It appears you don't have support to open PDFs in this web browser. To view this file, Open with your PDF reader
Abstract
Neural activity is known to oscillate within discrete frequency bands and the synchronization between these rhythms is hypothesized to underlie information integration in the brain. Since strict synchronization is only possible for harmonic frequencies, a recent theory proposes that the interaction between different brain rhythms is facilitated by transient harmonic frequency arrangements. In this line, it has been recently shown that the transient occurrence of 2:1 harmonic cross-frequency relationships between alpha and theta rhythms (i.e. falpha ≈ 12 Hz; ftheta ≈ 6 Hz) is enhanced during effortful cognition. In this study, we tested whether achieving a state of ‘mental emptiness’ during meditation is accompanied by a relative decrease in the occurrence of 2:1 harmonic cross-frequency relationships between alpha and theta rhythms. Continuous EEG recordings (19 electrodes) were obtained from 43 highly experienced meditators during meditation practice, rest and an arithmetic task. We show that the occurrence of transient alpha:theta 2:1 harmonic relationships increased linearly from a meditative to an active cognitive processing state (i.e. meditation < rest < arithmetic task). It is argued that transient EEG cross-frequency arrangements that prevent alpha:theta cross-frequency coupling could facilitate the experience of ‘mental emptiness’ by avoiding the interaction between the memory and executive components of cognition.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details


1 University of Leuven, KU Leuven, Belgium, Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, Research Group for Neurorehabilitation, Leuven, Belgium (GRID:grid.5596.f) (ISNI:0000 0001 0668 7884)
2 University Hospital of Psychiatry, The KEY Institute for Brain-Mind Research, Zurich, Switzerland (GRID:grid.412004.3) (ISNI:0000 0004 0478 9977)
3 University Hospital of Psychiatry, The KEY Institute for Brain-Mind Research, Zurich, Switzerland (GRID:grid.412004.3) (ISNI:0000 0004 0478 9977); University of Zurich, Institute of Pharmacology and Toxicology, Zurich, Switzerland (GRID:grid.7400.3) (ISNI:0000 0004 1937 0650); University of Zurich and ETH Zurich, Neuroscience Center Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland (GRID:grid.7400.3) (ISNI:0000 0004 1937 0650); University of Zurich, Zurich Center for Interdisciplinary Sleep Research, Zurich, Switzerland (GRID:grid.7400.3) (ISNI:0000 0004 1937 0650)
4 Kyoto University Graduate School of Medicine, Sakyo-ku, Department of Psychiatry, Kyoto, Japan (GRID:grid.258799.8) (ISNI:0000 0004 0372 2033)