This work is licensed under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
1. Introduction
It is well known that training both limbs facilitates performance through intermanual transfer from one limb to the other. For example, the training of a specific skill with one hand can improve the performance of the other hand [1–3]. These well documented transfer effects, originally handled under the term “cross-education,” have been described for a range of fine motor skills [1, 2], sport-specific skills (e.g., [4, 5]), and muscle strength transfer [6, 7].
Several models have been put forward to explain empirical observations of intermanual transfer effects. Those theories are primarily based on studies that investigated transfer effects by the use of behavioral, neuroimaging, or brain modulation methods [6–11]. According to a recent review by Ruddy and Carson (2013) [11], two different theoretical models can be distinguished: the bilateral access (also known as the callosal access) model and the cross-activation model. The bilateral access model supports the idea that motor engrams, evolved after unilateral training in the dominant hemisphere, can be accessed by the opposite hemisphere via the corpus callosum, which leads to increased task performance of the contralateral limb [12]. The cross-activation model is supported by observations that unilateral motor executions evoke increased neuronal excitability of both the contra- and ipsilateral motor cortices, leading to neural plasticity in both hemispheres (cf. [8]). However, the underlying neurophysiology of bilateral transfer effects remains unclear. One possible mechanism was summarized from a recent review on neuroimaging studies in which it is argued that the mirror neural system (MNS) could be involved during bilateral transfer [7]. The MNS has been identified as the neuroanatomical basis that matches observed actions with an internal motor representation of the observed action such that the respective neuronal structures are active when movements are observed (own or others), imitated, imagined, or executed [13, 14]. Zult et al. (2014) [7] argued that imitation plays a role in motor learning during intermanual transfer paradigms, which is supported by studies observing increased activations of brain areas during cross-education that overlap with areas containing mirror neurons [15–17].
Given the positive possibilities of intermanual transfer effects, in the recent past, interest emerged regarding a method that facilitates motor learning and intermanual transfer in the clinical context, known as mirror therapy [18]. This therapy uses a mirror that is placed in the midsagittal plane of a patient in order to provide visual feedback of an intact hand that is performing a motor task while the performer directs their gaze and attention onto the mirror. Simultaneously, the nontraining hand is hidden behind the mirror in a similar position. This superimposition provides an online visual illusion as if the contralateral nontraining limb (and impaired limb in patients) is moving as efficiently as the training limb. This therapy was originally used to treat phantom limb pain [19] and is currently thought to be supportive in patients with hemiparesis [20] and complex regional pain syndrome [21] and in stroke [22, 23]. The advantage of such a method is especially valuable for patients whose control of one hand is impaired or immobilized since in motor recovery programs the additional mirror visual feedback (MVF) can facilitate the recovery process [20]. In addition, the use of this novel technique has been repeatedly demonstrated to facilitate motor learning not only in patients practicing bilaterally, but also in healthy people performing unilateral motor training. Intermanual transfer was more pronounced by the use of MVF compared to other feedback modalities [24–26].
Those studies indicate that augmented visual feedback through a mirror facilitates intermanual transfer effects, while the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. Based on neuroimaging data, a recent review by Deconinck et al. (2015) [20] found that MVF-related neural activation patterns have substantial overlap with regions related to attention and action monitoring processes, both of which are strongly related to motor learning. Additionally and in line with another review on cross-education, increased neural activity of ipsilateral brain areas that are associated with the mirror neural system was reported in mirror training [6]. Since motor execution that is concurrently observed through a mirror (i.e., providing an illusion of movement of the contralateral hand, although not active) is a special kind of movement observation, it appears to be reasonable that the MNS could be involved [6, 7]. Therefore, the involvement of the MNS-related brain areas not only is proposed to play a role in intermanual transfer but also might be synergistically involved in the augmented transfer effects supported by visual feedback through a mirror [6].
So far, most studies concerning mirror-feedback are solely based on fine motor skills, so it remains unclear whether facilitation of intermanual transfer through MVF might also occur in tasks that require more complex (and sport-specific) motor abilities. It has been repeatedly claimed that the impact of augmented feedback methods depends on task complexity and skill level [27, 28], but MVF studies did not consider whether skill level or task complexity might influence the beneficial performance gains through MVF (i.e., whether performance gains differ in terms of experienced versus unexperienced or high versus low level of expertise in a complex motor skill). In sport science and other fields of expertise research, it is well established that skill level modulates motor execution [29–31], neural activity of the action observation network [32–34], action anticipation [35], focus of attention [36], and gaze behavior [37]. Moreover, experts MNS activation is differently with higher involvement of the MNS when observed movements are familiar compared to nonfamiliar (i.e., are part of their existing motor repertoire) [38, 39].
Therefore, the present study explores whether MVF may have beneficial effects on intermanual transfer in a sport-specific task and if it is modulated by skill level. To this end, we adapted the mirror therapy test apparatus and test protocol as reported by Hamzei et al. (2012) in order to allow participants of high and low proficiency in ball dribbling to perform a dribble task while they received either visual feedback of the trained hand through a mirror (i.e., visual illusion of the left hand) or direct feedback of the trained hand. Based on the existing literature on intermanual transfer effects, for which MVF has been shown to be supportive, we hypothesized that in a more complex sport-specific task we will find intermanual transfer effects that will be increased through MVF. More specifically, due to well-known novice-expert differences and the differential activation of the MNS, we hypothesized that athletes with experience in ball dribbling will profit more than novices from MVF.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
Initially 84 right handed participants took part in this study, but four of them were not able to complete the whole training program due to injury or other engagements. The remaining 80 participants were
2.2. Mirror Training and Slalom Course Setup
The mirror apparatus used in the present study was based on a typical mirror therapy box (e.g., Hamzei et al. 2012) [26] and was modified for the purpose of the present study. As depicted in Figure 1(a), a
[figures omitted; refer to PDF]
In addition to the mirror task, participants had to perform a dribbling slalom course (Figure 1(b)), which served as a transfer task. This course was constructed to allow participants to dribble through pylons, including five directional changes with the starting line also being the finish line. This course was constructed once for right hand dribbling (Figure 1(b), bottom) and once in a mirrored fashion for left hand dribbling (Figure 1(b), top).
2.3. Motor Tasks
Participants had to perform a stationary dribble task at the mirror box construction and a dynamic slalom dribble task at the slalom course. In the stationary dribble task, participants assumed a standing position with their feet placed in the respective positions (see Figure 1) and were asked to dribble a basketball (sized appropriately for their gender) with either the right or the left hand as often as possible in the two target fields in a predefined sequence, which was as follows: they were free to begin in either the left or the right field by dribbling two times in the respective field, then two times in the neighboring field, then going back again, and so on. In case of a dribbling error, which occurred when the basketball touched the wooden beams, they were instructed to proceed with dribbling and ignore the error if possible. However, in the case of a complete loss of ball control, they could take a new basketball from a box, which was directly positioned beside them, and proceed with dribbling in the respective field where the error happened. Participants’ resting hand had to be positioned behind the wall at the same height and position as the other hand. The task lasted 45 seconds, and successful dribbling was defined as the ball being dribbled in a field in the instructed sequence. They were not allowed to hold the ball or to bring the hand completely under the ball. In the slalom dribbling course, the task was to dribble the ball using only one hand while running as fast as possible through the cones that were placed on the floor as depicted in Figure 1.
2.4. Procedure
The experiment was executed in a sports hall. After arriving and receiving instruction as to the purpose of the study, participants were pseudorandomly assigned to either the mirror visual feedback (MVF) or the control (direct feedback) group. This pseudorandomization was separated into novice and experienced (according to dribbling expertise) participants, resulting in two groups (41 novice versus 39 experienced). In the expertise group 19 participants were assigned to the MVF and 20 to the control condition, while in the novice group 20 went to MVF and 21 to the control condition. Each participant began the experiment after an individual warm-up with the dynamic slalom dribbling task. For task familiarization, verbal instructions for the slalom course were provided first and then each participant was allowed to complete the slalom course two times in a self-paced manner. In a counterbalanced order within each group, participants started the first trial with either right hand or left hand dribbling while the time for course completion was recorded via a handheld stop watch. Three trials were performed with each hand.
Subsequently, participants were instructed to the stationary dribbling task at the mirror box. For task familiarization, participants were allowed to perform the task three times (45 sec) with rest breaks of 45 sec between trials for each hand. After familiarization, baseline measurements (pretest) included three trials of 45 sec with rest breaks of 45 sec between trials for each hand performed in a counterbalanced order with the right and the left hand.
In the first training block, which followed the baseline measurements, participants performed 10 trials of 45 sec of the motor task with the right hand with rest breaks of 45 sec between each trial. In the control training condition, participants learned the motor task in the same way as was requested in the baseline measurements, with direct visual feedback of the right hand while dribbling the basketball. In the MVF training group, participants were requested to direct their gaze and attention onto the mirror while performing the motor task so that the mirror provided an illusion as if the left hand was dribbling the ball. In all, each participant performed four training blocks with the right hand as described for the first training session. The training period as well as posttest measurements were completed in a two-week interval, and only one training block was allowed per day. After the training blocks, posttest measurements were conducted on a separate day in the same way as the baseline measurements.
2.5. Data Analysis
Pre- and posttest trials were recorded with an HD video camera directed at the target fields for subsequent analysis of scores and errors. An investigator blinded to expertise level analyzed the video material and assigned scores as defined above (i.e., one point was given for successfully dribbling in a target field). Moreover, the error score was counted as well, such as a loss of ball control. Thus, dribbling scores (further called “dribbling performance”) for the three trials with their respective errors (further called “dribbling error”) could be analyzed for the pre- and the posttest and for each hand. Likewise, the three scores of the three trials were measured for the pre- and posttests of the slalom course (further called “slalom performance”) for both hands. Statistical analysis presented here was performed by taking the mean values of the three pre- and posttest trials. We took the mean values since we could not find any statistical differences when we performed the same analysis using the median score or the best score.
2.6. Statistical Analysis
Shapiro-Wilks tests were used to check all variables for normal distribution; all scores of the stationary dribbling at the mirror box (i.e., dribbling performance) were normally distributed. However, error scores of the dribbling task at the mirror box (i.e., dribbling error) as well as times for the slalom course (i.e., slalom performance) were not normally distributed. Thus, ANOVAs and
Three-way ANOVAs with repeated measures “TIME” (Pre/Post) on the between-factor “CONDITION” (MVF/Control) and “EXPERTISE” (Novice/Experienced) were calculated separately for left and right hand performance to observe whether the feedback modalities (MVF versus active) influence a different performance improvement in the two expertise groups. Wherever sphericity was violated, Greenhouse-Geisser adjusted values were reported and
Finally, for the not-normally distributed variables (dribbling error and slalom performance), an index for performance changes was calculated by subtracting post- from prevalues. For these variables, between-subject performances changes between the MVF and control groups were analyzed by Mann-Whitney
2.7. Statistics on Baseline Performance
Since in the following analysis several between-subject analyses were performed and body height might influence dribbling behavior at a test setup not adjusted to body height, we calculated ANOVAs with the between-factor CONDITION and EXPERTISE for body height to determine whether experimental groups systematically differ in these variables. However, no effects emerged (all
We additionally tested whether our a priori group delineation criteria (active engagement in basketball or handball competition) were able to separate experienced from novice participants, that is, if it is mirrored in different baseline measures of our three outcome variables: Table 1 (values separated by the factor EXPERTISE) depicts the values of baseline (pretest) left and right hand performance from which one can see that left hand (
Table 1
Performance scores for all test variables for experts and novices independent of condition. Shown are mean values along standard deviations.
Expertise level | Task/variable | Pretest | Posttest | ||
Right hand | Left hand | Right hand | Left hand | ||
Novice | Dribbling performance | 85.52 ± 18.76 | 68.78 ± 16.63 | 104.33 ± 23.31 | 84.83 ± 20.98 |
Experienced | 105.32 ± 15.39 | 95.60 ± 14.67 | 123.42 ± 18.07 | 107.15 ± 15.87 | |
|
|||||
Novice | Dribbling error | 2.52 ± 1.79 | 4.59 ± 2.74 | 1.23 ± 1.11 | 3.09 ± 2.30 |
Experienced | 1.66 ± 1.84 | 2.99 ± 2.46 | 0.80 ± 0.93 | 1.93 ± 1.39 | |
|
|||||
Novice | Slalom performance (sec) | 9.09 ± 1.52 | 9.58 ± 1.69 | 8.80 ± 1.45 | 9.04 ± 1.57 |
Experienced | 8.47 ± 1.01 | 8.61 ± 1.13 | 8.01 ± 0.87 | 8.11 ± 0.86 |
3. Results
3.1. Global Analysis on the Central Parameter Dribbling Performance
As expected after four training sessions, a three-way ANOVA for the right hand dribbling performance revealed a highly significant performance improvement for the factor TIME (
3.2. Left Hand Dribbling Performance Improvements between Conditions
Due to the twofold and threefold interactions for left hand performance, we performed separate ANOVAs, one for the novice and one for the experienced group (since no interaction effects for right hand appeared, no further analyses were performed for right hand; see data analysis). As depicted in Figure 2(c), for the novice group a significant improvement for the factor TIME in left hand performance emerged (
[figures omitted; refer to PDF]
3.3. Post Hoc Measures for Left Hand Dribbling Performance Improvements
For post hoc analysis to further elucidate the significant
Table 2
Absolute and relative performance improvements for all test variables separated by CONDITION
CONDITION |
Task/variable | Performance improvements | Performance improvements |
||
Right hand | Left hand | Right hand | Left hand | ||
Control novice | Dribbling performance | 19.06 ± 15.33 | 16.50 ± 11.41 | 17.23 | 11.80 |
MVF novice | 18.55 ± 9.79 | 15.58 ± 9.33 | 14.91 | 10.27 | |
Control experienced | 17.70 ± 11.48 | 6.50 ± 8.94 | 18.21 | 6.11 | |
MVF experienced | 18.51 ± 11.93 | 16.87 ± 11.93 | 19.97 | 16.41 | |
|
|||||
Control novice | Dribbling error | −1.06 ± 1.45 | −1.82 ± 2.02 | 40.66 | 40.80 |
MVF novice | −1.53 ± 1.45 | −1.14 ± 1.75 | 42.99 | 24.35 | |
Control experienced | −1.35 ± 2.07 | −1.18 ± 2.25 | 63.81 | 35.91 | |
MVF experienced | −0.65 ± 1.13 | −0.70 ± 1.38 | 37.68 | 28.61 | |
|
|||||
Control novice | Slalom performance (sec) | −0.25 ± 0.64 | −0.64 ± 0.75 | 6.68 | 2.61 |
MVF novice | −0.33 ± 0.39 | −0.42 ± 0.42 | 4.43 | 3.63 | |
Control experienced | −0.42 ± 0.51 | −0.28 ± 0.39 | 3.41 | 4.93 | |
MVF experienced | −0.58 ± 0.83 | −0.65 ± 0.52 | 7.53 | 6.90 |
3.4. Dribbling Error Improvements between Groups
To observe whether the MVF group’s dribbling error decrease from pre- to posttest measures (i.e., performance improvements) differed from the control groups, we compared the difference in the pre- and posttest measures separated for the factors
3.5. Comparison of Slalom Performance Improvements between Groups
To detect whether the MVF group’s performance improvements in slalom dribbling were more pronounced than the control groups, we compared the difference scores from pre- to posttest measures separated for the factors
We found no differences between MVF and control in right hand improvements, neither for experienced nor for novice participants (both
4. Discussion
The purpose of the present approach was to find out whether mirror visual feedback is applicable within a sport-scientific context and whether the well-known improvements of motor learning through MVF are modulated by skill level. Intermanual transfer effects were found, such as all groups, regardless of proficiency level or feedback modality, improving performance with the nontraining hand. Compared to direct feedback, superior left hand performance gains of MVF participants were significant in the experienced group only, while performance gains in novice participants did not differ between feedback modalities. Interestingly, this pattern of observations was accompanied by the same effects in a (nontrained) transfer task in which dynamic instead of stationary dribbling was requested. The results support our first hypothesis, which predicted intermanual transfer effects in the present motor task, which will be increased through MVF. However, this hypothesis has to be modified, as MVF effects depend on skill level. Consequently, our second hypothesis that experts will profit more from MVF than novices has to be rejected in its current formulation, since only experienced players showed improvement. A comparable decrease in error scores across groups indicates that a change in the speed and accuracy relationship cannot account for our findings. Moreover, we could not find any differences between male or female participants, which, however, should be handled cautiously due to the limited participant number for each gender and factor.
4.1. Intermanual Transfer
The intermanual transfer effects observed in the present study are in line with other studies that observed transfer effects of different motor tasks. For example, transfer effects were found in several realistic sport-specific tasks [4, 5, 41], a pegboard task requiring fine motor skills [2], an inverted-reversed printing task [42], finger tapping [43], keyboard pressing [12], ball catching [44], and adaptation in visuomotor rotation tasks [45]. Thus, the present study extends those findings in so far that interlimb transfer effects are observable in a ball dribbling task, which, however, are modulated by skill level.
Interestingly, transfer effects of the control groups receiving direct feedback were stronger in novice participants compared to those who were experienced. However, ball dribbling is a well-learned motor behavior for handball and basketball players, but not for nonplayers. It is well-known that motor learning undergoes several stages [46] such that the task to be executed in the present study differed in several aspects with respect to skill level. It is thought that, during the initial motor learning phase, movements are unskilled and depend strongly on feedback along with high demands on attention [47, 48]. With sustained practice, movement aspects such as accuracy and velocity increase and become more automated, while dependence on feedback becomes less important [49]. Since experienced participants might have been less dependent on feedback, one could assume that the level of attention decreased in the direct feedback modality, which in turn resulted in a lesser degree of intermanual transfer effects to the left hand compared to novice participants. However, this is a speculative view which needs further investigation of the explicit role of attention in intermanual transfer.
4.2. Mirror Visual Feedback
The present results concerning MVF are twofold since intermanual transfer effects were more pronounced through MVF compared to normal feedback only in the experienced group. Novice participants, relatively unfamiliar with basketball dribbling, did not benefit more from mirror visual feedback compared to normal (direct) feedback. The latter finding is in contrast to studies that found pronounced transfer effects in healthy participants (for an overview see [20]). However, due to study protocol and feedback modalities, only the studies by Hamzei et al. [26] and Läppchen et al. [25] are directly comparable to the present approach. They compared learning simple fine motor tasks (e.g., a pegboard task) with either direct feedback from the training hand or MVF. Clear advantages of MVF over direct feedback were found in intermanual transfer from right to left hand. Based on accompanied functional MRI data Hamzei et al. (2012) found a mirror training specific neural network, including areas that are associated with the mirror neural system. Moreover, Läppchen et al. (2012) found different excitability changes (induced by TMS) in M1 in both hemispheres after mirror training. The
Therefore, from a neurophysiological point of view, different involvement of MNS-related brain regions might be responsible for the skill-level dependent results in the current experiment. The tasks used by [25, 26] were basic fine motor tasks that were comparable to fine motor executions that humans perform in everyday life such as putting a peg in a hole or using a teaspoon. Consequently, it might well be that those tasks become or were already familiar within the training process and had become part of the motor repertoire. Indeed, Hamzei et al. (2012) argued that observation of this embodied action (i.e., the tasks used in their study) activates MNS-related brain regions, likely due to motor simulation [26]. However, the ball dribbling task in the present study has no equivalence to the everyday life of nonplayers. Therefore, although speculative and not measured directly, one might argue that the ball dribbling task, being relatively unfamiliar for novice participants, did not activate the MNS to such an extent that it drives augmented transfer effects as suggested through MVF. In contrast, experienced ball dribblers, whose sport frequently requires ball dribbling skill, might had stronger involvement of MNS-related brain regions. Support for this interpretation comes from (motor-) expert studies. In expert dancers, as an example, different activations of the MNS with higher involvement of the MNS have been detected when observed movements are familiar, that is, part of the own motor repertoire [38, 39]. Along with the activation of ipsilateral motor areas through observing the right training hand in a mirror (i.e., illusion of a moving left hand) in accordance with the cross-activation model (cf. [8]), this resulted in performance gains of the left untrained hand which outweighed any performance gains through “common” intermanual transfer effects [6, 7].
Alternatively, considering the different task demands of ball dribbling concurrently with MVF or active feedback, it could well be that task complexity plays an essential role for our findings, which in turn is related to individual skill level [50, 51]. Thus, a simple explanation could be that for experienced players the active feedback condition was simple, while MVF was complex (or at least very unusual or more complex). In contrast, both feedback modalities were complex for novice participants. The unusual feedback through a mirror might have forced even experienced ball dribblers to direct attention more strongly back to task execution as visual feedback dependency becomes more relevant. Indeed, it has been indicated that with increasing task complexity the profit of concurrent feedback also increases [50]. The increase in attention of experienced players to task execution and thus to the illusion of the left hand might have, in accordance with the cross-activation model, increased neural involvement of ipsilateral brain areas, which in turn evoked the performance improvements of the left hand. Bearing this in mind, our results are well in line with the current knowledge and further suggest that MVF-induced transfer effects could depend on the two interrelated factors’ task complexity and the individual’s skill level [27, 52]. If so, mirror therapy studies in the future might consider these aspects to find the best means of motor rehabilitation.
Although the present exploratory approach requires further investigation into the role of task complexity, the role of attention, skill level, and additional comparisons to other feedback modalities, we propose that the present results support MVF as being a potential tool to support intermanual transfer effects for rehabilitation in a sport context when athletes suffer hand or arm immobilization. However, considering that the present approach is the first that attempted to transfer the mirror illusion paradigm to the sport context, the study has some limitations. First, no measurement of neural activity was implemented, so the interpretations of brain-related mechanisms are only indirect and warrant further investigation by concurrent neuroimaging techniques. Furthermore, comparisons to other feedback conditions such as purely observational feedback of a passive hand [53] or active left hand, motor imagery, and bilateral training or left handed participants have not been considered in this initial study. How far professional or high expertise players are differently affected by MVF compared to the medium expertise levels in this study would be an interesting point as well. In this line, a higher sample size including a high variance of skill levels might also reveal a possible relationship between baseline values and intermanual performance gains through MVF. Lastly, proprioceptive sensations from the hand behind the mirror when unintentionally moved might have interfered with the mirror illusion differently in the two expertise groups [54], a factor that should be systematically controlled in the future.
5. Conclusion
The present study found that mirror visual feedback facilitates intermanual transfer effects in sport, but only for participants that had experience with the movements being performed. Thus, this study introduced the role of skill level and task complexity to the field of mirror visual feedback, two interrelated factors that could provide new insights in the study of mechanisms underlying MVF.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that there are no known competing interests associated with this publication and there has been no significant financial support for this work that could have influenced its outcome.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank the handball and basketball sport clubs supporting this study. The authors also thank Fabian Hanpft, Sabine Mayer, Johannes Jester, Philipp Sprott, and Florian Bönnemann for help with data acquisition.
[1] G. Thut, N. D. Cook, M. Regard, K. L. Leenders, U. Halsband, T. Landis, "Intermanual transfer of proximal and distal motor engrams in humans," Experimental Brain Research, vol. 108 no. 2, pp. 321-327, 1996.
[2] K. Schulze, E. Lüders, L. Jäncke, "Intermanual transfer in a simple motor task," Cortex, vol. 38 no. 5, pp. 805-815, DOI: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70047-9, 2002.
[3] N. Birbaumer, "Motor learning: passing a skill from one hand to the other," Current Biology, vol. 17 no. 23, pp. R1024-R1026, DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.004, 2007.
[4] E. Haaland, J. Hoff, "Non-dominant leg training improves the bilateral motor performance of soccer players," Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports, vol. 13 no. 3, pp. 179-184, DOI: 10.1034/j.1600-0838.2003.00296.x, 2003.
[5] T. Stöckel, M. Weigelt, J. Krug, "Acquisition of a complex basketball-dribbling task in school children as a function of bilateral practice order," Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, vol. 82 no. 2, pp. 188-197, DOI: 10.1080/02701367.2011.10599746, 2011.
[6] G. Howatson, T. Zult, J. P. Farthing, I. Zijdewind, T. Hortobágyi, "Mirror training to augment cross-education during resistance training: a hypothesis," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 7, article 396,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00396, 2013.
[7] T. Zult, G. Howatson, E. E. Kádár, J. P. Farthing, T. Hortobágyi, "Role of the mirror-neuron system in cross-education," Sports Medicine, vol. 44 no. 2, pp. 159-178, DOI: 10.1007/s40279-013-0105-2, 2014.
[8] M. Lee, M. R. Hinder, S. C. Gandevia, T. J. Carroll, "The ipsilateral motor cortex contributes to cross-limb transfer of performance gains after ballistic motor practice," Journal of Physiology, vol. 588 no. 1, pp. 201-212, DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.183855, 2010.
[9] G. Dreisbach, H. Haider, R. H. Kluwe, "Preparatory processes in the task-switching paradigm: evidence from the use of probability cues," Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 28 no. 3, pp. 468-483, DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.468, 2002.
[10] S. Obayashi, "Possible mechanism for transfer of motor skill learning: implication of the cerebellum," The Cerebellum, vol. 3 no. 4, pp. 204-211, DOI: 10.1080/14734220410018977, 2004.
[11] K. L. Ruddy, R. G. Carson, "Neural pathways mediating cross education of motor function," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 7, article 397,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00397, 2013.
[12] H. G. Taylor, K. M. Heilman, "Left-hemisphere motor dominance in righthanders," Cortex, vol. 16 no. 4, pp. 587-603, DOI: 10.1016/s0010-9452(80)80006-2, 1980.
[13] G. Rizzolatti, L. Fadiga, V. Gallese, L. Fogassi, "Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions," Cognitive Brain Research, vol. 3 no. 2, pp. 131-141, DOI: 10.1016/0926-6410(95)00038-0, 1996.
[14] M. Jeannerod, "Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition," NeuroImage, vol. 14 no. 1, pp. S103-S109, DOI: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832, 2001.
[15] J. P. Farthing, J. R. Krentz, C. R. A. Magnus, T. S. Barss, J. L. Lanovaz, J. Cummine, C. Esopenko, G. E. Sarty, R. Borowsky, "Changes in functional magnetic resonance imaging cortical activation with cross education to an immobilized limb," Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, vol. 43 no. 8, pp. 1394-1405, DOI: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e318210783c, 2011.
[16] J. P. Farthing, J. R. Krentz, C. R. A. Magnus, "Strength training the free limb attenuates strength loss during unilateral immobilization," Journal of Applied Physiology, vol. 106 no. 3, pp. 730-736, DOI: 10.1152/japplphysiol.91331.2008, 2009.
[17] T. Hortobágyi, S. P. Richardson, M. Lomarev, E. Shamim, S. Meunier, H. Russman, N. Dang, M. Hallett, "Interhemispheric plasticity in humans," Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, vol. 43 no. 7, pp. 1188-1199, DOI: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31820a94b8, 2011.
[18] V. S. Ramachandran, D. Rodgers-Ramachandran, "Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors," Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, vol. 263 no. 1369, pp. 377-386, DOI: 10.1098/rspb.1996.0058, 1996.
[19] V. S. Ramachandran, E. L. Altschuler, "The use of visual feedback, in particular mirror visual feedback, in restoring brain function," Brain, vol. 132 no. 7, pp. 1693-1710, DOI: 10.1093/brain/awp135, 2009.
[20] F. J. A. Deconinck, A. R. P. Smorenburg, A. Benham, A. Ledebt, M. G. Feltham, G. J. P. Savelsbergh, "Reflections on mirror therapy: a systematic review of the effect of mirror visual feedback on the brain," Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, vol. 29 no. 4, pp. 349-361, DOI: 10.1177/1545968314546134, 2015.
[21] C. S. McCabe, R. C. Haigh, E. F. J. Ring, P. W. Halligan, P. D. Wall, D. R. Blake, "A controlled pilot study of the utility of mirror visual feedback in the treatment of complex regional pain syndrome (type 1)," Rheumatology, vol. 42 no. 1, pp. 97-101, DOI: 10.1093/rheumatology/keg041, 2003.
[22] E. L. Altschuler, S. B. Wisdom, L. Stone, C. Foster, D. Galasko, D. M. E. Llewellyn, V. S. Ramachandran, "Rehabilitation of hemiparesis after stroke with a mirror," The Lancet, vol. 353 no. 9169, pp. 2035-2036, DOI: 10.1016/s0140-6736(99)00920-4, 1999.
[23] G. Yavuzer, R. Selles, N. Sezer, S. Sütbeyaz, J. B. Bussmann, F. Köseoǧlu, M. B. Atay, H. J. Stam, "Mirror therapy improves hand function in subacute stroke: a randomized controlled trial," Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, vol. 89 no. 3, pp. 393-398, DOI: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.08.162, 2008.
[24] I. Nojima, T. Mima, S. Koganemaru, M. N. Thabit, H. Fukuyama, T. Kawamata, "Human motor plasticity induced by mirror visual feedback," The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 32 no. 4, pp. 1293-1300, DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.5364-11.2012, 2012.
[25] C. H. Läppchen, T. Ringer, J. Blessin, G. Seidel, S. Grieshammer, R. Lange, F. Hamzei, "Optical illusion alters M1 excitability after mirror therapy: A TMS Study," Journal of Neurophysiology, vol. 108 no. 10, pp. 2857-2861, DOI: 10.1152/jn.00321.2012, 2012.
[26] F. Hamzei, C. H. Läppchen, V. Glauche, I. Mader, M. Rijntjes, C. Weiller, "Functional plasticity induced by mirror training: the mirror as the element connecting both hands to one hemisphere," Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, vol. 26 no. 5, pp. 484-496, DOI: 10.1177/1545968311427917, 2012.
[27] G. Wulf, C. H. Shea, "Principles derived from the study of simple skills do not generalize to complex skill learning," Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, vol. 9 no. 2, pp. 185-211, DOI: 10.3758/bf03196276, 2002.
[28] A. Utley, S. Astill, "Motor control, learning and development," International Journal of Sports Science and Coaching, vol. 3 no. 2, pp. 297-299, DOI: 10.1260/174795408785100590, 2008.
[29] L. Jäncke, N. J. Shah, M. Peters, "Cortical activations in primary and secondary motor areas for complex bimanual movements in professional pianists," Cognitive Brain Research, vol. 10 no. 1-2, pp. 177-183, DOI: 10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00028-8, 2000.
[30] T. F. Münte, E. Altenmüller, L. Jäncke, "The musician's brain as a model of neuroplasticity," Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 3 no. 6, pp. 473-478, 2002.
[31] G. Schlaug, "The brain of musicians," Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 930, pp. 281-299, 2001.
[32] N. Balser, B. Lorey, S. Pilgramm, R. Stark, M. Bischoff, K. Zentgraf, A. M. Williams, J. Munzert, "Prediction of human actions: expertise and task-related effects on neural activation of the action observation network," Human Brain Mapping, vol. 35 no. 8, pp. 4016-4034, DOI: 10.1002/hbm.22455, 2014.
[33] G. Buccino, F. Lui, N. Canessa, I. Patteri, G. Lagravinese, F. Benuzzi, C. A. Porro, G. Rizzolatti, "Neural circuits involved in the recognition of actions performed by nonconspecifics: An fMRI Study," Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 16 no. 1, pp. 114-126, DOI: 10.1162/089892904322755601, 2004.
[34] Y. Chang, "Reorganization and plastic changes of the human brain associated with skill learning and expertise," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 8 no. 1, article 35,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00035, 2014.
[35] S. M. Aglioti, P. Cesari, M. Romani, C. Urgesi, "Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players," Nature Neuroscience, vol. 11 no. 9, pp. 1109-1116, DOI: 10.1038/nn.2182, 2008.
[36] S. Hüttermann, D. Memmert, D. J. Simons, "The size and shape of the attentional ‘spotlight’ varies with differences in sports expertise," Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, vol. 20 no. 2, pp. 147-157, DOI: 10.1037/xap0000012, 2014.
[37] R. Rienhoff, L. Fischer, B. Strauss, J. Baker, J. Schorer, "Focus of attention influences quiet-eye behavior: an exploratory investigation of different skill levels in female basketball players," Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, vol. 4 no. 1, pp. 62-74, DOI: 10.1037/spy0000031, 2015.
[38] B. Calvo-Merino, D. E. Glaser, J. Grèzes, R. E. Passingham, P. Haggard, "Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers," Cerebral Cortex, vol. 15 no. 8, pp. 1243-1249, DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhi007, 2005.
[39] B. Calvo-Merino, J. Grèzes, D. E. Glaser, R. E. Passingham, P. Haggard, "Seeing or doing? influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation," Current Biology, vol. 16 no. 22,DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.065, 2006.
[40] J. Cohen, Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavior Science, 1988.
[41] O. Senff, M. Weigelt, "Sequential effects after practice with the dominant and non-dominant hand on the acquisition of a sliding task in schoolchildren," Laterality, vol. 16 no. 2, pp. 227-239, DOI: 10.1080/13576500903549414, 2011.
[42] S. E. Parlow, M. Kinsbourne, "Asymmetrical transfer of training between hands: implications for interhemispheric communication in normal brain," Brain and Cognition, vol. 11 no. 1, pp. 98-113, DOI: 10.1016/0278-2626(89)90008-0, 1989.
[43] J. I. Laszlo, R. A. Baguley, P. J. Bairstow, "Bilateral transfer in tapping skill in the absence of peripheral information," Journal of Motor Behavior, vol. 2 no. 4, pp. 261-271, DOI: 10.1080/00222895.1970.10734884, 1970.
[44] S. M. Morton, C. E. Lang, A. J. Bastian, "Inter- and intra-limb generalization of adaptation during catching," Experimental Brain Research, vol. 141 no. 4, pp. 438-445, DOI: 10.1007/s002210100889, 2001.
[45] R. L. Sainburg, J. Wang, "Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information," Experimental Brain Research, vol. 145 no. 4, pp. 437-447, DOI: 10.1007/s00221-002-1140-7, 2002.
[46] U. Halsband, R. K. Lange, "Motor learning in man: a review of functional and clinical studies," Journal of Physiology Paris, vol. 99 no. 4–6, pp. 414-424, DOI: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.007, 2006.
[47] C. G. Atkeson, "Learning arm kinematics and dynamics," Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 12, pp. 157-183, DOI: 10.1146/annurev.ne.12.030189.001105, 1989.
[48] R. Shadmehr, F. A. Mussa-Ivaldi, "Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task," Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 14 no. 5, pp. 3208-3224, 1994.
[49] B. Preilowski, "Phases of motor-skills acquisition: a neuropsychological approach," Journal of Human Movement Studies, vol. 3, pp. 169-181, 1977.
[50] R. Sigrist, G. Rauter, R. Riener, P. Wolf, "Augmented visual, auditory, haptic, and multimodal feedback in motor learning: a review," Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, vol. 20 no. 1, pp. 21-53, DOI: 10.3758/s13423-012-0333-8, 2013.
[51] M. A. Guadagnoll, T. D. Lee, "Challenge point: a framework for conceptualizing the effects of various practice conditions in motor learning," Journal of Motor Behavior, vol. 36 no. 2, pp. 212-224, DOI: 10.3200/jmbr.36.2.212-224, 2004.
[52] T. Haerem, D. Rau, "The influence of degree of expertise and objective task complexity on perceived task complexity and performance," Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 92 no. 5, pp. 1320-1331, DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.5.1320, 2007.
[53] P. Reissig, R. Puri, M. I. Garry, J. J. Summers, M. R. Hinder, "The influence of mirror-visual feedback on training-induced motor performance gains in the untrained hand," PLoS ONE, vol. 10, article e0141828,DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141828, 2015.
[54] R. W. Selles, M. E. Michielsen, J. B. J. Bussmann, H. J. Stam, H. L. Hurkmans, I. Heijnen, D. De Groot, G. M. Ribbers, "Effects of a mirror-induced visual illusion on a reaching task in stroke patients: implications for mirror therapy training," Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, vol. 28 no. 7, pp. 652-659, DOI: 10.1177/1545968314521005, 2014.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright © 2016 Fabian Steinberg et al. This work is licensed under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Mirror training therapy is a promising tool to initiate neural plasticity and facilitate the recovery process of motor skills after diseases such as stroke or hemiparesis by improving the intermanual transfer of fine motor skills in healthy people as well as in patients. This study evaluated whether these augmented performance improvements by mirror visual feedback (MVF) could be used for learning a sport-specific skill and if the effects are modulated by skill level. A sample of 39 young, healthy, and experienced basketball and handball players and 41 novices performed a stationary basketball dribble task at a mirror box in a standing position and received either MVF or direct feedback. After four training days using only the right hand, performance of both hands improved from pre- to posttest measurements. Only the left hand (untrained) performance of the experienced participants receiving MVF was more pronounced than for the control group. This indicates that intermanual motor transfer can be improved by MVF in a sport-specific task. However, this effect cannot be generalized to motor learning per se since it is modulated by individuals’ skill level, a factor that might be considered in mirror therapy research.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer