It appears you don't have support to open PDFs in this web browser. To view this file, Open with your PDF reader
Abstract
Clinical evidence suggests that mindfulness meditation reduces anxiety, depression, and stress, and improves emotion regulation due to modulation of activity in neural substrates linked to the regulation of emotions and social preferences. However, less was known about whether mindfulness meditation might alter pro-social behavior. Here we examined whether mindfulness meditation activates human altruism, a component of social cooperation. Using a simple donation game, which is a real-world version of the Dictator’s Game, we randomly assigned 326 subjects to a mindfulness meditation online session or control and measured their willingness to donate a portion of their payment for participation as a charitable donation. Subjects who underwent the meditation treatment donated at a 2.61 times higher rate than the control (p = 0.005), after controlling for socio-demographics. We also found a larger treatment effect of meditation among those who did not go to college (p < 0.001) and those who were under 25 years of age (p < 0.001), with both subject groups contributing virtually nothing in the control condition. Our results imply high context modularity of human altruism and the development of intervention approaches including mindfulness meditation to increase social cooperation, especially among subjects with low baseline willingness to contribute.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details


1 College of Letters & Sciences, UC Berkeley, Berkeley, United States (GRID:grid.47840.3f) (ISNI:0000 0001 2181 7878); Los Angeles Center for Enriched Studies, Los Angeles, United States (GRID:grid.47840.3f)
2 Yale University, Human Nature Lab, Yale Institute for Network Science, New Haven, United States (GRID:grid.47100.32) (ISNI:0000000419368710)
3 Mindful Awareness Research Center and Cousins Center for Psychoneuroimmunology, Jane and Terry Semel Institute for Neuroscience at UCLA, and Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, United States (GRID:grid.19006.3e) (ISNI:0000 0000 9632 6718)
4 Yale University, Human Nature Lab, Yale Institute for Network Science, New Haven, United States (GRID:grid.47100.32) (ISNI:0000000419368710); Departments of Sociology, Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Medicine, Statistics and Data Science, and Biomedical Engineering, Yale Institute for Network Science, New Haven, United States (GRID:grid.47100.32)
5 Department of Epidemiology, UCLA Fielding School of Public Health, Los Angeles, United States (GRID:grid.19006.3e) (ISNI:0000 0000 9632 6718)