About the Authors:
Nicolas Poirel
Contributed equally to this work with: Nicolas Poirel, Grégoire Borst
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliations Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France, Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université de Caen, Caen, France, Institut Universitaire de France (IUF), Paris, France
Grégoire Borst
Contributed equally to this work with: Nicolas Poirel, Grégoire Borst
Affiliations Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France, Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université de Caen, Caen, France
Grégory Simon
Affiliations Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France, Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université de Caen, Caen, France
Sandrine Rossi
Affiliations Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France, Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université de Caen, Caen, France
Mathieu Cassotti
Affiliations Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France, Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université de Caen, Caen, France, Centre de Gestion Scientifique, Mines ParisTech, Paris, France
Arlette Pineau
Affiliations Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France, Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université de Caen, Caen, France
Olivier Houdé
Affiliations Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Paris, France, Laboratoire de Pychologie du Développement et de l’Éducation de l’enfant (LaPsyDÉ, Unité CNRS 3521), Université de Caen, Caen, France, Institut Universitaire de France (IUF), Paris, France
Introduction
In most situations in the visual environment, when the number of objects increases, a greater portion of space is occupied. Many empirical studies demonstrate that space and numbers are not only strongly associated at a behavioral level but also recruit overlapping cortical regions in the human brain [1], [2], [3], [4]. In situations in which numbers and length conflict (e.g., discrete linear arrays of coins with the same number of coins but different lengths), a fronto-parietal network is recruited [5]. The activation of this network allows one to surpass the spatial bias (i.e., length equals number) to correctly focus on the comparison of numbers [6], [7] (see [8] for a discussion on the involvement of the intraparietal sulcus in numerosity and length processing). This relationship between space and number appears early in cognitive development [9]. Young children tend to believe that the spatial extent of a group of objects reflects the number of objects in that group. Piaget’s famous conservation-of-number task [10] is a classical example of children’s inability to dissociate the number of objects in arrays from their spatial extents. In this task, children first determine whether two rows containing the same number of coins placed in a one-to-one correspondence are equal in number. Once the children have acknowledged this equality, one of the rows is transformed in length but not in number (i.e., the coins in the row are spread apart; see Figure 1), and the children are once again asked whether the two rows contain the same number of coins. Before 7 years of age, children erroneously believe that there are more objects in the longer row. This perceptual error has been reported by numerous developmental studies [11], [12], [13]. Errors made by young children in conservation-of-number tasks have been attributed either to children’s inability to fully grasp the concept of number [10], [14] or to children’s inability to inhibit a misleading perceptual strategy, or the ‘length-equals-number’ heuristic [15]. According to Piaget, children around 7 years of age begin to understand that any visuospatial transformations can be mentally canceled out by using the mirror transformation. Hence, the acquisition of the reversibility of cognitive operations allows children to determine that the two rows of coins contain the same numbers of objects, regardless of any apparent transformations in the conservation-of-number task. Thus, in Piaget’s theory, success in this task proves the solidity of the number concept and represents an important step in children’s acquisition of concrete logicomathematical skills. However, Neo-Piagetian authors have suggested that young children erroneously state that the longer row contains more coins because they fail to inhibit the inappropriate ‘length-equals-number’ heuristic [15], [16], [17]. Indeed, in line with Dempster’s claim that “conservation and class inclusion have more to do with the ability to resist interference than they do with the child’s ability to grasp their underlying logic” ([16], p. 15), Neo-Piagetians believe that the critical factor for success in Piaget’s numerical or logical tasks is not only the ability to activate the appropriate logicomathematical strategy but also, and most importantly, the executive ability to inhibit misleading visuospatial heuristics (here, the ‘length-equals-number’ heuristic). According to Neo-Piagetians, cognitive development is supported not only by the ability to acquire knowledge of incremental complexity [14] but also by the ability to inhibit previously acquired knowledge (for recent works regarding the importance of inhibition on cognitive development, see [18], [19], [20]). Note that the two accounts of children’s errors in the conservation-of-number task are not mutually exclusive. Piaget argued that children change their judgment after the coins are spread apart because they use the misleading ‘length-equals-number’ intuition. However, Piaget did not hypothesize, based on this observation, that children specifically need inhibitory control as an executive function of the brain (beyond logicomathematical cognition per se) to succeed at this developmental task.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 1. Example of conservation-of-number trial and percentages of explained variance between percentage of fMRI signal change during the conservation task and behavioral scores for the successful and the control groups of children.
Lower scores for the Stroop task reflect higher inhibitory control efficiency.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040802.g001
Recently, an fMRI study investigated the brain areas necessary for succeeding at Piaget’s conservation-of-number task [5]. During a functional imaging session, children were presented with a computerized version of Piaget’s classic conservation-of-number task. The results revealed that the acquisition of number conservation is supported by a parieto-frontal network that is involved in the monitoring of different possible and competitive task responses [21]. Thus, the acquisition of the number conservation principle might be directly related to the efficiency of the inhibitory control that allows one to inhibit erroneous intuitive perceptual heuristic responses.
The present work aims to further investigate the role of executive functions in the numerosity-length conflict. In our previous fMRI study [5], success in the conservation-of-number task elicited activations in the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus, a region critical to inhibitory control [22] and resisting interference [23]. In line with this result, we reasoned that if inhibitory control allows children to surpass the length-equals-number conflict, then the percentage of BOLD signal changes in the insula/inferior frontal gyrus should be related to children’s inhibitory control efficiency, as reflected by children’s performance on an adaptation of the Stroop task (a Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development [24]). To demonstrate that the level of neural activation in this area is directly related to the efficiency of inhibitory control rather than to other cognitive abilities, we also tested whether the percentage of BOLD signal changes in the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus was related to the children’s ability to manipulate information in working memory in a Backward Digit Span task [25]. Indeed, studies report that both inhibitory control and the manipulation of information in working memory rely on the inferior portions of the frontal lobes [26] (see [27] for a review). A control group consisting of children who were unable to perform the number-length interference items was also included. We reasoned that if inhibitory control allows children to surpass the length-equals-number conflict, then we should find no correlation between children’s performance in the Stroop task and the level of activation in the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus in the control group.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Two groups of children were created from the original sample tested in the fMRI study [5]. One group (hereafter referred to as the successful group) consisted of 16 children who accurately performed the number-length interference items (mean age: 9 years ± 21 months; 10 girls). The second group (hereafter referred to as the control group) consisted of 12 children who were unable to accurately perform the number-length interference items (mean age: 6 years ± 8 months; 6 girls). Only right-handed children were included because handedness affects performance on the Stroop task [28]. The local ethics committee (CPP Nord-Ouest III, France) approved the study. These children had no history of a neurological or psychiatric disorder. Written consent was obtained from the parents and the children themselves.
Behavioral Assessment
The children performed an animal Stroop task [24] and a Backward Digit Span task outside the scanner. In the animal Stroop task, the children performed three conditions (congruent, control, and incongruent), each consisting of 24 farm animal pictures presented on a page. Four farm animals were used in each condition (i.e., cow, duck, pig, and sheep). In all three conditions, participants were instructed to name the body of each animal regardless of the head attached to it. In the congruent condition, the head of each animal corresponded to the correct body. In the control condition, human heads were placed on animals’ bodies. Finally, in the incongruent condition, chimeric animals were shown (e.g., a cow’s head attached to a pig’s body). Reaction times (RTs) were recorded for each condition. The one-to-one correspondence condition in the conservation-of-number task is a baseline congruent condition in which length and number co-vary, whereas the experimental incongruent condition is a condition in which length and number conflict. Therefore, individual interference scores for the Stroop task were computed, following the same cognitive logic, by subtracting the congruent RTs from the incongruent RTs for each child. Lower scores reflect higher inhibitory control efficiency. This interference score reflects the time required to disengage from an automatic answer [29].
Children’s ability to manipulate information in working memory was assessed with the Backward Digit Span subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children [25]. In this task, the children listened to series of discrete digits and were asked to recall the series of digits in the reverse order of presentation. The children first performed two series of two digits. The series of digits were incrementally increased by one digit every two trials. The task was stopped when the children failed to recall two trials with the same number of digits. The working memory score was defined as the number of series correctly recalled.
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Paradigm
Images were acquired using a 3T MRI scanner (Achieva, Philips Medical System, Netherlands). In the first anatomical session, three-dimensional T1-weighted spoiled gradient images were acquired (field of view (FOV) = 256 mm, slice thickness = 1.33 mm, 128 slices, matrix size = 192×192 voxels). The subsequent fMRI session was conducted with T2*-weighted gradient echo planar images (repetition time of 2 s, echo time of 35 ms, and a flip angle of 80° for 31 axial slices, 3.5 mm thick, with a 224-mm FOV and a 64×64 grid; see Houdé et al., 2011 for a detailed description of the fMRI imaging protocol). Throughout this second MRI session, the children performed Piaget’s conservation-of-number task and were presented with conservation trials. Each trial consisted of two rows, each containing the same number of coins (5, 6, or 7). For each trial, the children were asked to judge the numerical equivalence of two rows when the rows had the same length. After the children responded, the coins in one of the rows were spread apart by apparent movements on the computer screen (see Figure 1). The children were again instructed to judge the numerical equivalence of the two rows when their length, but not their number, differed. The children responded by pressing the “same” button or the “not the same” button on the response box. The question “Is the number of objects the same in both rows?” was delivered verbally for each trial. Each trial remained on the screen until the children responded.
Functional Regions of Interest
Functional regions of interest (fROI) were defined according to a contrast analysis between successful and unsuccessful children. The brain activity of the unsuccessful group of children (i.e., children who made errors on the length-interference items inside the scanner during the conservation-of-number task) was subtracted from the brain activity of the successful group of children (i.e., children who correctly performed the length-interference items inside the scanner during the conservation-of-number task) [5]. This contrast revealed the brain areas that were activated more in the successful group and therefore the brain network necessary to succeed at Piaget’s conservation-of-number task. For each child, we extracted the BOLD values from the significant activations (p<.01, False Discovery Rate, with a minimum extent of 50 voxels in clusters). These regions were located in the bilateral ventral and dorsal visual pathways and in the bilateral parieto-frontal network (see Table 1). Posterior activations were identified in the calcarine sulcus and lingual, inferior occipital, and temporal gyri for the ventral pathway and in the cuneus and superior and middle occipital gyri for the dorsal pathway. The bilateral parieto-frontal network included the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), angular gyrus, insula/orbital part of the inferior frontal gyrus, and middle frontal gyrus with the orbital/opercular part of the right middle frontal gyrus. The left precentral and bilateral cerebellum were also more active in successful children.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. MNI coordinates and number of voxels of brain regions more activated in children who succeeded to the length-interference items than in children who were not able to efficiently perform the Piagetian task.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040802.t001
Results
Behavioral Data
We analyzed the RTs from the Stroop task in a one-way (condition: congruent vs. control vs. incongruent) repeated-measures analysis of variance. Note that data from one child of the successful group were excluded due to abnormal longer RTs for the congruent condition (47 s) compared to the control (31 s) and incongruent (36 s) conditions. Consistent with previous findings [24], children required more time to perform the incongruent condition (successful group: 32±9 s; control group : 72±28 s) than the congruent or the control conditions (27±7 s and 25±5 s, respectively, for the successful group, 39±10 s and 50±23 s, respectively, for the control group), F(2, 14) = 10.10, p = .0005 (successful group) and F(2, 11) = 19.9, p<.0001 (control group). The mean score of the Backward Digit Span test was 6±2 numbers correctly repeated in a backward order for the successful group, and 4±0.3 numbers correctly repeated in a backward order for the control group. Importantly, we found no significant correlation between the inhibitory control score and the Backward Digit Spans (successful group: p = .15; control group: p = .73), suggesting that the two tasks reflect different cognitive processes, namely inhibitory control and information manipulation in working memory.
fROI Data and Behavioral Scores
Regression analyses, which included gender and age as covariates, were computed to determine the degree of relationship between the activity of the fROIs and behavioral performance (i.e., the interference Stroop score and Backward Digit Span, see Table 2).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 2. F Ratio and p values of the parameter estimates in the regression analysis between the activity of brain regions necessary to surpass the length-numerosity interference and behavioral tests for children who succeeded to the length-interference items (successful group) and for children who were not able to efficiently perform the Piagetian task (control group).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040802.t002
Regarding the successful group, the main result was a double dissociation pattern of neurocognitive correlations in the two key fROIs from the parieto-frontal network necessary to succeed at the Piagetian conservation-of-number task (see Figure 1). A significant negative correlation was found between the interference scores and the percentage of BOLD signal changes in the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus. Backward Digit Spans positively correlated with the percentage of BOLD signal changes in the left intraparietal sulcus. Critically, Backward Digit Spans did not correlate with the percentage of BOLD signal changes in the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus (p = .13), and interference scores did not correlate with the percentage of BOLD signal changes in the left intraparietal sulcus (p = .21). It is worth noting that the same pattern of correlations was observed when the interference score was calculated by subtracting control RTs from incongruent RTs [24]. The interference score correlated with the percentage of BOLD signal changes in the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus (p = .02), but not with the IPS (p = .17).
Regression analyses revealed no correlation between the fROIs and the interference Stroop score or the Backward Digit Span for children in the control group who did not succeed at the conservation task (all p>.05, see Table 2).
Discussion
The current study examined whether success in a well-known Piagetian developmental task (i.e., the conservation-of-number task) is determined, at least partially, by children’s ability to inhibit the length-equals-number heuristic. Piaget’s conservation-of-number task is a challenging task for children younger than 7 years of age. Before this age, children erroneously indicate that longer arrays contain more objects when two arrays with the same number of elements have different lengths. Authors have suggested that executive processes, particularly inhibitory control, are critical for resisting number-length interference [17]. To demonstrate that inhibitory control is needed to accurately perform the conservation-of-number task, we computed the correlations between the percentage of BOLD signal changes in fROIs defined in a previous fMRI study [5] and behavioral performance that reflected either inhibitory control efficiency or the ability to manipulate information in working memory. Children’s inhibitory control efficiency–the extra time needed to process incongruent (e.g., a pig’s head on a duck’s body) or congruent (e.g., a duck’s head on a duck’s body) items in a Stroop task, mirroring the two types of items (length-number conflict vs. length-number covariation) presented in the conservation-of-number task–correlated with the level of neural activation in the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus. Notably, no correlation was observed between the activation in the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus and the ability to manipulate information in working memory.
These results suggest that the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus is specifically involved in the need to resist an interference; thus, this may be a core area of the brain network for the inhibitory control required to accurately perform Piaget’s conservation-of-number task. Given that no correlation was found between the activation of this region and the interference Stroop score of children who did not succeed at the conservation-of-number task (i.e., the control group), we suggest that the level of activation in the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus reflected the inhibitory control needed to correctly perform this Piagetian task. This interpretation is further supported by a meta-analysis showing that the right insula (1) is increasingly recruited with increasing executive efficiency during cognitive development [27], and (2) continues to be strongly involved in the inhibition process during adulthood [30]. Note that, in agreement with the processes that are suspected of being involved in the conservation-of-number task, we only studied the correlations between inhibition and working memory processes and brain activations. One could argue that the activity of the right insula might not be restricted to its role in inhibition. Activity in the right insula is also linked, for example, to the capacity to monitor error [31] and to manage response-challenging situations [32]. Nevertheless, the fact that this region correlated in the present study with inhibition but not with working memory scores strongly suggests that the right insula is involved in resistance to interference situations.
The ability to manipulate numerical information in working memory, as revealed by the Backward Digit Span, was related to the activation of the left IPS in children who succeeded at the Piagetian task. This relation was not present in the control group of children who did not resist the number-length interference. The IPS is classically viewed as a numerical area in adults and children [33], [34], [35], so it is coherent that activation in this area could be related to children’s ability to manipulate numbers. The ability to recall a series of digits in reverse order may be a critical ability that allows children to accurately perform the conservation-of-number task. According to Piaget [10], during the conservation-of-number task, children need to mentally reverse the visuospatial transformation that occurs after the initial equivalence phase to determine that two rows of coins of different lengths possess the same number of elements. Thus, children need to maintain the representation of the two rows of coins of different lengths in working memory while they mentally imagine what the two rows would look like when the coins that are spread apart return to their original position. We note that the IPS is recruited not only in number processing but also in length processing [36] as well as in the transformation of visuospatial mental images (as reviewed by Zacks [37], see also [38]). These results, in conjunction with the findings of the present work, suggest that the activation observed in the IPS when children accurately performed the conservation-of-number task may reflect the ability to imagine the mirror visuospatial transformation of a perceptual transformation of objects (such as the spreading of coins), which is an elementary form of a more formal reversibility (e.g., numerical reversibility).
Conclusion
In conclusion, the present findings provide the first evidence of a direct relationship between the activation of the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus, inhibitory control efficiency and the ability to resolve a length-numerosity conflict. We further characterized the parieto-frontal network involved in number conservation by providing evidence that the activation of the IPS may be related to the ability to reverse visuospatial transformations and operations. The data reported here, in conjunction with data collected in behavioral [17] and event-related studies [39], add support to the view that inhibitory control is necessary to overcome length-numerosity interference in children. In addition, we provided evidence that Neo-Piagetian and Piagetian accounts of numerical cognitive development are not mutually exclusive. After the visuospatial transformation (the spreading of the coins), children need to inhibit the misleading strategy (i.e., length-equal-number, supported by insula/inferior frontal gyri) and to activate the reversibility of the operation (supported by the IPS) to determine that the two rows possess the same number of elements.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: SR AP OH. Performed the experiments: SR AP OH. Analyzed the data: NP GB GS MC. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: NP GB GS SR MC. Wrote the paper: NP GB.
Citation: Poirel N, Borst G, Simon G, Rossi S, Cassotti M, Pineau A, et al. (2012) Number Conservation is Related to Children’s Prefrontal Inhibitory Control: An fMRI Study of a Piagetian Task. PLoS ONE 7(7): e40802. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040802
1. Dolores de Hevia M, Girelli L, Vallar G (2006) Numbers and space: a cognitive illusion? Experimental Brain Research 168: 254–264.M. Dolores de HeviaL. GirelliG. Vallar2006Numbers and space: a cognitive illusion?Experimental Brain Research168254264
2. Fias W, Brysbaert M, Geypens F, d’Ydewalle G (1996) The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition 2: 95–110.W. FiasM. BrysbaertF. GeypensG. d’Ydewalle1996The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect.Mathematical Cognition295110
3. Walsh V (2003) A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 483–488.V. Walsh2003A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity.Trends in Cognitive Sciences7483488
4. Dehaene S, Brannon E (2011) Space, time and number in the brain: Searching for the Foundations of Mathematical Thought. London: Academic Press. 374 p. S. DehaeneE. Brannon2011Space, time and number in the brain: Searching for the Foundations of Mathematical Thought. London: Academic Press.374 p
5. Houdé O, Pineau A, Leroux G, Poirel N, Perchey G, et al. (2011) Functional magnetic resonance imaging study of Piaget’s conservation-of-number task in preschool and school-age children: a neo-Piagetian approach. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 110: 332–346.O. HoudéA. PineauG. LerouxN. PoirelG. Perchey2011Functional magnetic resonance imaging study of Piaget’s conservation-of-number task in preschool and school-age children: a neo-Piagetian approach.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology110332346
6. Leroux G, Joliot M, Dubal S, Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N, et al. (2006) Cognitive inhibition of number/length interference in a Piaget-like task: Evidence from ERP and fMRI. Human Brain Mapping 27: 498–509.G. LerouxM. JoliotS. DubalB. MazoyerN. Tzourio-Mazoyer2006Cognitive inhibition of number/length interference in a Piaget-like task: Evidence from ERP and fMRI.Human Brain Mapping27498509
7. Leroux G, Spiess J, Zago L, Rossi S, Lubin A, et al. (2009) Adult brains don’t fully overcome biases that lead to incorrect performance during cognitive development: An fMRI study in young adults completing a Piaget-like task. Developmental Science 12: 326–338.G. LerouxJ. SpiessL. ZagoS. RossiA. Lubin2009Adult brains don’t fully overcome biases that lead to incorrect performance during cognitive development: An fMRI study in young adults completing a Piaget-like task.Developmental Science12326338
8. Dormal V, Andres M, Dormal G, Pesenti M (2010) Mode-dependent and mode-independent representations of numerosity in the right intraparietal sulcus. Neuroimage 52: 1677–1686.V. DormalM. AndresG. DormalM. Pesenti2010Mode-dependent and mode-independent representations of numerosity in the right intraparietal sulcus.Neuroimage5216771686
9. Lourenco SF, Longo MR (2011) Origins and development of generalized magnitude representation. In: Dehaene S, Brannon E, editors. pp. 225–244.SF LourencoMR Longo2011Origins and development of generalized magnitude representation.S. DehaeneE. Brannon225244editors.Space, time and number in the brain: Searching for the Foundations of Mathematical Thought.London: Academic Press. London: Academic Press.
10. Piaget J (1952) The child’s conception of number. New York: Basic Books (original in French, 1941). 258 p. J. Piaget1952The child’s conception of number. New York: Basic Books (original in French, 1941).258 p
11. Ping RM, Goldin-Meadow S (2008) Hands in the air: using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity. Developmental Psychology 44: 1277–1287.RM PingS. Goldin-Meadow2008Hands in the air: using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity.Developmental Psychology4412771287
12. Poirel N, Vidal M, Pineau A, Lanoë C, Leroux G, et al. (2011) Evidence of Different Developmental Trajectories for Length Estimation According to Egocentric and Allocentric Viewpoints in Children and Adults. Experimental Psychology 58: 142–146.N. PoirelM. VidalA. PineauC. LanoëG. Leroux2011Evidence of Different Developmental Trajectories for Length Estimation According to Egocentric and Allocentric Viewpoints in Children and Adults.Experimental Psychology58142146
13. Zhang Q, Shi J, Fan Y, Liu T, Luo Y, et al. (2008) An event-related brain potential study of children’s conservation. Neuroscience Letters 431: 17–20.Q. ZhangJ. ShiY. FanT. LiuY. Luo2008An event-related brain potential study of children’s conservation.Neuroscience Letters4311720
14. Piaget J (1984) Piaget’s theory. In: Mussen P, editor. J. Piaget1984Piaget’s theory.P. Musseneditor.Handbook of Child Psychology.New York: Wiley. New York: Wiley.
15. Houdé O (2000) Inhibition and cognitive development: object, number, categorization, and reasoning. Cognitive Development 15: 63–73.O. Houdé2000Inhibition and cognitive development: object, number, categorization, and reasoning.Cognitive Development156373
16. Dempster FN (1995) Interference and inhibition in cognition: an historical perspective. In: Dempster FN, Brainerd CJ, editors. pp. 3–26.FN Dempster1995Interference and inhibition in cognition: an historical perspective.FN DempsterCJ Brainerd326editors.Interference and inhibition in cognition.New York: Academic Press. New York: Academic Press.
17. Houdé O, Guichart E (2001) Negative priming effect after inhibition of number/length interference in a Piaget-like task. Developmental Science 4: 71–74.O. HoudéE. Guichart2001Negative priming effect after inhibition of number/length interference in a Piaget-like task.Developmental Science47174
18. Davidson MC, Amso D, Anderson LC, Diamond A (2006) Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4–13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia 44: 2037–2078.MC DavidsonD. AmsoLC AndersonA. Diamond2006Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4–13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching.Neuropsychologia4420372078
19. Diamond A, Barnett WS, Thomas J, Munro S (2007) Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science 318: 1387–1388.A. DiamondWS BarnettJ. ThomasS. Munro2007Preschool program improves cognitive control.Science31813871388
20. Diamond A, Lee K (2011) Interventions shown to Aid Executive Function Development in Children 4–12 Years Old. Science 333: 959–964.A. DiamondK. Lee2011Interventions shown to Aid Executive Function Development in Children 4–12 Years Old.Science333959964
21. Deary IJ, Penke L, Johnson W (2010) The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11: 201–211.IJ DearyL. PenkeW. Johnson2010The neuroscience of human intelligence differences.Nature Reviews Neuroscience11201211
22. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA (2004) Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 170–177.AR AronTW RobbinsRA Poldrack2004Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex.Trends in Cognitive Sciences8170177
23. Wager TD, Sylvester CY, Lacey SC, Nee DE, Franklin M, et al. (2005) Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fMRI. NeuroImage 27: 323–340.TD WagerCY SylvesterSC LaceyDE NeeM. Franklin2005Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fMRI.NeuroImage27323340
24. Wright I, Waterman M, Prescott H, Murdoch-Eaton D (2003) A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development: typical developmental trends. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 44: 561–575.I. WrightM. WatermanH. PrescottD. Murdoch-Eaton2003A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function development: typical developmental trends.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines44561575
25. Wechsler D (2003) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. French version published by the ECPA, Paris. D. Wechsler2003Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.French version published by the ECPA, Paris
26. McNab F, Leroux G, Strand F, Thorell L, Bergman S, et al. (2008) Common and unique components of inhibition and working memory: an fMRI, within-subjects investigation. Neuropsychologia 46: 2668–2682.F. McNabG. LerouxF. StrandL. ThorellS. Bergman2008Common and unique components of inhibition and working memory: an fMRI, within-subjects investigation.Neuropsychologia4626682682
27. Houdé O, Rossi S, Lubin A, Jolio M (2010) Mapping numerical processing, reading, and executive functions in the developing brain: an fMRI meta-analysis of 52 studies including 842 children. Developmental Science 13: 876–885.O. HoudéS. RossiA. LubinM. Jolio2010Mapping numerical processing, reading, and executive functions in the developing brain: an fMRI meta-analysis of 52 studies including 842 children.Developmental Science13876885
28. Beratis IN, Rabavilas A, Papadimitriou GN, Papageorgiou C (2010) Effect of handedness on the Stroop Colour Word Task. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition 15: 597–609.IN BeratisA. RabavilasGN PapadimitriouC. Papageorgiou2010Effect of handedness on the Stroop Colour Word Task.Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition15597609
29. MacLeod CM (1991) Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin 109: 163–203.CM MacLeod1991Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review.Psychological Bulletin109163203
30. Turner GR, Spreng RN (2012) Executive functions and neurocognitive aging: dissociable patterns of brain activity. Neurobiology of Aging 33(4): 826e1–13.GR TurnerRN Spreng2012Executive functions and neurocognitive aging: dissociable patterns of brain activity.Neurobiology of Aging33(4)826e113
31. Goldberg MC, Spinelli S, Joel S, Pekar JJ, Denckla MB, et al. (2011) Children with high functioning autism show increased prefrontal and temporal cortex activity during error monitoring. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1: 47–56.MC GoldbergS. SpinelliS. JoelJJ PekarMB Denckla2011Children with high functioning autism show increased prefrontal and temporal cortex activity during error monitoring.Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience14756
32. Strang N, Pruessner J, Pollak SD (2011) Developmental changes in adolescents’ neural response to challenge. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1: 560–569.N. StrangJ. PruessnerSD Pollak2011Developmental changes in adolescents’ neural response to challenge.Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience1560569
33. Ansari D, Dhital B (2006) Age-related changes in the activation of the intraparietal sulcus during nonsymbolic magnitude processing: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18: 1820–1828.D. AnsariB. Dhital2006Age-related changes in the activation of the intraparietal sulcus during nonsymbolic magnitude processing: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience1818201828
34. Kawashima R, Taira M, Okita K, Inoue K, Tajima N, et al. (2004) A functional MRI study of simple arithmetic: A comparison between children and adults. Cognitive Brain Research 18: 225–231.R. KawashimaM. TairaK. OkitaK. InoueN. Tajima2004A functional MRI study of simple arithmetic: A comparison between children and adults.Cognitive Brain Research18225231
35. Piazza M, Izard V, Pinel P, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S (2004) Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron 44: 547–555.M. PiazzaV. IzardP. PinelD. Le BihanS. Dehaene2004Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus.Neuron44547555
36. Dormal V, Pesenti M (2009) Common and specific contributions of the intraparietal sulci to numerosity and length processing. Human Brain Mapping 30: 2466–2476.V. DormalM. Pesenti2009Common and specific contributions of the intraparietal sulci to numerosity and length processing.Human Brain Mapping3024662476
37. Zacks JM (2008) Neuroimaging studies of mental rotation: A meta-analysis and review. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20: 1–19.JM Zacks2008Neuroimaging studies of mental rotation: A meta-analysis and review.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience20119
38. Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Ganis G (2006) The case for mental imagery. New York: Oxford University Press. 260 p. SM KosslynWL ThompsonG. Ganis2006The case for mental imagery. New York: Oxford University Press.260 p
39. Daurignac E, Houdé O, Jouvent R (2006) Negative priming in a numerical Piaget-like task as evidenced by ERP. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18: 730–736.E. DaurignacO. HoudéR. Jouvent2006Negative priming in a numerical Piaget-like task as evidenced by ERP.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience18730736
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2012 Poirel et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Although young children can accurately determine that two rows contain the same number of coins when they are placed in a one-to-one correspondence, children younger than 7 years of age erroneously think that the longer row contains more coins when the coins in one of the rows are spread apart. To demonstrate that prefrontal inhibitory control is necessary to succeed at this task (Piaget’s conservation-of-number task), we studied the relationship between the percentage of BOLD signal changes in the brain areas activated in this developmental task and behavioral performance on a Stroop task and a Backward Digit Span task. The level of activation in the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus was selectively related to inhibitory control efficiency (i.e., the Stroop task), whereas the activation in the left intraparietal sulcus (IPS) was selectively related to the ability to manipulate numerical information in working memory (i.e., the Backward Digit Span task). Taken together, the results indicate that to acquire number conservation, children’s brains must not only activate the reversibility of cognitive operations (supported by the IPS) but also inhibit a misleading length-equal-number strategy (supported by the right insula/inferior frontal gyrus).
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer