Introduction
Evidence from experimental and human studies (reviewed by Eubig et al. 2010) suggests that prenatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) can adversely affect children's behavior. During infancy, breastfeeding represents the primary, if not exclusive, source of postnatal PCB exposure and can lead to childhood blood PCB levels that exceed prenatal levels as measured in cord blood (Ayotte et al. 2003). However, the few epidemiologic studies that have assessed the association between postnatal exposure to PCBs and attention and activity, for example, have not reported consistent evidence of adverse effects (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003; Jacobson et al. 1990; Plusquellec et al. 2010; Verner et al. 2010; Vreugdenhil et al. 2002). This is surprising given the extensive brain development that occurs postnatally, including cell differentiation that has been shown to be altered by PCBs in human neuroprogenitor cells in vitro (Fritsche et al. 2005).
Failure to observe associations between postnatal PCB exposure and neurobehavior may be related to methodological and logistical challenges to adequately characterizing exposure during infancy and early childhood. Because PCB levels in breast milk correlate with prenatal levels as indicated by maternal and cord blood levels (Muckle et al. 2001), it is difficult to identify the independent role of lactational exposures on health measures. In addition, the association between lactational exposures and behavior is confounded by breastfeeding duration, which is positively associated with both infants' blood PCB levels (Ayotte et al. 2003) and improved child neurobehavior (Julvez et al. 2007). Furthermore, estimating PCB body burden in infancy and early childhood presents additional obstacles. Because collecting serial blood samples in infancy is often impractical due to cost and logistical and ethical considerations, researchers have used metrics based on breast milk PCB levels and duration of breastfeeding (Jacobson et al. 1990; Vreugdenhil et al. 2002) or single blood measurements in later childhood (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003; Jacobson et al. 1990). None of these approaches allow estimation of PCB exposure at different periods of postnatal development, some of which may be more sensitive to PCBs than others.
As an efficient alternative, we recently developed a pharmacokinetic model based on maternal and child physiology and biochemistry to estimate profiles of persistent organic pollutant levels in children's body lipids (including blood lipids) during and after breastfeeding (Verner et al. 2013). The predictive value of this model was subsequently evaluated using data from Inuit and Slovak birth cohorts in which children had their blood sampled at around 6 months of age (Verner et al. 2013). Simulations based on cord blood PCB-153 levels and information on maternal prepregnancy body weight and child gestational age, birth weight, sex, infant weight, and exclusive and total breastfeeding duration explained 74% and 57% of the variability in blood PCB-153 levels of infants enrolled in the Inuit and Slovak cohorts, respectively. Using a pharmacokinetic model to estimate month-by-month postnatal blood PCB-153 levels through age 11 months in Inuit children, we observed an association between measured cord blood PCB-153 levels and inattention and between estimated postnatal blood PCB-153 levels (especially around the 4th month) and activity at age 11 months (Verner et al. 2010). This finding, together with the fact that different neurodevelopmental processes occur in different regions of the brain during distinct prenatal and postnatal periods (Rice and Barone 2000), supports the potential for differential PCB effects on behavior depending on exposure timing. Whether the relation of early-life PCB exposures with behavior in later childhood varies with prenatal versus infancy exposure timing has yet to be studied. Identifying developmental periods of differential PCB susceptibility can be important to public health strategies for exposure mitigation.
In a longitudinal birth cohort of mothers and children living near a PCB-contaminated site in New Bedford, Massachusetts (USA) (Choi et al. 2006), cord serum PCB levels were associated with an increased risk of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)-related behaviors at 8 years of age measured using the Conners' Rating Scale for Teachers (CRS-T), a standardized questionnaire for estimating the frequency of adverse behaviors in children (Sagiv et al. 2010). In the same cohort, we aimed to evaluate the differential effects of prenatal and postnatal exposures on children's behavior by a) estimating children's monthly serum PCB-153 levels during the first year of life using the above-described validated pharmacokinetic model, and b) evaluating the association of measured cord serum and estimated monthly infant serum PCB-153 levels with behavior using quantile regression at the 50th and 75th percentiles of CRS-T scores. This statistical approach allowed us to evaluate whether PCB exposures are more strongly associated with ADHD-related behaviors in children with more adverse behaviors at 8 years (75th percentile of CRS-T scores), who may represent a more susceptible population because of genetic factors or other early environmental exposures.
Methods
Study population. Mothers residing in towns adjacent to the PCB-contaminated New Bedford Harbor were invited to participate in the New Bedford Cohort at the time of delivery if they were > 18 years of age, gave birth via vaginal delivery, and spoke English or Portuguese (Korrick et al. 2000). A total of 788 newborns were enrolled between 1993 and 1998. We restricted our analyses to singleton births (n = 782) with cord serum PCB measures (n = 746) and information on duration of exclusive and total breastfeeding (n = 581), and to those who participated in 8-year follow up assessments with complete CRS-T evaluations (n = 567). A total of 441 of the 567 children had complete covariate data and were included in our analyses. This study was approved by the human subjects committees of Harvard School of Public Health and Brigham and Women's Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts and Southcoast Hospitals Group, New Bedford, Massachusetts. Mothers gave written informed consent before examination.
Laboratory measurement methods. Cord blood sampling, analytical procedures, and PCB levels have been described elsewhere (Korrick et al. 2000). As part of previous research on prenatal PCB exposures in the New Bedford Cohort, cord blood was collected at birth, and cord serum levels of 51 PCB congeners were measured at the Harvard School of Public Health Organic Chemistry Laboratory (Boston, MA). Serum samples underwent liquid-liquid extraction followed by analysis by gas chromatography with electron capture detection. Because of limited sample volume, serum lipids were not measured. The concentration of lipids in cord serum was measured in 12 randomly selected, discarded anonymous samples from the study hospital where New Bedford Cohort infants were born (mean, 1.7 g lipid/L serum). Because the pharmacokinetic model uses PCB levels in lipids, we expressed cord serum PCB levels on a lipid basis using the same 1.7-g/L lipid concentration for all cord serum samples.
Postnatal exposure assessment. We used PCB-153 as a proxy for total PCB exposure because of its long half-life in humans (Ritter et al. 2011) and its correlation with other prevalent PCB congeners in this population (Spearman correlation coefficients of 0.72 for PCB-118, 0.81 for PCB-138, and 0.73 for PCB-180). We estimated children's serum lipid PCB-153 levels at each of the first 12 postnatal months using our previously published pharmacokinetic model (Verner et al. 2013). This model simulates lifetime exposure in the mother, transplacental diffusion during pregnancy, and transfer via breastfeeding after delivery (Figure 1). Both the mother and the child are represented in the model, and only body lipids are included because PCB-153 is highly lipophilic and binds almost exclusively to lipids. The model assumes a homogeneous distribution across body lipids, including serum lipids. Maternal exposure was parameterized as a constant daily dose (nanograms per kilogram body weight) calibrated to match measured cord serum PCB-153 levels at birth. Infants' postnatal exposure through age 12 months was attributed entirely to breastfeeding.
The pharmacokinetic model runs on subject-specific information to generate individualized profiles of serum PCB-153 levels during infancy. Here, we used the New Bedford Cohort participants' information on maternal age at delivery, prepregnancy body weight, and gestational weight gain; and child duration of exclusive and total breastfeeding, gestational age at birth, birth weight, and weight at the time of the 8-year behavioral assessment (birth weight and weight at 8 years were used to estimate each infant's growth curve between birth and 12 months). Data on duration of breastfeeding was collected as categories: no breastfeeding, < 1 month, 1-3 months, 4-6 months, 7-9 months, and > 9 months. Because the pharmacokinetic model uses continuous data, these categories were transformed to breastfeeding durations of 0, 0.5, 2, 5, 8, and 10 months, respectively. Because subject-specific information was unavailable for certain model inputs, we used the same assumptions for all individuals. Between the end of exclusive breastfeeding (when formula or solid food was introduced) and the end of total breastfeeding, we assumed breast milk consumption to be 50% of intake during exclusive breastfeeding. To model maternal weight changes postpartum, we assumed body weight to be 2 kg higher than prepregnancy body weight 6 months after delivery (Butte et al. 2003) and to return to prepregnancy body weight 12 months after delivery. The same assumptions were used in the model development and validation study (Verner et al. 2013).
Behavioral assessment. The CRS-T is a 59-item standardized questionnaire used to assess the frequency of a wide range of problem behaviors in children as reported by their teachers (Conners 1997). It has been used in research among nonclinical populations but is also used in the diagnosis and treatment of children with behavioral problems, including problems with attention, hyperactivity, and impulse control that are associated with ADHD. The CRS-T has been shown to be both sensitive and specific in differentiating children with clinically diagnosed ADHD from children with neurotypical behavior (Tripp et al. 2006). Teachers of children participating in the study's 8-year follow-up were asked to complete the CRS-T questionnaire. The four CRS-T subscales pertaining to ADHD-related behaviors were used in this analysis: a) Conners' ADHD index, b) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) Inattentive index (American Psychiatric Association 1994), c) DSM-IV Hyperactive-Impulsive index, and d) DSM-IV Total index (inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive behaviors combined) (Conners 1997). CRS-T scores are standardized to age- and sex-adjusted T-scores with a mean (± SD) of 50 ± 10 where more frequent adverse behaviors are characterized by higher CRS-T scores.
Covariate assessment. Information about mother-child sociodemographics, medical history, weight, lifestyle, and dietary habits, including infant feeding, were collected via study questionnaires administered during study evaluations of each child at approximately 2 weeks after birth and at ages 6 months (for a subset of the cohort) and 8 years. Additional medical history, weight, pregnancy history, and birth outcome data were collected by review of the study hospital's labor and delivery medical records as well as outpatient pediatric medical records with data abstracted between birth and age 8 years. In addition, child weight at 8 years was measured as part of the 8-year study evaluation.
Statistical analysis. We used multivariable quantile regression, a method that has not been used in prior analyses of behavior in this cohort, to evaluate the associations of measured (cord serum) and simulated (postnatal) serum lipid PCB-153 levels with children's behavior. Quantile regression differs from ordinary least square (OLS) linear regression in that it minimizes the sum of absolute residuals rather than the squared residuals, thus reducing the influence of outliers and making it more robust to departure from ordinary least square model assumptions (Koenker and Bassett 1978). Quantile regression's robustness to non-Gaussian distributions made it an appropriate regression method for the skewed CRS-T scores. In addition, quantile regression allows for assessment of associations at any quantile of the dependent variable by symmetrically (median) or asymmetrically (other quantiles) weighting positive and negative residuals. For example, in quantile regression at the 75th percentile, underpredictions (positive residuals) are given a weight of 0.75 whereas overpredictions (negative residuals) are given a weight of 0.25. This model would indicate the change in the 75th percentile of the outcome value associated with each unit change in a predictor variable. Quantile regression uses all available data regardless of the quantile of interest, thereby providing overall estimates of association. Evaluating associations at different percentiles is particularly useful when exposure is hypothesized to affect certain percentiles of the dependent variable to a greater extent than the mean or median (Beyerlein 2014). For example, children with poorer neurocognitive skills may be particularly susceptible to neurotoxicant effects, as observed in a study of lead and cognitive function by Miranda et al. (2009). In our study, one could hypothesize that children who are predisposed to ADHD-related behaviors due to genetic and early-life environmental factors--those most likely to be in the higher percentiles of CRS-T scores at age 8 years--may be more sensitive to early exposures to PCBs. We ran quantile regressions for the study at the 50th percentile (roughly analogous to OLS linear regression at the mean) and at the 75th percentile (more adverse behaviors) of CRS-T scores. We picked the 75th percentile a priori to optimize our assessment of PCB effects on children with worse scores without compromising the precision of the analyses. Because the distribution in estimated children's PCB-153 levels changes during development, and because regression coefficients are influenced by the mean and SD of exposure estimates, we expressed the change in CRS-T scores by the interquartile range (IQR) increase in PCB-153 levels for each time period to make coefficients comparable across exposure periods.
We included the following potential confounders (related to exposures and outcomes) or control variables (related to outcomes) in regression models based on a priori considerations: maternal prepregnancy weight, gestational weight gain categories (inadequate, adequate, excessive) using the Institute of Medicine recommendations (Rasmussen and Yaktine 2009), characteristics at delivery (age, marital status, education, parity), seafood consumption during pregnancy, use of tobacco and alcohol during pregnancy and use of illicit drugs in the year before delivery, and IQ at 8-year follow-up; total household income (at delivery) and Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) score (Caldwell and Bradley 1984) at 8-year assessment; and child sex, race, cord blood lead level, ADHD medication use, school type, and age at the CRS-T evaluation. Because simulated postnatal serum PCB-153 levels are strongly correlated with cord serum PCB-153 levels and duration of breastfeeding, we did not adjust for these two measures in our final regression models; however, we performed secondary sensitivity analyses adjusting for these factors. We also did secondary quantile regression analyses restricted to the subset of children who were ever breastfed.
Results
Table 1 provides a summary of study participants' sociodemographic, lifestyle, and exposure characteristics. At the time of delivery, mothers in this study were, on average, 27 years of age, 38% were unmarried, and about one-third lived in low-income households with annual incomes < $20,000. Only 31% of the women had a gestational weight gain within the recommendations of the Institute of Medicine (Rasmussen and Yaktine 2009), whereas 51% gained more weight. Children were on average 8 years of age at behavioral evaluation, were predominantly white (69%), and only 54% were ever breastfed. Complete covariate data were available for 441 of the 567 children identified for study. Of all children enrolled in the New Bedford Cohort, those in this analysis (n = 441) were generally more sociodemographically advantaged than those excluded (n = 347) (data not shown). For example, included mothers were older, had more education, were more likely to be married, and had a higher IQ; their children were younger at CRS-T examination and more likely to be breastfed than those excluded. Conversely, included mothers smoked more during pregnancy and their children had higher cord blood lead levels than those excluded (data not shown). A substantial proportion (29%) of mothers excluded from the analyses had missing prenatal smoking information, which may, in part, account for the apparent increased smoking among included participants.
We simulated PCB-153 levels in infants using a pharmacokinetic model that assumes that all postnatal exposure during the first year of life is from breastfeeding. Four representative plots of PCB-153 levels are shown for infants with different durations of breastfeeding in Figure 2. Estimated infants' serum PCB-153 increased during lactation, peaked at the end of breastfeeding, and subsequently decreased with cessation of postnatal exposure and dilution of body burden with growth and expanding body lipids. Among breastfed infants, the increase in estimated serum PCB-153 levels over time was steeper in infants whose measured cord serum level at birth was higher (Figure 2A and C compared with D). Levels of serum PCB-153 that were accumulated prenatally declined rapidly after delivery in infants who were not exposed via breastfeeding (Figure 2B). Because of the relatively low prevalence of breastfeeding in this population, median estimated serum PCB-153 levels declined from 38 ng/g lipids at birth to 30 ng/g lipids at 2 months and 14 ng/g lipids at 12 months (see Supplemental Material, Figure S1) despite the fact that levels increased in breastfed infants. Estimated PCB-153 levels in infants' serum were highly correlated with measured cord serum levels. The Spearman correlation coefficients between cord serum PCB-153 levels and postnatal estimates decreased from the first month (r = 0.93) to the 6th month (r = 0.70) and stabilized around 0.67 thereafter. On the other hand, the Spearman correlation between the total duration of breastfeeding and serum PCB-153 levels increased from 0.06 at birth to 0.68 at 6 months of age and stabilized around 0.70 thereafter (see Supplemental Material, Figure S2). This increasing correlation was expected because the duration of breastfeeding is the most influential model parameter during the first year (Verner et al. 2013).
In line with a previous study of the same cohort where children in the highest quartile of cord serum PCB levels had increased risks of ADHD-related behavior in a log risk regression analysis (Sagiv et al. 2010), results from the current quantile regression analyses also suggest that cord serum PCB-153 levels are associated with greater ADHD-related behavior as reflected in three of the four CRS-T subscales assessed (DSM-IV Total index, DSM-IV Hyperactive-Impulsive index, and Conners' ADHD index). However, these associations were statistically significant in quantile regression models only at the 75th percentile (Figure 3). The association between PCB-153 and ADHD-related behaviors at age 8 years was stronger with measured cord serum levels than with estimated postnatal serum levels; the strength and significance of the associations attenuated substantially over time within the first few months of estimated postnatal exposure (Figure 3). The most sustained association with postnatal exposure estimates was a modest statistically significant association of estimated serum PCB-153 levels through age 6 months with greater DSM-IV Hyperactive-Impulsive index and Conners' ADHD index behaviors at age 8 years (quantile regression models at the 75th percentile), but these associations also attenuated over time (Figure 3). For example, the increase in Hyperactive-Impulsive index behaviors at age 8 years associated with an IQR increase in PCB-153 levels was 0.9 point at birth [95% confidence interval (CI): 0.2, 2.5], 0.5 point at 2 months (95% CI: 0.3, 2.3), and 0.3 point at 12 months (95% CI: -0.2, 1.5). Because quantile regression does not assume any particular distribution of residuals, it is possible for CIs to be asymmetrical, which is the case in most of our regression models.
In sensitivity analyses, results were similar after models were adjusted for the total duration of breastfeeding (see Supplemental Material, Figure S3A). On the other hand, the modest associations with postnatal exposure disappeared when we adjusted for measured cord serum PCB-153 levels (see Supplemental Material, Figure S3B). Restricting analyses to breastfed children (see Supplemental Material, Figure S3C) did not affect regression coefficients substantially, though, consistent with the smaller number (n = 239), confidence intervals were wider than in main analyses. Results did not change materially when regression coefficients were expressed using the same IQR for all exposure periods (data not shown).
Discussion
Results from animal (Holene et al. 1998; Rice 2000) and in vitro studies (Fritsche et al. 2005), and the relatively high PCB exposures that occur consequent to breastfeeding (Ayotte et al. 2003; Lanting et al. 1998; Patandin et al. 1997) support likely adverse neurobehavioral impacts of PCB exposure in infancy, including adverse impacts on impulse control (Verner et al. 2010). We used a pharmacokinetic model to estimate monthly serum PCB-153 levels in the first year of life and to assess the relation of these exposure estimates with ADHD-related behavior at age 8 years, including hyperactivity and impulsivity, in the New Bedford Cohort.
Cord serum PCB-153 levels were significantly associated with the DSM-IV Total index, the DSM-IV Hyperactive-Impulsive index, and Conners' ADHD index in quantile regression models at the 75th percentile. Associations of estimated infant serum PCB-153 levels with ADHD-related behavior were most suggestive for performance on the DSM-IV Hyperactive-Impulsive and Conners' ADHD indices (Figure 3), but these associations, although statistically significant through age 6 months, diminished over time. Previous studies associating postnatal PCB exposures with adverse behavior have typically been done in animal models or human populations with higher PCB levels than were observed in the New Bedford Cohort. For example, monkeys exposed to a mixture of PCBs during their first 20 weeks of life (levels of 1,694-3,560 ng/g lipids) showed reduced performance on tasks of nonspatial discrimination, spatial delayed alternation, and fixed interval schedules at the age of 2.5-5 years, all of which are correlates of ADHD-related behaviors, including impulse control, in humans (Rice 2000). Similarly, our previous study of Inuit infants suggested that activity, in particular, may be susceptible to PCB exposure via breastfeeding in the first year of life but at higher exposure levels than were observed in New Bedford children (Verner et al. 2010). Specifically, in 150 Inuit infants from Northern Quebec (Canada), estimated postnatal serum PCB-153 levels during the 4th month of life (mean, 211 ng/g lipids; range, 5-1,548) were associated with increased spontaneous activity at 11 months (Verner et al. 2010). Plusquellec et al. (2010) observed an association between plasma PCB-153 levels at 5 years of age (mean, 153 ng/g lipids; range, 8-780) and increased concurrent spontaneous activity among 87 Inuit preschoolers. Although studies among Inuit children suggest that postnatal exposure to PCBs increases activity levels, Jacobson et al. (1990) reported an inverse association between total serum PCB levels at 4 years (mean serum total PCBs, 2.0-2.2 ng/mL) and a concurrent composite activity rating. Of note, no associations have been detected between attention and postnatal PCB exposure (Jacobson and Jacobson 2003; Plusquellec et al. 2010; Verner et al. 2010).
Certain factors such as genetics (Sharp et al. 2009) and early-life exposures (e.g., maternal smoking) (Latimer et al. 2012) are determinants of later-life inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive behaviors. Children who demonstrated ADHD-like behaviors at 8 years of age, that is, those at higher percentiles of CRS-T scores, were likely predisposed to these behaviors during development, when prenatal and lactational exposures to PCBs occurred. Interestingly, the association between cord serum PCB-153 and ADHD-related behaviors was stronger at the 75th percentile than at the 50th percentile of CRS-T scores. A possible interpretation of these findings is that predisposed individuals may be more vulnerable to PCB neurotoxic effects. Evidence of enhanced susceptibility to neurobehavioral toxicants among children who demonstrate suboptimal neurobehavioral performance has been reported by others. For example, in a study of childhood lead exposure and end-of-school-grade test scores, Miranda et al. (2009) observed stronger adverse associations between lead and test scores among children with worse compared with better scores. This hypothesis would need to be more thoroughly evaluated in prospective studies where more detailed information on predisposing factors, including genetic factors, is available.
Despite the fact that children were living near a PCB-contaminated site (Choi et al. 2006), PCB levels measured in cord serum were relatively low compared with other longitudinal cohort studies in North America and Europe (Longnecker et al. 2003). Low-level exposures experienced by the New Bedford Cohort children may explain the lack of clear association between postnatal PCB exposure and ADHD-related behaviors. In addition, limitations to our study may underlie our inability to detect a consistent association between estimated postnatal PCB levels and ADHD-related behavior. The pharmacokinetic model we used to estimate postnatal PCB levels, which considers exposure only through transplacental transfer and breastfeeding, was validated in cohorts where breastfeeding was highly prevalent and the resolution in the duration of breastfeeding was higher (continuous vs. categorical) (Verner et al. 2013). The low prevalence of breastfeeding (i.e., 54% of children were ever breastfed, 40% more than a month) and the limited resolution in the duration of breastfeeding measure, a model parameter that was highly sensitive in global sensitivity analyses of the pharmacokinetic model (Verner et al. 2013), likely introduced measurement error in the exposure predictions and consequently reduced our power to detect associations. Therefore, we cannot rule out the possibility that the absence of consistent associations of ADHD-related behaviors with postnatal PCBs is attributable to the reduced precision of postnatal exposure estimates in the New Bedford Cohort. Additional limitations to our study include pharmacokinetic model assumptions (e.g., postpartum weight changes, proportion of food intake attributable to breast milk during partial breastfeeding, constant maternal daily dose, postnatal exposure restricted to breastfeeding) and the lack of information on subject-specific cord blood lipids. Although these limitations may have diminished our ability to detect modest associations with postnatal exposure to PCBs, the use of a pharmacokinetic model enabled us to retrospectively investigate early PCB exposures in a cohort with no measured levels in infancy. Our results represent an important step toward assessing the potential for PCB exposures in infancy to affect subsequent behavioral development.
Conclusions
Epidemiologic and experimental evidence suggests that hyperactive or impulsive behavior may be associated with postnatal PCB exposures (Holene et al. 1998; Rice 2000; Verner et al. 2010). To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to assess the neurobehavioral impact of postnatal PCB exposures at low levels characteristic of contemporaneous general population samples. In the context of low-level exposure and limitations in postnatal exposure estimation where 60% of children were not breastfed or breastfed for < 1 month, our findings were inconclusive regarding the potential modest adverse impact of low-level PCB exposures in infancy on hyperactive or impulsive behavior in later childhood. Our results using quantile regression at the 75th percentile of CRS-T scores suggest that children observed to have more ADHD-like behavior at age 8 years, who were possibly predisposed by genetic and early-life environmental factors, may be more susceptible to both prenatal and postnatal exposure to PCBs. Further research is warranted to address the potential for low-level PCB exposure in infancy to affect later child neurobehavior and to assess sources of increased susceptibility to postnatal PCB effects.
American Psychiatric Association. 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. DSM-IV. 4th ed. Washington, DC:American Psychiatric Association.
Ayotte P, Muckle G, Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, Dewailly É, Inuit Cohort Study. 2003. Assessment of pre- and postnatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls: lessons from the Inuit Cohort Study. Environ Health Perspect 111:1253-1258; doi: 10.1289/ehp.6054.
Beyerlein A. 2014. Quantile regression--opportunities and challenges from a user's perspective. Am J Epidemiol 180(3):330-331.
Butte NF, Ellis KJ, Wong WW, Hopkinson JM, Smith EO. 2003. Composition of gestational weight gain impacts maternal fat retention and infant birth weight. Am J Obstet Gynecol 189(5):1423-1432.
Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. 1984. HOME Observation for Measurement of the Environment. Revised ed. Little Rock, AR:University of Arkansas at Little Rock.
Choi AL, Levy JI, Dockery DW, Ryan LM, Tolbert PE, Altshul LM, et al. 2006. Does living near a Superfund site contribute to higher polychlorinated biphenyl (PCB) exposure? Environ Health Perspect 114:1092-1098; doi: 10.1289/ehp.8827.
Conners CK. 1997. Conners' Rating Scales-Revised: Technical Manual. North Tonawanda, NY:Multi-Health Systems.
Eubig PA, Aguiar A, Schantz SL. 2010. Lead and PCBs as risk factors for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Environ Health Perspect 118:1654-1667; doi: 10.1289/ehp.0901852.
Fritsche E, Cline JE, Nguyen NH, Scanlan TS, Abel J. 2005. Polychlorinated biphenyls disturb differentiation of normal human neural progenitor cells: clue for involvement of thyroid hormone receptors. Environ Health Perspect 113:871-876; doi: 10.1289/ehp.7793.
Holene E, Nafstad I, Skaare JU, Sagvolden T. 1998. Behavioural hyperactivity in rats following postnatal exposure to sub-toxic doses of polychlorinated biphenyl congeners 153 and 126. Behav Brain Res 94(1):213-224.
Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. 2003. Prenatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and attention at school age. J Pediatr 143(6):780-788.
Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, Humphrey HE. 1990. Effects of exposure to PCBs and related compounds on growth and activity in children. Neurotoxicol Teratol 12(4):319-326.
Julvez J, Ribas-Fitó N, Forns M, Garcia-Esteban R, Torrent M, Sunyer J. 2007. Attention behaviour and hyperactivity at age 4 and duration of breast-feeding. Acta Paediatr 96(6):842-847.
Koenker R, Bassett G Jr. 1978. Regression Quantiles. Econometrica 46(1):33-50.
Korrick SA, Altshul LM, Tolbert PE, Burse VW, Needham LL, Monson RR. 2000. Measurement of PCBs, DDE, and hexachlorobenzene in cord blood from infants born in towns adjacent to a PCB-contaminated waste site. J Expo Anal Environ Epidemiol 10(6 pt 2):743-754.
Lanting CI, Fidler V, Huisman M, Boersma ER. 1998. Determinants of polychlorinated biphenyl levels in plasma from 42-month-old children. Arch Environ Contam Toxicol 35(1):135-139.
Latimer K, Wilson P, Kemp J, Thompson L, Sim F, Gillberg C, et al. 2012. Disruptive behaviour disorders: a systematic review of environmental antenatal and early years risk factors. Child Care Health Dev 38(5):611-628.
Longnecker MP, Wolff MS, Gladen BC, Brock JW, Grandjean P, Jacobson JL, et al. 2003. Comparison of polychlorinated biphenyl levels across studies of human neurodevelopment. Environ Health Perspect 111:65-70; doi: 10.1289/ehp.5463.
Miranda ML, Kim D, Reiter J, Overstreet Galeano MA, Maxson P. 2009. Environmental contributors to the achievement gap. Neurotoxicology 30(6):1019-1024.
Muckle G, Ayotte P, Dewailly É, Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL. 2001. Prenatal exposure of the northern Québec Inuit infants to environmental contaminants. Environ Health Perspect 109:1291-1299.
Patandin S, Weisglas-Kuperus N, de Ridder MA, Koopman-Esseboom C, van Staveren WA, van der Paauw CG, et al. 1997. Plasma polychlorinated biphenyl levels in Dutch preschool children either breast-fed or formula-fed during infancy. Am J Public Health 87(10):1711-1714.
Plusquellec P, Muckle G, Dewailly E, Ayotte P, Bégin G, Desrosiers C, et al. 2010. The relation of environmental contaminants exposure to behavioral indicators in Inuit preschoolers in Arctic Quebec. Neurotoxicology 31(1):17-25.
Rasmussen KM, Yaktine AL, eds. 2009. Weight Gain During Pregnancy: Reexamining the Guidelines. Committee to Reexamine IOM Pregnancy Weight Guidelines, Food and Nutrition Board and Board on Children, Youth, and Families. Washington, DC:National Academies Press. Available: http://iom.nationalacademies.org/en/Repo?rts/2009/Weight-Gain-During-Pregnancy-Re?examining-the-Guidelines.aspx [accessed 20 February 2013].
Rice DC. 2000. Parallels between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and behavioral deficits produced by neurotoxic exposure in monkeys. Environ Health Perspect 108(suppl 3):405-408.
Rice D, Barone S Jr. 2000. Critical periods of vulnerability for the developing nervous system: evidence from humans and animal models. Environ Health Perspect 108(suppl 3):511-533.
Ritter R, Scheringer M, MacLeod M, Moeckel C, Jones KC, Hungerbühler K. 2011. Intrinsic human elimination half-lives of polychlorinated biphenyls derived from the temporal evolution of cross-sectional biomonitoring data from the United Kingdom. Environ Health Perspect 119:225-231; doi: 10.1289/ehp.1002211.
Sagiv SK, Thurston SW, Bellinger DC, Tolbert PE, Altshul LM, Korrick SA. 2010. Prenatal organochlorine exposure and behaviors associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in school-aged children. Am J Epidemiol 171(5):593-601.
Sharp SI, McQuillin A, Gurling HM. 2009. Genetics of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuropharmacology 57(7-8):590-600.
Tripp G, Schaughency EA, Clarke B. 2006. Parent and teacher rating scales in the evaluation of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: contribution to diagnosis and differential diagnosis in clinically referred children. J Dev Behav Pediatr 27(3):209-218.
Verner MA, Plusquellec P, Muckle G, Ayotte P, Dewailly É, Jacobson SW, et al. 2010. Alteration of infant attention and activity by polychlorinated biphenyls: unravelling critical windows of susceptibility using physiologically based pharmacokinetic modeling. Neurotoxicology 31(5):424-431.
Verner MA, Sonneborn D, Lancz K, Muckle G, Ayotte P, Dewailly É, et al. 2013. Toxicokinetic modeling of persistent organic pollutant levels in blood from birth to 45 months of age in longitudinal birth cohort studies. Environ Health Perspect 121:131-137; doi: 10.1289/ehp.1205552.
Vreugdenhil HJ, Slijper FM, Mulder PG, Weisglas-Kuperus N. 2002. Effects of perinatal exposure to PCBs and dioxins on play behavior in Dutch children at school age. Environ Health Perspect 110:A593-A598.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences Sep 2015
Abstract
Background: Epidemiologic studies of postnatal PCB exposure and behavior have not reported consistent evidence of adverse associations, possibly because of challenges in exposure estimation. We previously developed a pharmacokinetic model to improve estimation of children's PCB exposure.
Objectives: We aimed to assess whether estimated serum PCB levels in infancy are associated with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)-related behaviors at 8 years of age among children whose cord serum PCB levels were previously shown to be associated with ADHD-related behaviors.
Methods: We used a pharmacokinetic model to estimate monthly serum polychlorinated biphenyl (PCB)-153 levels in 441 infants (ages 1-12 months) based on parameters such as breastfeeding and cord serum PCB-153 levels. Behavior was evaluated at age 8 using the Conners' Rating Scale for Teachers (CRS-T). Associations between PCB-153 levels and ADHD-related CRS-T indices were assessed using multivariable quantile regression at the 50th and 75th percentiles of CRS-T scores, where higher percentiles reflect more adverse behaviors.
Results: Cord serum PCB-153 levels (median, 38 ng/g lipids) were associated with ADHD-related behaviors, although statistical significance was observed with quantile regression models only at the 75th percentile. Associations with postnatal exposure estimates were attenuated. For example, hyperactive-impulsive behavior scores at age 8 years were 0.9 points (95% CI: 0.2, 2.5), 0.5 points (95% CI: 0.3, 2.3), and 0.3 points (95% CI: -0.2, 1.5) higher in association with interquartile range increases in serum PCB-153 at birth, 2 months, and 12 months of age, respectively.
Conclusions: Associations between estimated postnatal PCB-153 exposures and ADHD-related behaviors at 8 years of age were weaker than associations with PCB-153 concentrations measured in cord serum at birth.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer