INTRODUCTION
Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is a leading cause of death and disability worldwide. Although most people suffering from TBI recover, their quality of life is often poor in part due to hippocampal‐dependent memory‐related disorders (Blennow et al., ; Bryant, ). Although injury‐induced neurogenesis has been shown to mediate some aspects of recovery following TBI in animal models, mechanisms underlying this remain unclear as do the precise aspects of hippocampal function regulated by injury‐induced neurogenesis.
It is well established that newborn neurons are continually generated throughout life in the hippocampal dentate gyrus and olfactory bulbs (Christian, Song, & Ming, ; Ming & Song, ). These adult newborn neurons, especially in the dentate gyrus, are believed to participate in particular aspects of spatial and emotional memory formation (Besnard & Sahay, ; Christian et al., ; Ming & Song, ). Several studies also highlight that increases in neurogenesis, like voluntary exercise or exposure to enriched environments, enhance learning and memory formation (Creer, Romberg, Saksida, van Praag, & Bussey, ; Garthe, Roeder, & Kempermann, ; Thuret, Toni, Aigner, Yeo, & Gage, ). Interestingly, enhanced neurogenesis is also observed in a variety of brain injuries and many studies have shown that injury‐induced newborn dentate neurons are essential in recovery from some, though not all, cognitive deficits in animal models of brain injury (Blaiss et al., ; Cho et al., ; Fernandez‐Hernandez & Rhiner, ; Sun, ; Sun, Daniels, Rolfe, Waters, & Hamm, ).
The hippocampus plays a critical role in encoding similar events (or episodes) discretely and without interfering with previously stored memories (Anacker & Hen, ). It has been proposed that a distinct population of neurons in the dentate gyrus are recruited to encode or process information that associate experienced events with corresponding environmental stimuli, known as pattern separation (Amaral, Scharfman, & Lavenex, ; Kesner, ). Therefore, there is a low level of overlap of neurons in the dentate gyrus that become active even when two sensory stimuli are similar, which ensures the recall of associative memory underlying context‐appropriate responses. Mechanisms for intact pattern separation are required for fundamental aspects of memory formation and integration and many essential cognitive functions go awry if the ability of the dentate gyrus to regulate this is impaired (Besnard & Sahay, ; Kheirbek, Klemenhagen, Sahay, & Hen, ; Kheirbek, Tannenholz, & Hen, ; McHugh et al., ). For example, in fear memory, generalization of fear is protective, and an appropriate response to the likelihood of risk in the environment is considered both essential and adaptive. When overgeneralization of fear occurs due to damage in the hippocampus, the ability to discriminate a safe environment from an aversive one is detrimental and can result in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) or panic disorder (Besnard & Sahay, ; Kheirbek, Klemenhagen, et al., ).
Recent studies show that the ability of mice in distinguishing an aversive environment from a naïve one can be both diminished or enhanced. Impairments are seen when a specific NMDA receptor in adult dentate newborn neurons is selectively deleted, which inactivates these neurons, and enhancements are seen by deleting a proapoptotic gene specifically in adult newborn dentate gyrus neurons, which increases their number (Kheirbek, Klemenhagen, et al., ; Kheirbek, Tannenholz et al., ; McHugh et al., ; Sahay et al., ). Hence, newborn neurons in the adult dentate gyrus are believed to regulate pattern separation to ensure correct memory as the response is recalled in the context of specific sensory stimuli.
We used delta‐HSV‐TK transgenic mice to temporally abolish neurogenesis under the control of ganciclovir, and our previous studies demonstrate that injury‐induced newborn neurons in the dentate gyrus facilitate recovery in some, but not all, hippocampal‐dependent tasks (Blaiss et al., ; Yu, Kim, & Kernie, ; Yu, Zhang, Liebl, & Kernie, ). Here, we tested whether injury‐induced newborn neurons in the dentate gyrus facilitate performance in discriminating between aversive and naïve environments. Surprisingly, we found that performance improved in mice when neurogenesis was inhibited whether injury was performed or not. Furthermore, we also observed that the sequence in introducing the aversive and naïve environment affect the performance when neurogenesis was inhibited. Therefore, impaired neurogenesis improved contextual discrimination learning in a protocol‐dependent manner and this phenomenon is enhanced following injury.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals
All experimental procedures were conducted under the approval of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Columbia University, and experimental animals were humanely housed and cared for under the supervision of the Institute of Comparative Medicine. The previously generated nestin‐HSV‐TK transgenic mice (Jackson Laboratory #JAX:029671,
Controlled cortical impact injury
The standard protocol using a controlled cortical impact device (Leica Impact One, Leica Biosystems) was used to generate brain injuries as described previously (Blaiss et al., ; Yu et al., , ). Eight‐ or 9‐week‐old nestin‐HSV‐TK mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and then placed in a stereotactic frame. A midline incision was made, the soft tissues were reflected, and a 5 × 5 mm craniotomy was made between bregma and lambda and 1 mm lateral to the midline. The injury was generated on the left hemisphere with a 3 mm stainless steel tipped impact device with a deformation of 0.7 mm, constant speed of 4.4 m/s, and duration of 300 ms. After surgery, the scalp was fastened with sutures and the mice were allowed to recover. For sham operations, mice received all the procedures noted above, but the impact was not performed.
Administrations of 5′‐bromo‐deoxyuridine and valganciclovir
5′‐Bromo‐deoxyuridine (BrdU, 100 mg/kg, Sigma) was delivered via intraperitoneal injection once a day for 3 consecutive days starting 72 hr after surgery to verify the efficiency in ablating dividing neuronal progenitors while feeding with valganciclovir‐containing chow. Valganciclovir (Valcyte, Genetech) was mixed with regular grain‐based rodent diet to produce 900 mg/kg valganciclovir‐containing chow (Custom Animal Diets, LLC, Easton, PA) (Yu et al., ). Valganciclovir chow was provided ad libitum for valganciclovir‐treated groups until the conclusion of the experiments (Figure a). Sham‐operated and injured mice were housed separately to allow approximately equal intake.
Experimental design and the efficiency of inhibition of neurogenesis. (a) Eight‐week‐old delta‐HSV‐TK transgenic mice were used and underwent sham operation or CCI. One injection of BrdU was given to label the dividing cells for 3 consecutive days 72 hr after surgery. After 4 weeks of recovery, contextual fear discrimination was performed and the task took 13 or 19 days depending on the protocol used. (b) The quantification of newborn neurons in the dentate gyrus represented by BrdU/Prox1‐expressing cells revealed a 75% reduction in sham‐operated mice (unpaired two‐tail t test, **p = .0011). (c) The quantification of newborn neurons in the dentate gyrus represented by BrdU/Prox1‐expressing cells revealed a 66% reduction in CCI‐operated mice (unpaired two‐tail t test, **p = .001). (d) Representative pictures demonstrate the reduction of injury‐induced newborn neurons after being fed with valganciclovir‐containing chow (scale bar: 20 μm)
Contextual fear‐discrimination learning
The fear discrimination was conducted by an experimenter blinded to the experimental treatment conditions. The protocols of fear discrimination were described by other groups with some modifications (Sahay et al., ; Wehner & Radcliffe, ). Modifications were primarily in how many times the mice were exposed to A before B was introduced, the sequence of introduction of B to the mice, and total training days.
For fear discrimination learning, all experimental mice were exposed to context A in which a single 2‐s foot shock of 0.75 mA was delivered 3 min after placement in the chamber. Mice were left in the chamber for another 30 s after the shock, then were moved back to their home cage. Depending on experimental groups, mice were exposed to context B either before or after context A with at least a 1‐hr interval. The sequence of exposure in A or B was pseudorandomized and counterbalanced. An automated scoring system (EthoVision XT, Noldus) which digitalized the video signal and compared frame‐by‐frame changes in mouse position was used to measure the freezing levels in both contexts A (3 min before shock) and B (3 min in the chamber) each day.
Three different protocols were used to verify the impact of newborn neurons in discrimination (Figures a, a, a). In the extensive learning protocol (Figure a, protocol 3), experimental mice were exposed to context A with a 2‐s foot shock every day for further habituation, and context B was introduced starting from day 6 (the learning session). In the brief exposure version, two protocols were used. In both protocols, mice were exposed to context A with a 2‐s foot shock without exposure to B in the first day (conditioning). Starting on day 2, context B was introduced either before (Figure a, protocol 1) or after (Figure a, protocol 2) context A with at least a 1‐hr interval separating them. The sequence of exposure to A and B in protocol 2 were opposite to protocol 1.
(a) The sequence in exposing mice to contexts A and B in protocol 1. (b) The discrimination index indicated that the learning ability of sham‐operated mice changed along the learning period whether or not neurogenesis was intact, however, there was no significant difference between mice with intact neurogenesis and mice with impaired neurogenesis (p = .0297 with learning day as the variable, p = .233 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .3209 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Sidak's multiple comparisons). (c) A similar result was observed in the injured mice, the discrimination index suggested that no significant difference was found in injured mice regardless of whether or not neurogenesis was intact. Different from the sham‐operated mice, however, the index was not changed along the learning period in the injured mice (p = .2790 with learning day as the variable, p = .6057 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .1610 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Sidak's multiple comparisons). (d) To further investigate the performance of mice in context A, the percentage of freezing time along the learning period was analyzed. Consistent with the discrimination index, no significant difference was observed in sham‐operated mice with or without intact neurogenesis (p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .4295 with experimental group as the variable, and p < .0015 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Sidak's multiple comparisons). (e) Similar to sham‐operated mice, the performance of injured mice was alike in context A regardless of whether or not neurogenesis was intact (p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .6639 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .0424 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Sidak's multiple comparisons). (f) In context B, sham‐operated mice with impaired neurogenesis performed similarly as sham‐operated mice with intact neurogenesis (p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .5722 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .1366 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Sidak's multiple comparisons). (g) A similar performance was observed in injured mice whether or not neurogenesis was intact (p = .0003 with learning day as the variable, p = .8902 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .2903 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Sidak's multiple comparisons)
(a) The arrangement of contexts A and B, and the sequence of exposure to both contexts in protocol 2 (opposite to protocol 1). (b) Different from what was observed when protocol 1 was used, a subtle difference was observed in the sham‐operated mice with impaired neurogenesis. The mice showed discrimination at day 2, the first time context B was introduced (p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .1233 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .0610 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; *p < .05 in Sidak's post hoc analysis). (c) In injured mice, the discrimination index revealed that injured mice without neurogenesis had enhanced discrimination (p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .1233 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .0610 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 in Sidak's post hoc analysis). (d and e) The freezing time in context A demonstrated that mice without intact neurogenesis spent significantly less freezing time whether injured or not (d for sham‐operated mice, p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .001 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .5647 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; *p < .05 in Sidak's post hoc analysis; e for injured mice, p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .0114 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .2650 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; *p < .05 in Sidak's post hoc analysis). (f and g) A similar decrease in freezing time in both sham‐operated and injured mice without intact neurogenesis in context B (f for sham‐operated mice, p = .0022 with learning day as the variable, p = .0069 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .0287 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; *p < .05 in Sidak's post hoc analysis; g for injured mice, p = .0034 with learning day as the variable, p = .0128 with experimental group as the variable, and p < .0001 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; **p < .01, ***p < .001 in Sidak's post hoc analysis)
(a) The arrangement of contexts A and B, and the sequences of exposure to both contexts in protocol 3 was similar to protocol 2. (b) Mice associated the foot shock with context A successfully in a 5‐day training session regardless of injury or whether neurogenesis was intact or not (repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Tukey's multiple comparisons; time effect, p < .0001; experimental groups, p = .2306). (c and d) After the 5‐day training, there was no significant difference in indices using protocol 2 regardless of injury status or whether or not neurogenesis was intact (c for sham‐operated mice, p = .0094 with learning day as the variable, p = .0207 f with experimental group as the variable, p = .9760 for the interaction using repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; d for injured mice, p = .0773 with learning day as the variable, p = .2725 with experimental group as the variable, p = .7326 for the interaction using repeated measures two‐way ANOVA). (e and f) After extensive training, the decrease in freezing time in context A in both sham‐operated and injured mice with impaired neurogenesis when protocol 2 was used was not observed (e for sham‐operated mice, p = .0299 with learning day as the variable, p = .2630 with experimental group as the variable, p = .1726 for the interaction using repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; f for injured mice, p = .1221 with learning day as the variable, p = .4552 with experimental group as the variable, p = .2159 for the interaction using repeated measures two‐way ANOVA). (g and h) After extensive training, the decrease in freezing time in context B in both sham‐operated and injured mice with impaired neurogenesis when protocol 2 was used was not observed (g for sham‐operated mice, p = .0319 with learning day as the variable, p = .0176 with experimental group as the variable, p = .9205 for the interaction using repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; h for injured mice, p = .6211 with learning day as the variable, p = .6931 with experimental group as the variable, p = .5198 for the interaction using repeated measures two‐way ANOVA)
Immunohistochemistry
All mice were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane through the whole period of perfusion. The transcardiac perfusion was performed with 50 ml of 1x PBS, followed by 100 ml of 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in 1x PBS. Whole brains were then dissected and immersed in 4% PFA/1x PBS overnight for postfixation. Then, brains were embedded in 3% agarose/1x PBS and serial 50 μm sections were cut with a vibratome (VT1000S, Leica). All sections encompassing the hippocampus were collected sequentially in six‐well plates. Free‐floating sections were used for immunohistochemistry. For BrdU staining, all sections from a single well were washed with 1x PBS three times and rinsed with water. Then, sections were denatured with 1 N HCl for 45 min in 37°C water bath. After denaturation, sections were washed with 0.3% Triton X‐100/1x PBS for three times, 10 min each time. Then, sections were blocked with 5% normal donkey serum (017‐000‐121, Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories) in 0.3% Triton X‐100/1x PBS for 1 hr at room temperature. Rat anti‐BrdU antibody (1:500; Abcam Cat# ab6326
Quantification and microscopy
To quantify BrdU and BrdU/Prox1 cells, we used a traditional and well‐established method to account for oversampling as described before with modifications (Miles & Kernie, ; Yu et al., ). A Zeiss Imager M2 microscopy using Zeiss Neofluor 40x/1.3 oil lens was used to determine BrdU‐labeled cells and colocalization of BrdU and Prox1. Cell quantification was performed from the ipsilateral and contralateral sides of the dentate gyrus in injured and sham‐operated mice. We sampled a well of sections 50‐μm thick, coronally sectioned, and PFA‐fixed 300 μm apart. Sections from each well were stained with BrdU and Prox1 as free‐floating sections to allow equal antibody penetration between sections. Sections were mounted using Immunomount (Vectashield D, Vector Labs) with a refractive index of 1.495. Antibody penetration and signal intensity were found to be evenly distributed throughout the z‐axis in multiple sections from different mice. Each blade of the dentate gyrus was counted separately at 40x under Zeiss immersion oil 518F. Blinded quantification was performed on BrdU alone and BrdU/Prox1 fully colocalized cells in the granular layer of the dentate gyrus. By scanning serially through the z‐axis of each cell, a more precise discrimination of the colocalization of relevant cell markers was permissible. Since 1/6 of the total number of sections was counted, we multiplied our counts by 6 to obtain the number of cells per dentate gyrus.
Statistics
An unpaired two‐tailed t test was used to determine the significant decrease in the number of newborn neurons after valganciclovir chow was given. Whether mice were able to distinguish two similar contexts was determined by the discrimination index. The discrimination index was calculated with the following formula: (freezing time in A)/(freezing time in A + freezing time in B). Analyses of the discrimination index and freezing time in context A or B were done using repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Sidak's multiple comparisons. All the data were presented as mean ± SEM.
RESULTS
Valganciclovir inhibits neurogenesis in nestin‐HSV‐TK mice
We inducibly inhibited neurogenesis with previously generated and well‐characterized delta‐HSV‐TK transgenic mice (Blaiss et al., ; Yu et al., , ). Eight‐week old mice underwent either a sham operation or CCI (Figure a). Three days after surgery, BrdU was injected once per day for 3 consecutive days to label proliferating neural progenitors (Blaiss et al., ; Yu et al., , ). Starting from the day surgery was performed, mice were randomly assigned to be fed with either regular chow or chow mixed with valganciclovir until the experiment was concluded (Figure a). The discrimination learning task started 4 weeks after surgery, and three groups of mice were used for three different learning protocols. To ensure the efficiency of valganciclovir administration for inhibiting neurogenesis, immunofluorescent staining was performed to label BrdU and Prox1, a dentate gyrus‐specific neuronal marker, in brain sections collected from all mice fed with valganciclovir chow and samples were randomly chosen from each experimental group for quantification of newborn neurons represented by BrdU/Prox1‐labeled cells. Treatment with valganciclovir resulted in a significant inhibition of neurogenesis regardless of whether the mice underwent a sham or CCI procedure (Figure b–d). In sham‐operated mice, the reduction in the number of newborn neurons was roughly 75% on both sides of the dentate gyrus (Figure b, two‐tailed unpaired t test, p = .0011). In injured groups, the decrease was approximately 66% (Figure c and d, two‐tailed unpaired t test, p = .0011). Therefore, we concluded that HSV‐TK mice treated with valganciclovir caused a significant reduction in the number of newborn neurons in the dentate gyrus in both sham‐operated and injured mice.
Intact neurogenesis is not required for fear discrimination when the aversive stimulus is presented first
To investigate whether adult newborn neurons play a role in discriminating an aversive environment from a naïve one that shares similar cues, three protocols were used in contextual fear discrimination tasks. We first introduced context A to mice with an aversive foot shock at day 1 without exposure to the naïve context B (Figure a). Starting from day 2, context B was introduced to all experimental mice and context B was introduced before A in protocol 1 (Figure a). No significant difference was observed in sham‐operated mice without intact neurogenesis when the discrimination index was used as an indicator for distinguishing aversive from naïve contexts (Figure b, p = .0297 with learning day as the variable, p = .233 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .3209 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Sidak's multiple comparisons). The same phenomenon was also observed in injured mice even when neurogenesis was impaired (Figure c, p = .2790 with learning day as the variable, p = .6057 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .1610 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Sidak's multiple comparisons). When the freezing of mice in context A revealed by percentage of freezing time was examined in sham‐operated mice, once again no significant difference was observed in mice with or without intact neurogenesis (Figure d, p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .4295 with experimental group as the variable, and p < .0015 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Sidak's multiple comparisons). The same result was observed in injured mice (Figure e, p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .6639 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .0424 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Sidak's multiple comparisons). As expected, all experimental mice demonstrated similar freezing in context B regardless of treatment (Figure f for sham‐operated mice, p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .5722 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .1366 in the interaction; Figure g for injured mice, p = .0003 with learning day as the variable, p = .8902 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .2903 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA). Hence, all experimental mice demonstrated similar freezing times in contexts A and B in distinguishing the naïve context B from the aversive context A when protocol 1 was used.
Fear discrimination is impaired when neurogenesis is inhibited when the naïve stimulus is encountered first
In protocol 2, we reversed the sequence of A and B when compared to protocol 1 (Figure a), and found that the freezing times were quite different. In sham‐operated mice, those without intact neurogenesis showed a significant improvement in discrimination the first day when context B was introduced (Figure b, p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .1233 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .0610 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; *p < .05 in Sidak's post hoc analysis). However, mice with and without neurogenesis showed similar abilities in discrimination after day 2. In injured mice, an apparent improvement in discrimination was observed in mice with impaired neurogenesis when compared with injured mice with intact neurogenesis (Figure c, p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .1233 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .0610 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 in Sidak's post hoc analysis). To understand why an improved discrimination index was observed in mice with impaired neurogenesis, the freezing times in contexts A and B were examined. In sham‐operated mice, mice without intact neurogenesis spent less freezing time in both contexts A and B (Figure d and f, d for context A, p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .001 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .5647 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; *p < .05 in Sidak's post hoc analysis; f for context B, p = .0022 with learning day as the variable,, p = .0069 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .0287 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; *p < .05 in Sidak's post hoc analysis). A similar but more apparent outcome was observed in the injured groups (Figure e and f). In context A, injured mice with impaired neurogenesis demonstrated less freezing time (Figure e, p < .0001 with learning day as the variable, p = .0114 with experimental group as the variable, and p = .2650 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; *p < .05 in Sidak's post hoc analysis). A similar pattern of freezing time was observed in context B (Figure g, p = .0034 with learning day as the variable, p = .0128 with experimental group as the variable, and p < .0001 in the interaction in repeated measures two‐way ANOVA; **p < .01, ***p < .001 in Sidak's post hoc analysis). Hence, less neurogenesis resulted in a weaker association of aversive stimulus with context A and more activity in the naïve context B when protocol 2 was used.
Overtraining in the aversive context A eliminates the effect of impairing neurogenesis
To rule out the possibility that mice might not associate an aversive foot shock with context A when protocol 2 was used, we exposed mice in context A with an aversive foot shock for 5 consecutive days before context B was introduced. The naïve context B was introduced after exposure of context A on day 6, but the exposure sequence was similar to protocol 2 (Figure a). After a 5‐day training session, mice associated the aversive foot shock with context A successfully as indicated by similar percentage of freezing time in context A (Figure b, repeated measures two‐way ANOVA with Tukey's multiple comparisons; time effect, p < .0001; experimental groups, p = .2306). As expected, the discrimination index demonstrated that sham mice were able to distinguish context B from A whether neurogenesis was intact or not (Figure c, p = .0094 with learning day as the variable, p = .0207 with experimental group as the variable, p = .9760 for the interaction using repeated measures two‐way ANOVA). The ability in distinguishing contexts A and B in injured mice with or without intact neurogenesis was similar (Figure d, p = .0773 with learning day as the variable, p = .2725 with experimental group as the variable, p = .7326 for the interaction using repeated measures two‐way ANOVA). After examining the freezing time in both contexts, sham‐operated mice with or without intact neurogenesis spent similar freezing times in contexts A and B (Figure e and g). Moreover, similar freezing times were observed in injured mice whether they retained intact neurogenesis or not (Figure f and h). Therefore, the enforcement in association between aversive stimulus and corresponding context A diminished the phenomenon observed in mice with impaired neurogenesis when protocol 2 was used. Hence, we conclude that adult newborn neurons interfere with contextual discrimination in a protocol‐dependent manner.
DISCUSSION
Patients who recover from traumatic brain injury often suffer a variety of cognitive deficits, and treatments for these deficits remain supportive (Blennow et al., ; Hemphill, Dauth, Yu, Dabiri, & Parker, ; Yu, Washington, & Kernie, ). The recognition that injury‐induced neurogenesis occurs and appears to mediate some aspects of cognitive recovery, especially in the dentate gyrus in the hippocampus which is involved in learning and memory, raises the hope that manipulating neurogenesis might improve learning and memory after brain injury (Akers et al., ; Arruda‐Carvalho, Sakaguchi, Akers, Josselyn, & Frankland, ; Deng, Aimone, & Gage, ; Johnston, Shtrahman, Parylak, Goncalves, & Gage, ; Massa et al., ; Stone et al., ). Using mouse models to mimic traumatic brain injury, we previously demonstrated that injury‐induced neurogenesis is essential in spatial learning and memory after brain injury, but it does not appear to be required in contextual fear conditioning (Blaiss et al., ). It has been proposed that contextual fear conditioning is not a hippocampus‐dependent task and that the amygdala plays a critical role in mediating this particular kind of memory (Wehner & Radcliffe, ). Therefore, we employed the discrimination version of the contextual fear task, which is highly hippocampal dependent (Frankland, Cestari, Filipkowski, McDonald, & Silva, ; Wehner & Radcliffe, ) in order to determine the requirement of injury‐induced newborn neurons in successfully differentiating aversive and naïve contexts after TBI.
We observed that using a protocol where context B was introduced before the aversive context A at day 2 (protocol 1), mice were able to differentiate aversive and naïve contexts regardless of treatment. By using a different protocol where context A was introduced before B at day 2 (protocol 2), mice lacking neurogenesis unexpectedly retained higher discrimination indices regardless of whether they were injured or not. Moreover, both sham‐operated and injured mice lacking neurogenesis spent significantly less freezing time in both contexts A and B, though the observation was enhanced in the setting of CCI. This may be due to the increase in neurogenesis that occurs following injury compared with sham‐injured mice (Figure ), which supports the observation that more relative impairment of neurogenesis results in improved fear discrimination as demonstrated by a higher discrimination index in mice who are both injured and have impaired neurogenesis (Figure c). Extensive exposure to mice to the aversive foot shock in context A for 5 consecutive days (protocol 3) eliminated the impairment of fear discrimination memory observed with ablating neurogenesis seen in protocol 2 (Figure ).
Decreases in adult hippocampal neurogenesis impair pattern separation, whereas increases facilitate it (Clelland et al., ; Clemenson et al., ; Sahay et al., ; Tronel et al., ). However, our observations using various fear discrimination paradigms to test pattern separation are not consistent with this model. When we introduced the naïve context B before A in protocol 1, no significant difference was observed among experimental groups in the discrimination index. Based on data using this protocol alone, one may conclude that adult newborn neurons are not required in learning contextual discrimination. However, sham‐operated and injured mice with impaired neurogenesis spent less freezing time in both contexts A and B compared with mice with intact neurogenesis when context B was introduced after A at day 2. The reduction of freezing time in both contexts A and B in mice with impaired neurogenesis could result in an increase in the discrimination index, which suggests an improvement in contextual discrimination. Therefore, using the discrimination index alone to determine whether mice learn to distinguish aversive and naïve contexts might be misleading without further examining freezing time in the individual contexts of A and B. Furthermore, the performance of experimental mice varied according to the specific protocol employed. To our knowledge, this is the first report suggesting that variance of performance in contextual fear discrimination might be protocol dependent. Several studies raise similar concerns for contextual fear conditioning, although this task includes both hippocampal and amygdala‐dependent functions and its regulation appears more complicated than that for fear discrimination (Fujinaka et al., ; Huckleberry, Ferguson, & Drew, ). Hence, further investigation is needed to propose a standard protocol for contextual fear discrimination.
By performing extensive training, mice without intact neurogenesis associated the aversive stimulus with the corresponding context A successfully and were able to distinguish the naïve context B from the aversive context A during learning independent of injury. Interestingly, a previous study demonstrated that adult‐born neurons are necessary for extended contextual discrimination by employing a doxycycline‐dependent means of inducing overexpression of the proapoptotic protein, Bax, to trigger apoptosis in neural progenitors and thus inhibit neurogenesis (Tronel et al., ). Although they reported a lesser discrimination ratio in mice with impaired neurogenesis, details using a post hoc analysis were not reported and therefore, it is not clear if a significant difference existed between experimental groups along the 8‐day learning period. It is also possible that the two extra training days given in our study resulted in a more vivid association of the aversive stimulus with context A, which allowed mice to distinguish context B from A more easily whether or not their brains were injured or neurogenesis was intact.
Without intact neurogenesis, mice spent less freezing time in both contexts A and B when protocol 2 was used. The observed deficit was reversed when mice were trained to associate the aversive foot shock with context A successfully before introduction of the naïve context B. Therefore, the observed deficit in protocol 2 is due to a weak association of the foot shock with context A. However, this cannot explain what was observed when protocol 1 was used. Interestingly, the performance of mice, even controls, in contexts A and B varied in several publications according to various published protocols (Clemenson et al., ; Sahay et al., ; Tronel et al., ; Wu & Hen, ). The present study highlights the difficulty encountered in ascribing generalized phenomena such as fear discrimination in mice to particular aspects of either baseline or enhanced (by injury) adult hippocampal neurogenesis when the behavioral paradigms used are so exquisitely protocol dependent.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2017. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Introduction
Significant enhancement of neurogenesis is known to occur in response to a variety of brain insults such as traumatic brain injury. Previous studies have demonstrated that injury‐induced newborn neurons are required for hippocampus‐dependent spatial learning and memory tasks like the Morris water maze, but not in contextual fear conditioning that requires both the hippocampus and amygdala. Recently, the dentate gyrus, where adult hippocampal neurogenesis occurs, has been implicated in processing information to form specific memory under specific environmental stimuli in a process known as pattern separation.
Methods
To test whether injury‐induced newborn neurons facilitate pattern separation, hippocampus‐dependent contextual fear discrimination was performed using delta‐HSV‐TK transgenic mice, which can temporally inhibit injury‐induced neurogenesis under the control of ganciclovir.
Results
We observed that impaired neurogenesis enhanced the ability to distinguish aversive from naïve environments. In addition, this occurs most significantly following injury, but only in a context‐dependent manner whereby the sequence of introducing the naïve environment from the aversive one affected the performance differentially.
Conclusions
Temporal impairment of both baseline and injury‐induced adult neurogenesis enhances performance in fear discrimination in a context‐dependent manner.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer