It appears you don't have support to open PDFs in this web browser. To view this file, Open with your PDF reader
Abstract
The homeostatic need for sodium is one of the strongest motivational drives known in animals. Although the brain regions involved in the sensory detection of sodium levels have been mapped relatively well, data about the neural basis of the motivational properties of salt appetite, including a role for midbrain dopamine cells, have been inconclusive. Here, we employed a combination of fiber photometry, behavioral pharmacology and c-Fos immunohistochemistry to study the involvement of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system in salt appetite in rats. We observed that sodium deficiency affected the responses of dopaminergic midbrain neurons to salt tasting, suggesting that these neurons encode appetitive properties of sodium. We further observed a significant reduction in the consumption of salt after pharmacological inactivation of the nucleus accumbens (but not the medial prefrontal cortex), and microstructure analysis of licking behavior suggested that this was due to decreased motivation for, but not appreciation of salt. However, this was not dependent on dopaminergic neurotransmission in that area, as infusion of a dopamine receptor antagonist into the nucleus accumbens did not alter salt appetite. We conclude that the nucleus accumbens, but not medial prefrontal cortex, is important for the behavioral expression of salt appetite by mediating its motivational component, but that the switch in salt appreciation after sodium depletion, although detected by midbrain dopamine neurons, must arise from other areas.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details


1 University Medical Center Utrecht, Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, Department of Translational Neuroscience, Utrecht, The Netherlands (GRID:grid.7692.a) (ISNI:0000000090126352); Utrecht University, Department of Animals in Science and Society, Division of Behavioural Neuroscience, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Utrecht, The Netherlands (GRID:grid.5477.1) (ISNI:0000000120346234)
2 University Medical Center Utrecht, Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, Department of Translational Neuroscience, Utrecht, The Netherlands (GRID:grid.7692.a) (ISNI:0000000090126352)
3 Utrecht University, Department of Animals in Science and Society, Division of Behavioural Neuroscience, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Utrecht, The Netherlands (GRID:grid.5477.1) (ISNI:0000000120346234)
4 University Medical Center Utrecht, Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, Department of Translational Neuroscience, Utrecht, The Netherlands (GRID:grid.7692.a) (ISNI:0000000090126352); Sahlgrenska Academy at the University of Gothenburg, Institute of Physiology and Neuroscience, Gothenburg, Sweden (GRID:grid.8761.8) (ISNI:0000 0000 9919 9582)