ARTICLE
Received 17 Apr 2015 | Accepted 12 Dec 2015 | Published 25 Jan 2016
DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 OPEN
Evolutionary signals of selection on cognition from the great tit genome and methylome
Veronika N. Laine1,*, Toni I. Gossmann2,*, Kyle M. Schachtschneider3,4,*, Colin J. Garroway5, Ole Madsen3, Koen J.F. Verhoeven6, Victor de Jager1, Hendrik-Jan Megens3, Wesley C. Warren7, Patrick Minx7,Richard P.M.A. Crooijmans3, Pdraic Corcoran2, The Great Tit HapMap Consortiumw, Ben C. Sheldon5,Jon Slate2, Kai Zeng2, Kees van Oers1, Marcel E. Visser1,3 & Martien A.M. Groenen3
For over 50 years, the great tit (Parus major) has been a model species for research in evolutionary, ecological and behavioural research; in particular, learning and cognition have been intensively studied. Here, to provide further insight into the molecular mechanisms behind these important traits, we de novo assemble a great tit reference genome and whole-genome re-sequence another 29 individuals from across Europe. We show an overrepresentation of genes related to neuronal functions, learning and cognition in regions under positive selection, as well as increased CpG methylation in these regions. In addition, great tit neuronal non-CpG methylation patterns are very similar to those observed in mammals, suggesting a universal role in neuronal epigenetic regulation which can affect learning-, memory- and experience-induced plasticity. The high-quality great tit genome assembly will play an instrumental role in furthering the integration of ecological, evolutionary, behavioural and genomic approaches in this model species.
1 Department of Animal Ecology, Netherlands Institute of Ecology (NIOO-KNAW), P.O. Box 50, 6700AB Wageningen, The Netherlands. 2 Department of Animal and Plant Sciences, University of Shefeld, Shefeld S10 2TN, UK. 3 Animal Breeding and Genomics Centre, Wageningen University, P.O. Box 338, 6700AH Wageningen, The Netherlands. 4 Department of Animal Sciences, University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois 61801, USA. 5 Edward Grey Institute, Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, UK. 6 Department of Terrestrial Ecology, Netherlands Institute of Ecology (NIOO-KNAW), P.O. Box 50, 6700AB Wageningen, The Netherlands. 7 The Genome Institute, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri 63108, USA. * These authors contributed equally to this work. w A full list of consortium members appears at the end of the paper. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to V.N.L. (email: mailto:[email protected]
Web End [email protected] ) or to M.A.M.G. (email: mailto:[email protected]
Web End [email protected] ).
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10474 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 1
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474
Theory predicts that the ability to perceive, assess and learn from others should have tness benets under a wide range of conditions1. However, we know little about whether, or
how, natural selection acts on cognitive traits related to social living in any species2. Great tits learn socially in the wild3, solve complex learning tasks4 and there is evidence that cognitive abilities may have important tness implications5. A literature survey of records of technical (for example, tool use) and opportunistic (for example, exploring novel food items) innovation spanning 803 bird species from 76 families shows that great tits are among the top avian species in terms of overall number and diversity of foraging innovations6. This suggests rapid evolution in the great tit lineage of learning and cognition-associated traits compared with many other birds, making great tits an excellent model for studying complex social-cognitive behaviour.
In addition to studies of cognitive and learning abilities of great tits3,5, great tit research has contributed signicantly to our general understanding of life history evolution7, the effects of climate change on natural populations8,9, the allocation of resources to breeding10 including trade-offs between reproduction and immunity11, the extent and consequences of individual variation in rates of ageing12, inbreeding and inbreeding depression13,14, hostparasite coevolution15, territorial and foraging behaviour16,17 and the impact of variation in personality traits on other life history characters18. This considerable contribution of studies of great tits to our understanding of basic ecological and evolutionary processes is largely due to work on numerous long-term study systems throughout Europe.
To explore the molecular basis for learning and cognition, we developed a complete set of de novo molecular genomic tools to ascertain evidence of natural selection on genetic and epigenetic variation in great tits. Here, we test the hypothesis that learning and cognition have been important targets of selection in great tit evolution by exploring footprints of selection in the great tit genome. We nd an overrepresentation of genes related to neuronal functions, learning and cognition in regions under positive selection, as well as increased CpG methylation in these regions. In addition, great tit neuronal non-CpG methylation patterns are very similar to those observed in mammals. The development of high-quality genomic resources for great tits provides tools that will allow the integration of genomics into ecological, evolutionary and behavioural research in wild populations in this important study system.
ResultsGenome sequencing, assembly and annotation. We selected a male great tit (hereafter the reference bird) from a recent captive population (four generations in captivity) in the Netherlands for genome sequencing and de novo assembly (see Methods and Supplementary Information for a detailed description of genome assembly and annotation). We generated a total of 114 Gb of Illumina HiSeq sequence data; after gap-lling and removal of adaptor sequences, the assembly consisted of a total of 2,066 scaffolds with an N50 scaffold length of over 7.7 Mb and an N50 contig length of 133 kb. Taken together, the assembled contigs span 1.0 Gb. We were able to assign 98% of the assembled bases to a chromosomal location using the recently described high-density linkage map19 (Supplementary Data 1). The total number of chromosomes covered by the assembly is 29 with three additional linkage groups (Chr25LG1, Chr25LG2 and LGE22). The assignment of chromosomal locations to the majority of the assembly resulted in a very high quality reference genome and enabled detailed comparisons with other bird genomes.
A comparison between the genomes of the great tit and zebra nch (Taeniopygia guttata)20 (see Supplementary Fig. 1) showed only intra-chromosomal inversions, and not a single inter-chromosomal rearrangement, conrming high synteny in birds. For the genome annotation step, we combined RNA sequencing data from eight different tissues of the reference bird with gene models from chicken (Gallus gallus)21 and zebra nch20. The nal annotation resulted in a total of 21,057 transcripts for 13,036 high-condence gene predictions.
Population genomics. To obtain further insight into the evolutionary genetics of the great tit, an additional 29 wild individuals from across Europe were re-sequenced at an average depth of 10 (Supplementary Data 2, Fig. 1a). We found very little
differentiation among populations (FSTo0.02; Supplementary Data 3) which is in line with previous phylogenetic analyses of the spatial genetic structure of great tits in Europe using mtDNA markers22. This suggests a largely panmictic European great tit population22. Pairwise sequential Markovian coalescent analysis (PSMC)23 of the reference individual suggested a large effective population size that increased from an already large B2 105
individuals 1 Myr ago to B5.7 105 individuals 70 Kyr ago
(Fig. 1b). There was evidence that the expansion was interrupted by a very mild decline in population size beginning B110 Kyr ago, coinciding with the start of the last glacial period in Europe, followed by a quick recovery. Consistent with expectation for a population that has undergone a recent increase, there is a genome-wide excess of low-frequency variation, as indicated by negative Tajimas D values (Fig. 2b).
Selective sweeps. We found that several regions with reduced diversity coincide with regions of extremely negative Tajimas D (Fig. 2a,b), indicative of recent selective sweeps. To investigate the role of recent positive selection in greater detail, we conducted a genome-wide scan for selective sweeps and identied 813 genomic regions (Fig. 2, Supplementary Data 4) compromising roughly 0.2% of the genome. We extracted 460 genes that had human orthologues from these regions and assessed their functional importance by performing a gene ontology (GO) enrichment analysis. Thirteen different GO categories were overrepresented in these selected regions relative to the rest of the genome (Fig. 3, Supplementary Data 5a). Among these categories were dendrite, cognition and several other categories related to neuronal function. Therefore genes related to neuronal function are likely to have been targets of recent positive selection.
Learning and cognition. One of the striking genes presented in the learning/cognition category was early growth response protein 1 (EGR1), whose gene expression has been particularly well studied within vocal communication and social contexts in Passerines24,25. EGR1 is one of the immediate early genes, which are known to be important in learning and memory26. In zebra nches, its expression has been shown to be correlated with song learning and practice and is social context dependent25,27. To further investigate the role of EGR1 in great tit evolution, we obtained additional EGR1 sequences from 45 bird species (Supplementary Data 6). We found that rapid evolution of EGR1 is specic to the tit lineage (as signied by an elevated ratio of non-synonymous to synonymous substitution rate (dN/dS)) and has occurred after the split with the nearest sequenced relative, the ground tit and that this is not a feature related to the captive nature of the reference bird (Supplementary Fig. 2). These results are consistent with EGR1 being subject to frequent, recurrent positive selection. Another relevant gene from this category was the forkhead box protein P2 (FOXP2), a well-studied gene that
2 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10474 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 ARTICLE
affects speech and language development in humans28 and linked to song learning in birds29,30. The combination of these song-related genes and the other neuronal genes within sweep areas points to a role of these genes in the evolution of song learning and memory in great tits.
Rates of molecular evolution. To examine whether the identied GO categories in general play an important role in great tit evolution, we extracted all great tit genes that are associated with the enriched GO terms of the sweep regions (Fig. 3, Supplementary Data 5a) and conducted evolutionary rate (dN/dS) analyses by using orthologous genes from chicken and zebra nch. We found that median dN/dS for genes associated with the identied GO terms is reduced compared with all other GO-annotated genes (0.066 versus 0.087, P 1.4 10 22,
MannWhitney U-test), and yet these genes have signicantly more targets of probable positive selection based on a site test of positive selection (P 1.84 10 3, w2 test; see Methods for
details). Moreover, on a genome-wide scale, B1% of the genes in the great tit genome show evidence for positive selection based on their long-term evolutionary rates. These genes are generally enriched for neuronal traits, such as cerebellar Purkinje cell layer
formation and axon extension (Supplementary Data 5b). Taken together, it becomes apparent that selection on traits for brain function has played a major role in the evolution of great tits.
The great tit methylome. Epigenetic control of gene expression is increasingly recognized as playing a major role in many different cellular processes affecting a large variety of traits31, with DNA methylation of cytosines being the most widely studied epigenetic mark. Great tit DNA methylation patterns were investigated by performing whole-genome bisulte sequencing in whole brain and blood tissue of the reference bird. A total of 10.2 million CpG sites, representing 66.7% of all CpG sites, could be called in both tissues. The observed genome-wide methylation patterns in both tissues, including reduced methylation within CpG islands and at transcription start sites (TSS), are consistent with previous ndings in human and mouse cells32,33(Supplementary Fig. 3). We also observed low, but signicant non-CpG methylation in the brain tissue that was not observed in the blood (Fig. 4a). The neuronal non-CpG methylation patterns seen at 167.4 million sites (42% of all non-CpG sites) display similar genome-wide patterns to those seen for CpG methylation, including reduced methylation within CpG islands and at TSS, albeit at a much
NO
FI
SCT
EE
SE
Camb, UK
WW, UK
NL
DE
PL CZ
AT HU
BE
FR
COR
ES
750 km
Effective population size (x104)
20
10
0
70
60
50
40
30
105 106
Years (g =2, [afii9839]=2.0x109)
Figure 1 | The 29 re-sequenced great tits and their demographic history. (a) Map of the sampling locations of the 29 re-sequenced great tits (black) and reference individual (red). (b) Pairwise sequential Markovian coalescent analysis (PSMC) of the reference genome. The red line represents the average and the black lines indicate the condence interval as determined by bootstrapping (100 ).
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10474 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 3
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474
a
1.2
log 2 CLRTajimas DWattersons (in %)
b
0.8
0.4
0.0
1
0
1
2
3
20 PTPRZ1 FOXP2
c
PLK2
MEF2C
AMPH
CTNND2
GLP1R
BTBD9
15
10
5
EGR1
CHST10
SYNJ1
DDX11 DGKI
ATP8A1
JAKMIP1
Figure 2 | Genome-wide test statistics obtained from 29 re-sequenced great tits. (a) Genome-wide distribution of Wattersons Y and (b) Tajimas D measured in sliding window sizes of 50 kB and step size of 10 kB, as well as (c) CLR (composite likelihood score, measured as the sum of neighbouring sweep targets, see Methods) from the sweep analysis with labelled cognition-related genes (see Fig. 3 and Supplementary Data 5a) that were among the top 3% of gene-associated sweep targets (indicated by the dashed line). Chromosomes are separated by colour in ascending order according to their chromosome number. The Z chromosome is the furthest right. The solid lines in the upper two panels denote smoothing splines.
GO annotation P value Percentage of genes with this GO category
3.53x103
3.98x103
1.43x102
3.07x102
3.54x102
4.99x102
5.61x102
8.04x102
1.18x101
1.87x101
1.81x101
2.82x101
1.26x101
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
DendriteCytoskeleton organizationProtein oligomerizationSynaptic transmissionRegulation of phosphatase activity Synaptic membraneCognitionIon channel activityCell cycle phase transitionCilium Phosphatidylinositol-mediated signalling Cellular protein complex assembly GTPase binding
Figure 3 | Gene ontology (GO) enrichment analysis of sweep area genes by using human gene ontologies. The GO enrichment analysis detected 13 functional groups of GO terms across all sweep areas (rst column). P value denotes the corrected P value by using Bonferroni step-down method.
lower level. Similar non-CpG methylation has recently been found in embryonic stem cells and oocytes of many mammalian species34,35, as well as in the neuronal tissue of humans and mice33,35.
Both CpG and non-CpG methylation has been shown to negatively correlate with gene expression at TSS and in gene bodies in mouse neurons33, while hypomethylation of both CpG and non-CpG sites in gene bodies has been reported in highly expressed genes of human neurons35. Consistent with these results, both CpG and non-CpG methylation were negatively correlated with gene expression within gene bodies and at TSS in the great tit brain (Spearmans rho o 0.23, Po1.0 10 95 for
all comparisons, Fig. 4b,c). In addition, the negative correlation between non-CpG methylation and expression was observed in the regions directly upstream and downstream of gene bodies (Spearmans rho o 0.22, Po0.0001 for all comparisons,
Fig. 4c). Therefore, our ndings now extend a potential functional role of non-CpG methylation in neuronal tissue to Aves, suggesting evolutionary conservation of this epigenetic regulation in brains.
Methylation is correlated with rates of molecular evolution. To investigate the potential adaptive and evolutionary role of DNA
methylation, we rst assessed the methylation patterns of selective sweep genes in the brain. We observed higher CpG methylation at sweep gene bodies (Linear Mixed effect Model, LMM; w21 394:61, P 9.2 10 88) and in regions upstream
(LMM; w21 148:94, P 3.0 10 34) and downstream (LMM;
w21 292:70, P 1.8 10 65) of gene bodies compared with the
same regions in genes outside sweep regions (Fig. 4d). In addition, lower non-CpG methylation in sweep gene bodies (LMM; w21 57:17, P 4.0 10 14) and in regions upstream
(LMM; w21 10:10, P 0.001) and downstream (LMM;
w21 31:88, P 1.64 10 8) of gene bodies was observed
compared with the same regions in genes outside sweep regions (Fig. 4e). These patterns are not due to systematic differences in expression levels between sweep and non-sweep genes because the observed CpG and non-CpG methylation differences are in opposing directions. In addition, although the overall expression proles reveal a higher proportion of non-sweep genes with no or low expression (Supplementary Fig. 4e), differences seen between CpG and non-CpG methylation in sweep gene regions were also observed when comparing genes with similar expression levels (Supplementary Fig. 4ad). While previous studies in chickens and humans have also shown altered CpG methylation in regions under selective pressure36, it is unclear what causes the observed
4 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10474 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 ARTICLE
2.0%
Non-CpG methylation level
1.8%1.6%1.4%1.2%1.0%0.8%0.6%0.4%0.2%0.0%
Non-CpG methylation level
1.8%
1.6%1.4%1.2%1.0%0.8%0.6%0.4%0.2%0.0%
Blood Brain
Shores 10 Kb upstream
CpG islands Shores
TSS
Gene body
10 Kb downstream
2.5%
CpG methylation levelCpG methylation level
90% 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10%
0%
90% 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10%
0%
Non-CpG methylation level
2.0%
1.5%
1.0%
0.5%
0.0%
10 Kb upstream
TSS
Gene body
10 Kb downstream
10 Kb upstream TSS Gene body 10 Kb downstream
<1 FPKM1 10 FPKM10 100 FPKM 100 1,000 FPKM
Non sweep genes Sweep genes
Non-CpG methylation level
2.0%
1.6%1.4%1.2%1.0%0.8%0.6%0.4%0.2%0.0%
1.8%
10 Kb upstream TSS Gene body 10 Kb downstream 10 Kb upstream TSS Gene body 10 Kb downstream
Figure 4 | DNA methylation patterns across genomic features. (a) Non-CpG methylation patterns associated with CpG islands and gene bodies.(b) Neuronal CpG methylation in gene bodies and at TSS is negatively correlated with expression (Spearmans rank correlation, Spearmans rho o 0.23,
Po1.0 10 95 for all comparisons), presented as fragments per kilobase of transcript per million fragments mapped (FPKM). (c) Neuronal non-CpG
methylation at TSS, gene bodies and adjacent upstream and downstream regions is negatively correlated with expression (Spearmans rho o 0.23,
Po1.0 10 95 for all comparisons). (d) Increased neuronal CpG methylation at sweep gene bodies (Linear Mixed Effect Model, LMM; w21 394:61,
P 9.2 10 88) and adjacent upstream (LMM; w21 148:94, P 3.0 10 34) and downstream regions (LMM; w21 292:70, P 1.8 10 65).
(e) Decreased neuronal non-CpG methylation in sweep gene bodies (LMM; w21 57:17, P 4.0 10 14) and adjacent upstream (LMM; w21 10:10,
P 0.001) and downstream regions (LMM; w21 31:88, P 1.64 10 8). Shaded areas denote variances.
higher CpG and lower non-CpG methylation in great tit sweep regions. In addition, lowly methylated genes in the brain were found to evolve signicantly slower in comparison with highly methylated genes as shown by differences in the rate of nonsynonymous mutations between the two groups of genes (Po0.001 for all pairwise comparisons, U-test, Supplementary
Fig. 5). This pattern was observed for both CpG and non-CpG methylation at TSS and within gene bodies. Overall these results not only show, for the rst time, conserved neuronal non-CpG methylation patterns between birds and mammals, but also extend them by showing that methylation is correlated with rates of molecular evolution, thereby suggesting an important role for DNA methylation in evolution.
DiscussionWe have generated a high quality de novo assembled and annotated genome for the great tit, a model organism in ecology and evolutionary biology. Our study adds to the growing number of representative genomes sequenced across the bird family tree37,38. Further, unlike these recent genome studies, we assembled the great tit genome into chromosomes, which was useful for determining chromosomal regions of selective sweeps and larger-scale synteny conservation across species. The great tit genome assembly represents a powerful tool, especially when combined with the extensive availability of individual phenotypes of known birds for which DNA is available via routine blood sampling. Using sequence analysis of birds from a wide range of
European populations, we identied selective sweep areas enriched with genes related to learning and cognition. Using whole-genome methylation data, we not only revealed conserved non-CpG methylation patterns between birds and mammals, but also extended these observations by showing that methylation is correlated with rates of molecular evolution, thereby suggesting an important role for DNA methylation in evolution. Our de novo assembled genome will help us to reveal the genetic basis of phenotypic evolution, which is essential for understanding how wild species have adapted to our changing planet.
Methods
Genome sequence assembly and annotation. A blood sample from a male Parus major was obtained for whole-genome sequencing and stored in queens buffer. This reference bird originated from a captive population that was derived from wild-caught birds from the Netherlands four generations ago and has since been articially selected for avian personality39. In the great tit genome assembling,we relied on the creation of de Bruijn graphical structures, a directed graph that evolves from dened sequence length (kmer) progression (see Supplementary Methods for a detailed description of genome assembly and annotation). In brief, our genome assembling involved four principal steps that progressed from sequence quality revisions, to forming contigs from these sequence reads, to connecting contigs into scaffolds using paired-end sequence of large fragments (jumping libraries) and nally gap lling. In this study, the total input genome coverage of Illumina HiSeq sequences was B95 (fragments, 3 and 8 kb spanning
inserts) based on a genome size estimate of 1.2 Gb. The combined sequence reads were assembled using the ALLPATHS software40. This draft assembly was referred to as Parus_major 1.0.1. This version has been gap lled and cleaned of contaminating contigs. The assembly is made up of a total of 2,066 scaffolds with an N50 scaffold length of over 7.7 Mb (N50 contig length was 133 kb). The total assembled contigs spans 1.0 Gb, and has an assembled coverage of 40 using
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10474 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 5
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474
fragment reads aligned to the assembly. The nal assembly (Pmajor1.04) has been deposited at DDBJ/EMBL/GenBank under the accession JRXK00000000. The version described in this paper is version JRXK01000000. The genome is also publicly available in https://genomes.bioinf.nioo.knaw.nl/
Web End =https://genomes.bioinf.nioo.knaw.nl/ .
Assembly chromosome builds. Assembled scaffolds were assigned to specic chromosomes using two Parus major linkage maps19 constructed from different populations; one at Wytham Woods, Oxford (UK) and the other at de Hoge Veluwe (Netherlands), which is the population that the reference bird descended from. Flanking sequences of single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) positioned on the linkage map were aligned against the assembled scaffolds using BLAT. Scaffolds that contained multiple SNP markers were then oriented and positioned on the basis of the positions of the SNP markers on the linkage map. The order of the SNP markers on the scaffolds and the linkage map were in good agreement (Supplementary Fig. 6). Regardless of whether the Netherlands or UK map was used, the Spearman rank correlation coefcient between the linkage map marker order and the assembly marker order was 0.99 or greater for nearly all the chromosomes. Exceptions were only on microchromosomes with just a few markers, for example, linkage group 25LG22. If anything, correlation coefcients were slightly higher when the UK map was used, despite the genome assembly being performed on a Netherlands bird. This indicates that the assembly is equally applicable to other great tit populations as it is to the source population. Most discrepancies between the orders on the sequence and linkage map were caused by the lower resolution of the linkage map involving SNPs that were less than 1 cM apart. Two scaffolds (22 and 28) appeared to be chimeric and were manually split between contigs Contig22.251-Contig22.250 and contigs Contig28.53-Contig28.51, respectively. Because of a lack of any genetic marker on Contig28.52, this contig was not assigned to a specic chromosome.
Small scaffolds that were assigned to a chromosome based on only a single marker were oriented based on the zebra nch-great tit comparative map, taking into account the orientation of the anking scaffolds assigned to that chromosome. Next, we used MUMMER to align all unassigned scaffolds against the zebra nch genome20 and larger scaffolds that unambiguously mapped to a specic region of the zebra nch genome at a location between assigned scaffolds were assigned to that location in the genome. The orientation of these scaffolds was chosen in relation to the adjacent mapped scaffolds, thus minimizing the number of rearrangements. In total, 300 scaffolds could be assigned to a chromosomal location based on the linkage map19 totalling 975,330,736 bp and 124 scaffolds were assigned to a chromosomal location based on the alignment with the zebra nch genome, totalling 23,489,268 bp (Supplementary Data 1). Although the nal genome assembly therefore is not completely de novo, these contigs only represent2.4% of the assembled sequence and 97.6% of the assembly represents a truly de novo genome assembly.
RNA sequencing and assembly. RNA was extracted from eight tissues (bone marrow, homogenized half of the brain, breast let, higher intestine, kidney, liver, lung and testis) from the reference bird and was then used to prepare tissue-specic tagged Illumina sequencing libraries. The tagged libraries were pooled and sequenced using ve lanes on one owcell of Illumina HiSeq 2000 (same run on the machine). This resulted in 100 bp paired-end unstranded RNA sequencing data. The number of reads per tissue ranged from 98 to 229 million with a total number of 1.25 billion paired-end reads (Supplementary Data 7). The sequence reads were checked for quality with FastQC (http://www.bioinformatics.babraham.ac.uk/projects/fastqc/
Web End =http://www.bioinformatics.babraham.ac.uk/pro http://www.bioinformatics.babraham.ac.uk/projects/fastqc/
Web End =jects/fastqc/ ) and low-quality sequences were trimmed with Fastq-Mcf41 resulting in a nal number of 1,096,140,415 paired-end reads that were used for the annotation. The RNA sequencing data per individual tissue has been submitted to Genbank (GT_BoneMarrow SRS863935, GT_Brain SRS866013, GT_BreastFilet SRS86603, GT_HigherIntestine SRS866033, GT_Liver SRS866035, GT_Kidney SRS866036, GT_Lung SRS866044, GT_Testis SRS866048).
The combined 1 billion reads from all eight tissues were simultaneously de novo assembled using the Trinity software package42 v. r2013-02-25. Because of the high depth of the RNA sequencing data, we rst normalized the data. Following the normalization, the Trinity assembly was subsequently run using the default settings. We obtained a total of 101,289 assembled transcripts ranging in size from 201 to 16,061 bp, with an average size of 1,335 bp. We also did a reference-based RNA assembling by using the normalized RNA sequencing data in Tophat version v2.0.10 (ref. 43; Bowtie v2.1.0 (ref. 44)).
Genome annotation. For the genome annotation, both PASA v. 2-r20130605 (ref. 45) and MAKER v. 2.31.5 (ref. 46) pipelines were used (Supplementary Fig. 7). First PASA (program to assemble spliced alignments) was used for the identication of spliced transcripts and the grouping of the identied transcripts belonging to the same gene. PASA was run using the assembled Trinity transcripts and the Cufinks gtf output le from Tophat/Bowtie as input. Alignment within PASA was done using gmap. The PASA analysis resulted in a total of 74,229 different assembled transcripts with an average size of 1,564 bp. A comparison (using blast) with the annotated transcriptomes of chicken21, zebra nch20, ycatcher47 and ground tit48 (derived from Ensembl release 75 or NCBI refseq release 66) indicated that these transcripts represented 13,626 different genes in these other birds.
In the MAKER pipeline, the output of PASA was used as EST evidence. From the ab initio predictors, AUGUSTUS version 3.0.2 (ref. 49) was applied by using the chicken gene model. The same RNA-seq de novo assembly as used in PASA was included in MAKER. In addition to de novo assembly, reference-based RNA-seq assembly was used. Last, protein evidence from zebra nch (version 3.2.4.75) and chicken (version 4.75) obtained from Ensembl version 75 was aligned to the great tit genome. By combining these two pipelines, we obtained 21,057 transcripts for 13,036 great tit genes. See additional information about functional annotation and repeat/RNA masking in Supplementary Information and Supplementary Data 8.
Resequencing and SNP calling. We analysed 29 wild great tit individuals (Supplementary Data 2) covering a wide range of the species distribution (Fig. 1); 10 individuals were from the Wytham population in Oxford (UK), and the remaining 19 birds were sampled from 15 European populations. Each bird was sequenced to E10 coverage. Paired-end sequencing libraries of each sample
were built with an insert size of 300 bp and sequencing was performed on a HiSeq 2000 platform with a read length of 100 bp. The raw reads were trimmed and ltered with Sickle (https://github.com/najoshi/sickle
Web End =https://github.com/najoshi/sickle) using default parameters and a length restriction of 75. We then used the BurrowsWheeler Aligner50 to map the ltered raw reads onto the assembled great tit reference genome. Subsequently, we removed duplicates and conducted local realignments following the best practices of the GATK pipeline51. We used ANGSD52 to call SNPs based on the genotype likelihoods estimated by the GATK model from the mapped reads51; this approach has been shown to produce more accurate estimates of the Site Frequency Spectrum (SFS) than other widely used SNP-calling pipelines when sequencing coverage is low (o10 )53, which is critical for our analysis because our average
coverage is 10 . However, for comparison, we also called SNPs using three
additional approaches: (1) GATK using data from each individual separately (the Single approach); (2) GATK using data from all individuals simultaneously (the Multi approach); (3) Platypus, which calls SNPs directly from the BurrowsWheeler Aligner alignments, independent of the GATK pipeline54.
We also tested the reliability of the SNP data, see details in Supplementary Information and Supplementary Data 9 and 10 and Supplementary Fig. 9.
Population differentiation and demography. We found very little differentiation among populations (FSTo0.015; Supplementary Data 3). The most differentiated population pairs were SpainUK (ES versus WW, UK; Fig. 1) and FranceSpain (FRES; Fig. 1) with FST 0.012.
We analysed the demographic history with pairwise sequential Markovian coalescent analysis (PSMC23). PSMC estimates rates of coalescent events across a single genome and uses these to infer Ne (the effective population size) in the past23. The model relies signicantly on condently called polymorphic sites and requires that both alleles are called. Therefore we conducted this analysis with variants called on the high coverage reference genome sequence. We set the mutation rate to 2.0 10 9 per year per site and the generation time to 2 years.
Genome-wide diversity and Tajimas D. We obtained genome-wide sliding window estimates (step size 10 kb, window size 50 kb) of Wattersons Y (Fig. 2) and Tajimas D (Fig. 2) along each chromosome based on the SNP calls from ANGSD. For most macrochromosomes, diversity is increased towards the chromosome ends, and there are remarkable local drops of diversity on chromosomes 3, 6 and Z (Fig. 2; more details about low diversity in chromosome Zin Supplementary Information and Supplementary Fig. 10). By calculating Wattersons Y for synonymous sites in protein-coding genes we found aclear negative correlation between chromosome length and diversity levels (Supplementary Fig. 8); we observed the same pattern when diversity was estimated using all sites (Supplementary Fig. 8).
Selective sweep detection. We scanned the genome for target regions of positive selection using Sweepnder55 which uses local deviations of the site frequency spectrum (SFS) compared with a standard neutral model or chromosome/genome-wide reference to infer the action of positive selection. We used SNP calls from the ANGSD pipeline to construct SFS. ANGSD assumes that each variable position is biallelic and determines allele frequencies based on the genotype likelihoods56. Whole-genome alignments constructed with MAUVE57 for zebra nch20, ycatcher47, ground tit48 and chicken21 were used to infer the ancestral state based on maximum parsimony. Sites for which the ancestral state could not be reconstructed with condence were included in the analysis as folded sites (unpolarized). The chromosome-wide SFS was used as reference for each chromosome as recommended by Sweepnder, which helps to reduce false positives caused by past demographic changes. We used a dense grid size of 100 bp and extracted sweep targets that had a composite likelihood score (CLR) within the top 1%. Neighbouring sweep targets were merged to sweep regions, with the total CLR score of a sweep region being the sum of the CLR score of each sweep target. The top 3% of genes that overlapped with or were near (within 5 kB anking the sweep region) sweep regions larger than 300 bp were extracted (Supplementary Data 4). Targets for positive selection included the low diversity region of chromosome 3, 6 and Z. Further details of the Sweepnder pipeline can be found in the Supplementary Materials and Methods.
6 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10474 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 ARTICLE
Multiple alignment construction for substitution rate analyses. Natural selection affects the composition of genomes and we were interested in estimating gene-specic rates of molecular evolution (dN/dS) and nding evidence for the action of positive selection in protein coding genes. Since alignment quality is crucial for reliably conducting tests of positive selection using substitution rate analyses, we chose a very conservative approach. We used homologous genes from zebra nch and chicken, the two best annotated bird genomes to date. We only analysed genes with a homologue in both, and used MUSCLE58 along with ZORRO59 to exclude positions of low alignment certainty. To obtain the corresponding multiple DNA codon alignments, protein alignments along with the unaligned DNA sequences were prepared with PAL2NAL60. Altogether, we constructed E11,107 triplet alignments. Substitution rates were calculated using
PAML61 to obtain the nonsynonymous to synonymous substitution rate ratio (dN/dS o). o values o1, 1 and 41 indicate purifying selection, neutral
evolution and diversifying (positive) selection, respectively. Rates of molecular evolution (dN/dS) for each gene were obtained from the one-ratio model M0 from PAML that assumes a constant o for the whole gene phylogeny. In addition, a site test was used to detect positive selection. Specically, we compared the likelihoods calculated using model M8, which assumes a proportion of sites to evolve under positive selection, and model M7, which does not assume a site class with o exceeding one. Positive selection was inferred when the model M7 and M8 were signicantly different as assessed by a likelihood-ratio test that assumes that 2DlnL (twice the log likelihood difference) is w2 distributed with two degrees of freedom.
The P values were adjusted for multiple testing (Benjamini and Hochberg) with a false discovery rate of 0.2; w2 test results for an enrichment of positively selected genes were qualitatively similar for FDRs 0.1, 0.3, 0.4 and 0.5 (P 2.17 10 5,
3.97 10 3, 2.58 10 3 and 2.17 10 3, respectively).
GO enrichment analyses. Human orthologues were obtained for the great tit genes by using a combination of Ensembl and Uniprot databases. In the sweep areas, orthologues were found for 460 genes. Functional relatedness of GO terms was done using the Cytoscape plugin ClueGo 2.1.4 (ref. 62). ClueGo constructs and compares networks of functionally related GO terms with kappa statistics.
A two-sided hypergeometric test (enrichment/depletion) was applied with GO term fusion, network specicity was set to medium and false discovery correction was carried out using the Bonferroni step-down method. We used both human(08.10.2015) and chicken gene ontologies (08.10.2015) for comparison. With human gene ontologies, we detected 13 functional groups of GO terms across all sweep areas (Supplementary Data 5a). These groups were largely involved in functions concerning dendrite, cytoskeleton organization, protein oligomerization, synaptic transmission, regulation of phosphatase activity, synaptic membrane and cognition. We also did a GO enrichment analysis for the positively selected genes, as dened by the PAML-based analysis, in the same way as for the sweep genes, and obtained 12 functional GO groups (Supplementary Data 5b). When using the chicken orthologues for both sweep and positively selected genes, the results were comparable but with lower signicance levels (Supplementary Data 5c,d) because the chicken genes were not as well GO annotated as the human ones.
To further conrm our GO enrichment results, we randomly selected 50 sets of genes from outside the sweep area, each containing B460 genes (the same number as in our selective sweep set), and analysed them using the same Cluego settings. We found that the GO term groups signicantly enriched in our selective sweep set appeared no more than three times except for phosphatidylinositol-mediated signalling GO group which appeared seven times. This additional analysis further supports the robustness of our results.
Test of accelerated evolution in great tit EGR1. We used BLAST to obtain orthologous sequences of the EGR1 gene from 45 additional bird species37,38 downloaded from Ensembl version 75 or from Gigascience (http://gigadb.org/dataset/101000
Web End =http://gigadb.org/ http://gigadb.org/dataset/101000
Web End =dataset/101000 , Supplementary Fig. 2, detailed species list provided in Supplementary Data 6) to test whether there was additional evidence for the action of positive selection during the evolution of EGR1 in birds. Pairwise dN/dS rates revealed that there is a substantial increase in dN/dS between great tit and ground tit relative to other pairwise values (Supplementary Fig. 2, upper panel). We also tested whether the increased xations are unique to the captive reference bird by using variable positions from the 29 re-sequenced birds, the reference bird and the ground tit genome and found no evidence for this (Supplementary Fig. 2 lower panel).
Brain gene expression. To compare expression levels and methylation in the brain, 200,793,186 trimmed paired-end reads from the brain were aligned against the assembled genome using Tophat version v2.0.10 (ref. 43; Bowtie v2.1.0(ref. 44)) with the same settings as described above, except that multiple hits were preltered against the genome (-M option) and the reads were rst aligned against the nal annotation (-G option). The brain Tophat alignment was analysed with Cufinks v2.2.0 using the same settings as above, except that the annotation was also included (-g option). Expression levels of brain genes were extracted from the Cufinks output.
Methylation analysis. Blood and brain DNA libraries were constructed according to the Epitect whole-genome bisulte sequencing workow (Illumina) with 18 PCR
cycles. Whole-genome sequencing data were generated using the Illumina HiSeq 2,500 platform at Business Unit Bioscience, Wageningen UR. The number of paired-end reads (101 bp) were 358M and 292M for the brain and the blood, respectively. Raw sequencing reads were trimmed for quality (Z20) and adaptor sequence using trim_galore v.0.1.4 (http://www.bioinformatics.babraham.ac.uk/projects/trim_galore/
Web End =http://www.bioinformatics.babraham.ac.uk/ http://www.bioinformatics.babraham.ac.uk/projects/trim_galore/
Web End =projects/trim_galore/ ). The methylation data has been submitted to NCBI with accession numbers SRR2070790 and SRR2070791 for the brain and the blood, respectively. Trimmed sequences were aligned to the reference genome using BSseeker2 v2.0.6 (ref. 63) with Bowtie2 v.2.1.0 (ref. 44) in the local alignment mode. A total, 97.63% and 99.93% of the genome was covered to an average depth of 31.88 and 33.04 in brain and blood, respectively. Methylation levels for
each site were determined using the BSseeker2 methylation call script. All the analyses were done using sites covered by a minimum of 10 reads in both the samples. Only genes found to be 1:1:1 orthologues with chicken and zebra nch were used for methylation analysis. Gene bodies (annotated gene boundaries excluding the 50 5% of genes) and TSS (300 bp upstream to 50 bp downstream of the annotated starting position of each gene) for which we had information from at least 50% of the potential methylation sites were used in the dN/dS and expression correlation analysis. Average methylation levels for TSS and gene bodies were calculated for each individual gene. The upper (highly methylated) and lower (lowly methylated) quartiles were compared for differences in their evolutionary rates (dN/dS) using a MannWhitney U-test. Correlations between the average methylation level of a given region (TSS or gene body) per gene with expression were performed using Spearmans rank correlation. A sliding window approach was used to infer differences in methylation levels between sweep and non-sweep gene regions. Non-sweep genes located on scaffolds were not included in the analysis, as these regions were not tested for sweeps. For this, genes were divided into different regions: the gene body (described above), 10 kb upstream and 10 kb downstream of the gene body and the TSS region (described above). Each gene body was subdivided into 40 bins, with the length of each bin therefore depending on the length of the gene. We calculated the mean methylation levels of these 40 bins with an overlap between neighbouring bins of 250 bp. Upstream and downstream regions were divided into bins of exactly 250 bp with an overlap of 125 bp between consecutive bins.
To compare TSS methylation between sweep and non-sweep genes, we conducted a t-test with equal variance assumed. Only TSS regions with a minimum of 10 covered sites were used for comparative analysis. To compare methylation levels between sweep and non-sweep genes for other gene regions, we conducted LMM analyses with methylation level as the dependent variable, sweep (yes or no) as a xed factor and bin as a random factor. A likelihood-ratio test was conducted comparing the model with and without sweep as a xed factor. All criteria were met for conducting parametric analyses.
References
1. Rendell, L. et al. Cognitive culture: theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 6876 (2011).
2. Leadbeater, E. What evolves in the evolution of social learning? J. Zool. 295, 411 (2015).
3. Aplin, L. M. et al. Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature 518, 538541 (2015).
4. Titulaer, M., van Oers, K. & Naguib, M. Personality affects learning performance in difcult tasks in a sex-dependent way. Anim. Behav. 83, 723730 (2012).
5. Cole, E. F., Morand-Ferron, J., Hinks, A. E. & Quinn, J. L. Cognitive ability inuences reproductive life history variation in the wild. Curr. Biol. 22, 18081812 (2012).
6. Overington, S. E., Morand-Ferron, J., Boogert, N. J. & Lefebvre, L. Technical innovations drive the relationship between innovativeness and residual brain size in birds. Anim. Behav. 78, 10011010 (2009).
7. Boyce, M. S. & Perrins, C. M. Optimizing great tit clutch size in a uctuating environment. Ecology 68, 142153 (1987).
8. Nussey, D. H., Postma, E., Gienapp, P. & Visser, M. E. Selection on heritable phenotypic plasticity in a wild bird population. Science 310, 304306 (2005).
9. Charmantier, A. et al. Adaptive phenotypic plasticity in response to climate change in a wild bird population. Science 320, 800803 (2008).
10. Pettifor, R. A., Perrins, C. M. & McCleery, R. H. Individual optimization of clutch size in great tits. Nature 336, 160162 (1988).
11. Knowles, S. C. L., Nakagawa, S. & Sheldon, B. C. Elevated reproductive effort increases blood parasitaemia and decreases immune function in birds: a meta-regression approach. Funct. Ecol. 23, 405415 (2009).
12. Bouwhuis, S., Sheldon, B. C., Verhulst, S. & Charmantier, A. Great tits growing old: selective disappearance and the partitioning of senescence to stages within the breeding cycle. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 276, 27692777 (2009).
13. Van Noordwijk, A. J. & Scharloo, W. Inbreeding in an island population of the great tit. Evolution 35, 674688 (1981).
14. Greenwood, P. J., Harvey, P. H. & Perrins, C. M. Inbreeding and dispersal in the great tit. Nature 271, 5254 (1978).
15. Richner, H. Host-parasite interactions and life-history evolution. Zoology 101, 333344 (1998).
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10474 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 7
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474
16. Krebs, J. R. Territory and breeding density in the great tit , Parus major L. Ecology 52, 222 (1971).
17. Mappes, J. & Alatalo, R. V. Effects of novelty and gregariousness in survival of aposematic prey. Behav. Ecol. 8, 174177 (1997).
18. Van Oers, K. & Naguib, M. in Animal Personalities: Behavior, Physiology and Evolution (eds Carere, C. & Maestripieri, D.) 520 (Chicago Univ. Press, 2013).
19. Van Oers, K. et al. Replicated high-density genetic maps of two great tit populations reveal ne-scale genomic departures from sex-equal recombination rates. Heredity 112, 307316 (2014).
20. Warren, W. C. et al. The genome of a songbird. Nature 464, 757762 (2010).21. ICGSC. Sequence and comparative analysis of the chicken genome provide unique perspectives on vertebrate evolution. Nature 432, 695716 (2004).
22. Kvist, L. et al. Evolution and genetic structure of the great tit (Parus major) complex. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 270, 14471454 (2003).
23. Li, H. & Durbin, R. Inference of human population history from individual whole-genome sequences. Nature 475, 493496 (2011).
24. Clayton, D. F. The genomics of memory and learning in songbirds. Annu. Rev. Genomics Hum. Genet. 14, 4565 (2013).
25. Hara, E., Kubikova, L., Hessler, N. A & Jarvis, E. D. Role of the midbrain dopaminergic system in modulation of vocal brain activation by social context. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 34063416 (2007).
26. Dragunow, M. A role for immediate-early transcription factors in learning and memory. Behav. Genet. 26, 293299 (1996).
27. Bolhuis, J. J., Zijlstra, G. G. O., den Boer-Visser, A. M. & Van Der Zee, E. A. Localized neuronal activation in the zebra nch brain is related to the strength of song learning. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 22822285 (2000).
28. Enard, W. et al. Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature 418, 869872 (2002).
29. Teramitsu, I. & White, S. A. FoxP2 regulation during undirected singing in adult songbirds. J. Neurosci. 26, 73907394 (2006).
30. Haesler, S. et al. Incomplete and inaccurate vocal imitation after knockdown of FoxP2 in songbird basal ganglia nucleus area X. PLoS Biol. 5, e321 (2007).
31. Goldberg, A. D., Allis, C. D. & Bernstein, E. Epigenetics: a landscape takes shape. Cell 128, 635638 (2007).
32. Ball, M. P., Li, J. B., Gao, Y., Lee, J. & Leproust, E. Targeted and genome-scale methylomics reveals gene body signatures in human cell lines. Nat. Biotechnol. 27, 361368 (2009).
33. Guo, J. U. et al. Distribution, recognition and regulation of non-CpG methylation in the adult mammalian brain. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 215222 (2014).
34. Shirane, K. et al. Mouse oocyte methylomes at base resolution reveal genome-wide accumulation of non-CpG methylation and role of DNA methyltransferases. PLoS Genet. 9, e1003439 (2013).
35. Lister, R. et al. Human DNA methylomes at base resolution show widespread epigenomic differences. Nature 462, 315322 (2009).
36. Natt, D. et al. Heritable genome-wide variation of gene expression and promoter methylation between wild and domesticated chickens. BMC Genomics 13, 59 (2012).
37. Jarvis, E. D. et al. Whole-genome analyses resolve early branches in the tree of life of modern birds. Science 346, 13201331 (2014).
38. Zhang, G. et al. Comparative genomics reveals insights into avian genome evolution and adaptation. Science 346, 13111320 (2014).
39. Drent, P. J., van Oers, K. & van Noordwijk, A. J. Realized heritability of personalities in the great tit (Parus major). Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 270, 4551 (2003).
40. Gnerre, S. et al. High-quality draft assemblies of mammalian genomes from massively parallel sequence data. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 15131518 (2011).
41. Aronesty, E. Comparison of sequencing utility programs. Open Bioinforma. J. 7, 18 (2013).
42. Grabherr, M. G. et al. Full-length transcriptome assembly from RNA-Seq data without a reference genome. Nat. Biotechnol. 29, 644652 (2011).
43. Kim, D. et al. TopHat2: accurate alignment of transcriptomes in the presence of insertions, deletions and gene fusions. Genome Biol. 14, R36 (2013).
44. Langmead, B., Trapnell, C., Pop, M. & Salzberg, S. L. Ultrafast and memory-efcient alignment of short DNA sequences to the human genome. Genome Biol. 10, R25 (2009).
45. Haas, B. J. et al. De novo transcript sequence reconstruction from RNA-seq using the Trinity platform for reference generation and analysis. Nat. Protoc. 8, 14941512 (2013).
46. Cantarel, B. L. et al. MAKER: an easy-to-use annotation pipeline designed for emerging model organism genomes. Genome Res. 18, 188196 (2008).
47. Ellegren, H. et al. The genomic landscape of species divergence in Ficedula ycatchers. Nature 491, 756760 (2012).
48. Qu, Y. et al. Ground tit genome reveals avian adaptation to living at high altitudes in the Tibetan plateau. Nat. Commun. 4, 2071 (2013).
49. Stanke, M., Tzvetkova, A. & Morgenstern, B. AUGUSTUS at EGASP: using EST, protein and genomic alignments for improved gene prediction in the human genome. Genome Biol. 7(Suppl 1): S11.18 (2006).
50. Li, H. & Durbin, R. Fast and accurate short read alignment with Burrows-Wheeler transform. Bioinformatics 25, 17541760 (2009).
51. DePristo, M. A. et al. A framework for variation discovery and genotyping using next-generation DNA sequencing data. Nat. Genet. 43, 491498 (2011).
52. Korneliussen, T., Albrechtsen, A. & Nielsen, R. ANGSD: Analysis of Next Generation Sequencing Data. BMC Bioinformatics 15, 356 (2014).
53. Han, E., Sinsheimer, J. S. & Novembre, J. Characterizing bias in population genetic inferences from low-coverage sequencing data. Mol. Biol. Evol. 31, 723735 (2014).
54. Rimmer, A. et al. Integrating mapping-, assembly- and haplotype-based approaches for calling variants in clinical sequencing applications. Nat. Genet. 46, 912918 (2014).
55. Nielsen, R. et al. Genomic scans for selective sweeps using SNP data. Genome Res. 15, 15661575 (2005).
56. Kim, S.Y. et al. Estimation of allele frequency and association mapping using next-generation sequencing data. BMC Bioinformatics 35, 231 (2011).
57. Darling, A. E., Mau, B. & Perna, N. T. progressiveMauve: multiple genome alignment with gene gain, loss and rearrangement. PLoS ONE 5, e11147 (2010).
58. Edgar, R. C. MUSCLE: multiple sequence alignment with high accuracy and high throughput. Nucleic Acids Res. 32, 17921797 (2004).
59. Wu, M., Chatterji, S. & Eisen, J. A. Accounting for alignment uncertainty in phylogenomics. PLoS ONE 7, e30288 (2012).
60. Suyama, M., Torrents, D. & Bork, P. PAL2NAL: Robust conversion of protein sequence alignments into the corresponding codon alignments. Nucleic Acids Res. 34, W609W612 (2006).
61. Yang, Z. PAML 4: phylogenetic analysis by maximum likelihood. Mol. Biol. Evol. 24, 15861591 (2007).
62. Bindea, G. et al. ClueGO: A Cytoscape plug-in to decipher functionally grouped gene ontology and pathway annotation networks. Bioinformatics 25, 10911093 (2009).
63. Guo, W. et al. BS-Seeker2: a versatile aligning pipeline for bisulte sequencing data. BMC Genomics 14, 774 (2013).
Acknowledgements
We thank Eveline Verhulst for help with the methylome data, Christa Mateman for lab assistance, Martijn Derks for calculating the sliding windows, Tieshan Xu for the help with the Trinity assembly, Louise Dittmar for the help in dN/dS and diversity analysis, Christian Huber for help on the sweep analysis and Jun-Mo Kim who designed the SNP chip. K.M.S. was supported by a grant from the Cooperative Research Program for Agriculture Science & Technology Development (PJ009103) of the Rural Development Administration, Republic of Korea. T.I.G., P.C. and K.Z. were supported by a BBSRC grant (BB/K000209/1) and a NERC grant (NE/L005328/1) awarded to K.Z., C.J.G. was funded by Natural Environment Research Council (NERC) (NE/K01126X/1). K.J.F.V. was funded by the Dutch Organisation for Scientic Research, NWO VIDI grant (864.10.008). B.C.S. was funded by ERC Advanced Grant (250164) and by a Wolfson Merit Award from the Royal Society. J.S. was funded by a European Research Council (ERC) Starting grant, Avian EGG (202487) and a Natural Environment Research Council (NERC), The Great Tit HapMap Project (NE/J012599/1). M.E.V. was supported by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientic Research (NWO-VICI grant) and the European Research Council (ERC-2013-AdG 339092).
Author contributions
M.A.M.G., M.E.V. and K.v.O. designed the research. P.M., W.C.W. and M.A.M.G. did the genome assembly. V.N.L., V.d.J. and H.J.M. did the genome annotation. R.P.M.A.C. isolated DNA and RNA and coordinated RNA sequencing. T.I.G., P.C. and K.Z. performed and coordinated the diversity and signatures of selection analyses. C.J.G. did the effective population size analyses. O.M. coordinated the methylome analysis and performed the gene expression analyses. K.v.O. and K.J.F.V. initiated the methylome sequencing and K.v.O., K.M.S., O.M., K.J.F.V. and T.I.G. analysed the methylome data. B.C.S., K.v.O., M.E.V. and TGTHC provided great tit DNA samples and J.S. coordinated the re-sequencing of additional birds. J.S., K.v.O. and M.A.M.G. performed the comparison between the linkage maps and genome assembly. V.N.L. did the GO analyses. V.N.L., T.I.G., K.M.S., C.J.G., K.v.O. and M.A.M.G. wrote the manuscript.
All the authors provided input during the writing of the manuscript.
Additional information
Accession Codes: The raw sequences of reference great tit have been deposited tothe NCBI Sequence Read Archive (SRA; http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/sra
Web End =http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/sra ) under the accession number SRS1185780. The nal assembly (Pmajor1.04) has been deposited in DDBJ/EMBL/GenBank under the accession JRXK00000000. The version described in this paper is version JRXK01000000. The RNAseq reads for the eight tissues have been deposited to the NCBI Sequence Read Archive under the accession numbers GT_BoneMarrow SRS863935, GT_Brain SRS866013, GT_BreastFilet SRS866031, GT_HigherIntestine SRS866033, GT_Liver SRS866035, GT_Kidney SRS866036, GT_Lung SRS866044, GT_Testis SRS866048. The whole-genome bisulte sequencing reads for the two tissues have been deposited to the NCBI Sequence Read Archive under
8 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10474 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 ARTICLE
the accession numbers GT_Brain_BS SRS964344 and GT_Blood_BS SRS964345. The reads for the 29 re-sequenced birds have been deposited to the NCBI Sequence Read Archive under the accession number SRP066678.
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =http://www.nature.com/ http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =naturecommunications
Competing nancial interests: The authors declare no competing nancial interests.
Reprints and permission information is available online at http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =http://npg.nature.com/ http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =reprintsandpermissions/
How to cite this article: Laine, V. N. et al. Evolutionary signals of selection on cognition from the great tit genome and methylome. Nat. Commun. 7:10474 doi: 10.1038/ncomms10474 (2016).
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articles Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if the material is not included under the Creative Commons license, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to reproduce the material. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Web End =http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
The Great Tit HapMap ConsortiumFrank Adriaensen8, Eduardo Belda9, Andrey Bushuev10, Mariusz Cichon11, Anne Charmantier12,Niels Dingemanse13, Blandine Doligez14, Tapio Eeva15, Kjell Einar Erikstad16, Slava Fedorov17, Michaela Hau13, Sabine Hille18, Camilla Hinde19, Bart Kempenaers13, Anvar Kerimov10, Milos Krist20, Raivo Mand21, Erik Matthysen8, Reudi Nager22, Claudia Norte23, Markku Orell24, Heinz Richner25, Tore Slagsvold26, Vallo Tilgar21, Joost Tinbergen27, Janos Torok28, Barbara Tschirren29, Tera Yuta30
8Evolutionary Ecology Group, Department of Biology, University of Antwerp, B-2020 Antwerp, Belgium. 9Departamento de Ciencia Animal, IGIC, Universidad Politcnica de Valencia, C/Paranimf n1, E-46730 Ganda (Valencia), Espana. 10Faculty of Biology, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Moscow 119234, Russia. 11Inst. of Environmental Sciences, Jagiellonian University, Gronostajowa 7, 30-387 Krakw, Poland. 12CEFE-CNRS, UMR 5175, 1919, route de
Mende, F34293 Montpellier Cedex 5, France. 13Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Department of Behavioural Ecology & Evolutionary Genetics, Eberhard-Gwinner-Stra, House 5, 82319 Seewiesen (Starnberg), Germany. 14UMR CNRS 5558LBBE, Biomtrie et Biologievolutive, UCB Lyon 1 - Bt. Grgor
Mendel, 43 bd du 11 novembre 1918, 69622 VILLEURBANNE cedex, France. 15Section of Ecology, Department of Biology, University of Turku, Turku 20014, Finland. 16Norwegian Institute for Nature Research, FRAM-High North Research Centre for Climate and the Environment, 9296 Troms, Norway.
17Department of Vertebrate Zoology, Moscow State University, Moscow 119899 Russia. 18Institute of Wildlife Biology and Game Management, University of Natural Resources and Life Science, A-1180 Vienna, Austria. 19Behavioural Ecology Group, Department of Animal Sciences, Wageningen University, Wageningen 6708 PB, The Netherlands. 20Department of Zoology and Laboratory of Ornithology, Faculty of Science, Palack University, Olomouc 77147, Czech Republic. 21Department of Zoology, Institute of Ecology and Earth Sciences, University of Tartu, Vanemuise 46, Tartu 51014, Estonia. 22Institute of Biodiversity, Animal Health and Comparative Medicine, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8QQ, UK. 23Department of Life Sciences, Institute of Marine Research IMAR/CMA, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal. 24Department of Biology, University of Oulu, P.O. Box 3000, 90014 Oulu, Finland.
25Evolutionary Ecology Lab, Institute of Ecology and Evolution, University of Bern, Bern 3012, Switzerland. 26Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary Synthesis (CEES), Department of Biosciences, University of Oslo, P.O. Box 1066, Blindern, 0316 Oslo, Norway. 27Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary Studies (CEES), Univ. of Groningen, PO Box 11103, NL-9700 CC Groningen, The Netherlands. 28Behavioural Ecology Group, Department of Systematic Zoology and Ecology, Etvs Lornd University, Budapest H-1117, Hungary. 29Institute of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, CH-8057 Zurich, Switzerland. 30Graduate School of Environmental Science, Hokkaido University, N10 W5 Sapporo, Hokkaido 060-0810, Japan.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10474 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10474 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 9
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Nature Publishing Group Jan 2016
Abstract
For over 50 years, the great tit (Parus major) has been a model species for research in evolutionary, ecological and behavioural research; in particular, learning and cognition have been intensively studied. Here, to provide further insight into the molecular mechanisms behind these important traits, we de novo assemble a great tit reference genome and whole-genome re-sequence another 29 individuals from across Europe. We show an overrepresentation of genes related to neuronal functions, learning and cognition in regions under positive selection, as well as increased CpG methylation in these regions. In addition, great tit neuronal non-CpG methylation patterns are very similar to those observed in mammals, suggesting a universal role in neuronal epigenetic regulation which can affect learning-, memory- and experience-induced plasticity. The high-quality great tit genome assembly will play an instrumental role in furthering the integration of ecological, evolutionary, behavioural and genomic approaches in this model species.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer