This work is licensed under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
1. Introduction
Prolonged immobilization and inactivity after injury and/or surgery may lead to serious motor and cognitive dysfunctions, especially in older adults [1, 2]. More precisely, immobilization in the acute period after hip arthroplasty has become more and more frequent in this population due to the increasing amount of people suffering from osteoporosis, which can result in hip fractures [3]. Despite the knowledge that immobilization not only negatively affects cardiovascular and pulmonary parameters but also increases the risk of falls and movement disorders [1, 4], patients are generally inactive during this period due to their limited ability to participate in physical exercise. In addition, immobilization leads to several impairments of motor function [5]. Motor impairments that follow short periods of inactivity are believed to be principally driven by changes occurring at the cortical level rather than the muscular level [6–8]. Indeed, a significant reduction of the cortical motor area representing the immobilized limb could be observed [9].
To counteract at least some of these risk factors, mental simulation techniques such as action observation (AO) or motor imagery (MI) have been proposed as feasible alternatives to stimulate the motor system [10, 11]. The AO therapy requires subjects to observe a video clip or watch actions performed by an operator [12, 13] while MI represents the mental simulation of motor actions without any corresponding motor output [14].
In the last decade, a growing number of AO- or MI-based interventions were successfully conducted that aimed at fostering rehabilitation of patients, for instance, after stroke [12, 15, 16], Parkinson’s disease (for review, see [17]), or orthopedic injury and/or surgery [18, 19]. The use of AO in healthy participants has been shown to limit the reduction of brain area that is normally induced by immobilization [20]. Thus, activation of cerebral visuomotor systems during AO seems sufficient to counteract negative cortical plasticity induced by immobilization. Similarly, MI was demonstrated to counteract the slowdown of sensorimotor processes induced by short-term immobilization [21]. The reason for the efficiency of those mental simulation techniques is considered to rely on activation of overlapping brain areas during AO, MI, and physical execution of the motor task [14, 22]. Traditionally, AO and MI have been considered as independent intervention methods. Recently, however, more and more evidence emerged that proposes increased efficiency when combining AO with MI (AO + MI), meaning that MI is performed during AO (for reviews, see [23, 24]). In this context, AO + MI has revealed higher corticospinal excitability [25, 26] as well as greater activity in motor areas of the brain [27] compared to either AO or MI alone. Noteworthy some studies even reported oversummative activity [27, 28] compared to the sum of brain activity during independent AO and independent MI. These findings were interpreted as evidence that neural correlates of MI and AO might merge rather than compete with each other [24]. However, due to a lack of intervention studies, it is not clear to date whether the combination of AO and MI (AO + MI) in addition to common physical therapy has important implications for neurorehabilitation and motor (re-)learning in patients (for reviews, see [23, 24]).
The aim of the present study therefore was to evaluate AO + MI in a clinical setting by comparing the rehabilitation outcomes of patients undergoing an AO + MI intervention with those of patients that were treated in the conventional way only. One major aim of our study was to minimize the period of inactivity and to start with an early rehabilitation. Finally, our main hypothesis was that the combination of AO and MI (AO + MI) would result in better rehabilitation outcome than the conventional intervention alone.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
Participants were recruited from the general database of Valdoltra Orthopaedic Hospital, University of Primorska, Ankaran, Slovenia. From 405 subjects that were assessed for eligibility, 26 volunteered to participate in the “PANGeA hip study, Valdoltra 2015.” Finally, 21 participants successfully completed both (PRE and POST) measurements (for baseline characteristics, see Table 1). Due to primary osteoarthritis of the hip, the cementless total hip arthroplasty (THA) was performed through direct lateral approach in all participants. The enrolment, randomization, and final analysis procedures are shown in the CONSORT flow diagram (Figure 1). Prior to the study, all participants were physically screened by medical doctors and interviewed by the research team. The exclusion criteria were as follows: previous THA, severe acute metabolic, neuromuscular and cardiovascular diseases, excessive obesity (over 45% fat), elevated/high body temperature or other life-threatening situations, infectious diseases, cancer, bleeding, failure of vitally important organs, complete physical exhaustion, mild cognitive impairment or dementia, critical ischemia of the lower limbs, and patients unable to intend the measurement and rehabilitation protocols. All participants were right-handed and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All procedures were carried out in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the National Medical Ethics Committee. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the study, and no payment was provided for participation in this study.
Table 1
Baseline characteristics of PANGeA hip study participants.
Intervention group ( |
Control group ( |
||
Gender | 2 women | 5 women | |
Age (y) | 64.4 ± 4.1 | 63.1 ± 5.6 | 0.550 |
Height (cm) | 171.8 ± 5.1 | 168.6 ± 13.8 | 0.528 |
Weight (kg) | 86.6 ± 8.7 | 76.0 ± 15.7 | 0.088 |
Grip strength, dominant (kg) | 35.3 ± 9.1 | 32.3 ± 15.6 | 0.605 |
Total hip replacement, (right side, N) | 7/10 | 7/11 | |
MoCA score | 27.9 ± 1.4 | 28.1 ± 1.4 | 0.755 |
Education duration (y) | 12.4 ± 3.0 | 11.8 ± 2.4 | 0.636 |
Note: data are mean ± SD. MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment.
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]2.2. Nonphysical Training Intervention
Participants in the intervention group received next to a standard rehabilitation protocol with physiotherapists an additional nonphysical intervention (AO + MI of locomotor tasks) for approximately 30 minutes per day in the hospital, then 3 times per week in their homes, for a period of 2 months. At the same time and for the same amount of time, participants who were randomly allocated to the placebo control group were asked to actively observe the documentary videos on the television (in hospital and at home). Participants in both groups spent approximately 4–6 days in the hospital before being discharged. All hospital sessions were surveyed and supervised by the authors of the study. Home sessions were supervised by one experimenter during the beginning of the training and then by conference (Skype) video calls. Participants in the control group were also contacted via phone or Skype at least once per week within the period of two months to ensure a comparable level of commitment and to monitor their personal notes regarding the time they spent watching educational documentaries.
Each session of the nonphysical intervention program started with a short relaxation protocol. Afterwards, participants watched video clips presented on a 13-inch tablet PC showing a healthy person filmed from behind performing different locomotor tasks. The difficulty of the presented tasks was progressively increased from week 1 to 8: participants started observing a person walking slowly with assistive devices (e.g., walker and crutches), continuing with normal and fast-paced gait, walking upstairs and downstairs, and walking on narrow (normal and narrow sport bench) and unstable surfaces (soft mats, sand beach, and surfaces covered with snow). When participants reached the level where they could easily imagine themselves performing the presented tasks, an additional task was added such as performing the same locomotor task while holding a glass or a jug full of water. The videos were of different lengths (30 s to 60 s of cyclic locomotor tasks) and were displayed in blocks of 60 to 120 s (see also underneath for more details). Thus, the videos were repeated between two and four times while the participants watched the video and at the same time imagined performing the task that was shown in the video (AO + MI). However, as older adults indicated to have some problems to watch and at the same time “feel the sensations that arise from doing the task” (kinesthetic motor imagery), we presented first the video and asked them to do AO + MI followed by a period in which subjects should close their eyes and imagine the sensations that arise from doing the task (MI). First, this combination of AO + MI and MI was only foreseen in order to familiarize participants with AO + MI. However, this method proved very efficient to maintain the participants’ motivation high, and therefore, the entire nonphysical training was conceptualized to maintain blocks of AO + MI followed by the same amount of time for MI. Starting with 60 seconds of AO + MI and 60 seconds of MI, participants in the intervention group were encouraged to prolong their AO + MI and MI sessions up to 120 seconds for each task throughout the whole training period (two months). Altogether 30 different videos were used as shown in Table 2. Thus, during each training day, at least one to two new videos were presented. Before watching the videos, the participants were instructed as follows: “On the following video you will see a locomotor task, which will be repeated several times. Observe this task and imagine performing the task yourself during the entire period of the video. After several repetitions (60–120 s) you will be asked to close your eyes and to continue performing mentally the task until you hear the stop sign. From time to time, I will encourage you performing the task. Now try to relax and when you are ready, press the spacebar to start the video while concentrating as much as possible on the task.”
Table 2
Example of cognitive training intervention blocks for three successive trainings.
Length | AO + MI | Training 1 | Training 2 | Training 3 |
2 minutes each | AO + MI | Video 1 | Video 2 | Video 4 |
MI | ||||
AO + MI | ||||
MI | ||||
Break | ||||
AO + MI | Video 1 | Video 3 | Video 4 | |
MI | ||||
AO + MI | ||||
MI | ||||
Break | ||||
AO + MI | Video 2 | Video 3 | Video 5 | |
MI | ||||
AO + MI | ||||
MI |
Note: each training session duration was approximately 30 minutes.
2.3. Outcome Measures
All measurements were carried out in a separate and quiet room to avoid any external disturbances from the hospital environment. All tests were performed twice, prior surgery (PRE measurements) and 60 days postsurgery (POST measurements).
2.3.1. Timed Up and Go Test (TUG)
The TUG test was administered to quantify the functional mobility of patients [29]. Patients were asked to rise from the chair, walk around an obstacle that was 3 m away, and return to take a seat as quickly and safely. A practice trial was given, and then two timed trials were recorded and averaged [30]. Time to completion was monitored with a stopwatch.
2.3.2. Four Step Square Test (FSST)
Patients were asked to perform stepping over 25 mm high obstacles in four different directions as quickly and safely as they could (FSST). A practice trial was given, and two timed trials were performed where the best of both was used for further analysis [31]. Time to completion was monitored with a stopwatch.
2.3.3. Single- and Dual-Task Walking
Spatiotemporal gait parameters were measured with the 2D OptoGait system (Microgate, Bolzano, Italy) in the following four 1-minute conditions in a randomized order: walking at their preferred, self-selected speed; brisk walking to the best of their capacity; and both speeds under a dual-task condition. The dual-task conditions were composed of walking and at the same time subtracting by threes from a randomly chosen number between 400 and 500 (serial 3s). Prior to walking, a familiarization trial was given to each participant. Participants were instructed to subtract as many numbers as possible, with their focus prioritized to task correctness prior to the speed of subtraction. Gait speed and swing-time variability were taken into further consideration [4]. For the cognitive task, the amount of subtracted numbers and errors was monitored for each condition.
2.3.4. Single- and Dual-Task Postural Control
The postural task consisted of standing as still as possible in a tandem foot placement position. Participants were asked to focus on a black point placed approximately one meter in front of them at eye level. A force plate (AMTI HE600600-2k, Advanced Mechanical Technology Inc., Watertown, MA, USA) was used to measure displacements of the center of pressure (COP) in both mediolateral and anteroposterior directions. From these values, total sway path, frequency, and amplitude were calculated separately for each direction. This postural task was either performed as a single task or combined with a secondary cognitive task. The cognitive task and its instructions were identical as for walking (see Section 2.3.3).
2.4. Statistical Analyses
The data were analyzed with IBM SPSS Statistics 24.0 software for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Il, USA). The Shapiro-Wilk test was performed to all datasets to test for normality of distribution. Baseline differences between both groups were assessed with independent sample t-test. Interactions were tested by a 2-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) where the group (intervention and control groups) was used as the between subject variable and time (PRE and POST measurements) as the within subject variable. In case of significance, post hoc comparisons with Bonferroni corrections were applied. For ordinal parameters and not-normally distributed data, a Friedman’s ANOVA was used. Statistical significance was set at the level of
3. Results
From all participants that were included and randomized, 10 participants from the intervention group (age: 64.4 ± 4.1 years; height: 171.8 ± 5.1 cm; weight: 86.6 ± 8.7 kg) and 11 participants from the control group (age: 63.1 ± 5.6 years; height: 168.6 ± 13.8 cm; weight: 76.0 ± 15.7 kg) were considered for statistical analyses. Functional outcome data did not violate normality of distribution as assessed by Shapiro-Wilk test (all
3.1. TUG Test
At PRE, the intervention group did not differ from the control group when performing the TUG (
[figures omitted; refer to PDF]
3.2. FSST Test
At PRE, the intervention group did not differ from the control group when performing the FSST (
3.3. Single- and Dual-Task Walking
At PRE, no significant differences were found between both groups for any of the assessed gait parameters (all
For the single-task fast-paced walking condition, there were no significant interaction effects for gait speed (
For swing-time variability parameter, there was a significant time × group interaction (
For cognitive performance, there were no differences between the two groups at PRE for neither the subtracted numbers (all
3.4. Single- and Dual-Task Postural Control
At PRE, no significant differences in any of the postural parameters were found between both groups (all
There were no significant time × group interactions for any of the COP parameters (total sway path, frequency, and amplitude) in both mediolateral and anteroposterior directions, as well as in postural single- and dual-task conditions (all
For the cognitive performance assessed during the serial threes subtraction task, there were no differences between the two groups at PRE, neither for the numbers calculated (
4. Discussion
The principal finding of the current study is that two months of additional nonphysical training resulted in better functional and cognitive rehabilitation outcomes in patients with unilateral total hip replacement than with the standard rehabilitation program alone. More specifically, our study supports the feasibility of AO + MI combined with kinesthetic MI in order to accelerate and improve the acute phase of rehabilitation (up to two months after the surgery). The intervention group had better outcomes than the control subjects in physical tests that measured functional mobility and stepping over obstacles, tasks that were actually part of the mental training. In contrast, no differences between the intervention and control groups were found in tasks that were not mentally trained such as static balance. This underlines the task specificity of the mental training intervention.
There is increasing evidence that the combination of AO and MI (AO + MI) leads to greater brain activity in motor areas than either AO or MI alone (for reviews, see [23, 24]). However, the functional implications of these observations for rehabilitation are not clear, yet, as the applicability and effectiveness of AO + MI was scarcely investigated in functional settings. Some of the first studies that concentrated on behavioral outcomes were conducted in the sports domain, where AO + MI was initially entitled “video-guided imagery.” In this context, it was demonstrated that training with AO + MI was more effective in order to learn golf putting [32] or increase elbow flexors strength [33] than the same training with MI. Similarly, Sun et al. [34] demonstrated better rehabilitative outcome in a group practicing concurrent AO + MI than a group that first observed and then imagined the same actions. However, less clear results were obtained after nonphysical balance training with comparable outcomes when using either AO + MI or MI alone [35].
In the present study, we initially wanted to evaluate the influence of AO + MI in a clinical setting by comparing AO + MI with the normal rehabilitation procedure. However, as subjects indicated difficulties to watch and at the same time “feel the sensations that arise from doing the task” (kinesthetic motor imagery), we presented first the video and asked them to do AO + MI followed by a period in which subjects should close their eyes and imagine the sensations that arise from doing the task (MI). Although this kind of mental training intervention intermingles AO + MI and MI, and consequently, the influence of each single mental simulation technique cannot be differentiated, we considered it most important to apply a motivating, comprehensive, and feasible mental training intervention. Furthermore, out of the 405 screened patients, we expected to recruit a higher number of “suitable” participants for the current study that would have allowed the comparison of the current two groups with an additional group performing solely MI. However, although this was not possible, the current approach nevertheless allows the comparison between the standardized (best practice) rehabilitation procedures with the same rehabilitation process amplified with mental simulation of locomotor tasks.
From the behavioral perspective, the current mental simulation training of locomotor tasks in THA patients was highly efficient (a) to counteract surgery-induced impairments that became obvious in the control group (TUG; gait speed and swing-time variability during dual-task walking) and (b) to even improve some motor-cognitive skills already two months after surgery (FSST; cognitive performance during postural tasks and during walking). Another interesting point is the task specificity of the mental simulation approach. Only in tasks that were part of the mental training, participants demonstrated improved task performance compared to the control group whereas in tasks that were not mentally trained, no differences between groups were obvious. Furthermore, the AO + MI training-related effects were mostly seen in the more demanding tasks, such as dual-task walking, where participants needed to perform a secondary cognitive task while walking. The present results are in general agreement with one of our previous studies where nonphysical training led to enhanced performance only in the most attention-demanding walking condition [4].
The underlying mechanisms of mental simulation programs are believed to rely on overlapping brain areas during motor execution and MI as well as during motor execution and AO [14, 22]. In this context, Jeannerod postulated the well-accepted hypothesis that “the motor system is part of a simulation network that is activated under a variety of conditions in relation to action, either self-intended or observed from other individuals” [36]. Recent studies proposed the combination of AO and MI (AO + MI) as this combined approach was found to elicit greater [25, 37] and in some cases even oversummative activity [27, 28] compared to the sum of brain activity during independent AO and independent MI. These findings were interpreted as evidence that neural correlates of MI and AO might merge rather than compete with each other [24]. In this sense, the involvement of distinct brain structures that can solely be activated by either MI or AO [38, 39] would provide supplementary and complementary activation compared to one or the other modality alone. In addition, the activation of common AO and MI brain regions, mostly premotor and motor areas, may induce an overlapping activity [22, 40] that would further augment brain activity with AO + MI. Concerning the execution of AO + MI, it was suggested that the mental simulation of a movement would be facilitated by visual guidance, which allows the participants to update their internal representations. In addition, the visual stimulus generated by AO may help participants to create a visual image, allowing them to focus on kinesthetic modality [41], known to be more effective to activate the motor neural processes than visual imagery only [42]. In line with this, patients in our intervention group reported to be able to perform better kinesthetic MI directly after having seen a video of the task.
With advanced age mental imagery capacities can be altered, especially the temporal features of the imagined action [43]. Our training therefore started with relatively short videos, and the task exposure time was progressively increased. It has been argued that the lack of sensory feedback might be one of the main reasons to explain the decreased ability to perform motor imagery with age [44]. This argues in favor of using AO + MI approaches, since AO may help old adults to compensate for MI ability deficiencies. This is further supported by the finding in elderly subjects that revealed better effectiveness of AO + MI in activating brain areas, than either AO or MI alone [25].
5. Conclusion
The present study highlights the benefits of AO + MI interventions for rehabilitation purposes, especially when participants are immobilized after surgery. The results demonstrate that the integrated AO + MI approach was an efficient tool to enhance the functional rehabilitation outcomes of postsurgical orthopedic patients. Remarkably, gains were shown to be exclusively improving the tasks that were actually mentally simulated during the training so that future nonphysical training studies should take into account this task specificity.
In conclusion, AO + MI approaches represent an affordable, safe, and not very time-consuming tool to optimize individual’s rehabilitation process. Furthermore, it allows starting the rehabilitation process in the early phase following surgery, when the patient is not able to perform a regular physical training. Once mastered, short sessions of AO + MI performed at home on a regular basis can significantly improve the outcomes of the rehabilitation process, even in frail populations.
Disclosure
An earlier version of this work was partly presented at the 21st Annual Congress of the European College of Sport Science (ECSS), Vienna, Austria, in July 2016.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors have no conflicts to report.
Acknowledgments
This study was a part of the research project PANGeA (Physical Activity and Nutrition for Quality Ageing), supported by a grant Cross-border Cooperation Program Slovenia-Italy 2007–2013, ID no. CB147, from European Regional Development Fund. The authors would like to thank the participants in the study, all members of the research team as well as students of Applied Kinesiology of University of Primorska who contributed to the smooth undertaking of the study, especially Kaja Teraž for the help with data collection. The authors thank Natalia Zigmund for the daily and initial screening of the patients and all other involved orthopedic specialists and staff of Valdoltra Orthopaedic Hospital.
[1] R. Pišot, U. Marusic, G. Biolo, S. Mazzucco, S. Lazzer, B. Grassi, C. Reggiani, L. Toniolo, P. E. di Prampero, A. Passaro, M. Narici, S. Mohammed, J. Rittweger, M. Gasparini, M. Gabrijelčič Blenkuš, B. Šimunič, "Greater loss in muscle mass and function but smaller metabolic alterations in older compared with younger men following 2 wk of bed rest and recovery," Journal of Applied Physiology, vol. 120 no. 8, pp. 922-929, DOI: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00858.2015, 2016.
[2] E. E. Devore, T. G. Fong, E. R. Marcantonio, E. M. Schmitt, T. G. Travison, R. N. Jones, S. K. Inouye, "Prediction of long-term cognitive decline following postoperative delirium in older adults," The Journals of Gerontology: Series A, vol. 72 no. 12, pp. 1697-1702, DOI: 10.1093/gerona/glx030, 2017.
[3] J. Y. Reginster, P. Gillet, C. Gosset, "Secular increase in the incidence of hip fractures in Belgium between 1984 and 1996: need for a concerted public health strategy," Bulletin of the World Health Organization, vol. 79 no. 10, pp. 942-946, 2001.
[4] U. Marusic, V. Kavcic, B. Giordani, M. Gerževič, R. Meeusen, R. Pišot, "Computerized spatial navigation training during 14 days of bed rest in healthy older adult men: effect on gait performance," Psychology and Aging, vol. 30 no. 2, pp. 334-340, DOI: 10.1037/pag0000021, 2015.
[5] M. Bassolino, M. Bove, M. Jacono, L. Fadiga, T. Pozzo, "Functional effect of short-term immobilization: kinematic changes and recovery on reaching-to-grasp," Neuroscience, vol. 215, pp. 127-134, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.04.019, 2012.
[6] S. Facchini, M. Romani, M. Tinazzi, S. M. Aglioti, "Time-related changes of excitability of the human motor system contingent upon immobilisation of the ring and little fingers," Clinical Neurophysiology, vol. 113 no. 3, pp. 367-375, DOI: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00009-3, 2002.
[7] R. Huber, M. F. Ghilardi, M. Massimini, F. Ferrarelli, B. A. Riedner, M. J. Peterson, G. Tononi, "Arm immobilization causes cortical plastic changes and locally decreases sleep slow wave activity," Nature Neuroscience, vol. 9 no. 9, pp. 1169-1176, DOI: 10.1038/nn1758, 2006.
[8] L. Avanzino, M. Bassolino, T. Pozzo, M. Bove, "Use-dependent hemispheric balance," Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 31 no. 9, pp. 3423-3428, DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4893-10.2011, 2011.
[9] J. Liepert, M. Tegenthoff, J.-P. Malin, "Changes of cortical motor area size during immobilization," Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Electromyography and Motor Control, vol. 97 no. 6, pp. 382-386, DOI: 10.1016/0924-980X(95)00194-P, 1995.
[10] T. Mulder, S. de Vries, S. Zijlstra, "Observation, imagination and execution of an effortful movement: more evidence for a central explanation of motor imagery," Experimental Brain Research, vol. 163 no. 3, pp. 344-351, DOI: 10.1007/s00221-004-2179-4, 2005.
[11] S. Grospretre, C. Ruffino, F. Lebon, "Motor imagery and cortico-spinal excitability: a review," European Journal of Sport Science, vol. 16 no. 3, pp. 317-324, DOI: 10.1080/17461391.2015.1024756, 2016.
[12] D. Ertelt, S. Small, A. Solodkin, C. Dettmers, A. McNamara, F. Binkofski, G. Buccino, "Action observation has a positive impact on rehabilitation of motor deficits after stroke," NeuroImage, vol. 36, pp. T164-T173, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.043, 2007.
[13] E. Sarasso, M. Gemma, F. Agosta, M. Filippi, R. Gatti, "Action observation training to improve motor function recovery: a systematic review," Archives of Physiotherapy, vol. 5 no. 1,DOI: 10.1186/s40945-015-0013-x, 2015.
[14] M. Jeannerod, "The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery," Behavioral and Brain Sciences, vol. 17 no. 2, pp. 187-202, DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X00034026, 1994.
[15] A. J. Butler, S. J. Page, "Mental practice with motor imagery: evidence for motor recovery and cortical reorganization after stroke," Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, vol. 87 no. 12,DOI: 10.1016/j.apmr.2006.08.326, 2006.
[16] M. Franceschini, M. Agosti, A. Cantagallo, P. Sale, M. Mancuso, G. Buccino, "Mirror neurons: action observation treatment as a tool in stroke rehabilitation," European Journal of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, vol. 46 no. 4, pp. 517-523, 2010.
[17] D. Caligiore, M. Mustile, G. Spalletta, G. Baldassarre, "Action observation and motor imagery for rehabilitation in Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review and an integrative hypothesis," Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, vol. 72, pp. 210-222, DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.005, 2017.
[18] N. Schott, M. O. Frenkel, H. Korbus, K. L. Francis, "Mental practice in orthopedic rehabilitation: where, what, and how? A case report," Movement & Sport Sciences - Science & Motricité, vol. 82, pp. 93-103, DOI: 10.1051/sm/2013099, 2013.
[19] J. H. Villafañe, C. Pirali, M. Isgrò, C. Vanti, R. Buraschi, S. Negrini, "Effects of action observation therapy in patients recovering from total hip arthroplasty arthroplasty: a prospective clinical trial," Journal of Chiropractic Medicine, vol. 15 no. 4, pp. 229-234, DOI: 10.1016/j.jcm.2016.08.011, 2016.
[20] M. Bassolino, M. Campanella, M. Bove, T. Pozzo, L. Fadiga, "Training the motor cortex by observing the actions of others during immobilization," Cerebral Cortex, vol. 24 no. 12, pp. 3268-3276, DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bht190, 2014.
[21] A. Meugnot, N. F. Agbangla, Y. Almecija, L. Toussaint, "Motor imagery practice may compensate for the slowdown of sensorimotor processes induced by short-term upper-limb immobilization," Psychological Research, vol. 79 no. 3, pp. 489-499, DOI: 10.1007/s00426-014-0577-1, 2015.
[22] J. Grèzes, J. Decety, "Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: a meta-analysis," Human Brain Mapping, vol. 12 no. 1,DOI: 10.1002/1097-0193(200101)12:1<1::AID-HBM10>3.0.CO;2-V, 2001.
[23] S. Vogt, F. D. Rienzo, C. Collet, A. Collins, A. Guillot, "Multiple roles of motor imagery during action observation," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 7,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00807, 2013.
[24] D. L. Eaves, M. Riach, P. S. Holmes, D. J. Wright, "Motor imagery during action observation: a brief review of evidence, theory and future research opportunities," Frontiers in Neuroscience, vol. 10,DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00514, 2016.
[25] A. Mouthon, J. Ruffieux, M. Wälchli, M. Keller, W. Taube, "Task-dependent changes of corticospinal excitability during observation and motor imagery of balance tasks," Neuroscience, vol. 303, pp. 535-543, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.031, 2015.
[26] D. J. Wright, S. A. McCormick, J. Williams, P. S. Holmes, "Viewing instructions accompanying action observation modulate corticospinal excitability," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 10,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00017, 2016.
[27] W. Taube, M. Mouthon, C. Leukel, H. M. Hoogewoud, J. M. Annoni, M. Keller, "Brain activity during observation and motor imagery of different balance tasks: an fMRI study," Cortex, vol. 64, pp. 102-114, DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.09.022, 2015.
[28] M. Sakamoto, T. Muraoka, N. Mizuguchi, K. Kanosue, "Combining observation and imagery of an action enhances human corticospinal excitability," Neuroscience Research, vol. 65 no. 1, pp. 23-27, DOI: 10.1016/j.neures.2009.05.003, 2009.
[29] D. Podsiadlo, S. Richardson, "The timed “Up & Go”: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons," Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 39 no. 2, pp. 142-148, DOI: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1991.tb01616.x, 1991.
[30] M. Jakovljević, "Časovno merjeni test vstani in pojdi: pregled literature," Fizioterapija, vol. 21 no. 1, pp. 38-47, 2013.
[31] N. Sonc, D. Rugelj, "Normativne vrednosti časovno merjenega testa korakanja v štirih kvadratih," Fizioterapija, vol. 22 no. 1, pp. 31-37, 2014.
[32] D. Smith, P. Holmes, "The effect of imagery modality on golf putting performance," Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, vol. 26 no. 3, pp. 385-395, DOI: 10.1123/jsep.26.3.385, 2004.
[33] C. J. Wright, D. Smith, "The effect of PETTLEP imagery on strength performance," International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, vol. 7 no. 1, pp. 18-31, DOI: 10.1080/1612197X.2009.9671890, 2009.
[34] Y. Sun, W. Wei, Z. Luo, H. Gan, X. Hu, "Improving motor imagery practice with synchronous action observation in stroke patients," Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, vol. 23 no. 4, pp. 245-253, DOI: 10.1080/10749357.2016.1141472, 2016.
[35] W. Taube, M. Lorch, S. Zeiter, M. Keller, "Non-physical practice improves task performance in an unstable, perturbed environment: motor imagery and observational balance training," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 8,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00972, 2014.
[36] M. Jeannerod, "Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition," NeuroImage, vol. 14 no. 1, pp. S103-S109, DOI: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832, 2001.
[37] K. L. Macuga, S. H. Frey, "Neural representations involved in observed, imagined, and imitated actions are dissociable and hierarchically organized," NeuroImage, vol. 59 no. 3, pp. 2798-2807, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.083, 2012.
[38] F. Filimon, J. D. Nelson, D. J. Hagler, M. I. Sereno, "Human cortical representations for reaching: mirror neurons for execution, observation, and imagery," NeuroImage, vol. 37 no. 4, pp. 1315-1328, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.008, 2007.
[39] B. Lorey, T. Naumann, S. Pilgramm, C. Petermann, M. Bischoff, K. Zentgraf, R. Stark, D. Vaitl, J. Munzert, "How equivalent are the action execution, imagery, and observation of intransitive movements? Revisiting the concept of somatotopy during action simulation," Brain and Cognition, vol. 81 no. 1, pp. 139-150, DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.09.011, 2013.
[40] S. Hétu, M. Grégoire, A. Saimpont, M. P. Coll, F. Eugène, P. E. Michon, P. L. Jackson, "The neural network of motor imagery: an ALE meta-analysis," Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, vol. 37 no. 5, pp. 930-949, DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.017, 2013.
[41] P. Holmes, C. Calmels, "A neuroscientific review of imagery and observation use in sport," Journal of Motor Behavior, vol. 40 no. 5, pp. 433-445, DOI: 10.3200/JMBR.40.5.433-445, 2008.
[42] C. M. Stinear, W. D. Byblow, M. Steyvers, O. Levin, S. P. Swinnen, "Kinesthetic, but not visual, motor imagery modulates corticomotor excitability," Experimental Brain Research, vol. 168 no. 1-2, pp. 157-164, DOI: 10.1007/s00221-005-0078-y, 2006.
[43] P. Personnier, A. Kubicki, D. Laroche, C. Papaxanthis, "Temporal features of imagined locomotion in normal aging," Neuroscience Letters, vol. 476 no. 3, pp. 146-149, DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.04.017, 2010.
[44] X. Skoura, P. Personnier, A. Vinter, T. Pozzo, C. Papaxanthis, "Decline in motor prediction in elderly subjects: right versus left arm differences in mentally simulated motor actions," Cortex, vol. 44 no. 9, pp. 1271-1278, DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.07.008, 2008.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright © 2018 Uros Marusic et al. This work is licensed under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
This study aimed at determining whether the combination of action observation and motor imagery (AO + MI) of locomotor tasks could positively affect rehabilitation outcome after hip replacement surgery. Of initially 405 screened participants, 21 were randomly split into intervention group (
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details

1 Institute for Kinesiology Research, Science and Research Centre Koper, Koper, Slovenia; Department of Health Sciences, Alma Mater Europaea-ECM, Maribor, Slovenia
2 EA4660, C3S Culture Sport Health Society, Université de Franche-Comté, Besançon, France
3 Institute for Kinesiology Research, Science and Research Centre Koper, Koper, Slovenia
4 Valdoltra Orthopaedic Hospital, Ankaran, Slovenia
5 Department of Medicine, Movement and Sport Sciences, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland