ARTICLE
Received 4 Sep 2014 | Accepted 3 Dec 2014 | Published 13 Jan 2015
T.J.H. Morgan1,2, N.T. Uomini3,4,5, L.E. Rendell1, L. Chouinard-Thuly1,6, S.E. Street1,7, H.M. Lewis1,8, C.P. Cross1,7, C. Evans1, R. Kearney1, I. de la Torre9, A. Whiten7 & K.N. Laland1
Hominin reliance on Oldowan stone toolswhich appear from 2.5 mya and are believed to have been socially transmittedhas been hypothesized to have led to the evolution of teaching and language. Here we present an experiment investigating the efcacy of transmission of Oldowan tool-making skills along chains of adult human participants (N 184) using ve different transmission mechanisms. Across six measures, transmission
improves with teaching, and particularly with language, but not with imitation or emulation. Our results support the hypothesis that hominin reliance on stone tool-making generated selection for teaching and language, and imply that (i) low-delity social transmission, such as imitation/emulation, may have contributed to the B700,000 year stasis of the Oldowan technocomplex, and (ii) teaching or proto-language may have been pre-requisites for the appearance of Acheulean technology. This work supports a gradual evolution of language, with simple symbolic communication preceding behavioural modernity by hundreds of thousands of years.
1 Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of Biology, University of St Andrews, Fife KY16 9AJ, UK. 2 Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720, USA. 3 Department of Archaeology, Classics and Egyptology, University of Liverpool, Liverpool L69 3GS, UK.
4 Department of Linguistics, Max-Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig 04103, Germany. 5 Department of Primatology, Max-Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig 04103, Germany. 6 Department of Biology, McGill University, Montral, Quebec, Canada H3A 1B1. 7 Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, Fife KY16 9JP, UK. 8 Department of Anthropology, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK. 9 Institute of Archaeology, University College London, London WC1H 0PY, UK. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to N.T.U. (email: mailto:[email protected]
Web End [email protected] ) or to K.N.L. (email: mailto:[email protected]
Web End [email protected] ).
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 6:6029 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 1
& 2015 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved.
DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029
Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029
From 2.5 million years ago, early hominins were skilled stone knappers, capable of producing more than 70 sharp akes from a single cobble core by striking it with a hammerstone
(termed the Oldowan technocomplex13; Fig. 1a, Supplementary Note 1 and Supplementary Figure 4). Existing remains show systematic ake detachment, maintenance of aking angles and repair of damaged cores4. This complexity, along with present-day tool-making experiments5, implies that Oldowan technology was learned and required considerable practice1,6. Furthermore, the technologys continual existence and wide geographic spread, along with hints of regional traditions3,7, indicate that it was socially transmitted, although the underlying psychological mechanisms remain poorly understood8.
Whether Oldowan stone tool making has implications for the evolution of human language and teaching (dened as active information donation9) is debated10,11. Positions range from the view that Oldowan tool making indicates a major development in hominin cognition8, such as teaching or language12, to the hypothesis that chimpanzee-like emulation or imitation (reproducing the object manipulations or motor patterns of others, respectively) is sufcient to transmit knapping technology13. Accordingly, accounts of the evolution of language range from a gradual emergence beginning 2 mya (refs 14,15) to a relatively sudden appearance 50100 kya (ref. 16). However, a difculty with positing complex Oldowan communication is the apparent stasis in Oldowan technology for more than 700,000 years until Acheulean tools appear B1.7 mya (refs 17,18). The absence of clear cultural change during this window seems inconsistent with the presence of language, and remains an outstanding mystery more generally19.
Across disciplines, researchers are increasingly turning to gene-culture co-evolutionary accounts to explain the evolution of human cognitive abilities, including teaching and
language10,13,2031. Central to such hypotheses is the idea that cultural traits can both shape and be shaped by genetic evolution, and a number of examples of gene-culture co-evolution are now known from human evolution2630. Hominin stone tool manufacture is a particularly interesting candidate case as the appearance of such technology 2.5 myaat the dawn of Homo and its continued deployment for millions of years, means it could have played a protracted role in human evolution. Furthermore, due to the challenging ecological niche that early hominins occupied20,32 and the difculty of acquiring tool-making skills6, tness benets were likely associated with the ability to make and deploy effective cutting tools32 as well as the ability to rapidly transmit the skills33, and so a co-evolutionary relationship between tool making and cognition, specically teaching and language, would seem plausible. Accordingly, Oldowan stone tool production could have generated selection for more complex forms of social transmission that enhanced the delity of information transmission. This could have resulted in a form of social transmission sufcient to transmit Acheulean technology reliably, and which would then generate selection for further increases in the complexity of social transmission, and so on. If this hypothesis is correct, changes in hominin cognition, including those underlying the appearance of Acheulean technology, could have depended upon selection generated by a reliance on Oldowan technology. In support of this hypothesis, archaeological remains show that changes to hominin morphology, including increased overall brain size, follow the advent of Oldowan tool making3. Other recent work has linked the cultural evolution of technologies to the capacity for high-delity social transmission9,3335. However, hitherto such studies have either been theoretical or limited to somewhat articial and abstract tasks. Accordingly, whether hominin lithic technology and social transmission genuinely represents a case of gene-culture co-evolution is currently unclear.
Hammer stone
Core Platform
edge
Flake
Platform
angle
Reverse engineering Imitation/emulation
Verbal teaching
!
Basic teaching
Gestural teaching
Trained experimenter
Trained experimenter
Figure 1 | Experimental design and structure. (a) A diagram of the stone knapping process. The hammerstone strikes the core with the goal of producing a ake. The platform edge and angle are important to the success of knapping. (bf) The ve learning conditions. (g) The structure of the experiment. For each condition, six chains were carried out (four short and two long); one of two trained experimenters started each chain (equally within each condition).
2 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 6:6029 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
& 2015 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029 ARTICLE
Experiments with contemporary humans have provided insights into the cognitive and motor processes supporting lithic technology23,24, and could also establish which mechanisms support its transmission. However, research on the social transmission of tool making is very limited. For instance, a review of Acheulean tool making found that reduction strategies were highly consistent across individuals36. The authors suggest true imitation (that is, reproducing the motor pattern of another individual through observational learning) is the minimal form of social transmission that could produce such consistency36. Furthermore, an unpublished experimental study found that demonstrative gestures were sufcient for the co-operative procurement and initial reduction of bedrock slabs37. Only two studies have directly investigated the ability of contemporary adult humans to make tools following different means of social transmission, both comparing the efcacy of speech with symbolic gestural communication. One investigated the acquisition of Levallois technology38 (a complex technology prevalent from 30030 kya) and reported no differences between the conditions. However, the measure of performance was a binary (yes/no) assessment by the experimenter, leaving the possibility that more subtle differences existed but were undetected. The second investigated bifacial knapping39 (a technique associated with Acheulean technology). Although the tools produced in both conditions showed similar shape, symmetry and quality, the two groups used different techniques, with verbally taught participants more accurately replicating the technique of the instructor (even though they lacked the skill to enact it effectively)39. As verbal and gestural communication are both symbolic forms of communication, further differences may yet emerge if a wider range of social transmission mechanisms, including imitation, emulation and subtle forms of pedagogy, are considered. This is particularly relevant to the manufacture of Oldowan technology, where the debate over the underlying transmission mechanisms is at its ercest.
Here we present a large-scale experimental study testing the capability of ve social learning mechanisms to transmit Oldowan stone knapping techniques across multiple transmission events. By establishing the relative rates of transmission resulting from different means of communication, we aimed to provide insights into which forms of communication might have been selected for as a result of reliance on tool use. The mechanisms investigated are summarized as (i) reverse engineering, (ii) imitation/ emulation, (iii) basic teaching, (iv) gestural teaching and (v) verbal teaching (Fig. 1bf). In total, 184 participants took part, producing over 6,000 pieces of int, each of which was weighed, measured and assessed for quality using a novel metric that we developed and veried. We nd that, across six measures, performance increases with teaching and, particularly, language. However, there is little evidence that imitation/emulation enhances transmission. Our ndings support a gene-culture co-evolutionary account of human evolution in which reliance on Oldowan tools would have generated selection favouring teaching and, ultimately, language. We suggest that Oldowan cultural evolution was limited, in part, by low-delity social transmission mechanisms. The appearance of Acheulean tools indicates the evolution of higher-delity social transmission, with teaching and/or some basic form of symbolic communication as plausible candidates. Accordingly, this work supports an early origin for language.
ResultsPerformance across conditions. Across numerous measures of individual performance, we consistently found that teaching and language, but not imitation or emulation, enhanced the
acquisition of stone knapping skills relative to reverse engineering (see Table 1). For instance, total ake quality only showed clear improvement with gestural or verbal teaching (Fig. 2a), with language nearly doubling performance relative to reverse engineering, and also improving performance relative to imitation/emulation and basic teaching. The number of viable akes produced shows a similar pattern (Fig. 2b), with substantial increases relative to reverse engineering requiring gestural or verbal teaching. Moreover, unlike all forms of teaching, imitation/ emulation did not increase the proportion of akes that were viable (Fig. 2c). Neither was there evidence for an increase in the rate of manufacture of viable akes with imitation/emulation; only verbal teaching was clearly associated with an increase (Fig. 2d). Similarly, only verbal teaching led to a clear increase (430%) in the volume of core reduced (Fig. 2e). Finally, although there was no evidence that imitation/emulation increased the probability of a viable ake per hit, gestural teaching doubled and verbal teaching quadrupled this probability (Fig. 2f). Across the six measures there is strong evidence that verbal teaching increases performance relative to gestural teaching. Thus, teaching, but particularly verbal teaching, greatly facilitated the rapid transmission of aking, whereas there is little evidence that imitation/emulation did so.
Performance along chains. In all conditions, as expected, performance decreased along chains relative to the trained experimenter as information was lost. However, with teaching, transmission was sufciently improved that performance declined steadily along chains, whereas without teaching, the drop in performance along chains was so severe that performance immediately fell to oor levels (that is, the minimal level of performance we observed, likely representing participants intuitive understanding of stone knapping). For instance, with verbal teaching, the probability that each hit produced a viable ake (Fig. 2g), the number of viable akes produced, and the proportion of akes that were viable (Fig. 2h) all decreased steadily along chains, approaching the baseline performance observed with reverse engineering and imitation/emulation (see Table 2). Analyses of the utterances by participants in the verbal teaching condition showed that both the total number of utterances spoken and the proportion of teaching-related utterances that were correct also decreased along the chain (Fig. 2i). The rate of decline varied with topic, with knowledge of both the exterior platform angle and force-carrying ridges rapidly lost, but information concerning the platform edge being preserved for longer and with greater accuracy (see Table 2).
For a full listing of all model estimates, see Supplementary Tables 16.
DiscussionThe central nding of this work is that the social transmission of Oldowan technology is enhanced by teaching and, in particular, by language. This is in line with a gene-culture co-evolutionary account of human evolution and supports the hypothesis that Oldowan stone tool manufacture generated selection favouring increasingly complex teaching and language13,24,40. Although the learning period in this experiment (at 5 min long) is clearly unrealistically short compared with the length of time that Oldowan hominins likely had available to learn, particularly given available data showing that precise control of conchoidal fracture can take decades to acquire41 and anthropological data showing that knapping skills are acquired across an apprenticeship lasting several years42, a short learning period is sufcient to examine the relative rates of transmission, which is the focus of this work. As such, we cannot rule out the possibility that with a longer learning
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 6:6029 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 3
& 2015 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved.
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029
Table 1 | Effects of different transmission mechanisms on performance.
Variable Condition
Reverse engineering Imitation/emulation Basic teaching Gestural teaching Verbal teaching Total quality 13.0 (9.2, 17.9) 15.7 (11.1, 21.4) 15.4 (11.1, 20.7) 19.8 (14.6, 26.7) 23.6 (17.0, 31.9)
Number of viable akes 15.76 (12.1, 0.47) 18.31 (14.07, 23.56) 19.56 (15.08, 25.37) 21.73 (16.77, 28.32) 25.22 (19.42, 33.02) Proportion of akes that are viable 0.55 (0.48, 0.62) 0.58 (0.52, 0.64) 0.72 (0.66, 0.77) 0.72 (0.67, 0.77) 0.73 (0.68, 0.78) Viable akes per minute 1.96 (1.33, 2.87) 1.98 (1.35, 2.85) 2.55 (1.78, 3.69) 2.95 (2.03, 4.36) 3.37 (2.26, 5.19) Proportion of core knapped 0.44 (0.35, 0.54) 0.46 (0.37, 0.56) 0.53 (0.43, 0.63) 0.51 (0.43, 0.62) 0.59 (0.48, 0.71) Probability of a viable ake per hit 0.03 (0.02, 0.05) 0.04 (0.03, 0.06) 0.06 (0.04, 0.08) 0.07 (0.05, 0.10) 0.10 (0.07, 0.16)
Estimated values for parameters at the rst position in the chain for different conditions. Quoted values are median model estimates and their 95% central credible intervals.
40 ** **
**
*
Total quality of all flakes
Viable flakes per
minute knapping time
40 0.800.750.700.650.600.550.50
35
30
25
20
15
**
*
**
**
35 30 25
Number of viable
flakes produced
Proportion of flakes
that are viable
* *
20 15 10
5
0.8
**
**
*
**
4
3
2
Expected proportion
of core reduced
0.7
0.6
0.5
0.4
Probability of a viable
flake per hit
Total number of utterances
0.15
0.10
0.05
0.00
*
0.80 100 10.9
0.8
0.7
0.6
0.5
90 80 70 60 50 40
0.14
Probability of a viable
flake per hit
0.120.100.080.060.040.02
Proportion of flakes
that are viable
0.750.700.650.600.550.50
Probability teaching
utterance is correct
1 2 3 4 5 Position along chain
1 2 3 4 5
Position along chain
1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Position along chain
Figure 2 | Performance across conditions and along chains. Values shown are the median model estimates and the corresponding 95% central credible intervals. More complex forms of communication, in particular verbal teaching, increased several measures of participant performance, including(a) the total quality of all akes, (b) the number of viable akes, (c) the proportion of akes that were viable, (d) the rate at which viable akes were made, (e) the proportion of the core knapped and (f) the probability that each hit resulted in a viable ake. The brackets marked with double asterisks indicate contrasts for which there is strong evidence of a difference (95% credible interval excluding 0), single asterisks indicate cases for which there is weak evidence of a difference (90% credible interval excluding 0). The red bracket in c indicates that the increase in performance from imitation/emulation to basic teaching is greater than the increase between all other adjacent conditions. (g,h) Although verbal and gestural teaching increased the probability of a viable ake per hit and the proportion of akes that were viable, performance in these conditions decreased along chains such that across conditions performance was similar by position 5. With reverse engineering, performance did not decline along chains, suggesting it was already at oor levels. Position 1 corresponds to the rst participant, not the trained experimenter. (i) With verbal teaching, both the total number of utterances (left hand bars) and the probability a teaching utterance was correct (right hand bars) decreased along chains. Key: reverse engineering: blue (n 37), imitation/emulation:
green (n 34), basic teaching: yellow (n 38), gestural teaching: orange (n 37), verbal teaching: red (n 38).
period, performance across conditions would have converged. However, given that knapping skills are known to take years to develop fully6,41, we suspect that increasing the time spent
learning would initially only increase the differences in performance across conditions, with any convergence only occurring after extensive learning. Given their magnitude, the
4 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 6:6029 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
& 2015 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029 ARTICLE
Table 2 | Effects of position along chains on performance.
Variable Condition Gradient/rate of change Extent of change Number of viable akes Verbal teaching 0.07 ( 0.10, 0.04)
Proportion of akes that are viable Basic teaching 0.06 ( 0.10, 0.01)
Gestural teaching 0.11 ( 0.15, 0.06)
Verbal teaching 0.08 ( 0.13, 0.03)
Probability of a viable ake per hit Imitation/emulation 0.08 ( 0.12, 0.05)
Basic teaching 0.04 ( 0.08, 0.00)
Gestural teaching 0.12 ( 0.16, 0.08)
Verbal teaching 0.33 ( 0.38, 0.28)
Total utterances Verbal teaching 1.2 (0.63, 14.0) 42.2 ( 29.3, 58.9)
Proportion of teaching utterances correct Verbal teaching 1.4 (0.56, 45.8) 4.0 ( 1.4, 6.9)
Platform angle teaching accuracy Verbal teaching 3.99 (0.0, 128.1) 0.75 (3.21, 1.91)
Ridge teaching accuracy Verbal teaching 0.42 (0.1766, 1.10) 3.69 ( 1.95, 6.75)
Platform edge teaching accuracy Verbal teaching 0.00 (0.0, 0.09) 1.18 (4.78, 4.12)
Force required teaching accuracy Verbal teaching 0.00 (0.0, 0.03) 0.53 (4.73, 3.489)
Quoted values are median model estimates and their 95% central credible intervals. Where only the gradient is given, a negative change corresponds to a decrease along chains; where both rate and
extent are given, the rate is a scalar quantity and a negative extent corresponds to a decrease along chains. Values in italics represent cases where the 95% credible interval did not exclude 0, but the
90% interval did (that is, weak, but not strong evidence).
observed differences in performance between conditions would likely translate into signicant tness differences in the shorter term. Key to our ndings support of a gene-culture co-evolutionary account of human technology and cognition is the continuous improvement in the rate of transmission observed with increasingly complex forms of communication. For example, if verbal teaching provided transmission benets, but simpler forms of teaching did not, then the co-evolutionary process would not be able to account for the evolution of these simpler forms of teaching. Likewise, if the transmission of tool making benetted from simple teaching, but gained no further benet from verbal teaching, then the co-evolutionary process would stop with simpler forms of teaching and could not explain the evolution of verbal teaching.
Accordingly, our data imply that Oldowan tool making would have created a continuous selective gradient leading from observational learning to much more complex verbal teaching. This process need not have taken place entirely within the Oldowan, but was probably already underway during the Oldowan and likely continued well after, as Oldowan tools continued to be made for hundreds of thousands of years beyond the Oldowan time period. Furthermore, assuming that the transmission of more complex technologies also benets from more complex means of communication, later technologies would have reinforced the gene-culture co-evolutionary dynamic. Such a process could have lasted for millions of years (and may be ongoing29), with more complex communication allowing the stable and rapid transmission of increasingly complex technologies, which in turn generate selection for even more complex communication and cognition, and so forth. Although this places little necessary constraint on when teaching and language may have evolved, our central contribution is to provide evidence that Oldowan tools, produced by hominins since at least2.5 mya, were involved in this dynamic.
A second signicant nding of this work is that the rate of
transmission of Oldowan tool making is, at best, minimally enhanced by the addition of imitation/emulation relative to reverse engineering. That the low level of performance with imitation/emulation and reverse engineering is stable along chains (and that performance with teaching and language collapses to this level) suggests a baseline level of performance reliant on little transmitted knowledge, and which could well be achieved through intuition and individual trial-and-error learning. We suggest that the rapid decline of performance with
teaching and language to this baseline merely reects the short learning time employed in this study. Previous transmission chain studies have established that periods of individual practice can bolster the stability of socially transmitted knowledge43. This suggests that with more time to learn, with bouts of teaching and language integrated with periods of individual practice, the benets of teaching and language would likely have been preserved for longer. Likewise, a benet of observational learning relative to reverse engineering may well appear over a longer learning period. However, our data suggest that any such benet is likely to be less than the benet that would be derived through teaching across a similar timespan because of the improved rate of transmission with teaching. Accordingly, although we do not suggest that imitation is insufcient to transmit the technology per se, our ndings support other recent work in implying that observation alone is an inefcient means to acquire stone tool-making skills23,44,45.
Limited information concerning tool manufacture can, no doubt, be rapidly acquired through imitation or emulation; for instance, the basics of core, hammerstone or ake selection36, the requirement to strike the core with the hammerstone and some idea of the force are required. However, it seems plausible that the rapid striking action associated with tool manufacture hinders the transmission of more subtle information crucial to knapping, such as details of the point of percussion or the platform edge and angle, through observation alone. It is here that teaching (for example, slowing down the striking action, pointing to appropriate targets, demonstrating core rotation, manual shaping of pupils grasp) and verbal instruction likely provide immediate benets to the pupil. Indeed, transcripts from the verbal teaching condition show that abstract knapping concepts, such as the platform angle, were transmitted between individuals in the verbal teaching condition (see Supplementary Fig. 3). It may well be the capacity for arbitrary labels such as platform angle that facilitates transmission with verbal teaching; such labels break the task into constituent parts, can be used to identify the important elements and provide a clear framework with which pupils can go on to teach others. Language not only allows transmission of the skill itself, but also the ability to transmit the skill to others effectively.
Third, our ndings have implications for one of the most enduring puzzles of human evolution: the apparent stasis of the Oldowan technocomplex, which lasted 700,000 years8,11,19,45. Our experiment suggests that Oldowan technological change
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 6:6029 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 5
& 2015 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved.
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029
could have been restricted by low-delity forms of social transmission that prevented the spread of innovations. This suggestion is supported by the slow spread of Oldowan technology across Africa, which indicates that this technology was difcult for Oldowan hominins to transmit3. Furthermore, the acquisition of Oldowan knapping skills is not trivial even for modern humans, as shown by our nding that the benets of teaching and language were rapidly lost in transmission. Although we cannot conclusively identify what form Oldowan transmission might have taken, our data indicate imitation or emulation as likely candidates. In naturalistic contexts, the relatively poor transmission that we observed with imitation and emulation could well be too slow and imprecise for innovations to be transmitted reliably, leaving the technology unable to increase in complexity until more effective communication had evolved.
The suggestion that low-delity social transmission is a limiting factor on technological development might contribute to an understanding of why human culture is so complex compared with the behavioural traditions of non-human animals46,47. Although human social transmission has allowed the cumulative elaboration of a vast number of technologies and behaviours, non-human animal social transmission has not. It seems possible that this is because non-human animal social transmission, which appears to be largely limited to forms of observational learning less sophisticated than those of humans43, lacks the delity required to transmit more complex innovations, thus constraining cumulative cultural evolution34,35,48. Even the modest knapping ability of extensively trained bonobos49,50 may rely on their prior training in symbolic communication51. Although it is plausible that a similar co-evolutionary process has operated to a lesser degree in some other species, such as other apes52, it remains an open question as to why their tool use did not generate selection for the higher-delity social transmission (teaching and language) observed in humans. One possibility is that the technologies of other apes are either sufciently simple that they can be acquired through more basic mechanisms or so hard to acquire that they can only rarely be transmitted successfully, removing the benet to teaching9. Task difculty might also explain a previous experimental nding that simple transmission mechanisms were sufcient for cumulative cultural evolution in the context of human paper-plane design53; this task may be sufciently simple that teaching is of little benet. Alternatively, ape reliance on tool use could be insufcient for the benets of tool-use to outweigh the costs of complex social transmission, thus preventing teaching from increasing tness9. Any of these constraints would undermine selection for higher-delity social transmission, hindering the co-evolutionary process.
Given that our ndings support a co-evolution of Oldowan tool use and complex communication, it might seem puzzling that the Oldowan stasis should last so long. If the selective advantage was present, why did more complex communication not evolve for 700,000 years? A likely explanation is that more complex communication may well have evolved during the Oldowan, but that this alone was insufcient for the evolution of stone tool technology. The appearance of Acheulean tools may have additionally been contingent on the evolution of other aspects of cognition, such as technical comprehension or the hierarchical planning of actions5456, as well as demographic and socio-ecological factors57,58. Accordingly, the extraordinary length of the Oldowan stasis could indicate that a large number of limiting factors needed to be overcome before innovations could appear and spread.
Given this, our ndings imply that the appearance of Acheulean tools 1.7 mya (refs 17,18) reects, in part, the
evolution of mechanisms of transmission that facilitated the more effective transmission of Oldowan tool-making, but also enabled the reliable transmission of the sub-goals and techniques required to make the distinctive and regularly shaped Acheulean tools59. We cannot specify the form of this transmission with precision. However, given the observation that chimpanzees are capable of some form of observational learning, yet cannot produce stone tools approaching the quality of the earliest known Oldowan examples13, combined with the complexity of Acheulean technology36, we suggest that teaching in the form of facilitated observation (similar to our basic teaching condition) is the minimal plausible form of social transmission for Acheulean hominins and that rudimentary forms of language are a possibility. However, although our ndings suggest that Oldowan hominins would have benetted from modern language, the suggestion that modern language evolved during the Oldowan seems unlikely given how slowly technology evolved thereafter. This leaves open the possibility that the transmission of Acheulean technology was reliant on a form of (gestural or verbal) proto-language12,60,61. This need not imply that Acheulean hominins were capable of manipulating a large number of symbols or generating complex grammars. Our ndings imply that simple forms of positive or negative reinforcement, or directing the attention of a learner to specic points (as was common in the gestural teaching condition), are considerably more successful in transmitting stone knapping than observation alone. This is supported by existing theoretical work that suggests positive and negative feedback greatly enhances the rate of transmission33. Whether or not simple symbolic communication was present during the Acheulean, we anticipate that the gene-culture co-evolutionary dynamic between tools and communication was, and that it would continue beyond the Acheulean, generating selection favouring the use of symbols for increasingly subtle and abstract concepts, and contributing to the eventual evolution of modern language capabilities.
In sum, our data support the hypothesis that a gene-culture co-evolutionary dynamic between tool use and social transmission was on-going in human evolution, starting at least 2.5 mya and potentially continuing to the present. The simplicity and stasis of Oldowan technology are indicative of a limited form of social transmission, such as observational learning, that only allowed the transmission of the broadest concepts of stone knapping technology. Whatever its nature, this was sufcient to support limited transmission among individuals with prolonged contact, but insufcient to propagate innovations more rapidly than they were lost, and would have contributed to the stasis in the Oldowan technocomplex. However, hominin reliance on stone technology would have generated selection for increasingly complex communication that allowed the more effective spread of stone-tools. Under this continued selection, teaching, symbolic communication and eventually verbal language may have been favoured, allowing the ready transmission of abstract aking concepts, such as the role of the exterior platform angle in choosing where to strike38, which our ndings shown are effectively transmitted by language. Given the increased complexity of the later Acheulean and Mousterian lithic technologies, with their reliance on long sequences of hierarchically organized actions36,38 and other abstract concepts, our results imply that hominins possessed a capacity for teachingand potentially simple proto-languageas early as1.7 mya.
Methods
Participants and materials. One hundred and eighty-four participants took part in the study. This sample size was chosen based on effect sizes observed in previous transmission chain studies. Participants were students at the University of
6 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 6:6029 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
& 2015 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029 ARTICLE
St Andrews recruited through the Universitys experimental sign-up system. Across the experiment, we used 2 tonnes of Brandon int from Norfolk, UK, broken up into cores of roughly 1 kg. We also used 100 granite hammerstones collected from the coastline near Stonehaven, Scotland.
Experimental design. Adult human participants (N 184) rst learned, were
tested on their ability, and then helped others to learn, to knap stone akes using a granite hammerstone and int core, across ve cumulatively complex transmission conditions (see Fig. 1bf): (i) Reverse Engineering: pupils were provided with a core and hammerstone for practice, but saw only the akes manufactured by their tutor and not their tutor themselves; (ii) Imitation/Emulation: in addition to having their own core and hammerstone, pupils also observed their tutor making akes, but could not interact with them; (iii) Basic Teaching: in addition to demonstrating tool production, tutors could also manually shape the pupils grasp of their hammerstone or core, slow their own actions and reorient themselves to allow the pupil a clear view (this condition replicates teaching reported in non-human primates62);(iv) Gestural Teaching: tutors and pupils could also interact using any gestures, but no vocalizations and (v) Verbal Teaching: tutors and pupils were also permitted to speak. Participants were assigned to conditions at random and blinding was not possible. The test given to participants to assess their ability was to make as many good-quality akes as possible from a single core. This reected pressures on hominin knappers to make the most of the limited availability of high-quality knapping materials.
Participants were arranged into transmission chains63 in which information was passed along chains of participants, with each participant learning from the previous participant and acting as tutor to the next participant. For each condition, we carried out four short chains (r5 participants) and two long chains(r10 participants) per condition (see Fig. 1g). Experimenters trained in stone knapping (T.J.H.M. and N.T.U.) acted as tutor to the rst participant.
To ensure participant motivation, we paid participants between d10 and d20, with the value dependent upon their performance when tested. In the teaching conditions (conditions 35), participants payment was also dependent upon how well their pupils went on to perform; thus tutors were motivated to teach effectively. In the imitation/emulation condition (condition 2), participants payment was also dependent upon how well they performed when demonstrating, this was to motivate demonstrators to focus on their own performance and not to teach the pupil.
Procedure. Upon arrival, participants were briefed on the experimental procedure and their consent was required to proceed (ethical approval was given by St Andrews UTREC, code: BL6376). Before they learnt to knap, and to ensure that participants understood what Oldowan tools were used for, participants were given an information sheet, int akes of varying quality, chamois leather and wooden sticks. They were then given 5 min to use these items to gain an understanding of what made a good-quality sharp cutting ake. The information sheet gave only very brief information on the history and uses of Oldowan stone tools, and not any information as to how to make them beyond striking a int core with a hammerstone.
The learning/teaching period lasted for 5 min, after which participants were interrupted. After the learning phase, the pupil then advanced to the test phase. Participants were instructed to take as long as they needed for the test phase, however, if they had not stopped within 18 min, the experimenter encouraged them to nish and after 20 min the experimenter instructed them to stop (only 12.5% of participants used the full 20 min). After the test phase (if applicable), participants went on to teach the next pupil. Once the procedure was complete, participants were debriefed and paid before leaving.
Data. All ints used by participants were bagged throughout the experiment. In total, participants produced 6,214 pieces of int greater than 2 cm in diameter. All of these pieces were weighed, measured and assessed for viability (that is, whether they had possible use as a cutting tool) and quality (using a novel metric, which we developed, that took into account ake mass, cutting edge length and diameter; see Supplementary Methods for details). Any pieces less than 2 cm across were not coded, as 2 cm was considered to be the minimum size for a ake to possibly have utility as a butchery tool64. We also weighed participants cores both before and after knapping. Participants behaviour during the experiment was recorded using video cameras and we subsequently measured the length of time participants spent knapping and the number of times participants struck their core with their hammerstone. We also transcribed everything participants said while in the verbal teaching condition and split it into utterances (N 1,481) for analysis. In
particular, all utterances were coded as either correct or incorrect, which was determined relative to established knapping practices. The robustness of ake viability ratings, as well as video coding, was tested by triple and double coding, respectively, a subset of the data. In both cases, the level of agreement between coders was very high (see Supplementary Methods for details of the double/triple coding procedure).
Analyses. We analysed the data using Bayesian GLMMs tted using MCMC methods in OpenBUGS65,66. We modelled six different measures of individual
performance: (i) the number of viable akes produced, (ii) the total quality of akes produced, (iii) the proportion of akes that were viable, (iv) the rate at which viable akes were produced, (v) the probability of a viable ake per hit and (vi) the proportion of their core successfully reduced. These measures were modelled as a function of condition, position along the chain, interactions between condition and position, initial core mass and random repeat-level effects.
For a full description of the experimental procedure and all analyses, see Supplementary Methods. For a comparison of the model results with the raw data, see Supplementary Figs 1 and 2.
References
1. Roche, H. et al. Early hominid stone tool production and technical skill 2.34 Myr ago in West Turkana, Kenya. Nature 399, 5760 (1999).
2. Semaw, S., Renne, P., Harris, J. W. K. & Feibel, C. S. 2.5-Million-year-old stone tools from Gona, Ethiopia. Nature 385, 333336 (1997).
3. Schick, K. & Toth, N. in Oldowan Case Stud. into Earliest Stone Age (eds Toth,N. & Schick, K.) (Gosport: Stone Age Institute, 2006).4. Delagnes, A. & Roche, H. Late Pliocene hominid knapping skills:the case of Lokalalei 2C, West Turkana, Kenya. J. Hum. Evol. 48, 435472 (2005).
5. Toth, N. Behavioral inferences from early stone artifact assemblages: an experimental model. J. Hum. Evol. 16, 763787 (1987).
6. Callahan, E. The Basics of Biface Knapping in the Eastern Fluted Point Tradition: A Manual for Flintknappers and Lithic Analysts (Eastern States Archaeological Federation, 1979).
7. Braun, D. R., Plummer, T., Ditcheld, P. W., Bishop, L. C. & Ferraro, J. V. in Interdiscip. Approaches to Oldowan (eds Hovers, E. & Braun, D. R.) 99110 (Springer, 2009).
8. Hovers, E. in Origins of Human Innovation and Creativity. (ed. Elias, S.). Developments in Quaternary Science Vol. 16 (ed. van der Meer, J. J. M.) 5168 (Elsevier B.V., 2012).
9. Fogarty, L., Strimling, P. & Laland, K. N. The evolution of teaching. Evolution 65, 27602770 (2011).
10. Gibson, K. & Ingold, T. Tools, Language and Cognition in Human Evolution (Cambridge Univ., 1993).
11. Ambrose, S. H. Paleolithic technology and human evolution. Science 291, 17481753 (2001).
12. Bickerton, D. Adams Tongue (Hill and Wang, 2009).13. Wynn, T., Hernandez-Aguilar, A., Marchant, L. F. & McGrew, W. C.An apes view of the Oldowan revisited. Evol. Anthropol. 20, 181197 (2011).
14. Belfer-Cohen, A. & Goren-Inbar, N. Cognition and communication in the Levantine Lower Palaeolithic. World Archaeol. 26, 144157 (1994).
15. DErrico, F. et al. Archaeological evidence for the emergence of language, symbolism, and music an alternative multidisciplinary perspective. J. World Prehistory 17, 170 (2003).
16. Mellars, P. Why did modern human populations disperse from Africa ca. 60,000 years ago? A new model. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 93819386 (2006).
17. Beyene, Y. et al. The characteristics and chronology of the earliest Acheulean at Konso, Ethiopia. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 15841591 (2013).
18. Lepre, C. J. et al. An earlier origin for the Acheulian. Nature 477, 8285 (2011).
19. De la Torre, I. The origins of stone tool technology in Africa: a historical perspective. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 366, 10281037 (2011).
20. Blumenschine, R. J. Early Hominid Scavenging Opportunities: Implications of Carcass Availability in the Serengeti and Ngorongoro Ecosystems (B. A. R. Archaeopress, 1986).
21. Enquist, M., Ghirlanda, S., Jarrick, A. & Wachtmeister, C.-A. Why does human culture increase exponentially? Theor. Popul. Biol. 74, 4655 (2008).22. Sterelny, K. Language, gesture, skill: the co-evolutionary foundations of language. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 367, 21412151 (2012).
23. Stout, D., Toth, N., Schick, K., Stout, J. & Hutchins, G. Stone tool-making and brain activation: position emission tomography (PET) studies. J. Archaeol. Sci. 27, 12151223 (2000).
24. Uomini, N. T. & Meyer, G. F. Shared brain lateralization patterns in language and Acheulean stone tool production: a functional transcranial Doppler ultrasound study. PLoS ONE 8, e72693 (2013).
25. Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J. & Henrich, J. The cultural niche: why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108(Suppl 2), 1091810925 (2011).
26. Tishkoff, S. A. et al. Convergent adaptation of human lactase persistence in Africa and Europe. Nat. Genet. 39, 3140 (2007).
27. Durham, W. H. Coevolution: Genes, Culture and Human Diversity (Stanford Univ., 1991).
28. Hnemeier, T. et al. Evolutionary responses to a constructed niche: ancient Mesoamericans as a model of gene-culture coevolution. PLoS ONE 7, e38862 (2012).
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 6:6029 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 7
& 2015 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved.
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029
29. Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J. & Myles, S. How culture shaped the human genome: bringing genetics and the human sciences together. Nat. Rev. Genet. 11, 137148 (2010).
30. Richerson, P. J., Boyd, R. & Henrich, J. Gene-culture coevolution in the age of genomics. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107(Suppl 2), 89858992 (2010).
31. Feldman, M. W. & Laland, K. N. Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. Trends Ecol. Evol. 5347, 453457 (1996).
32. Potts, R. Hominin evolution in settings of strong environmental variability. Quat. Sci. Rev. 73, 113 (2013).
33. Castro, L. & Toro, M. A. The evolution of culture: from primate social learning to human culture. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 1023510240 (2004).
34. Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B. & Laland, K. N. Identication of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science 335, 11141118 (2012).
35. Lewis, H. M. & Laland, K. N. Transmission delity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 367, 21712180 (2012).
36. Shipton, C. B. K., Petraglia, M. & Paddayya, K. Stone tool experiments and reduction methods at the Acheulean site of Isampur Quarry, India. Antiquity 83, 769785 (2009).
37. Petraglia, M., Shipton, C. B. K. & Paddayya, K. in Hominid Individ. Context Archaeol. Investig. Low. Middle Palaeolithic Landscapes, Locales Artefacts (eds Gamble, C. & Porr, M.) (Routledge, 2005).
38. Ohnuma, K., Aoki, K. & Akazawa, T. Transmission of tool-making through verbal and non-verbal communication-preliminary experiments in Levallois ake production. Anthropol. Sci. 105, 159168 (1997).
39. Putt, S. S., Woods, A. D. & Franciscus, R. G. The role of verbal interaction during experimental bifacial stone tool manufacture. Lithic Technol. 39, 96112 (2014).
40. Stout, D. in Stone Tools Evol. Hum. Cogn. (eds Nowell, A. & Davidson, I.) 159184 (Univ. Press of Colorado, 2010).
41. Nonaka, T., Bril, B. & Rein, R. How do stone knappers predict and control the outcome of aking? Implications for understanding early stone tool technology.J. Hum. Evol. 59, 155167 (2010).42. Stout, D. Skill and cognition in stone tool production: An Ethnographic Case Study from Irian Jaya. Curr. Anthropol. 43, 693723 (2002).
43. Hoppitt, W. J. E. & Laland, K. N. Social Learning: An Introduction to Mechanisms, Methods, and Models 320 (Princeton Univ., 2013).
44. Uomini, N. T. The prehistory of handedness: archaeological data and comparative ethology. J. Hum. Evol. 57, 411419 (2009).
45. Stout, D., Semaw, S., Rogers, M. J. & Cauche, D. Technological variation in the earliest Oldowan from Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. J. Hum. Evol. 58, 474491 (2010).
46. Laland, K. N. & Galef, Jr B. G. The Question of Animal Culture 320 (2009).47. Whiten, A. The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and ancestral apes. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 366, 9971007 (2011).
48. Tomasello, M. in Chimpanzee Cult. (eds Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M. & Heltne, P. G.) (Harvard Univ., 1994).
49. Roffman, I., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Rubert-Pugh, E., Ronen, A. & Nevo, E. Stone tool production and utilization by bonobo-chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109, 1450014503, doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1212855109/-550 /DCSupplemental.www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1212855109
Web End =10.1073/pnas.1212855109 (2012).
50. Toth, N. & Schick, K. The Oldowan: the tool making of early hominins and chimpanzees compared. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 38, 289305 (2009).
51. Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Fields, W. M. & Spircu, T. The emergence of knapping and vocal expression embedded in a Pan/Homo culture. Biol. Philos. 19, 541575 (2004).
52. Whiten, A. & van Schaik, C. P. The evolution of animal cultures and social intelligence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 362, 603620 (2007).
53. Caldwell, C. a. & Millen, A. E. Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution: is imitation necessary? Psychol. Sci. 20, 14781483 (2009).
54. Stout, D. Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 10501059 (2011).
55. Pelegrin, J. in Use Tools by Human Non-human Primates (eds Berthelet, A. & Chavaillon, J.) doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof
Web End =10.1093/acprof (Oxford Univ., 1993).
56. Dennett, D. Darwins Dangerous Idea (Simon & Schuster, 1995).57. Powell, A., Shennan, S. J. & Thomas, M. G. Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324, 12981301 (2009).
58. Potts, R. Environmental hypotheses of hominin evolution. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 27, 93136 (1998).
59. Gowlett, J. in Axe Age Acheulian Tool-Making from Quarry to Discard (eds Goren-Inbar, N. & Sharon, G.) (Equinox, 2006).
60. Donald, M. Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition (Harvard Univ., 1991).
61. Corballis, M. C. The Lopsided Ape: Evolution of the Generative Mind (Oxford Univ., 1993).
62. Boesch, C. Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Anim. Behav. 41, 530532 (1991).
63. Mesoudi, A. & Whiten, A. The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 34893501 (2008).
64. Key, A. J. M. & Lycett, S. J. Are bigger akes always better? An experimental assessment of ake size variation on cutting efciency and loading. J. Archaeol. Sci. 41, 140146 (2014).
65. Lunn, D. & Spiegelhalter, D. The BUGS project: Evolution, critique and future directions. Stat. Med. 28, 30493067 (2009).
66. Ntzoufras, I. Bayesian Modeling Using WinBUGS (Wiley, 2009).
Acknowledgements
Research supported in part by an ERC Advanced Grant to K.N.L. (EVOCULTURE, ref: 232823) and grants to N.T.U. from the British Academy (Centenary Project Lucy to Language: the Archaeology of the Social Brain) and the Leverhulme Trust (ECF 0298). We are grateful to Gillian Brown, Richard Byrne, Jane Rees and Stephen Shennan for helpful comments on earlier drafts. We thank John and Val Lord for supplying us with int.
Author contributions
T.J.H.M., N.T.U., L.E.R. and K.N.L. designed the experiment; T.J.H.M., N.T.U., L.E.R.,L.C-.T., S.E.S., H.M.L., C.P.C. and C.E. executed the experiment; T.J.H.M., N.T.U.,I.D.l.T. and R.K. coded the data; T.J.H.M. carried out the analyses; all authors contributed to the preparation of the manuscript.
Additional information
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =http://www.nature.com/ http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =naturecommunications
Competing nancial interests: The authors declare no competing nancial interests.
Reprints and permission information is available online at http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =http://npg.nature.com/ http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =reprintsandpermissions/
How to cite this article: Morgan, T. J. H. et al. Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nat. Commun. 6:6029 doi: 10.1038/ncomms7029 (2015).
8 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 6:6029 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms7029 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
& 2015 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Nature Publishing Group Jan 2015
Abstract
Hominin reliance on Oldowan stone tools--which appear from 2.5 mya and are believed to have been socially transmitted--has been hypothesized to have led to the evolution of teaching and language. Here we present an experiment investigating the efficacy of transmission of Oldowan tool-making skills along chains of adult human participants (N=184) using five different transmission mechanisms. Across six measures, transmission improves with teaching, and particularly with language, but not with imitation or emulation. Our results support the hypothesis that hominin reliance on stone tool-making generated selection for teaching and language, and imply that (i) low-fidelity social transmission, such as imitation/emulation, may have contributed to the ~700,000 year stasis of the Oldowan technocomplex, and (ii) teaching or proto-language may have been pre-requisites for the appearance of Acheulean technology. This work supports a gradual evolution of language, with simple symbolic communication preceding behavioural modernity by hundreds of thousands of years.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer