The goal of this study was to assess the relationship between the Five Factor Model of personality and the belief in the paranormal. Participants (N=307) were students from the University of Zagreb, Croatia. The measures used were the IPIP version of the Five Factor Model questionnaire (Goldberg et al., 2006) along with the Revised Paranormal Belief Scale (Tobacyk, 1988). The factor analysis of the latter yielded three previously unreported paranormal belief dimensions named: General paranormal belief, Traditional religious belief and Rituals and practices. The most significant personality correlations with all three factors were found for Openness, Conscientiousness and Neuroticism, respectively. Results to an extent confirmed previous findings, suggesting that open individuals accept more General paranormal beliefs, while rejecting Traditional religious belief. Conscientiousness displayed an opposite trend being positively related to Traditional religious beliefs and negatively to General paranormal beliefs. Finally, Neuroticism was related to Rituals and practices, i.e. superstitions, divinations and occult control of life outcomes. The relations are however modest ranging from 0.11 to 0.25. Both the novel paranormal belief structure and its significance to personality are discussed within an evolutionary perspective, with guidelines for further research highlighted in the end.
Keywords: paranormal beliefs, personality traits, Tobacyk, Five Factor Model, evolution
INTRODUCTION
From ghosts to haunted houses and lucky rabbit paws, paranormal beliefs are fascinating phenomena. Gallup polls show that about three in four Americans profess at least one paranormal belief (Moore, 2005)with possession by the devil (41%), extra sensory perception (41%) and haunting (37%) being currently most popular. In Croatia, although more moderate, the results differ only slightly, with one out of four people believing in the possession by the devil, lucky charms and ghosts (Marinovic-Jerolimov, 2005). However, despite a rather large body of research dealingwith these phenomena (for overview see Irwin, 1993, 2009), the contribution of personality to formation and persistence of paranormal beliefs has not been adequately explored. Studies on this subject have been hindered by conceptual and methodological issues, making the available findings for the most part uninterpretable and inconclusive. The aimof our study was therefore to explore the dimensionality of paranormal beliefs in a Croatian student sample and examine the relations between paranormal beliefs and personality dimensions using two of the most widely used and validated measures from both fields.
Paranormal beliefs; Defining and measuring an elusive concept
Psychological research into the paranormal is as old as psychology itself,withWilliamJames, known as the father ofNorth American psychology, being a prominent researcher into the supernatural (Coon, 1992). Regardless of its historic roots however, the progress of paranormal belief research has been slow, hindered mostly by the lack of consensus on the object of the study. What started as an exploration of superstitions (Irwin, 2009), soon grew to include various other constructs, but without any agreement over exactly what should be considered paranormal belief. The latest definition suggested they pertain to phenomena not empirically attested to the satisfaction of the scientific establishment, with the addition of arising in a broader community as a non-scientific commonsense endeavor to account for anomalous experiences (Irwin, 2009). Nevertheless, a consensus is far from being reached, making it a source of significant confusion between the researchers, and causing the construct of paranormal beliefs to be considered a methodological nightmare. A plethora of available questionnaires (e.g. Gallagher et al., 1994; Thalbourne & Delin, 1993; Tobacyk, 1988) also differ significantly in their views on dimensionality and the scope of paranormal beliefs. This in turn makes comparison of the findings from studies using different measures extremely questionable. Fortunately, a somewhat informal agreement has been achievedwith the (Revised) Paranormal Belief Scale (RPBS) conceived by Tobacyk and Milford (1983; Tobacyk, 1988) and used in most of the studies on paranormal belief since themid eighties (Irwin, 1993, 2009; Lawrence, 1995a). Out of a variety of items included, the authors (Tobacyk & Milford, 1983) extracted seven broad factors they considered to represent seven distinct dimensions of paranormal belief. The factors were named; Traditional religious beliefs, Psi,Witchcraft, Superstition, Spiritualism, Extraordinary life forms and Precognition. Although still accepted by some researchers, the seven dimensions were later thoroughly criticized for being logically and empirically unfounded (Lawrence, 1995a; 1995b). However, the liberal criteria of inclusion allowed for the scale to be considered a reliable taxonomy of paranormal beliefs, enabling various other researchers to explore the structure of the RPBS in search of broader and theoreticallymore valid dimensions. The suggestions varied from five (Lawrence & De Cicco, 1997; Lawrence et al., 1997), four (Hartman, 1999) and two (Lange et al., 2000), to even one latent factor (Thalbourne et al., 1995). Unfortunately, all of these proposed structures have been poorly empirically tested, and for the most part without any theoretical explanation. This made the dimensionality of paranormal beliefs still an open issue. Especially since there have been indications that the structure of paranormal beliefs, assessedwith RPBS, is culturally dependent (Díaz-Vilela & Álvarez-González, 2004; Tobacyk & Thomas, 1997).
Origins of paranormal beliefs and the role of personality
Besides the dimensionality, another significant issue in understanding paranormal beliefs are the highly inconsistent results regarding the relationship with various personality characteristics. Fivemajor factors of personality (Costa &McCrae, 1992) are amongst the most neglected domains in that aspect.Much of the early research with personality correlations of paranormal belief focused on smaller scope constructs such as locus of control (Tobacyk &Milford, 1983), and sensation seeking (Tobacyk &Milford, 1983) dogmatism (Alcock & Otis, 1980; Thalbourne et al., 1995), narcissism (Tobacyk &Mitchell, 1987) and even psychopathological indices like schizotipy (Goulding, 2005; Tobacyk and Wilkinson, 1990), manic-depression (Thalbourne & French, 1995) and schizophrenia (Thalbourne, 1994). However, with regard to the now widely accepted and almost paradigmatic, Five Factor Model of personality (Costa & Mc- Crae, 1992), there hasn't been much research that we could relate to or drawinstances from. Even studies dealingwith somewhat comparable dimensions of personality (e.g. Eysenck's PEN, 1991) have been few in between, and their findings were always highly inconsistent. For example, even though there has been some evidence on paranormal believers being more extraverted (Thalbourne & Haraldsson, 1980), all subsequent research using different measures of paranormal beliefs and personality found no such relation (e.g. Rattet & Bursik, 2001; Thalbourne et al., 1995).On the other hand, the dimension that has found themost support in being related to paranormal beliefs isNeuroticism.Windholz andDiamant (1974)were the first to characterize believers in paranormal as impulsive, reflective, neurotic and even schizoid, as opposed to controlled, practical, and well-adjusted non-believers. That finding was later replicated using various measures of both Neuroticism and paranormal belief (e.g. Gallagher et al., 1994; Thalbourne et al., 1995). In more recent studies, Neuroticism was found to be the best predictor of paranormal beliefs within Eysenck's three factor model (Williams et al., 2007), a significant predictor of both positive and negative superstitions (Wiseman & Watt, 2004), astrology (Fichten & Sunerton, 1983), and a useful predictor ofmagical-religious beliefs in a large Finnish sample (N=3261) (Lindeman & Aarnio, 2006). Despite the majority of confirming studies, some nevertheless found no relationship betweenNeuroticismand paranormal beliefs (e.g.MacDonald, 2000; Tobacyk, 1982). It is also worth noting that Neuroticism is one of the dimensions of the Five Factor model most consistently related to religiosity (Saroglou, 2002; Saroglou & Jaspard, 2000). Despite the statistically significant relation however, the direction has been both positive and negative, making it difficult to interpret. Studies exploring Conscientiousness and Agreeableness as correlates of paranormal belief have been somewhatmore consistent, albeit very scarce. Surprisingly, although those two dimensions play a major role in explaining the relationship between religiousness and personality (see Saroglou, 2002), and efficiently differentiate traditional religious beliefs frommore spiritual approaches to religion (Saucier, 2000; Saucier & Skrzypiska, 2006), their relation to other paranormal beliefs has not been well documented. In those rare studies only Conscientiousness was shown to have a negative relation to paranormal belief (i.e. Egan et al., 1999). Finally, Openness to experience, characterized by receptiveness to new ideas, approaches, and experiences (McCrae & Costa, 1997), was also fairly consistently shown to correlate with general paranormal belief (Egan et al., 1999; MacDonald, 2000; Smith et al., 2009). However, as with all other dimensions, the diverse measures of both personality and paranormal beliefs used, along with a small number of studies conducted, make it impossible to conclusively determine the relevance of this personality dimension in explaining and predicting paranor- mal beliefs.
Due to the above stated issues, the two problems addressed in our study were; the structure of the RPBS; and the relation between paranormal belief and personality. The first problem is an important one, since there is still little agreement between the researchers on the number of dimensions defining paranormal beliefs. Overestimating as well as underestimating the number of distinct constructswithin the RPBS could pose a threat in an attempt to find real relationships with personality dimensions and explain themin a theoretically sound way.With regard to the second problem, we can put forth several hypotheses. Firstly, due to the lack of significant findings in recent studies, conducted with somewhat comparable measures (e.g. Williams et al., 2007), we do not expect to find a significant relationship between paranormal beliefs and extraversion. Furthermore, asAgreeableness andConscientiousness dimensions have seldom been studied outside of traditional religious beliefs, our inferences can be made mostly on the basis of prior religiosity research. In that regard, we could expect Conscientiousness to be positively correlated to traditional religious beliefs (Saroglou, 2002). This is presumed due to conscientious people being characterized by impulse control, need for orderliness, and low flexibility (Costa &McCrae, 1992). Also, since all other paranormal beliefs are usually labeled as unconventional or even bizarre, it is sound to presume that negative correlation would be found between those scales and Conscientiousness (Egan et al., 1999). Regarding Agreeableness, which emphasizes both compliance and prosocial tendencies, we could presume that if adherence to traditional authority- based religion were strongly normative in a culture, endorsement of attitudes representing doctrines of this religion should become highly desirable for individuals and thus more related to this dimension (Saucier & Skrzypiska, 2006). Since the population of Croatia is predominantly Catholic, the measurement of tradition-oriented religiousness should become strongly affected by desirability responding, and thus related to an extent to Agreeableness. Furthermore, the prosocial tendencies displayed by the Agreeableness dimension, should manifest themselves through a benevolent worldview promoted by various spiritual claims. These presumptions are supported by the prior positive relationship with both traditional and spiritual dimensions of religiosity (Saroglou, 2002; Saucier, 2000; Saucier & Skrzypiska, 2006), and could imply a positive relation of Agreeableness with Traditional religious belief and Spiritualismsubscales of RPBS. Since it has been implied how a need for security could be the underlying cause of more neurotic people adopting paranormal beliefs (Thalbourne et al., 1995), we also expect to find a significant correlation between Neuroticismand paranormal beliefs.More specifically, Neuroticism is presumed to be most strongly correlated with Superstition, Traditional religious beliefs and possibly Precognition, as those subscales imply a certain determination, predictability and a sense of control. However, Neuroticism has also been negatively related to Religious fundamentalism and Orthodoxy (Saroglou, 2002), making it difficult to formulate adequate predictions on the direction of this relationship. Finally, a more precise hypothesis can be made in regard to theOpenness dimension. This dimension has been the most important predictor of religiosity (Saucier & Skrzypinska, 2006), and paranormal beliefs (Egan et al., 1999; Mac- Donald, 2000; Smith et al., 2009) within the Five FactorModel. Presumably, receptiveness to new ideas, approaches, and experiences as captured by the Openness factor (McCrae & Costa, 1997), leads away from traditionally rigid religious beliefs and towardmore stimulating beliefs andworldviews.We therefore expect Openness to be negatively related to Traditional paranormal beliefwhile positively related to all concepts regarding Psi, Spirituality, Precognition, and Extraordinary life forms.
METHOD
Participants
The participants were 307 undergraduate students fromthe University of Zagreb, of whom88.3%were women. The average age of participants was 21.9 years (SD = 2.5, range 19 - 45). All participantswere students of social and humanistic disciplines, most of themstudying psychology (N=217) and educational-rehabilitation sciences (N=59). The majority (78.4%) reported being Christian, with 16.1% being atheist or agnostic, and the rest reported some other religious affiliation. The study was conducted in classroomsettings. Subjects were approached at the beginning of the class and asked to participate in the study. Participation was voluntary, and the subjects willing to participate were given enough time to fill out includedmeasures. Although fairly common and comparable to most of the studies on this subject, we must note that this was a convenience sample which could have an effect on the results.
Measures
Paranormal beliefs were measured using the Revised Paranormal Belief Scale (RPBS) by Tobacyk (1988). The 26-itemscale provides a result on seven dimensions of paranormal beliefs: Traditional religious belief (e.g. There is a devil), Psi (e.g. A person's thoughts can influence the movement of a physical object), Witchcraft (e.g.Witches do exist), Superstition (e.g. Black cats can bring bad luck), Spiritualism (e.g. Your mind and soul can leave your body and travel), Extraordinary life forms (e.g. The Loch Ness monster of Scotland exists) and Precognition (e.g. Astrology is the way to accurately predict the future). The responses are scored on a seven-point Likert scale. Higher rating indicates stronger endorsement on all but one item from the Psi subscale (23. Mind reading is not possible) which in turn needs to be negatively scored (Tobacyk &Milford, 1983). The sumof all items is indicative of a general tendency to adopt paranormal belief, while subscales can be used to measure specific dimensions of paranormal belief (Tobacyk, 1988). Since this was the first use of this scale in Croatia, in order to assure the adequacy of translation, a standard translation/back-translation procedure was conducted by the authors. During this procedure none of the items were perceived as culturally unrecognizable and unfamiliar, therefore no itemswere replaced. The only slight reformulation was in renaming the "Abominable snowman of Tibet" to "Yeti", since the former has no direct translation in Croatian.
Personality factors were assessed by a 50-item International Personality ItemPool (IPIP, Goldberg et al., 2006; http://ipip. ori.org/) aimed tomeasure Costa andMcCrae's (1992) Five Factor Model. This measure consists of 50 short statements in which every dimension:Neuroticism, Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and Openness, are represented by 10 items. We decided to use the Five Factor oriented IPIP measure, in order to include the Openness dimension, which in the Big Five structure is replaced by Intellect. Although there is a significant overlap between the two dimensions (Saucier, 1992), we chose the former due to the number of existing studies regarding its relationship to the paranormal beliefs. The IPIP measure was validated on a Croatian sample by Mlacic and Goldberg (2007), while Gracanin, Kardum and Krapic (2004) adapted the Openness scale. Both adaptations replicated previous findings on the dimensionality of the measures, and showed high reliabilities of all the subscales on the Croatian sample.
RESULTS
Factor structure of the RPBS (Tobacyk, 1988)
We focused on the exploration of the original Tobacyk's seven factor structure using the principal axis factor analysis with oblique rotation. In the original study by Tobacyk andMilford (1983), the authors used varimax rotation in their analysis.However, due to subsequent criticism showing high scale intercorrelations, alongwith lack of theoretical explanationwhichwould support the orthogonality (Lange et al., 2000; Lawrence et al., 1997; Lawrence & De Cicco, 1997), a consensus on scales not being orthogonal has been reached (Lawrence et al., 1997; Tobacyk & Thomas, 1997).
As hypothesized, our analysis yielded an interpretable seven factor oblique structure accounting for 69.7% of the variance, which is in accordance with previous assertions (Tobacyk & Milford, 1983).1 Furthermore, most of the items loaded on their intended factors with the exception of items that have been problematic in the past studies. More specifically, item 23 from the Psi subscale, being the only item in negative form, which was not correlated with the rest of the scale in a Spanish sample (Díaz-Vilela & Álvarez-González, 2004); items 26 and 21 are both part of the Precognition subscale extensively criticized by Lawrence (1995a) for being composed of two different factors (Tobacyk et al., 1989) mainly precognitive systems (i.e. astrology) and gifted persons (i.e. clairvoyants); and item 20 (There is life on other planets) from the Extraordinary life forms subscale that was argued not to represent paranormal belief but a scientific probability (Lawrence, 1995a). However, the seven factor structure did not prove to be the most suitable factor solution for our data due to both the Scree test (Cattell, 1966) and Horn's (1965) Parallel analysis criteria. We noted a clear discontinuity in the sizes of the eigenvalues between the third and the fourth factor (Figure 1), indicating that the three factor structure should be extracted. Thiswas also confirmed by subsequent parallel analysis that showed all eigenvalues after the third could have been produced by chance. We therefore proceeded with the extraction of the three factors using the principal axis factor analysis with oblique rotation.
This factor analysis (Table 1) yielded an interpretable three factor structure explaining 52.47% of the total variance. Upon examination of the itemstructure, the three factorswere named General paranormal belief (GPB), Traditional religious belief (TRB), and Rituals and practices (RAP).
In this solution, the first factor consists mostly of items from Psi and Spiritualism subscales, with Precognition,Witchcraft and Extraordinary life forms items partially included. The second factor contains only the items from Tobacyk's (1988) Traditional religious beliefs scale, whilst the third factor contains items fromSuperstition subscale and the rest of the items from the Precognition,Witchcraft and Extraordinary life forms subscales. The first and the third factor were most strongly correlated (0.42, p< 0.001). The correlation between the first and the second factor were smaller but still significant (0.16, p< 0.001), whilst the second and the third factor are not significantly correlated (0.10, p> 0.05). The internal consistency reliability estimates (Cronbach alpha) of the three extracted scales ranged from 0.90 for General paranormal belief to 0.84 for Traditional religious belief and 0.80 for Rituals and practices. The reliabilities of seven RPBS subscales calculated according to Tobacyk (1988) were also within acceptable levels, being the highest for Traditional religious beliefs (0.84) and Psi (0.81) while Extraordinary life forms showed rather low reliability (0.53). The Five Factor structure was replicated similarly to previous studies (Mlacic & Goldberg, 2007) with the five factors explaining 47.46% of variance. The reliabilities for each of the Five Factor domains were high, ranging from 0.90 for Conscientiousness to 0.72 for Openness and are comparable to those reported by Mlacic and Goldberg (2007).
Correlations of the two RPBS (Tobacyk, 1988) factor solutions and the Five Factor Personality dimensions
The correlations of personality factors and the scales derived from the seven factor solution (Table 2) show a significant, albeit weak, relationship. As presumed, Conscientiousness,Neuroticism and Openness show highest correlations with paranormal beliefs.More specifically, Conscientiousness confirmed the predictions of being related positively to Traditional religiosity (0.19), and negatively to Psi belief (-0.13). Neuroticism was shown to be positively correlated to Superstition (0.19), Extraordinary life forms (0.16) and Precognition (0.14). Finally, the correlations between personality and paranormal beliefs were the highest for the Openness factor. Its negative relationship with Traditional religious beliefs (-0.25), and positive with belief in Psi (0.16) and Spiritualism (0.25) replicated, to an extent, the findings by Smith et al. (2009). Agreeableness has shown meaningful correlation only with Traditional religious beliefs (0.15).
However, when examining the correlations of personality with the three factor structure of paranormal beliefs (Table 3) the dynamic becomes more evident and easily interpretable. Neuroticism was significantly related to Rituals and practices (RAP) factor (0.23) since that factor subdued all its significant correlates from the seven factor structure. Conscientiousness retained its negative relationship to Psi through the General paranormal belief (GPB) factor (-0.13), while the positive relationship with Traditional religious belief (TRB) remained nearly the same (0.17), as the new factor consisted of all the items from Tobacyk's (1988) Traditional religious beliefs subscale. Agreeableness andOpenness preserved their relationship to TRB as well, being 0.13 and -0.25 respectively. Openness was also positively related to the GPB (0.19), presumably since GPB includes items from both Spirituality and Psi domains. Finally, a significant correlation between Extraversion and GPB emerged in this structure.
DISCUSSION
In this paper we set out to explore the dimensionality of paranormal beliefs in a Croatian student sample and examine the relations between paranormal beliefs and personality using two of the most widely used and validated measures from both domains.Most of our initial hypotheses regarding relation between personality and paranormal beliefs were proven true, with some of the previously non established relations opening room for novel approaches to the subject. As in most of the previous empirical research on the dimensionality of the RPBS, we were able to replicate the seven factor structure suggested by the authors (Tobacyk & Milford, 1983). The problematic items mostly related to the previously criticized Precognition scale (Lawrence, 1995a) and other items that could be easily misinterpreted as non-paranormal statements (e.g. There is life on other planets). However, another, more parsimonious structure of paranormal belief was shown to exist. This finding is in accord with a large number of studies that either presumed a lesser number of factors on the basis of the logical analysis (Lawrence, 1995a; 1995b), or empirical studies suggesting that a lesser number of factors could provide a better fit of the data (e.g.Hartman, 1999). After analyzing the data, both the Scree test (Cattell, 1966) and parallel analysis (Horn, 1965) suggested the data supported three factors. Upon extracting those factors, we found the items distributed meaningfully and most significantly on the three factors, with no items being left out or having dual projections. This finding was in accord with the proposition by the authors (Tobacyk & Thomas, 1997) that the seven RPBS dimensions do not necessarily represent the "correct" structure of paranormal beliefs, but rather a construction dependent on a socio-cultural context and the level of analysis selected by the investigator. Each of the seven RPBS dimensions can therefore be represented by higher-order dimensions that might prove to be more meaningful and useful in construction of an integrative model of the explored domain (Tobacyk, 1995). Tobacyk & Thomas (1997) also state that together with statistical indicators, the new paranormal belief dimensions should also have a heuristic value and be able to explain important psychological and behavioral processes of the person in order for a dimensionality to be considered relevant. We will therefore address these issues through the findings of this study. In regard to the seven factor structure (Table 2), significant correlationswere found between all the personality dimensions we presumed to be related to paranormal beliefs. More specifically, Neuroticism, Conscientiousness and Openness confirmed the hypothesis regarding their relation to Superstition, Traditional religious beliefs, and Psi, respectively. Some results however were not predicted, as for instance the positive relation between Neuroticism and Extraordinary life forms, or the lack of negative correlation between Conscientiousness and the rest of the paranormal belief subscales. The lack of significant relations between Openness and paranormal subscales beyond Spiritualism and Psi were also not predicted, for it was believed that the search for exciting and non conformist ideas, inherent to the Openness factor (McCrae & Costa, 1997), would leave them more prone to such belief as precognition and extraordinary life forms. Findings regarding Agreeableness also did not support fully our hypothesis since it was significantly related only to Traditional religious belief. Taking this into consideration, we can better understand our three factor model and how it might indicate a novel way of understanding the paranormal belief structure and its relation to personality. The newly formed General paranormal belief (GPB) factor consists of Psi and Spiritualism scales, along with other items from the Witchcraft, Precognition and Extraordinary life forms scales that were not explicitly related to a person (i.e. a witch), a system (i.e. astrology) or to an extent to a creature (i.e. Loch Ness monster). Upon reviewing the included items, it is evident how this factor combines all the non explicit paranormal beliefs that focus on an idea of non physical transcendent energy. Traditional religious belief factor retained the items of Tobacyk's Traditional religious beliefs (TRB) scale, indicating a strong independence of religious beliefs from other paranormal beliefs. Finally, Rituals and practices (RAP) factor contains Superstition subscale along with items explicitly depicting Witchcraft (i.e. Witches do exist), divination systems (e.g. Astrology is a way to accurately predict the future) and a cryptozoological creature (i.e. Loch Ness monster). It is worth noticing how this division separated meaningfully the previously questioned scale of Precognition (Lawrence, 1995a) and also implied a structural issue for theWitchcraft scale. Division of the Extraordinary life forms scale however, was treated as a random fluctuation, rather than a meaningful division due to extremely low support for both cryptozoological claims. Bearing all this in mind, the possible significance of personality in explaining the structure of our three factor model can be seen through comparison of the results reported in Tables 2 and 3. There we can see how all dimensions of paranormal belief that were relevant to a single of the three most important personality factors in this relation (i.e. Neuroticism, Conscientiousness and Openness) went on to constitute a higher order paranormal belief factor. The dimensionality of paranormal beliefs could therefore easily be regarded through its relevance to personality. Starting from Neuroticism, it is presumed that the mediator between the two is the need for a sense of understanding and meaning of the physical and social world (Alcock, 1981). Anxious individuals would therefore have a strong need for control. In an attempt to overcome perceived uncertainty in their surroundings, they would be more susceptible to superstitions since they provide an illusory sense of control over life's unpredictable events (Irwin, 2000). This relation between Neuroticism and superstition has been repeatedly confirmed (Williams et al., 2007; Wiseman & Watt, 2004), however, due to the traditional division of scales by Tobacyk (1988), the wider picture could have eluded us. For example, sinceNeuroticismwas related to RAP factor (0.23, p?0.001), we could hypothesize that this factor represents aspects of paranormal beliefs relevant for anxiousness regulation. It included beliefs in witches and astrology which, along with Superstition, imply a possibility of predicting and controlling the outsideworld and avoiding negative life outcomes (e.g., knocking on wood, asking for a miracle cure or looking for a job only when Jupiter aligns with Mars). Should we accept that interpretation, it becomes clear how there could be a deeper, more salient structure hidden behind those descriptive paranormal dimensions, and it might very well be related to personality. In regard to Conscientiousness, we have confirmed our hypothesis of positive relation to Traditional religious belief (0.17, p?0.001). This presumably stems from the characteristics of conscientious people;mainly orderliness, self-discipline, self-control, impulse control, desirability, conformity, restraint (Costa &McCrae, 1992) and even orthodoxy (Saroglou, 2002). Also, since the church explicitly judges all other forms of spiritualism and alternate religiousness, negative relationships of Conscientiousness with General paranormal belief (-0.13, p?0.005) were rightfully expected. Here again we might see how, by using the three factors, no valuable information in regard to personality was lost as Tobacyk's (1988) original subscale related with Conscientiousness remained intact. Finally, in accordance with our hypothesis, Openness was related to Psi (0.16, p?0.001) and Spiritualism (0.35, p?0.001), while being negatively related to Traditional religious beliefs (-0.26, p?0.001), presumably through receptiveness to new ideas, preference for novelty, intellectual curiosity and liberal values (Costa & McCrae, 1992). This relation was further replicated in the three-factor paranormal structure without loss of meaningful information, as both Psi and Spiritualismformed a unitary factor of General paranormal belief and the Traditional religious beliefs factor remained the same as in the seven factor solution. Although we presumed that Openness would to some extent be related to all paranormal beliefs, it was related only to the subscales later included in the General paranormal belief factor, implying ameaningful, possibly personality driven latent structure that goes beyond simple descriptive categories.
Finally,we propose howan evolutionary viewpointmight prove useful in enlightening this division and its reflection upon personality dimensions.More specifically, we will attempt to view the three factors of paranormal belief and their relation to personality through three currently proposed interpretations of the evolution of paranormal ideation that can roughly be labeled as - byproduct, exaptation and adaptation. It is our belief that only with adopting findings from evolutionary religious studies can we hope to offer a meaningful hierarchical and integrative model of paranormal beliefs, and its relation to personality dimensions. First, we will view it through the research implying the innateness of paranormal beliefs (e.g. Boyer, 1994, 2003; Barrett, 2000). Founded on well defined, cognitive determinants instead of hypothetical ontological domains, this approach utilizes knowledge from developmental, evolutionary and cognitive psychology. Within it, all paranormal, superstitious and magical beliefs are interpreted as a by-product of cognitive processes and as ontological confusions of our core knowledge about the world;mainly, our intuitive physics, intuitive biology and intuitive psychology (Lindeman & Aarnio, 2007), implying how they will in some form be present without any social influence or interpersonal need. This concept of ontological confusions, however, precisely sums up the wide variety of paranormal beliefs that are represented in the newly formed General paranormal beliefs dimension. It is interesting to note how certain personality dimensions, mainly Openness, are attracted to this sort of imagination, and still very little is known on the interaction between the personality and our supernaturalizing propensity. On the second level of this evolutionary analysis, we focus on the active role paranormal ideas and beliefs have taken in order to secure meaning to individuals able to perceive them. Norenzayan and Hansen (2006) offer a good examplewith an evolutionary adoption of the terrormanagement theory (Greenberg et al., 1997), implying how, after evolving to self-consciousness, humans faced paralyzing consequences of fear of death. It is proposed that the most "fit" individuals adapted by using our pre-existing propensity toward supernatural and creating non-realistic but comforting beliefs that allow them to survive or find comfort. Again, a parallel could be drawn to our third factor, Rituals and practices, which seemingly describes precisely that aspect of paranormal beliefs. Its relevance to anxiety control is shown by significant correlation with Neuroticism. Finally, the third level is our capacity for supernatural ideation and its relation to organized religion. This evolutionary view promotes the idea how our ability to perceive a divine force, which prescribed and controlled a set of strict rules, could have been a necessary prerequisite for cooperation and prevention of cheating behavior in large social groups. Thus, our supernaturalizing propensity is viewed as a possible source of fitness in a competitive social and cultural scene (e.g. Alcorta & Sosis, 2005; Bering et al., 2005; Norenzayan & Shariff, 2008; Wilson, 2002). We can see how this aspect of paranormal ideation has also emerged as a distinct factor in our structure (Traditional religious belief), again relating to one specific personality trait; i.e. Conscientiousness. Since the role of, not only paranormal beliefs, but also personality dimensions is already recognized through an evolutionary perspective (Buss, 1991, 2009), this relation might prove relevant and it is our suggestion for it to be further explored in the future. If a division of paranormal beliefs, according to the above stated evolutionary perspectives, is to be confirmed, we might finally approach a broad and meaningful theory of paranormal beliefs, as well as shed more light on evolutionary origins of personality structure. This integration could also once more enable us to conclude how, as Dobzhansky (1973) once stated for biology - nothing in psychology makes sense except in the light of evolution.
There are however some limitations to our study. First and foremost, we point to the limitations of our sample, all being drawn from a rather young and educated population, who for various educational and intellectual reasonsmight approach the subject of paranormal beliefs in a specific manner. Furthermore, our conclusions were both determined and limited by the measures used. Although the RPBS captures a wide sample of paranormal beliefs, there is a good reason to believe that many of those beliefs are left out, as for example alternativemedicine beliefs, various omens of luck, vital energy beliefs and even food related paranormal beliefs (Lindeman & Aarnio, 2006; Lindeman et al., 2000). Further studies should work towards implementation of both global and culture specific items in order to provide more validity to these conclusions. As for personality, a facet level approach might prove useful in plotting specificmechanisms within a trait, responsible for paranormal belief acquisition. Also, wemust note how the relations between the two domains were modest; making the exploration on a wider sample and the use of more specific, facet level measures even more necessary. Finally, since our theoretical venture in this paper to an extent surpasses the implications of our data, it is evident that a more intensive work on this aspect of paranormal belief is needed, and the integration of the two strongly emerging fields of cognitive and evolutionary psychology with the psychology of individual differences seems to be a promising path. Apart from personality measures, future researchers into this evolutionary-individualist paradigm would do well to include other already well explored and evolutionary relevant factors (e.g. intelligence, gender, mating value, etc.) Hopefully, it will be able to bring us closer to understanding this complex and intriguing human characteristic and help us finally enlighten what Sagan (1995) prosaically depicted as the "demon haunted world".
Paranormalna vjerovanja i osobine licnosti u Hrvatskoj
Igor MIKLOUSIC, Boris MLACIC, Goran MILAS
Institut drustvenih znanosti Ivo Pilar, Zagreb
Cilj ove studije bilo je istrazivanje povezanosti peterofaktorskoga modela licnosti i paranormalnih vjerovanja. Sudionici istrazivanja (N=307) bili su studenti Sveucilista u Zagrebu (Hrvatska). Od mjera je primijenjena IPIP verzija upitnika peterofaktorskoga modela (Goldberg i sur., 2006.) te revidirana skala paranormalnih vjerovanja (Tobacyk, 1988.). Faktorska analiza revidirane skale paranormalnih vjerovanja upozorila je na, do sada nezabiljezenu, trofaktorsku strukturu, pri cemu su dobivene dimenzije nazvane: generalna paranormalna vjerovanja, tradicionalna religijska vjerovanja i rituali i prakse. Najznacajnije povezanosti licnosti s dimenzijama paranormalnih vjerovanja nadene su za faktore otvorenosti, savjesnosti i neuroticizma. Rezultati su djelomicno potvrdili prijasnje nalaze, pokazujuci sklonost otvorenijih pojedinaca prema generalnim paranormalnim vjerovanjima i odbacivanju tradicionalnih religijskih vjerovanja. Dimenzija savjesnosti pokazala se pozitivno povezana s tradicionalnim religijskim, a negativno s generalnim paranormalnim vjerovanjima. Neuroticizam se pokazao povezan s ritualima i praksama, tj. praznovjerjem, proricanjem i okultnim praksama. Povezanosti su se pokazale umjerenima (0,11 - 0,25), a novootkrivena struktura paranormalnih vjerovanja i njezina povezanost s osobinama licnosti promatrana je kroz evolucijsku perspektivu. Ogranicenja i smjernice za buduce istrazivace naznacene su na kraju.
Kljucne rijeci: paranormalna vjerovanja, osobine licnosti, Tobacyk, peterofaktorski model, evolucija
1 Due to space limitations, the seven- -factor structure of paranormal beliefs was omitted from the text. Interested researchers can obtain detailed results on the psychometric properties of the RPBS from the first author upon request.
Alcock, J. E. (1981), Parapsychology: Science or Magic? A Psychological Perspective, Elmsford, Pergamon Press.
Alcock, J. E. and Otis, L. P. (1980), Critical Thinking and Belief in the Paranormal. Psychological Reports, 46: 479-482. doi:10.2466/pr0.1980. 46.2.479
Alcorta, C. S. and Sosis, R. (2005), Ritual, Emotion, and Sacred Symbols: The Evolution of Religion as an Adaptive Complex. Human Nature, 16 (4): 323-359. doi:10.1007/s12110-005-1014-3
Barrett, J. L. (2000), Exploring the Natural Foundations of Religion. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4 (1): 29-34. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99) 01419-9
Bering, J. M., McLeod, K. A. and Shackelford, T. K. (2005), Reasoning about Dead Agents Reveals Possible Adaptive Trends. Human Nature, 16 (4): 360-381. doi:10.1007/s12110-005-1015-2
Boyer, P. (1994), The Naturalness of Religious Ideas. A Cognitive Theory of Religion, Berkeley-Los Angeles, University of California Press.
Boyer, P. (2003), Religious Thought and Behaviour as By-Products of Brain Function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7 (3): 119-124. doi:10. 1016/S1364-6613(03)00031-7
Buss, D. M. (1991), Evolutionary Personality Psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 42: 459-491. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.42.020191.002331
Buss, D. M. (2009), How Can Evolutionary Psychology Successfully Explain Personality and Individual Differences? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4 (4): 359-366. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01138.x
Cattell, R. B. (1966), The Scree Test for the Number of Factors. Multivariate Behaviour Research, 1 (2): 245-276. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr 0102_10
Coon, D. J. (1992), Testing the Limits of Sense and Science: American Experimental Psychologists Combat Spiritualism, 1880-1920. American Psychologist, 47 (2): 143-151. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.47.2.143
Costa, P. T. and McCrae, R. R. (1992), Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and the NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Odessa, FL, Psychological Assessment Resources.
Díaz-Vilela, L. Y. and Álvarez-González, C. J. (2004), Differences in Paranormal Beliefs across Fields of Study from a Spanish Adaptation of Tobacyk's RPBS. Journal of Parapsychology, 68 (2): 405-421.
Dobzhansky, T. (1973), Nothing in BiologyMakes Sense Except in the Light of Evolution. The American Biology Teacher, 35: 125-129.
Egan, V., Auty, J.,Miller, R., Ahmadi, S., Richardson, C. and Gargan, I. (1999), Sensational Interests and General Personality Traits. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 10 (3): 567-582. doi:10.1080/09585189908402160
Eysenck, H. J. (1991), Dimensions of Personality: 16, 5, or 3? - Criteria for a Taxonomic Paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences, 12 (8): 773-790. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(91)90144-Z
Fichten, C. S. and Sunerton, B. (1983), Popular Horoscopes and the "Barnum Effect." Journal of Psychology, 114 (1): 123-134. doi:10.1080/ 00223980.1983.9915405
Gallagher, C., Kumar, V. K. and Pekala, R. J. (1994), The Anomalous Experiences Inventory: Reliability and Validity. Journal of Parapsychology, 58: 402-428.
Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H.W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R. and Gough, H. G. (2006), The International Personality Item Pool and the Future of Public-Domain Personality Measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40 (1): 84-96. doi:10.1016/j.jrp. 2005.08.007
Goulding, A. (2005), Healthy Schizotypy in a Population of Paranormal Believers and Experients. Personality and Individual Differences, 38 (5): 1069-1083. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.07.006
Gracanin, A., Kardum, I. and Krapic, N. (2004), Odnos pridjevske i upitnicke mjere dimenzija petofaktorskog modela licnosti [The Relation between Adjective and Questionnaire Measures of the Five Factor Model of Personality Dimensions]. Psihologijske teme, 13 (1): 33-46.
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S. and Pyszczynski, T. (1997), Terror Management Theory of Self-Esteem and Cultural Worldviews: Empirical Assessments and Conceptual Refinements. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 29: 61-139. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60016-7
Hartman, S. E. (1999), Another View of the Paranormal Belief Scale. The Journal of Parapsychology, 63 (2): 131-141.
Horn, J. L. (1965), A Rationale and Test for the Number of Factors in Factor Analysis. Psychometrika, 30 (2): 179-185. doi:10.1007/BF02289447
International Personality Item Pool. A Scientific Collaboratory for the Development of Advanced Measures of Personality Traits and Other Individual Differences. http://ipip.ori.org/ (19. 2. 2010)
Irwin, H. J. (1993), Belief in the Paranormal: A Review of the Empirical Literature. Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 87 (1): 1-39.
Irwin, H. J. (2000), Belief in the Paranormal and a Sense of Control over Life. European Journal of Parapsychology, 15: 68-78.
Irwin, H. J. (2009), The Psychology of Paranormal Belief: A Researcher's Handbook, Hertfordshire, University of Hertfordshire Press.
Lange, R., Irwin, H. J. and Houran, J. (2000), Top-Down Purification of Tobacyk's Revised Paranormal Belief Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 29 (1): 131-156. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00183-X
Lawrence, T. R. (1995a), How Many Factors of Paranormal Belief are There?: A Critique of the PBS. Journal of Parapsychology, 58: 3-25.
Lawrence, T. R. (1995b),Moving on from the Paranormal Belief Scale: A Final Reply to Tobacyk. Journal of Parapsychology, 59: 131-140.
Lawrence, T. R. and De Cicco, P. (1997), The Factor Structure of the Paranormal Belief Scale: More Evidence in Support of the Oblique Five. Journal of Parapsychology, 61 (3): 243-251.
Lawrence, T. R., Roe, C. A. and Williams, C. (1997), Confirming the Factor Structure of the Paranormal Beliefs Scale: Big Orthogonal Seven or Oblique Five? The Journal of Parapsychology, 61 (1): 13-31.
Lindeman, M. and Aarnio, K. (2006), Paranormal Beliefs: Their Dimensionality and Correlates. European Journal of Personality, 20 (7): 585-602. doi:10.1002/per.608
Lindeman, M. and Aarnio, K. (2007), Superstitious, Magical, and Paranormal Beliefs: An IntegrativeModel. Journal of Research in Personality, 41 (4): 731-744. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.06.009
Lindeman, M., Keskivaara, P. and Roschier, M. (2000), Assessment of Magical Beliefs about Food and Health. Journal of Health Psychology, 5 (2): 195-209. doi:10.1177/135910530000500210
MacDonald, D. A. (2000), Spirituality: Description,Measurement, and Relation to the Five FactorModel of Personality. Journal of Personality, 68 (1): 153-197. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.t01-1-00094
Marinovic Jerolimov, D. (2005), Tradicionalna religioznost u Hrvatskoj 2004.: izmedu kolektivnoga i individualnoga [Traditional Religiosity in Croatia in 2004: Between the Collective and Individual. Sociologija sela, 43 (2): 303-338.
McCrae, R. R. and Costa, P. T., Jr. (1997), Conceptions and Correlates of Openness to Experience. In: R. Hogan, J. A. Johnson and S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of Personality Psychology (pp. 825-847). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B978-012134645-4/50032-9
Mlacic, B. and Goldberg, L. R. (2007), An Analysis of a Cross-Cultural Personality Inventory: The IPIP Big-Five Factor Markers in Croatia. Journal of Personality Assessment, 88 (2): 168-177. doi:10.1080/00223890 701267993
Moore, D. W. (2005.), Three in Four Americans Believe in Paranormal. Gallup Poll, Princeton, NJ: Gallup. http://www.gallup.com/poll/16915/ Three-Four-Americans-Believe-Paranormal.aspx (16. 4. 2010)
Norenzayan, A. and Hansen, I. G. (2006), Belief in Supernatural Agents in the Face of Death. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32 (2): 174-187. doi:10.1177/0146167205280251
Norenzayan, A. and Shariff, A. F. (2008), The Origin and Evolution of Religious Prosociality. Science, 322 (5898): 58-62. doi:10.1126/science. 1158757
Rattet, S. L. and Bursik, K. (2001), Investigating the Personality Correlates of Paranormal Belief and Precognitive Experience. Personality and Individual Differences, 31 (3): 433-444. doi:10.1016/S0191- 8869(00)00148-3
Sagan, C. (1995), The Demon-Haunted World: Science as a Candle in the Dark, New York, Random House.
Saroglou, V. (2002), Religion and the Five Factors of Personality: AMeta- -Analytic Review. Personality and Individual Differences, 32 (1): 15-25. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00233-6
Saroglou, V. and Jaspard, J.-M. (2000), Personality and Religion: From Eysenck's Taxonomy to the Five-Factor Model. Archive for the Psychology of Religion, 23 (1): 41-70. doi:10.1163/157361200X00050
Saucier, G. (1992), Openness versus Intellect: Much Ado about Nothing? European Journal of Personality, 6 (5): 381-386. doi:10.1002/per .2410060506
Saucier, G. (2000), Isms and the Structure of Social Attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78 (2): 366-385. doi:10.1037//0022-3514. 78.2.366
Saucier, G. and Skrzypinska, K. (2006), Spiritual But Not Religious? Evidence for Two Independent Dispositions. Journal of Personality, 74 (5): 1257-1292. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00409.x
Smith, C. L., Johnson, J. L. and Hathaway,W. (2009), Personality Contributions to Belief in Paranormal Phenomena. Individual Differences Research, 7: 85-96.
Thalbourne, M. A. (1994), Belief in the Paranormal and Its Relationship to Schizophrenia-Relevant Variables: A Confirmatory Study. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33 (1): 78-80. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1994.tb01097.x
Thalbourne, M. A. and Delin, P. S. (1993), A New Instrument for Measuring the Sheep-Goat Variable: Its Psychometric Properties and Factor Structure. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research, 59: 172-186.
Thalbourne, M. A., Dunbar, K. A. and Delin, P. S. (1995), An Investigation into Correlates of Belief in the Paranormal. Journal of the Society for Physical Research, 89: 215-231.
Thalbourne, M. A. and French, C. C. (1995), Paranormal Belief, Manic- Depressiveness, and Magical Ideation: A Replication. Personality and Individual Differences, 18 (2): 291-292. doi:10.1016/0191-8869 (94)00146-J
Thalbourne, M. A. and Haraldsson, E. (1980), Personality Characteristics of Sheep and Goats. Personality and Individual Differences, 1 (2): 180-185. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(80)90040-9
Tobacyk, J. J. (1982), Paranormal Belief and Trait Anxiety. Psychological Reports, 51: 861-862. doi:10.2466/pr0.1982.51.3.861
Tobacyk, J. J. (1988), A Revised Paranormal Belief Scale, Unpublished manuscript, Louisiana Tech University, Ruston, LA.
Tobacyk, J. J. (1995), Final Thoughts on Issues in the Measurement of Paranormal Beliefs. Journal of Parapsychology, 59: 141-145.
Tobacyk, J. J. and Milford, G. (1983), Belief in Paranormal Phenomena: Assessment Instrument Development and Implications for Personality Functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44 (5): 1029-1037. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.44.5.1029
Tobacyk, J. J. and Mitchell, T. (1987), Out-of-Body Experience Status as a Moderator of Effects of Narcissism on Paranormal Beliefs. Psychological Reports, 60: 440-442. doi:10.2466/pr0.1987.60.2.440
Tobacyk, J. J., Nagot, E. and Mitchell, T. (1989), Prediction of Future Events Scale: Assessment of Beliefs about Predicting the Future. The Journal of Social Psychology, 129 (6): 819-823. doi:10.1080/00224545. 1989.9712090
Tobacyk, J. J. and Thomas, A. (1997), How the Big Orthogonal Seven is Really the Oblique Seven. Journal of Parapsychology, 61: 337-342.
Tobacyk, J. J. and Wilkinson, L. V. (1990), Magical Thinking and Paranormal Beliefs. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 5: 255-264.
Williams, E., Francis, L. J. and Robbins, M. (2007), Personality and Paranormal Belief: A Study among Adolescents. Pastoral Psychology, 56 (1): 9-14. doi:10.1007/s11089-007-0094-x
Wilson, D. S. (2002), Darwin's Cathedral: Evolution, Religion, and the Nature of Society, Chicago, IL, University of Chicago Press.
Windholz, G. and Diamant, L. (1974), Some Personality Traits of Believers in Extraordinary Phenomena. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 3: 125-126.
Wiseman, R. andWatt, C. (2004),Measuring Superstitious Belief:Why Lucky Charms Matter. Personality and Individual Differences, 37 (8): 1533-1541. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.02.009
Igor MIKLOUSIC, Boris MLACIC, Goran MILAS
Institute of Social Sciences Ivo Pilar, Zagreb
UDK: 159.923.2(497.5):133
Izvorni znanstveni rad
Primljeno: 27. 9. 2010.
Igor Miklousic, Institute of Social Sciences Ivo Pilar, Marulicev trg 19/1, P. O. Box 277, 10 000 Zagreb, Croatia.
E-mail: [email protected]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Institut Drustvenih Znasnosti Ivo Pilar 2012
Abstract
The goal of this study was to assess the relationship between the Five Factor Model of personality and the belief in the paranormal. Participants (N=307) were students from the University of Zagreb, Croatia. The measures used were the IPIP version of the Five Factor Model questionnaire (Goldberg et al., 2006) along with the Revised Paranormal Belief Scale (Tobacyk, 1988). The factor analysis of the latter yielded three previously unreported paranormal belief dimensions named: General paranormal belief, Traditional religious belief and Rituals and practices. The most significant personality correlations with all three factors were found for Openness, Conscientiousness and Neuroticism, respectively. Results to an extent confirmed previous findings, suggesting that open individuals accept more General paranormal beliefs, while rejecting Traditional religious belief. Conscientiousness displayed an opposite trend being positively related to Traditional religious beliefs and negatively to General paranormal beliefs. Finally, Neuroticism was related to Rituals and practices, i.e. superstitions, divinations and occult control of life outcomes. The relations are however modest ranging from 0.11 to 0.25. Both the novel paranormal belief structure and its significance to personality are discussed within an evolutionary perspective, with guidelines for further research highlighted in the end. [PUBLICATION ABSTRACT]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer