About the Authors:
Masaya Suzuki
Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft
Affiliation: R&D Department, Tokai Optical Co., Ltd., Okazaki, Japan
Naoya Kumagai
Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft
Affiliation: R&D Department, Tokai Optical Co., Ltd., Okazaki, Japan
Koji Inui
Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Writing – original draft
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliations Department of Integrative Physiology, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Okazaki, Japan, Institute for Developmental Research, Aichi Human Service Center, Kasugai, Japan
ORCID logo http://orcid.org/0000-0001-5867-9990
Ryusuke Kakigi
Roles Writing – original draft
Affiliation: Department of Integrative Physiology, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Okazaki, Japan
Abstract
Photophobia is a common condition in which bright light causes an unpleasant feeling due to increased sensitivity to light. In addition to discomfort, photophobia may be accompanied by visual dysfunction. The present study was conducted in order to examine whether visual evoked cortical responses contribute to the assessment of visual dysfunction due to bright light. Visual evoked magnetic fields (VEFs) following the presentation of a uniform bright light of 200–3700 cd/m2 in the lower visual field were recorded in 10 healthy volunteers and the effects of five color lenses: yellow, blue, gray, green, and colorless, were examined. VEFs were subjected to a multi-dipole analysis that resulted in the separation of several source activities, including the retina, V1, V2, V6, and fusiform gyrus. Source activity in the retina corresponding to the ERG b-wave exhibited a reduced amplitude and elongated peak latency with the yellow lens. Its latency strongly correlated with transmittance at 450 nm. On the other hand, cortical activities in V1 and the fusiform gyrus were stronger with the yellow lens than with the other color and colorless lenses. Only blue-light blocking showed significant effects. The result showing that the yellow lens enhanced V1 and fusiform activities indicated that processing in these areas was improved when subjects used this lens. The combination of delayed retinal activity and increased visual cortex activity may be an objective indicator of the effects of a color lens on visual function.
Figures
Fig 6
Table 2
Fig 1
Fig 2
Fig 3
Fig 4
Table 1
Fig 5
Fig 6
Table 2
Fig 1
Fig 2
Fig 3
Citation: Suzuki M, Kumagai N, Inui K, Kakigi R (2018) Effects of color lenses on visual evoked magnetic fields following bright light. PLoS ONE 13(8): e0201804. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201804
Editor: José M. González-Méijome, Universidade do Minho, PORTUGAL
Received: December 21, 2017; Accepted: July 23, 2018; Published: August 2, 2018
Copyright: © 2018 Suzuki et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: All relevant data are within the paper.
Funding: This study was supported by the ImPACT Program (2015-PM11-23-01) of the Council for Science, Technology, and Innovation (Cabinet Office, Government of Japan) to MS, NK, and KI (http://www.jst.go.jp/impact/). Tokai Optical Co., Ltd., provided support in the form of salaries for MS and NK. The specific roles of these authors are articulated in the 'author contributions' section. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Background
Photophobia is a state of increased sensitivity to light in which light feels too bright and uncomfortable. Therefore, photophobia is an internal experience, similar to pain, and its objective assessment is difficult. Photophobia is not a rare symptom; it is associated with various conditions, including migraine, blepharospasm, eye diseases, such as dry eye, and mental diseases, such as panic disorder or depression (for a review, see [1]). Although photophobia is considered to represent abnormal activation in a specific neural circuit, the underlying mechanisms have not yet been elucidated in detail. In addition to the difficulties associated with its objective assessment, the treatment of photophobia using a tinted lens is problematic. Subjects with photophobia wear darkly tinted lenses [2]; however, one principal treatment of photophobia is to decrease the dark adapted state [1]. Therefore, objective assessments and the appropriate use of tinted lens are important. The wavelength of light has been shown to affect photophobia [3,4]. Stringham et al. assessed photophobia using electromyography and showed increased sensitivity to light with decreasing wavelengths [4].
Several neuroimaging studies examined photophobia with a focus on photophobia-related brain activation [5–7]. The findings of these studies provided information on the mechanisms underlying the perception of photophobia, for example, photophobia-induced activation in the so-called pain matrix [6]. In addition to discomfort and pain, photophobia may be accompanied by visual dysfunction, and distinctions must be made between them [8]. In the present study, we focused on the effects of colored lenses on cortical activation in visual areas in order to investigate the mechanisms responsible for visual disturbances caused by bright light. Visual evoked magnetic fields (VEFs) following flash stimuli were recorded in ten healthy subjects and compared among five color lens conditions: yellow, blue, gray, green, and non-color, using a 306-channel whole head MEG system.
Methods
The experiment was performed on ten (four females and six males) healthy volunteers, aged 25–48 years (35.0 ± 5.8) with normal corrected visual acuity (20/20) and without neurological and ophthalmic disorders. Among the ten subjects tested, one was emmetropic and the remaining nine were myopic. The mean spherical equivalent (S + C/2) was -3.2 ± 1.7 D for the left eye and -3.0 ± 1.5 D for the right. During the experiment, they were corrected to normal vision (20/20) with ophthalmic lenses. The present study was approved in advance by the Ethics Committee of the National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Okazaki, Japan, and written consent was obtained from all subjects.
Stimulus and recordings
Since it was not possible to use electric devices in a shielded room because of magnetic noise, the visual stimulus was presented by a digital light processing projector placed outside the shielded room (Mirage 2000, Christie Digital System Inc., Kitcherner, Canada) at an interval of 1575 ms ± 5%. The stimulus was a flash of 300 ms and was applied to the subjects through a small window at a distance of 2 m from the eye. The stimulus was applied to the lower visual field (Fig 1A), which evoked greater VEF responses than the upper visual field [9,10]. Fig 1B shows the power of the flash as a function of the wavelength measured by a spectral radiance meter (CS-2000, Konika Minolta Japan, Tokyo). Clear notches were observed at approximately 500 and 590 nm in the curve, and were attributed to the properties of the projector. VEFs were recorded as described elsewhere [10,11]. Magnetic signals were recorded with a filter of 0.1–200 Hz at a sampling rate of 1004 Hz. The window of analysis was from 100 ms before to 600 ms after the stimulus, and the prestimulus period was used as the DC baseline. Epochs with MEG signals larger than 2.7 pt/cm were rejected from averaging. One hundred epochs without artifacts were collected for each condition.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 1. Stimulus.
A, Stimulus. B, Relative spectral distribution of the light source.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201804.g001
Procedures
In Experiment 1, the effects of the strength of the light on VEFs were examined using stimuli of 3.7 × 103, 1.2 × 103, or 0.2 × 103 cd/m2. Stimuli were presented in a random order. In Experiment 2, VEFs following a flash of 3.7 × 103 cd/m2 were compared among five lens conditions: yellow (CCP LY, Tokai Optical Co., Ltd.), blue, gray, green, and control (colorless). The luminous transmittance of each color lens was adjusted to 70% of the light source. Fig 2 shows the spectral transmittance curve of each lens. Recordings for each lens condition were performed one by one, and the order was randomized across subjects.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 2. Spectral transmittance curves of color lenses.
A colorless lens and four color lenses were used. Luminous transmittances for the color lenses were adjusted to an optical density of 70% of the light source.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201804.g002
Analysis
A multi-dipole analysis was performed using the brain electric source analysis (BESA) software package (NeuroScan, Mclean, VA) in order to separate temporally overlapping sources as described elsewhere [11,12]. The location of the estimate dipoles was expressed in Talairach coordinates using BrainVoyager (QX 1.4, Brain Innovation BV, Maastricht, the Netherlands). The latency and amplitude of the source strength waveform obtained were measured for each source activity.
A one-way repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used for statistical comparisons of latency and amplitude among three intensity conditions (Experiment 1) or five lens conditions (Experiment 2) followed by paired t-tests between the colorless lens and color lenses corrected with Bonferroni adjustments for multiple comparisons. Due to large inter-individual variations in the data obtained, a Wilcoxon signed-rank test was also used in order to confirm the significance of differences. A p value less than 0.05 was considered to be significant. Data are expressed as the mean ± standard deviation.
Results
Experiment 1
Fig 3 shows an example of VEFs in a subject. Early activities were recorded in the frontal region in addition to the main MEG responses in the occipital area. The results of multi-dipole analyses revealed that early activity to originated from the eye. Based on the location of activation and temporal dynamics with a peak at approximately 60 ms, this activity was considered to correspond to the b-wave of electroretinograms (ERGs). The dipole for this activity was estimated to be located in the cornea in most subjects (Fig 3B). Since the b-wave reflects the positivity of the cornea relative to the posterior pole, the intracellular current in the anterior direction in the present study matched the electric field of the b-wave. As shown in Fig 3C, retinal source activity increased its amplitude with stronger light intensities as expected.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 3. VEFs induced by bright light.
A, Top view of MEG waveforms of a representative subject in Experiment 1. B, Location of the estimated dipole in the eye superimposed on a subject’s own MR image. C, Grand-averaged source strength waveforms of retinal activity.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201804.g003
Although activated locations in the occipital area and the number of dipoles necessary to explain the evoked response slightly differed among subjects, dipoles were generally estimated to be located in the eye, primary visual cortex (V1), V2, V6, and fusiform gyrus (FG). V1 activity, with an upward current peaking at approximately 100 ms (M100), was estimated to be located in the calcarine fissure. The source responsible for the second major component at approximately 160 ms (M160) was located in the bilateral FG on the roof of the cerebellum. V2 sources pointing lateral in both hemispheres [12] were active before M100. A V6 source with an upward current and characteristic location in the midline of the parieto-occipital sulcus [9,12] was estimated in a few subjects. The mean Talairach coordinates of cortical sources are listed in Table 1. The grand-averaged waveforms of each source strength waveform are shown in Fig 4A. All of these source activities exhibited greater amplitudes and shorter response latencies for stronger light stimuli (Table 1).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 4. Grand-averaged source strength waveforms of each cortical activity.
Waveforms for Experiment 1 (A) and Experiment 2 (B) are shown.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201804.g004
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. The mean latency and amplitude of each source activity in Experiment 1.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201804.t001
Experiment 2
Fig 4B shows grand-averaged source strength waveforms for each source activity. The yellow lens exerted specific effects on retinal and cortical responses (Figs 4B and 5). ANOVA results showed that the latency of the b-wave significantly differed among the five color lenses (F(4,32) = 9.26, p = 4.3 × 10−5). Post hoc paired comparisons showed that the b-wave latency of the yellow lens was significantly longer than that of the colorless lens (t-test, p = 0.0022; Wilcoxon, p = 0.012) (Fig 5A, Table 2). The peak amplitude of the b-wave did not significantly differ among the lens conditions (F(4,32) = 1.19, p = 0.34). However, when amplitude was measured at the peak latency for the colorless condition, it significantly differed among the five conditions (F(4,32) = 5.82, p = 0.0013). Post hoc tests showed a significant difference between the yellow and colorless lenses (t-test, p = 0.034; Wilcoxon, p = 0.015), suggesting that viewing through the yellow lens reduced the amplitude of the early part of the b-wave, which prolonged peak latency. The latency of the b-wave strongly correlated with transmittance at 400–500 nm (Fig 5B and 5C), indicating that blue light contributed to shaping the early part of the b-wave. Despite the reductions induced in retinal activity by the yellow lens, some cortical activities were enhanced under this condition. ANOVA results showed that the effects of the lens color were significant on the amplitude of the source activities in V1 (F(4.32) = 3.93, p = 0.01) and L-FG (F(4,24) = 6.26, p = 0.0014). When compared with the colorless lens, post hoc tests revealed that only the yellow lens had significant effects on V1 (t-test, p = 0.024; Wilcoxon, p = 0.025) and L-FG (t-test, p = 0.018; Wilcoxon, p = 0.018) activities. R-FG activity was slightly enhanced by the yellow lens (8.4 nAm for the yellow lens vs 2.9 nAm for the colorless lens, t-test, p = 0.13; Wilcoxon, p = 0.12). Regarding peak latency, the effects of the lens color was significant for V1 (F(4,32) = 8.08, p = 0.00013). Similar to the effects on amplitude, only the V1 latency of the yellow lens differed significantly from that of the colorless lens (t-test, p = 0.024; Wilcoxon, p = 0.012). Fig 6 shows a comparison of field distributions between colorless and yellow lens conditions in a representative subject. In this case, the field distribution was very similar between the two conditions with the exception that a characteristic quadrupole pattern distribution was present at 150–160 ms for the yellow lens condition only. Neural sources were estimated to be located in FG in both hemispheres (Fig 6B).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 5. Correlation between b-wave latency and transmittance at 450 nm.
A, Grand-averaged source strength waveforms of retinal activity in Experiment 2. The averaged waveforms of both eyes are shown. B, Relationship between the mean peak latency of retinal activity (b-wave) and transmittance of color lenses. C, Correlation coefficient plotted against wavelength.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201804.g005
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 6. Effects of the yellow lens on VEF topography.
Comparison of VEF topographies following a bright light stimulation between colorless and yellow lenses in a representative subject. Topographies for the recorded MEG signals (upper panel) and the model (lower panel) are shown. Note the similar topographies at early latencies, but lack of a clear quadrupole pattern distribution for the colorless lens condition at 150–160 ms. B, Dipole location for dipoles estimated in the fusiform gyrus region superimposed on a subject’s MR image.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201804.g006
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 2. The mean latency and amplitude of each source activity in Experiment 2.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201804.t002
Discussion
Retinal and cortical responses
Since the yellow lens had the largest impact, at least some of the present retinal activity observed originated from cones sensitive to short wavelengths (S-cones). On the other hand, the result showing that the blue or green lens had negligible effects on retinal activity suggested the lesser contribution of middle (M) and long (L) wavelength-sensitive cones. It currently remains unclear which cells contributed to shaping the response observed under the yellow lens condition with a longer response latency (Fig 6A). The response with the colorless lens was considered to be mainly composed of rod and S-cone components. Under the yellow lens condition, the S-cone component was reduced, which resulted in a longer implicit time for the b-wave. M- or L-cone ERGs have shorter latencies than S-cone ERGs, while S-cone ERGs have shorter latencies than rod ERGs [13,14]. Although the bright light was repeatedly presented at least 100 times under each condition in the present study, light adaptation may not have been complete because we used a black background. In a previous study using multifocal ERGs, Hoffmann et al. [15] reported minor effects of blue-light filtering. Although the methodologies used in their study and the present study differed, the range of filtering appeared to be the most important factor. The transmittance of the 450-nm wavelength in the present study was almost 0%, but was approximately 50% in their study. Therefore, a wavelength range of 400–530 nm, in which the present transmittance was clearly lower than theirs, appeared to be responsible for the reduced and delayed retinal response.
The beneficial effects of blue-light filtering on visual function have been reported in previous studies on contrast sensitivity [16–19], brightness [20], glare [21–25], reaction time [26], and scattering (described below) and VEPs in monkeys [27]. Previous findings also support the wavelength of light affecting the emotional aspects of photophobia. Main et al. [28] examined the discomfort threshold to light of short, medium, and long wavelengths in patients with migraine, patients with tension-type headache, and healthy controls, and found that patients with migraine had a significantly lower threshold for short and long wavelengths than the other two groups. Other studies reported that patients with photophobia preferred yellow tinted lens [29]. In the present study, cortical activities in V1 and FG were increased by the yellow lens in spite of reduced retinal activity. Since these cortical areas are in the ventral pathway of visual processing, this result suggested a functional change related to object recognition when bright light was viewed through a yellow lens, and, thus, the combination of reduced retinal activity and enhanced activity in the visual ventral pathway may be an objective marker for these functional changes. A case study by Horiguchi et al. [30] describing three patients with the total lack of photophobia due to bilateral lesions in the ventral occipital lobe appears to support this notion.
Effects of wavelength
This combination was specifically observed when subjects wore the yellow lens, implying that short wavelength light contributed to the disturbance of vision. Several mechanisms have been suggested to contribute to the results obtained in the present study. Bright light causes uncomfortable sensations (discomfort glare) as well as vision impairments (disability glare) [31]. Disability glare is caused by scattered intraocular light that casts a veiling luminance on the retina and, thus, reduces image contrast and chromatic discrimination [32]. Scattered light in the cornea and lens is predominantly of a short wavelength [33] and is partly due to small particles [34]. Therefore, the effects of the yellow lens on retinal and cortical activities in the present study may come from reduced light scattering. In the presence of glare, previous clinical studies showed that a yellow intra-ocular lens (IOL) improved driving performance [21,22] as well as recovery from photostress by a bright flash [23,25]. In 150 young healthy subjects, Hammond et al. [24] examined glare disability, photostress recovery, and their relationship with macular pigmentation using intense white light. They found that macular pigmentation correlated with these visual variables, which supported yellow intraocular filters enhancing chromatic contrast, reducing glare discomfort and dazzle, and thereby enhancing detail by absorbing blue haze [35]. Lenticular fluorescence also causes light scattering and affects visual function [27,36–39]. Lenticular fluorescence occurs due to the emission characteristics of certain compounds associated with lens proteins and has been shown to originate from chromophores with activation wavelengths of approximately 340–360 nm (emission at 420–440 nm) and 420–435 nm (emission at 500–520 nm) [27,38–42]. In the present study, the transmittance of light of this wavelength range markedly differed between the yellow and other lenses.
The second possible mechanism is chromatic aberration. Since lenses do not focus different wavelengths of light in a point, the retinal image formed by white light is blurred. Therefore, light scattering described above and chromatic aberration may cause a blur in the retina, and, thus, blue-light filtering is expected to improve contrast sensitivity. Campbell and Gubisch [43] measured contrast thresholds for monochromatic yellow light and white light, and observed a lower threshold for the former for intermediate frequencies. Similar effects on the contrast sensitivity of blue-light filtering were demonstrated in subjects with intraocular lenses [17,19]. Wolfesohn et al. [18] reported that yellow lenses reduced contrast sensitivity to a white-on-black grating and enhanced it to a white-on-blue grating in low to mid-range spatial frequencies. However, a study by Kelly et al. [44] using 52 healthy young subjects found no significant differences in contrast sensitivity between yellow and neutral lenses. Blue-light filtering generally appears to exert positive effects on contrast sensitivity under photopic [16,17] or mesopic conditions [16] at middle-range spatial frequencies [17], but not at high frequencies [45].
The third possibility is the color opponent system. Kinney et al. [26] measured reaction times for the square wave gratings of various spatial frequencies and contrasts viewed through yellow and neutral goggles, and observed that the yellow goggle increased reaction times to low-luminance gratings of middle frequencies. They suggested that the beneficial effects of the yellow lens came from enhancements in the output of the chromatic system due to reductions in subtractive effects in the yellow-blue opponent channel. Their findings showing that the yellow lens did not affect reaction times to gratings of high frequencies were consistent with those in previous studies showing no effects of a yellow lens on visual acuity [45], and precluded simple explanations for the effects of yellow lenses, such as a reduction in scattering light [26]. These non-optical factors may have contributed to the effects of the yellow lens observed in the present study.
Although the present study did not clarify the extent to which these mechanisms contributed to the results obtained, the findings of a VEP study in rhesus monkeys by Zuclich et al. [27] are noteworthy. They showed that the measured fluorescence of the lens was sufficiently high to imply the degradation of visual function, and in order to confirm this, they presented a 413-nm laser beam to the monkey’s eye while recording VEPs to monochromatic sine-wave gratings. The findings obtained showed that the amplitude of VEPs decreased by 15~25% when 413-nm wavelength light was projected, which they attributed to the veiling glare associated with the fluorescence induced because under the experimental conditions, a direct glare with light scattering of 413 nm did not explain the decline. Van den Berg stated that a value of approximately 20% for the VEP amplitude reduction matched the expected value of veiling luminance caused by 413-nm wavelength light calculated from excised human lenses [38]. To the best of our knowledge, there is one VEP study that investigated the effects of blue-light filtering in humans [46]. In contrast to the present results, they found no effects on VEP amplitudes. This difference may be attributed to the yellow lens used; we used a yellow lens that cut off light with a wavelength shorter than 450 nm (Fig 2), while transmittance at 450 nm of the IOL used by Hoffmann et al. was approximately 50%. Another study also failed to show a significant effect of the same yellow lens on ERGs [15]. Therefore, light with wavelengths of 380–530 nm, at which transmittance in the two studies clearly differed, appeared to have an impact on retinal and cortical activities. This was in agreement with the findings of Zuclich et al. [27] showing that a wavelength of 413 nm was effective.
Conclusions
The present results demonstrated that retinal activity (b-wave) in response to bright light was reduced when subjects viewed it through a yellow lens, while cortical activities in V1 and FG were enhanced, suggesting changes in visual processing by blue-light filtering. It is interesting to note that Good and Hou reported that children with cortical or cerebral visual impairment (CVI), which often accompanies photophobia, exhibited better visual acuity under low- than high-luminance viewing conditions, while no significant differences were observed between the two conditions in normal children [47]. In addition, the amplitude of VEP to low-luminance grating was greater than that to high-luminance in the CVI group, which is consistent with the present results. Therefore, blue-light filtering may be beneficial for these patients. The present results may be useful for selecting the color and depth of color lenses; however, future clinical studies are warranted.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the ImPACT Program (2015-PM11-23-01) of the Council for Science, Technology, and Innovation (Cabinet Office, Government of Japan) to MS, NK, and KI (http://www.jst.go.jp/impact/). Tokai Optical Co., Ltd. provided support in the form of salaries for MS and NK. The specific roles of these authors are articulated in the 'author contributions' section. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Citation: Suzuki M, Kumagai N, Inui K, Kakigi R (2018) Effects of color lenses on visual evoked magnetic fields following bright light. PLoS ONE 13(8): e0201804. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201804
1. Digre KB, Brennan KC. Shedding light on photophobia. J Neuroophthalmol. 2012;32:68–81. pmid:22330853
2. Lebensohn L. The nature of photophobia. Arch Ophthalmol. 1934;12: 380–390.
3. Main A, Vlachonikolis I, Dowson A. The wavelength of light causing photophobia in migraine and tension-type headache between attacks. Headache 2000;40:194–199. pmid:10759921
4. Stringham JM, Fuld K, Wenzel AJ: Action spectrum for photophobia. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2003;20:1852–1858. pmid:14570098
5. Malecaze FJ, Boulanouar KA, Demonet JF, Guell JL, Imbert MA. Abnormal activation in the visual cortex after corneal refractive surgery for myopia: demonstration by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Ophthalmology 2001;108:2213–2218. pmid:11733261
6. Moulton EA, Becerra L, Borsook D. An fMRI case report of photophobia: activation of the trigeminal nociceptive pathway. Pain 2009;145:358–363. pmid:19674842
7. Emoto H, Suzuki Y, Wakakura M, Horie C, Kiyosawa M, Mochizuki M, et al. Photophobia in essential blepharospasm—a positron emission tomographic study. Mov Disord. 2010;25:433–439. pmid:20014062
8. van den Berg TJ. On the relation between glare and straylight. Doc Ophthalmol 1991;78:177–181. pmid:1790738
9. Portin K, Vanni S, Virsu V, Hari R. Stronger occipital cortical activation to lower than upper visual field stimuli. Neuromagnetic recordings. Exp Brain Res. 1999;124:287–294. pmid:9989434
10. Suzuki M, Nagae M, Nagata Y, Kumagai N, Inui K, Kakigi R. Effects of refractive errors on visual evoked magnetic fields. BMC Ophthalmol. 2015;15:162. pmid:26553029
11. Tsuruhara A, Nagata Y, Suzuki M, Inui K, Kakigi R. Effects of spatial frequency on visual evoked magnetic fields. Exp Brain Res. 2013;226:347–355. pmid:23417649
12. Inui K, Kakigi R: Temporal analysis of the flow from V1 to the extrastriate cortex in humans. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:775–784. pmid:16835365
13. Gouras P, MacKay CJ. Electroretinographic responses of the short-wavelength-sensitive cones. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1990;31:1203–1209. pmid:2365554
14. Gouras P, MacKay CJ, Yamamoto S. The human S-cone electroretinogram and its variation among subjects with and without L and M-cone function. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1993;34:2437–2442. pmid:8325751
15. Hoffmann MB, Spors F, Langenbucher A, Walter S, Behrens-Baumann W, Reupsch J, et al. Minor effect of blue-light filtering on multifocal electroretinograms. J Cataract Refract Surg. 2010;36:1692–1699. pmid:20870115
16. Yap M. The effect of a yellow filter on contrast sensitivity. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1984;4:227–232. pmid:6472852
17. Niwa K, Yoshino Y, Okuyama F, Tokoro T. Effects of tinted intraocular lens on contrast sensitivity. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1996;16:297–302. pmid:8796198
18. Wolffsohn JS, Cochrane AL, Khoo H, Yoshimitsu Y, Wu S. Contrast is enhanced by yellow lenses because of selective reduction of short-wavelength light. Optom Vis Sci. 2000;77:73–81. pmid:10701805
19. Rodríguez-Galietero A, Montés-Micó R, Muñoz G, Albarrán-Diego C. Blue-light filtering intraocular lens in patients with diabetes: contrast sensitivity and chromatic discrimination. J Cataract Refract Surg. 2005;31:2088–2092. pmid:16412920
20. Kelly SA. Effect of yellow-tinted lenses on brightness. J Opt Soc Am A. 1990;7:1905–1911 pmid:2231102
21. Gray R, Perkins SA, Suryakumar R, Neuman B, Maxwell WA. Reduced effect of glare disability on driving performance in patients with blue light-filtering intraocular lenses. J Cataract Refract Surg. 2011;37:38–44. pmid:21183098
22. Gray R, Hill W, Neuman B, Houtman D, Potvin R. Effects of a blue light-filtering intraocular lens on driving safety in glare conditions. J Cataract Refract Surg. 2012;38:816–822. pmid:22520305
23. Hammond BR, Bernstein B, Dong J. The Effect of the AcrySof natural lens on glare disability and photostress. Am J Ophthalmol. 2009;148:272–276. pmid:19426959
24. Hammond BR Jr, Fletcher LM, Elliott JG. Glare disability, photostress recovery, and chromatic contrast: relation to macular pigment and serum lutein and zeaxanthin. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013;54:476–481. pmid:23211814
25. Hammond BR. Attenuating photostress and glare disability in pseudophakic patients through the addition of a short-wave absorbing filter. J Ophthalmol. 2015;2015:607635. pmid:25838942
26. Kinney JA, Schlichting CL, Neri DF, Kindness SW. Reaction time to spatial frequencies using yellow and luminance-matched neutral goggles. Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1983;60:132–138. pmid:6846488
27. Zuclich JA, Glickman RD, Menendez AR. In situ measurements of lens fluorescence and its interference with visual function. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1992;33:410–415. pmid:1740373
28. Main A, Vlachonikolis I, Dowson A. The wavelength of light causing photophobia in migraine and tension-type headache between attacks. Headache. 2000;40:194–199. pmid:10759921
29. Blackburn MK, Lamb RD, Digre KB, Smith AG, Warner JE, McClane RW, et al. FL-41 tint improves blink frequency, light sensitivity, and functional limitations in patients with benign essential blepharospasm. Ophthalmology. 2009;116:997–1001. pmid:19410958
30. Horiguchi H, Kubo H, Nakadomari S. Lack of photophobia associated with bilateral ventral occipital lesion. Jpn J Ophthalmol. 2011;55:301–303. pmid:21559913
31. Mainster MA, Turner PL. Glare's causes, consequences, and clinical challenges after a century of ophthalmic study. Am J Ophthalmol. 2012;153:587–593. pmid:22445628
32. Steen R, Whitaker D, Elliott DB, Wild JM. Age-related effects of glare on luminance and color contrast sensitivity. Optom Vis Sci. 1994;71:792–796. pmid:7898887
33. Coppens JE, Franssen L, van den Berg TJ. Wavelength dependence of intraocular straylight. Exp Eye Res. 2006;82:688–692. pmid:16293245
34. Costello MJ, Johnsen S, Metlapally S, Gilliland KO, Frame L, Balasubramanian D. Multilamellar spherical particles as potential sources of excessive light scattering in human age-related nuclear cataracts. Exp Eye Res. 2010;91:881–889. pmid:20888812
35. Walls GL, Judd HD. The intra-ocular colour-filters of vertebrates. Br J Ophthalmol. 1933;17:641–675. pmid:18169162
36. Weale RA. Age and human lenticular fluorescence. J Biomed Opt. 1996;1:251–261. pmid:23014723
37. Elliott DB, Yang KC, Dumbleton K, Cullen AP. Ultraviolet-induced lenticular fluorescence: intraocular straylight affecting visual function. Vision Res. 1993;33:1827–1833. pmid:8266638
38. van den Berg TJ. Quantal and visual efficiency of fluorescence in the lens of the human eye. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1993;34:3566–3573. pmid:8258514
39. Zuclich JA, Previc FH, Novar BJ, Edsall PR: Near-UV/blue light-induced fluorescence in the human lens: potential interference with visual function. J Biomed Opt. 2005;10:44021. pmid:16178654
40. Lerman S. Lens proteins and fluorescence. Isr J Med Sci. 1972;8:1583–1589. pmid:4647824
41. van den Berg TJ, Ijspeert JK. Light scattering in donor lenses. Vision Res. 1995;35:169–177. pmid:7839606
42. van den Berg TJ: Visual efficiency of scattering and fluorescence in the human eye lens. J Biomed Opt. 1996;1:262–267. pmid:23014724
43. Campbell FW, Gubisch RW. The effect of chromatic aberration on visual acuity. J Physiol. 1967;192:345–358. pmid:6050153
44. Kelly SA, Goldberg SE, Banton TA. Effect of yellow-tinted lenses on contrast sensitivity. Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1984;61:657–662. pmid:6517120
45. Pokorny J, Graham CH, Lanson RN. Effect of wavelength on foveal grating acuity. J Opt Soc Am. 1968;58:1410–1414. pmid:5683293
46. Hoffmann MB, Behrens-Baumann W, Langenbucher A, Reupsch J, Viestenz A. Effect of blue-light filtering on multifocal visual-evoked potentials. J Cataract Refract Surg. 2012;38:85–91. pmid:22001106
47. Good WV, Hou C. Sweep visual evoked potential grating acuity thresholds paradoxically improve in low-luminance conditions in children with cortical visual impairment. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2006;47:3220–3224. pmid:16799070
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2018 Suzuki et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Photophobia is a common condition in which bright light causes an unpleasant feeling due to increased sensitivity to light. In addition to discomfort, photophobia may be accompanied by visual dysfunction. The present study was conducted in order to examine whether visual evoked cortical responses contribute to the assessment of visual dysfunction due to bright light. Visual evoked magnetic fields (VEFs) following the presentation of a uniform bright light of 200–3700 cd/m2 in the lower visual field were recorded in 10 healthy volunteers and the effects of five color lenses: yellow, blue, gray, green, and colorless, were examined. VEFs were subjected to a multi-dipole analysis that resulted in the separation of several source activities, including the retina, V1, V2, V6, and fusiform gyrus. Source activity in the retina corresponding to the ERG b-wave exhibited a reduced amplitude and elongated peak latency with the yellow lens. Its latency strongly correlated with transmittance at 450 nm. On the other hand, cortical activities in V1 and the fusiform gyrus were stronger with the yellow lens than with the other color and colorless lenses. Only blue-light blocking showed significant effects. The result showing that the yellow lens enhanced V1 and fusiform activities indicated that processing in these areas was improved when subjects used this lens. The combination of delayed retinal activity and increased visual cortex activity may be an objective indicator of the effects of a color lens on visual function.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer