Financial incentives are a popular policy tool for promoting private land conservation. In the United States, federal conservation programs can be traced to the Dust Bowl in the 1930s (Claassen et al. ). Their purview has expanded from soil conservation to a wide range of conservation practices and programs, such as wildlife habitat conservation (Ciuzio et al. ). Similarly, Australia and the United Kingdom also have a long history of voluntary incentive programs (Kamal et al. ). As Kamal et al. () describe, these incentive programs utilize both outright purchasing of private land and conservation easements. In this article. we focus on a third strategy that does not have the same level of conservation security as the first two categories: financial incentive programs and contracts that allow landowners to voluntarily participate in conservation activities while maintaining their ownership of the land (Kamal et al. ). These programs generally have a fixed term (e.g., 1–20 years) (Kuhfuss et al. ), although the contracts may be extended, depending on factors such as landowner preference, funding availability, and program policies. Financial incentives may be monetary (e.g., cash payments), nonmonetary (e.g., tax credits, technical assistance), or cost share agreements with landowners (Doremus ). This approach has been lauded as an effective way to encourage voluntary behavior change by landowners, who are often resistant to government intervention on their lands through regulation (Doremus ) or simply struggling to make ends meet on their lands (Swann & Richards ). Conservation incentive programs create substantial environmental benefits, such as the production of critical wildlife habitat on private lands (Ciuzio et al. ; DiGaudio et al. ).
Public agricultural institutions, state wildlife and natural resources agencies, nongovernmental organizations, and collaborations of agencies and organizations across the globe invest large sums in implementing voluntary financial incentive programs. While no bundled expenditure total is available on a global scale, the magnitude of investment is easily apparent on a discrete basis. For example, United States Department of Agriculture's Natural Resource Conservation Service (NRCS) conservation program expenditures under the current U.S. Farm Bill (the Agricultural Act of 2014) were $5.7 billion in 2015 (ERS ). Expenditures on agri‐environment measures in the European Union amounted to 20 billion EUR from 2007 to 2013 (European Commission ).
Given the substantial expenditure globally, an increasing emphasis is being made on strategically prioritizing investments. Recent changes in U.S. Farm Bill programs to become more regional and highly focused on critical conservation outcomes (e.g., Working Lands for Wildlife, Regional Conservation Partnership Program (RCPP)) reflect this shift (Ciuzio et al. ; Reimer ). For example, the RCPP creates regional conservation initiatives that are up to 5 years in length and generates assistance from multiple partners using fund‐matching and a competitive application process (Reimer ). Strategic investment decisions, such as which RCPP initiative to fund, have been primarily defined biologically, rather than socially. Programs tend to be justified based upon the conservation status of a species or another resource that will benefit from conservation practices, and they are typically evaluated based on biophysical outcomes (e.g., acres treated, bird response). Further, while programs are rarely designed in terms of targeted social outcomes (e.g., landowner conservation stewardship attitude change, durability of landowner behavior change), positive social outcomes appear to be assumed (Burton et al. ; Race & Curtis ).
Landowner adoption of conservation practices (Prokopy et al. ; Baumgart‐Getz et al. ) and participation in voluntary incentive programs (e.g., Sorice et al. ; Reimer & Prokopy ) have been well studied. However, research on landowners’ continued use of conservation practices through time is lacking, which Reimer et al. () highlight as problematic given that most conservation gains rely on sustained behavior changes. In particular, little empirical research has focused on whether and why landowners continue their conservation behaviors after short‐term financial incentive payments end (Swann & Richards ). We term this “persistence,” as it is a continuation of a course of action or behavior (see Figure ). The term “persistence” has also been used in literature outside of landowner research, such as on household energy conservation behavior related to behaviors continuing after an intervention has ended (Frey & Rogers ). Existing research on landowner continuation of agricultural innovations in general has used the terms continuance, maintenance, or confirmation (e.g., Dunn et al. ), but these terms do not distinguish the important context of postprogram behavior. The reverse behavior, when a landowner returns to the precontract management behavior s/he was previously using (i.e., returning a retired field to crop production, as in the case of the Conservation Reserve Program (CRP)), has been termed disadoption (Claassen & Ribaudo ), discontinuance (Rogers ), or reversion (Kuhfuss et al. ). While it is often assumed that satisfied landowners progress from enrollment to reenrollment (if permitted) to persistence (Figure ), what remains largely unknown is what happens when payments stop: persistence or reversion? And why?
Steps of landowner participation in incentive programs—the model for persistence.
Given the need for attention to persistence of landowner conservation activities, the purpose of this article is to explore (1) the rationale of persistence as a goal for conservation incentive programs; (2) what information exists to substantiate the assumption that persistence happens postprogram when payments end, particularly in the cases of behaviors that must be continued on a regular basis; (3) what is known about factors that predict persistence (or not); and (4) the policy and program implications of this review, as well as research needs.
Conservation incentive programs around the world appear to have ambitions for landowner activities extending beyond payment contracts. Programs and policies have aimed to develop stewardship ethics and enduring attitude change, with long‐term management changes implied as a result. In Australia, for example, conservation programs such as the National Landcare Program are based on the idea that investments will lead to long‐term changes in landowner management practices (Race & Curtis ). Specifically, Curtis & De Lacy () concluded that the National Landcare Program was meant to achieve sustainability goals, in part, by developing a stewardship ethic among participants. In the European Union, cultural changes, such as proenvironment attitude shifts, are also an implicit goal of voluntary agri‐environmental programs (Burton et al. ). Similarly, changing landowner stewardship ethics and attitudes are considered important goals for the NRCS in the United States. Language used by the former NRCS chief (White ), specific conservation programs (e.g., Florida NRCS ), and the agency's National Planning Procedures Handbook (NRCS ) provide evidence to support this claim. Changes in landowner attitudes toward conservation and land stewardship may have immediate benefits for management effectiveness under contract, but we expect the primary agency motivation is more likely to affect long‐term behavioral changes.
While official positions on landowner persistence are mostly implicit, persistence could be an important consideration for conservation incentive programs to achieve conservation goals while most strategically using funds. It has been argued that short‐term conservation incentive programs are “temporary bribes, shallow in operation, and transitory in effect” (Morris & Potter ) unless they create lasting changes in participant attitudes and behaviors (Burton et al. ). If conservation funding is limited, landowner persistence becomes crucial to obtaining long‐term ecological benefits through voluntary incentive programs. The most direct benefit of persistence outcomes is cost‐effective conservation. This thinking is reflected in a U.S. Department of Agriculture Economic Research Service report:
The ultimate goal of transition incentive payments, however, is to encourage farmers to adopt conservation management practices for the long run. When a short‐term payment leverages a long‐term change in practices, the environmental benefits can be much larger than if the practice is dropped when the payments end. (Claassen et al. )
While Claassen et al. () were referring to farming management practices that would be profitable in the long term, the cost–benefit logic is the same for other conservation practices that use short‐term contracts.
Persistence is particularly important in cases where a limited pool of landowners are available for enrollment because a small number of landowners meet the program requirements, own appropriate land for the conservation practices, or express interest in enrolling in a program. If saturation of potential program participants occurs and there is a limit on reenrollment, then it is even more important that persistence occurs. If no persistence occurred, there would be few new landowners to enroll, while past enrollees would no longer be conducting the conservation behavior, having reverted to past practices. In this case, conservation behavior could only be achieved again through enrollment in a different program, if the pool of landowners expanded (e.g., fewer restrictions), or if landowners changed their mind and persisted with the behavior. An example of this saturation likely occurred in the NRCS Waterbird Habitat Enhancement Program in northern California (Figure ). The number of producers enrolled in the program peaked in 2014, then decreased each year as contracts were phased out of the program and landowners were unable to reenroll due to contract time limits. A follow‐up program was created under an RCPP; about half the previous participants reenrolled. Persistence remains a concern, however, as these RCPP contracts are still only for a maximum of 3 years.
Number of producers enrolled each year in the NRCS Waterbird Habitat Enhancement Program in northern California (MBCP ). Most producers enrolled for the maximum of 3 years. The sharp decrease in the number of enrolled producers is likely a result of saturation of willing landowners in the program area.
For conservation programs that provide wildlife habitat using short‐term incentive funding, persistence could be highly important for wildlife conservation goals. Newly created or appropriately managed habitat may revert without landowner behavioral persistence. The effects of this unstable habitat provision would depend upon the natural disturbance regime of the habitat involved and the natural history of the wildlife species targeted. If the habitat is dynamic by nature, then species that rely on it likely evolved to move with the habitat; short‐term contracts changing location on the landscape likely mimics how the habitat originally moved. Waterbirds using postharvest rice fields provide a good example of this scenario since water is naturally patchy on the landscape depending on precipitation (Elphick ). Many birds (e.g., waterfowl, shorebirds; Farmer & Parent ; Ackerman et al. ) and some fish (e.g., Sacramento splittail [Pogonichthys macrolepidotus]; Sommer et al. ) are mobile and can take advantage of newly formed habitat. The above‐mentioned NRCS Waterbird Habitat Enhancement Program incentivized the flooding of rice fields which provided habitat for many waterbirds using this spatially dynamic flooded habitat. For this type of program, if persistence does not occur and reenrollment is not an option, conservation gains would be ephemeral regardless of whether the system is naturally dynamic. If the system is less dynamic or if its dynamism is on longer time scales (e.g., fire regimes), the relatively short length of conservation contracts may not provide enough time to meet specific conservation goals. In addition, if target species are not highly mobile (e.g., small, terrestrial species such as giant garter snake [Thamnophis gigas; Halstead et al. ]) and appropriate connectivity to existing habitat does not exist, these species may have a harder time taking advantage of short‐term habitat. Even worse, short‐term habitat could become an ecological trap (Hale & Swearer ) if individuals settle in habitat that disappears as land reverts, rather than persists, when payments end.
Consistent, renewable funding—at program and landowner contract levels—could secure long‐term environmental benefits and address issues such as enrollment pool saturation and habitat stability. Yet, this is not always a viable solution and is not the current direction of strategic conservation efforts, at least in the United States. For example, RCPP initiatives are limited to 5 years of funding with no option for an individual initiative to be renewed in its current form. Thus, facilitation of persistence of landowner conservation behaviors beyond the life of payments becomes even more critical.
A recent literature review by Swann & Richards () highlighted the scarcity of research on landowners’ conservation behavior postincentive payments. The authors reviewed behavioral persistence, which they termed “long‐term behavior change,” following conservation incentive programs in developed countries. The literature review analyzed 42 papers from peer‐reviewed journals and gray literature pertaining to conservation behavior persistence. Yet, based on our analysis of these papers, only five papers attempted to explain persistence intentions/outcomes with empirical evidence, limiting the utility of this meta‐analysis. We found three additional empirical studies of persistence through our literature search from August to October 2016 using Web of Science and Google Scholar. Two sets of search phrases were used “(‘conservation program’ OR ‘incentive program’ OR ‘agri‐environment* program’) AND (disadopt* OR confirmation OR reversion OR maintenance OR persist*)” and “(landowner OR farmer OR ‘private land’) AND (‘post contract’ OR ‘after contract’ OR ‘long term’) AND (behavior OR practice OR management).” Articles were also forward and backward citation searched within those databases. Geographically, these eight total empirical studies were located in the United States (5), Australia (1), Europe (1), and Colombia (1) (Table ).
Empirical studies focused on behavioral persistence following conservation incentive programsStudy authors | Location | Program | Behavioral intention |
Skaggs et al. () | New Mexico, United States | Conservation Reserve Program | Farmland retirement |
Johnson et al. () | Texas, High Plans Region, United States | Conservation Reserve Program | Farmland retirement |
Roberts & Lubowski () | Contiguous United States | Conservation Reserve Program | Farmland retirement |
Jackson‐Smith et al. () | Little Bear River watershed, Northern Utah, United States | Little Bear River Watershed Project | *Water quality best management practices |
Ramsdell et al. () | Nebraska, United States | Rocky Mountain Bird Observatory mountain plover program | Practices related to plover nest protection, marking |
Kuhfuss et al. () | France | Mesures Agro‐Environnementales territorialisees | Range of agri‐environmental practices |
Race & Curtis () | Southeast Australia | Multiple natural resource management programs | Natural resource management practices |
Hayes () | Colombia | Payment for Ecosystem Services Silvopastoral program | Silvopastoral management practices |
*This study measured actual landowner behavior instead of behavioral intention.
Seven of these eight empirical persistence studies focused on behavioral intentions, rather than documenting actual behavioral persistence (Table ). This is potentially problematic because stated intentions, while informative, do not fully predict human behavior (Webb & Sheeran ). The reliability of intentions to causally determine behavior is particularly weak in contexts that involve social reactions, low behavioral control, and the potential for habit‐forming (Webb & Sheeran ), all of which may be relevant for land management behaviors. These studies of landowner intentions following conservation program contracts show highly variable persistence rates. Landowner intentions to persist with conservation practices following incentive programs have been as low as 31% (Johnson et al. ) or as high as about 85% (Jackson‐Smith et al. ; Hayes ).
In the United States, persistence research has been primarily concentrated on quantifying reversion rates after CRP participation, with less attention on understanding why landowners persist or revert. A few publications surveyed landowner intentions to return CRP land to crop production after program exit, or modeled this same land use decision (for example, Skaggs et al. ; Johnson et al. ; Roberts & Lubowski ). While these studies are valuable, a focus on land retirement programs has limitations because research suggests conservation practice type has an influence on persistence outcomes. Jackson‐Smith et al. () found an actual persistence rate of about 83% for structural, planting, clearing, and leveling agricultural best management practices, in contrast with continuous management practices that were continued by less than half of the landowners once payments ended. Hayes () found similar results in a Colombian program designed to decrease deforestation from agricultural expansion by improving pasture quality and promoting sustainable pasture management. After program participation, 85% of participants intended to continue using fences created from live trees, while only 41% intended to continue using pasture fertilizers.
While these few studies on landowner persistence intentions offer some initial insights, in order to fully understand, predict, and influence persistence likelihoods it is critical to better understand the social mechanisms underlying persistence outcomes. Social science theory‐based explanations for behavioral persistence can help us understand why conservation behaviors may or may not persist beyond the life of incentive program payments. We have identified five theory‐based pathways by which landowners’ conservation behaviors could be expected to persist or revert when conservation incentive payments end. Explanatory frameworks for behavioral persistence have been recently developed in other research contexts, including home energy use (Frey & Rogers ) and public health (Kwasnicka et al. ). Our proposed pathways (Table ) are informed by these frameworks and by the extensive literature on landowner behavior, including the limited research on landowner persistence. While presented as five independent pathways, these pathways likely covary and influence/interact with each other.
Proposed pathways for understanding landowner persistence behavior and example studiesLandowner persistence pathway | Description | Related landowner studies |
Landowner cognitions | Landowners’ attitudes toward, or perceptions of, the conservation behavior support continued use | Hayes (); Race & Curtis (); Kuhfuss et al. () |
Sustaining motivations | Motivations for performing a conservation behavior endure | Skaggs et al. (); Johnson et al. (); Ramsdell et al. () |
Habit forming | Performance of conservation behaviors becomes habitual | Sheeder & Lynne () |
Resources | Sufficient resources to maintain conservation behavior and/or overcome opportunity costs are available | Skaggs et al. (); Roberts & Lubowski (); Kuhfuss et al. () |
Social influence | Social norms and cultures support conservation behaviors | Burton et al. (); Kuhfuss et al. () |
Landowner cognitions including environmental attitudes (Baumgart‐Getz et al. ), attitudes toward specific practices (e.g., Dayer et al. ), and an individual's perception of their ability to perform a given behavior (behavior control; Fishbein & Ajzen ) (e.g., Lynne et al. ) can influence landowner adoption of conservation practices. Cognitions such as these can also be changed by specific interventions (Frey & Rogers ), or through repeated performance of a behavior (Kwasnicka et al. ). If incentive program participation generated positive changes in landowner cognitions, persistence outcomes postincentive payments would be more likely.
Landowner attitudes and attitude changes have long been considered important for behavioral outcomes before and after conservation programs (Morris & Potter ; Wilson & Hart ). While attitudes can be difficult to change, research in other settings has shown that behavior change can lead to attitude change (Lokhorst et al. ). Yet, empirical evidence of landowner attitude change from conservation program participation has been mixed (Burton & Paragahawewa ). Previous research has focused on general attitudes toward conservation or the environment (Burton & Paragahawewa ), overlooking the possibility of attitude change toward specific conservation practices. Attitudes toward specific practices would be expected to have a closer causal influence on persistence outcomes, and deserve further scrutiny in this context. Positive program outcomes may influence landowner attitudes toward conservation practices and increase behavior persistence. For example, French farmers that perceived a higher quality of life during participation in an agri‐environmental program were more likely to state their intention to persist with conservation behaviors after payments ended (Kuhfuss et al. ). Similarly, Race & Curtis () suggested that Australian landowners were more likely to maintain natural resource management behaviors they perceived to be successful.
Landowner perceptions of behavioral control have been shown to affect conservation practice use. For example, landowners with a greater sense of control over the process of conservation practice adoption are more likely to adopt those practices (Lynne et al. ). Perceptions of behavioral control are likely important after conservation programs as well, with landowners who feel more in control and capable of implementing conservation practices more likely to do so following an incentive program. Additionally, there is some evidence that incentive programs can increase landowner perceptions of behavioral control over specific conservation practices (Hayes ), which could facilitate persistence outcomes.
Changes in landowner cognitions can also be negative and counter persistence. For example, Hayes () found that 89% of new conservation program participants thought the program would greatly improve their land, compared to only 47% among landowners who had completed the program. Negative experiences with a conservation behavior or program could cause a landowner to feel the behavior was not as desirable or controllable as initially thought (Kwasnicka et al. ). This reevaluation process could explain Kuhfuss et al.’s () finding that French farmers who had experienced technical difficulties during conservation practice implementation had 50% lower persistence intentions.
Landowner motivations, such as enjoyment of a behavior or satisfaction with behavioral outcomes, could provide a basis for behavioral persistence if the motivations endure beyond incentive payments (de Snoo et al. ; Kwasnicka et al. ). Landowner motivations to perform conservation behaviors have been broadly categorized as motivation to receive external rewards such as financial incentives (extrinsic motivation) or for internal satisfaction (intrinsic motivation). Intrinsic motivation is hypothesized to be more durable and more likely to result in persistence effects (Ryan & Deci ; Kwasnicka et al. ), although not all landowners have intrinsic motivation. Extrinisic rewards that may endure beyond financial incentives include spillover benefits from conservation practices, such as enhanced recreation opportunities. For example, Baumgart‐Getz () found that farmers motivated by hunting opportunities were more likely to maintain fallow field best management practices.
Landowner motivations have been significantly correlated with landowner postprogram intentions. Skaggs et al. () found farmers who were motivated to stop erosion on their property were more likely to persist with land retirement following CRP contracts. In another study of post‐CRP decision‐making, farmers who considered the financial value of the crop base in their decisions were more likely to revert to cropping after CRP contracts (Johnson et al. ). Finally, Ramsdell et al. () found landowner self‐determined motivation (a composite of intrinsic motivation and some types of extrinsic motivation) was positively related with farmer intentions to persist with mountain plover conservation practices.
Like the landowner cognitions discussed previously, participation in conservation programs could affect landowner motivations (de Snoo et al. ). When landowners receive financial incentives to conduct conservation behaviors, this may disrupt and “crowd out” existing internal motivation to perform that behavior (Rode et al. ). If “crowding out” occurred for a landowner with intrinsic motivation, persistence would be less likely to result. In contrast, under some conditions, financial incentives may cause the opposite effect and “crowd in” intrinsic motivation (Rode et al. ). “Crowding in” could be expected to increase intrinsic motivation and persistence outcomes regardless of prior motivation. Unfortunately, limited empirical evidence of crowding effects precludes conclusions about specific conditions in which crowding would influence persistence outcomes (Rode et al. ).
Research on public health and home energy conservation behaviors have proposed habits as another mechanism for persistence effects (Frey & Rogers ; Kwasnicka et al. ). Habits are defined as “an automated tendency to repeat a specific behavioral response,” such as turning off lights when you leave a room (Frey & Rogers ). Behaviors that are repeated frequently in the same environmental context are best suited for habit formation (Frey & Rogers ). Habits have been shown to exert a greater influence when behaviors are performed daily or weekly, while they are less important for annual behaviors (Ouelette & Wood ). Habits are relevant for a wide range of environmental behaviors including transportation use, waste reduction behavior, and home energy conservation behavior (Klockner ).
One commonly used indicator for habit strength is an individual's past behavior (see Fishbein & Ajzen ). Using a similar definition, Sheeder & Lynne () found that landowners who had implemented conservation tillage in the past were more likely to adopt the behavior again, and concluded habits were responsible for this effect. Yet, this assumption that past behavior equates with habit formation is problematic as past behavior could operate in other ways (e.g., changing attitudes). The role of habits in landowner behavior has not been well studied. We expect that habits may particularly influence frequently repeated and simple conservation actions such as mowing fields or cutting shrubs.
Landowners may require resources such as time, capital, labor, knowledge, and equipment to continue conservation behaviors after incentive programs end. Resource needs are context‐dependent but, in general, landowners with sufficient resources to maintain behaviors will be more likely to demonstrate behavioral persistence following conservation programs (Kwasnicka et al. ). While financial interests are not the primary consideration for every landowner, the financial costs and benefits of conservation practices remain a significant factor in landowner decision‐making (e.g., Kuhfuss et al. ; Dunn et al. ). This relationship suggests persistence will be a challenge when payments end for landowners below a certain and unknown level of financial means.
Some conservation practices (e.g., forest management through timber harvest) may yield financial benefits for a landowner in addition to incentive payments. Practices that directly benefit a landowner and are either inexpensive or otherwise profitable to continue would thereby be better suited for persistence. For example, Kuhfuss et al. () found that when incentivized practices had generated a better sale value for farm products (e.g., ecofriendly crops) farmer persistence intentions were higher. Incentive payments would primarily help landowners overcome initial investment costs required to transition to these new practices (Frey & Rogers ).
Conservation behaviors can also diverge from a landowner's financial interests, such as by reducing financially driven land use for habitat protection. Persistence could be discouraged if conservation practices were too costly for landowners to maintain or if opportunity costs of continuing conservation behaviors were high. Opportunity costs are certainly relevant for land retirement programs such as CRP, where there is evidence farmers reverted CRP parcels to conventional production when commodity prices for corn peaked around 2012 (Wright & Wimberly ). Higher farmland quality (Roberts & Lubowski ) and larger enrolled areas (Skaggs et al. ) have also been significantly related to farmer intentions to revert to cropping after land retirement programs. Other variables that may affect opportunity costs could include enrolled cover type (e.g., trees/wildlife habitat or grasses/legumes; Roberts & Lubowski ), and property location.
Landowners make management decisions in social contexts that may or may not support persistence of conservation practices. A landowner's decision‐making may be influenced by their beliefs about what other landowners are doing (descriptive norms) and what behaviors are socially approved (injunctive norms) (Fishbein & Ajzen ). These two types of social norms are tied to the specific community and culture in which a landowner is embedded. Social norms can have important impacts on persistence outcomes; Kuhfuss et al. () found that farmer intentions to persist following a conservation incentive program were affected by information about other participants’ persistence intentions. Additionally, landowners who perceived social acknowledgement of their conservation behaviors were more likely to state an intention to persist after the program (Kuhfuss et al. ). Social contexts are so critical that some authors have argued that conservation programs and practices must be adapted for existing norms and cultural frameworks (Burton et al. ).
Social norms are particularly relevant when conservation practices produce highly visible aesthetic consequences that can be judged by a landowner and their community. In these contexts, the persistence suitability of a conservation practice may depend on whether the resulting appearance of managed land is socially supported (Burton & Paragahawewa ; Riley ). For example, in a farming community that values visible outcomes of skilled behavior, stonewall construction and meadow habitat management, are likely better suited for persistence than unmanaged habitat reserves (Riley ).
It is plausible that cultural preferences and norms may change through conservation program exposure (de Snoo et al. ). After a 10‐year period of incentive program participation, Riley () found evidence for an alignment between farmer concepts of good farming and environmentally friendly farming practices. Conservation incentive programs that work with social norms and incorporate norm‐based messaging may more effectively achieve persistence outcomes. Yet, research indicates that norms are often less predictive of landowner behavior, compared to other psychological variables such as attitudes and perceived behavior control (Dayer et al. ).
Despite the importance of landowner behavior after voluntary conservation incentive program payments end, there is a scarcity of research on behavioral persistence. While agencies worldwide emphasize the development of a stewardship ethic or attitude change as positive social benefits of their programs, we identified a much richer set of five pathways by which conservation behavior could persist when incentive payments end. Notably, the evidence is mixed and often context or practice‐specific, limiting conclusions about whether persistence should be expected or not. Basing any policy or programmatic decisions on an assumption that incentive program participation will lead to behavioral persistence when payments end appears problematic. This is particularly the case for management practices requiring costly and frequent landowner behaviors, as opposed to structural investments that are one‐time costs. Based on our pathways, we believe practices with the following characteristics may be more likely to lead to persistence after payments end:
Similarly, persistence may be more likely with certain types of landowners. Particularly, those with financial resources to implement conservation practices without funding, those with intrinsic motivations, and those more (or less, depending on the context) driven by norms.
Given the above insights and others from our review, we offer the following recommendations for incentive program design and policies:
It is also apparent that social science research on landowner behavior persistence is essential. Priorities for such research include:
Such research priorities might be incorporated into monitoring and evaluation program priorities, such as the Conservation Effects Assessment Program (CEAP), a multiagency effort of the United States Department of Agriculture. Created in 2004, CEAP is charged with quantifying environmental effects of conservation practices and programs to better guide conservation policy (Briske et al. ). While CEAP has documented substantial environmental gains resulting from NRCS private lands conservation programs (e.g., Tomer et al. ), the long‐term fate of many environmental benefits derived from short‐term private land conservation programs depends on landowner decision‐making after contracts expire. Thus, it would be worthwhile for this program to invest in understanding landowner conservation behavior in general, and persistence specifically. In order to ensure this research is possible, legal and administrative barriers (such as the 2008 Farm Bill's elimination of access to parcel‐level data for enrolled lands) preventing social scientists from accessing conservation program participant information must be addressed (Rissman et al. ).
From a conservation perspective, landowner conservation behavior persistence after incentive program payments end is desirable, providing for current and future conservation outcomes from current investments of taxpayer dollars. Yet, persistence cannot be assumed to be the outcome. Research to enhance understanding of the extent of behavioral persistence and its drivers must be prioritized, followed by strategic programs and policies that incorporate these insights.
We thank Ronald B. Meyers for his thoughts at the initiation of this project and his review of an early version of this manuscript, as well as two anonymous reviewers and Editorial Board member Lynn Dicks. Tara Craig designed the graphic for Figure 1. Support was provided by the S.D. Bechtel, Jr. Foundation and the Virginia Tech Open Access Subvention Fund.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2018. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Voluntary incentive programs are a keystone policy tool for increasing private landowner conservation behavior. Although landowner participation in conservation incentive programs is well studied, limited empirical research has focused on whether and why landowners continue to conduct conservation practices on their land after payments end, which we term persistence. The assumption is that a landowner who participates in an incentive program will likely continue the conservation practice after the payments end. This assumption fits with conservation policies that limit the number of years or times a landowner can receive payments for a given practice. If persistence occurs, it would provide cost‐effective outcomes from conservation funding investments. However, there is little published information to support persistence. Based on the narrow body of research on persistence of landowner conservation behavior, as well as persistence research in other fields, we identified five pathways that may support persistence outcomes and insights for when persistence could be expected. We then provide recommendations for policy, practice, and research. With billions of dollars invested annually in programs to incentivize landowners to take conservation action, an empirical examination of landowner conservation behavior persistence is sorely needed for shaping more effective incentive programs and policies.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
1 Department of Fish & Wildlife Conservation, College of Natural Resources and Environment, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, VA, USA
2 Point Blue Conservation Science, Petaluma, CA, USA