This work is licensed under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
1. Aging and Mental Diseases
Life expectancy has increased in most of the world’s population, mainly in developed countries. According to data from the United Nations, the proportion of persons over 60 years will be 20% of the total population in 2050, doubling the current elderly population [1]. While the mechanisms underlying the biological process of aging are not completely understood, in general terms aging is characterized by a progressive decrease of physiological function and an increased vulnerability to death [2].
Regarding cognitive and neural changes, aging involves a progressive decline in specific brain networks and cognitive processes, such as memory encoding and retrieval, and executive control functions, all of which are progressively impaired [3]. Executive functions are responsible for flexible and adaptive behaviours [4]; in other words, they are “the means by which our brain optimizes the flexible use of limited cognitive resources to currently prioritized tasks” [5]. Memory, on the other hand, constitutes the ability of living organisms to retain and utilize acquired information. Both memory and executive functions include subprocesses that are differently affected by aging. For instance, long-term memory is usually impaired in elder people, but the retrieval of word meaning is not [6]. Memory and executive functions are interrelated cognitive processes, and both play a critical role in complex situations of everyday life [4]. Executive functions usually control acts of remembering: for example, guiding event retrieval by recalling the sources of the information that subjects aim to remember [6, 7]. Along this line, memory decline in aging could stem from a disruption of the executive functions that influence memory, and not necessarily to a disruption in memory itself [6, 8]: if a subject tries to fruitlessly recall a friend’s name, it could imply damage both in memory storage and in executive functions, by being unable to focus on the process of retrieval.
The relationship between cognitive function and neuronal connectivity has been studied in several recent investigations. Particularly in aging, an increase of incoherent function in specific brain networks diminishes the efficiency and efficacy of information processing in the brain [3]. One of the networks that have been reported to be altered in old age include the default mode network (DMN) [9, 10], which is associated to mind wandering or spontaneous thought, self-reflective thinking, inner speech, incidental self-processing, stimulus-independent thought, momentary attentional lapses, task-unrelated thought, and autobiographical memory, among other processes. Other networks associated with high cognitive functions, like the attentional and task-positive networks (TPN), which include the dorsal and ventral frontoparietal attentional networks [11–13], are also modified through aging [3, 14–17]. On the other hand, lifestyle choices across the lifespan contribute positively to the process of aging [18–20]. Chronic stress, anxiety, or depression, for example, are common mental health conditions [21, 22] that accelerate many of the detrimental aspects of aging, such as cognitive decline, memory loss, and disruption of the DMN and attentional networks [23, 24].
Some reports indicate that abnormal functioning of the DMN could predispose individuals to mental diseases, like depression, anxiety, attention deficit, and posttraumatic stress disorders [23]. All of these factors, in the long term, affect the quality of life in the elderly. Prolonged periods of untreated mental illness over the lifespan, together with the neurological decline associated with aging, may have a detrimental effect on the quality of life of the elderly person. Our lifestyle not only affects our present mental state and brain function, but also influences how aging unfolds [18–20]. In this context, developing strategies that decrease the damaging aspects of aging, as well as of neurodegenerative and neuropsychiatric disorders, is a priority for promoting healthy aging. Enhancing cerebral resilience through pharmacological and nonpharmacological treatments may represent one approach. In this sense, we propose the regulation of the DMN and TPN as a means of promoting neuroprotection and optimizing aging.
2. Default Mode Network, Aging, and Mental Disease
The DMN is a member of a set of coherent fluctuations of brain activity called resting-state networks (RSN), which are activated when an individual is not engaged in any activity or superior cognitive process [25–27]. The RSN were initially described in functional magnetic resonance imaging studies, in which the interest was not the response to stimuli, but the brain activity that occurred when the subject was at rest [27, 28]. The importance of these networks has been discovered during the last two decades, with several interesting findings, such as brain energy consumption at rest. It has been reported that, in an average adult human, the intrinsic activity of the brain at rest represents 20% of all energy consumed, much more than other fundamental organs, such as the heart or liver [29]. Interestingly, when the type of brain activity changes, for example, while performing a task, energy consumption is slightly greater than the resting state (5% or less [29, 30]). Finally, from the total energy utilized by the brain in the resting state during wakefulness, 80% is used in neuronal firing and glutamate and GABA recycling [31]. These data suggest that the majority of brain energy consumption is related to the maintenance of basal activity and neuronal communication, which probably include the RSN, among others. In the same vein, numerous studies propose a fundamental role of the RSN, and especially the DMN, with normal brain function across the lifespan. A relationship between the RSN and various neurological and psychiatric conditions has been established [32–36], as have changes in RSN patterns during normal aging [37] and in mild cognitive impairment [38]. Finally, genetic studies have shown that RSN patterns have a hereditary component [38], and animal studies suggest that RSN are phylogenetically conserved in primates [38] and rats [39].
Among the different RSN, the DMN has gained special prominence due to its attenuation during active tasks [40]. The DMN is characterized by the synchronous activation of several separated regions in the brain, including the medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, precuneus, inferior parietal lobule, and inferolateral temporal cortex [23, 27, 41].
The DMN shows a high level of simultaneous activation during rest, while their activity diminishes during the performance of goal-directed tasks [27, 42, 43]. These results come from a classical approach that compares activity patterns within, and interconnectivity between, DMN brain regions during rest versus when commencing a goal-directed activity that modulates this network. However, while most studies indicate that activity is similar between rest and task, in general DMN activity seems to be attenuated rather than extinguished when we are on task versus off task. This attenuation becomes an activation transition between DMN and TPN when subjects change from a rest condition (passive fixation with eyes open) to common
Decomposition of connectivity data, in both humans and macaque models, shows that one extreme of the principal gradient of the brain’s connectivity is constituted by regions associated with sensory and motor functions, while the other extreme is constituted by the cortical regions that define the DMN [50]. Hence, DMN cortical structures show greater geodesic distance from the sensory and motor cortices. This enables the DMN to process information that is not domain specific to immediate sensory inputs, constituting a hub of multimodal representational information [50].
The DMN is also known to support self-referential processing, being the neurophysiological correlate of autobiographical memory, self-reflective thinking [27, 40, 51], envisioning of future events, considering the thoughts and perspectives of others [27, 52], and mind wandering [53]. All of these cognitive processes, according to studies that relate the activity of DMN to metacognitive processes, could be important in building and updating internal models of the world, based on memories about oneself or others. Despite the significance of the activation of this network, the function of the DMN is not fully understood. Previous studies indicate that the inability to suppress the DMN can lead to attentional lapses [54], such as episodes related to decreased performance in attentional tasks. These episodes have been associated with behavioral variability across groups, and this variability, in turn, has been proposed to be a marker of mind wandering [55]. Furthermore, depression, anxiety, stress, and attention deficit disorder are conditions characterized by a diminishing state of vigilance and an increase of inner dialogue toward negative ruminant thoughts [23]. Something distinctive occurs in neuropsychiatric and neurological diseases, such as schizophrenia and epilepsy, which are not always associated with an inability to suppress DMN activity [41, 56–60]. Neuropsychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia, appear to be related to antagonistic activity during tasks that demand attention [61]. In Alzheimer’s disease, on the other hand, the phenomenon is accompanied by a decreased activity and connectivity of the DMN, probably due to a decreased metabolism and physiological disruptions from plaque deposition [32, 62]. In task-related performance, older individuals exhibit a reduced deactivation of the DMN or an increase in its baseline activity [62–65], indicating that an inability to inhibit or shift from the default mode to task-activated attentional networks results in lower cognitive performance [63, 65]. Therefore, the balance of the activation and deactivation of the DMN appears to be important in maintaining healthy brain function, including executive functions, memory, and attention. All these data have been used to propose the DMN as a candidate biomarker of mental diseases [41]. We suggest that DMN activity could constitute a model for understanding the dynamics of cognitive decline in elderly people. In addition, we suggest that its regulation could be achieved by specific meditation practices and used as a way to promote optimal aging and prevent neurological and neuropsychiatric diseases.
3. Meta-Awareness: Attention Network vs. DMN
The DMN is a network with coherent activation during periods of rest, i.e., when brain activity related to attention is spontaneously oriented to internal thoughts, without awareness of theme [66, 67]. Consequently, in healthy individuals DMN activity correlates negatively with the goal-oriented TPN [44, 68]. The latter, opposite networks, are preferentially active in attention-demanding tasks [69], when the brain is focused on a cognitive task (internal or external).
The attentional network includes the lateral prefrontal cortex, premotor cortex, lateral parietal regions, occipital regions, anterior cingulate cortex, and insula [67]. From the point of view of cognitive neuroscience, the attentional system could be divided into three subsystems that perform different but interrelated functions: (i) alerting, (ii) orientation, and (iii) conflict monitoring [70]. These divisions were established by Posner and Petersen in the early ‘90s, based on behavioral studies of normal adults or patients with different forms of brain injury. The original review suggested that this classification is based on three basic concepts of the attention system: first, the brain’s attention system is anatomically separated from other processing systems, such as the motor or sensory systems; second, attention involves diverse anatomical areas of the brain; and third, the areas involved in attention perform different functions and can be specified in cognitive terms [70, 71]. This classification has evolved to include other elements, such as self-regulation [71], but the three basic elements are considered fundamental for the understanding of how attention works [12, 71]. Following this classification, the following functions were determined: (i) alerting or vigilance of an impending stimulus, which involves the thalamus and the right frontal and right parietal cortex, maintaining a state of high sensitivity to incoming stimuli [72, 73]; (ii) orientation or selection of relevant information from multiple stimuli, which involves the superior parietal cortex, temporal parietal junction, frontal eye fields, and superior colliculus [71–73]; and (iii) monitoring and resolution of conflict between computations in different neural areas (executive attention) [74], which includes the anterior cingulate cortex, lateral ventral cortex, prefrontal cortex, and basal ganglia. This network is also especially important in the detection of cognitive, homeostatic, or emotional subjective events, and it provides signals to the executive network to act in accordance with the current objective [71, 72]. However, when the attentional stream is abruptly interrupted and lacks goal-directed stimuli, for instance, during mind-wandering episodes, the DMN shows a high degree of functional connectivity between regions. This default activity has been related to states in which a subject is awake and alert, but not actively involved in an attention-demanding or goal-directed task [27, 43]. This suggests the existence of an interplay between the DMN and attentional networks’ activities. Success in goal-oriented activities seems not to necessarily require the absence of mind wandering, but the individual’s ability to detect when the mind is not on task, in order to reorient attention back [67]. Therefore, the activation of the circuitry associated with attention is critical not only in focusing on an object of attention, but also in noticing when the attention is focused or not on a particular task. In this way, an individual can detect when the mind is disengaged from a task and starts to wander. This process is called meta-awareness (“metaconsciousness” or “metacognitive awareness”) and is defined as one’s explicit knowledge of the current contents of thought, feelings, and perceptions [75, 76]. Thus, as meta-awareness is the mental ability that strengthens the capacity to be aware of the internal and external world, it could be used to intentionally initiate, direct, and/or sustain attentional processes [75].
Interestingly, the anticorrelation of the DMN and TPN, and the nature of TPN, suggests a simple strategy to control the DMN, potentiated by meta-awareness. This could be a useful intervention as it implies a modification of brain activity through an individual’s regulation of their behavior.
4. Meditation: Voluntary Control of Brain Networks
One of the simplest techniques that alter patterns of brain activity of the DMN and TPN is meditation [41]. Meditation can be defined as a form of mental training that aims to improve psychological capacities, such as attentional and emotional self-regulation, perspective taking, and meta-awareness [74, 75, 77]. This includes several methods, organized under the concept of contemplative practices [75]. As Dorjee suggests, contemplative practices could be defined as several techniques that promote a metacognitive self-regulatory capacity of the mind, modulated by motivational, intentional, and contextual factors of contemplative practices [78]. This ability is based on the voluntary control of attentional focus, and it involves maintaining attention on the immediate experience, away from distractions such as self-referential thinking and mind wandering [79, 80]. This allows introspective awareness of mental processes and behavior, which is indispensable in the self-regulatory processes that support well-being [78].
Among these practices, mantra meditation from the Yoga tradition, Shamata and Vipassana meditation from the Buddhist tradition, self-inquiry from Vedanta, or secular variations like mindfulness or loving-kindness utilize several techniques and elements, like sounds or words, breath, body scan, external objects such as the flame of a candle, or simply observation of the present experience [75]. In general terms, all meditation techniques include the cultivation of focused, relaxed, and steady attention on the immediate experience in a state of nonjudgmental acceptance [42, 74, 75].
One of the most interesting classifications of the contemplative practices in neuroscience divides meditation in three super families, based on their primary cognitive mechanisms: (i) the constructive family, (ii) the deconstructive family, and (iii) the attentional family (Figure 1). Although they all use attention as a common ground, the first two are less related to this aspect of cognition. The first family is associated with repairing a maladaptive self-schema (perspective taking and reappraisal) and the second with exploration of the processes of perception, emotion, and cognition (self-inquiry) [75]. The third family is essentially concerned with the control of attention.
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]The attentional family potentiates the capacities to manipulate the orientation and aperture of attention and to disengage and reorient attention where it is needed. Hence, this family of practices could be summarized as focusing on training the ability to increase meta-awareness and diminish experiential fusion [75]. The attentional family is divided into two subcategories. The first is called focused attention (FA), and it involves attending to a chosen sensory or mental object (such as sounds, words, breath, images, thoughts, or even feelings), at the exclusion of everything else [41, 67]. In this practice, the participant remains attentive, and each time the mind begins to wander the person is trained to guide the focus of attention back with nonjudgmental awareness (Figure 1). The second type is called open monitoring (OM), which implies directing the attention to whatever arises in the field of consciousness; an unspecific focus is proposed for this technique, and the practitioner is instructed to be aware of everything that arises from the internal or external worlds [23, 41] (Figure 1).
Both FA and OM influence DMN activity [42]. Brewer et al. investigated the impact of FA meditation (concentration), OM meditation (choiceless awareness), and loving-kindness meditation (a member of the constructive family) on the DMN, showing that, in the three types of meditations, the main nodes of the DMN were deactivated in experienced meditators. The authors also found a strong coupling between the posterior cingulate, dorsal anterior cingulate, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices, regions involved in self-monitoring and cognitive control during meditation [42]. In the case of FA meditation, the way attentional networks activate in response to the wandering mind in experienced meditators is known with some detail [67]. A follow-up resting state study has revealed that connectivity in attentional networks are directly associated with hours of practice, indicating that training in FA increases the attentional control of the practitioner in everyday life [67, 81].
OM is quite different from FA, because the practice is to observe thoughts and sensations while remaining unreactive, contributing to reduced emotional reactivity. The goal of OM is to understand the constant impermanence of reality while maintaining present awareness; some amount of mind wandering is allowed, but it is crucial to keep one’s self disengaged from one’s own train of thoughts. OM can be divided into two subtypes: one that focuses attention fully on the object (object-oriented OM) and those that focus attention on the quality of awareness during the experience (awareness-oriented OM) [75]. Investigations reporting the effects of OM meditation on the DMN have reported variable results, probably due to a lack of discrimination of these subtypes. While some publications indicate that OM, like FA, diminishes DMN activity [42, 81], it has also been reported that it results in an increased activation of the precuneus, a DMN hub, in contrast to FA [23, 82]. However, in both types of meditation a sustained effect in the control of the DMN occurs, beyond the period of practice, suggesting long-lasting effects in daily life [83]. Therefore, due to the fact that meditation, in general, is a mental training, it involves attention and the ability to maintain focus on a particular object. The effect of both types of meditation (OM and FA) could be interpreted as an increase in brain efficiency, promoted by their intensive training that improves the sustained attention and the cognitive control mechanisms as shown in several works [83, 84].
In experienced meditators of FA (as with OM), active control of task-positive brain regions are likewise observed, for example, in conflict monitoring, working memory, cognitive control, and emotional regulation, particularly through massive self-regulation in frontoparietal and insular areas. These regions and cognitive processes are relevant in cognitive decline, as well as in neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders [42, 82].
During selective attention, 7-14 Hz alpha rhythms are modulated in early sensory cortices, and this has been associated with an improved filtering of inputs to these brain areas [85, 86]. In attentional tasks, such as those guided by visual or somatic stimuli, a topographical distribution of alpha power reduction in the specific subregion associated with the task has been reported, as well as an increment in alpha power in unattended locations. This correlates with better performance in the task [87]. In meditation, particularly FA on body and breath-related sensations, enhanced modulation of 7-14 Hz alpha power is observed, which improves with increased hours of practice [88]. This modulation plays a key role in filtering inputs to the primary sensory neocortex and organizing the flow of sensory information in the brain [88, 89]. This top-down alpha rhythm modulation depends on the contextual cue, allowing the practitioner to better detect and regulate the wandering mind from its somatic focus, and could also be used to modulate pain sensation or compete with internal rumination in the case of depression [89].
Finally, it is not just the voluntary regulation of network connectivity that is associated with meditation, but also structural changes in the brain. A systematic review and meta-analysis [90] reports that certain brain structures are consistently altered when comparing meditation practitioners with control subjects. The majority of these regions are part of the DMN, as highlighted in a recent review [74]. Interestingly, there exists an anterior-posterior axis in the direction of the structural changes, with frontal and temporal areas reported to be thicker and parietal and occipital areas reported to be thinner in meditation practitioners versus controls [91]. Regions thicker in practitioners include areas involved in self-monitoring and integration of cognitive and emotional cues [92], reward processing, conflict monitoring, and self-regulation [93–95], memory (re)consolidation [88], and visual attention and perception [90, 91], in addition to exteroceptive and interoceptive body (meta)awareness [96]. Regions thinner in practitioners include areas involved in the processing of one’s body in a spatial context [97], higher-order body image, self-related processing, and attentional shifting [91, 98]. However, it is not straightforward to interpret the direction of the alterations found. For instance, training that increases cognitive functions has been associated with decreased grey matter volumes [91, 99], putatively because of selective elimination of synapses [91, 100].
5. Integration of Basic and Clinical Research and Contemplative Practices in the Elderly
Despite the growing evidence, in basic and clinical research, focused on the study of the brain mechanisms underlying contemplative practices and their connection with clinically relevant outcomes, this body of research still faces major challenges with respect to the study, design, and practice classifications [101]. However, the effect of contemplative practices on network organization, specifically the DMN and TPN, has been related to neurological and neuropsychiatric diseases particularly relevant during senescence. If we consider that one-third of Alzheimer’s disease is related to modifiable risk factors [102], such as certain negative psychoaffective states [103, 104], reducing this risk factor in 25% of individuals could prevent approximately three million cases of the disease worldwide [105]. Thus, it is reasonable to hypothesize that mental training for cognitive and affective regulation through contemplative practices has an important role in preventing cognitive decline, as well as neurocognitive disorders [74, 106, 107]. The effects of meditation on preserving age-related changes in cognitive functioning has been well established, specifically in areas such as the thalamus, insula, amygdala, hippocampus, and anterior cingulate cortex [108, 109].
Yoga and mindfulness meditation are two of the most studied contemplative practices, whose effects on aging we will detail below.
5.1. Yoga
Classic Yoga, or Raja Yoga, is a progressive method which includes different groups of practices aimed toward self-regulation [77]. This starts with ethics (when interacting with others), self-discipline, physical postures, and regulation of the breath, and ends in progressive states of sustained attention (withdrawal of the senses, concentration, meditation, and samadhi, a continuous state of meditative consciousness) [77]. This combination of practices allows, in a very low-cost experience, an improvement in lifestyle, constituting an integral preventive approach, as well as a complementary therapy to classical pharmacological approaches for mental diseases and diseases of aging. This is particularly pertinent when considering that polypharmacy is common in elderly people, and it increases the risk of drug interactions and adverse reactions up to 82% [110].
Yoga, like all contemplative practices, contains characteristics that promote the metacognitive capacity of the mind and self-regulation, including the potentiation of cognitive functions, such attention and memory in children and adults [111, 112], emotional regulation [113, 114], and prosocial behavior [113, 115]. A particular characteristic of the system is the modulation of both autonomic and cognitive aspects [77, 111–114, 116–120]. The practice of Yoga involves physical exercises (postures and regulation of breathing) to improve flexibility and strength, alongside cognitive training (meditation) to stimulate attention and meta-awareness [77]. Various sequences of postures derived from Hatha Yoga have been shown to have effects on stress and promote well-being [117]. The practice of postures involves constant short periods of focused attention on specific parts of the body, a kind of practice which could be classified differently to those previously described, called meditative movement [121]. Meditative movement includes forms of exercise that use movement, in conjunction with attention to body sensations, developing attention, proprioception, and a state of relaxation [77, 121]. The practice is followed by breathing exercises, such as alternate nostril breathing, which could aid in modulating autonomic responses and heart rate variability and improve memory and attention in adults [112, 119, 122–124]. The combination of these practices could improve the quality of life of its practitioners, fostering resilience and resistance and promoting healthy aging [117, 122, 125]. These exercises, together with ethical behavior and a positive attitude towards practice, including consistency and detachment from results (motivational/intentional factors), are the foundation and distinctive characteristic of the yogic practice [77].
A cross-sectional study in elderly female Yoga practitioners compared 21 women who had practiced Yoga for at least 8 years to 21 women naïve to Yoga, demonstrating that the first group showed a significantly greater brain cortical thickness in a left lobe cluster, including portions of the lateral middle frontal gyrus, anterior superior frontal gyrus, and dorsal superior frontal gyrus [126]. Furthermore, age-related decline in fluid intelligence was lower, while resting state functional brain networks were more resilient to damage. In addition, small-world brain architecture was stronger in long-term Yoga practitioners and meditation practitioners combined (16 of each practice type) compared to fifteen controls who were similar in age, education, exercise, and engagement in cognitive activities [127]. In an experimental study carried out by Vasudev et al., 24 elderly with depression were randomized to practice automatic self-transcending meditation based on mantra repetition (a form of FA) during a 12-week program, and were compared to 23 waitlist controls, with both groups receiving normal treatment for depression [128]. Depression and anxiety improved in meditation practitioners compared to treatment alone.
An eight-week meditation training, compared to relaxation training, resulted in decreased intraconnectivity in the DMN, salience network, and somatomotor network. Also, the meditator groups showed decreased connectivity strength between the DMN and other nodules (salience, frontoparietal, somatomotor, and visual networks) in tests for simple effects. There was lower nodal connectivity in the left posterior cingulate gyrus (associated with the DMN), bilateral paracentral lobule, and middle cingulate gyrus posttraining in the meditation group. No impact was observed on global level network organization in the resting state following this short-term intervention. In a separate study of the same participants, positive connectivity between the posterior cingulate cortex, precuneus, and the pons was observed in meditators [108, 129]. Lavretsky et al. randomly assigned elderly carers of family members with dementia to kirtan kriya yogic meditation (
There are several meditation techniques that are part of yogic practice, mostly related to the attentional family, in particular FA [108, 129, 130], and may include body scan, attention on breath, and mantra repetition [131, 132]. However, despite the type of meditation, the practice of Yoga involves gradually sustaining more attention on the subject or perceiver of the experience than on the object of the experience (like mantra, breath, or body), and this meditation seeks to establish permanent self-awareness by separating the object of the experience from the subject or perceiver of the experience. This separation could include even the body, senses, and all kinds of thoughts and emotions of the practitioner, because all of these elements could be considered objects that can be detected by the perceiver or the subject [75, 133]. From this point of view, meditation together with Yoga techniques also has aspects of awareness-oriented OM [75, 133].
The DMN is involved in self-referential processes. Different types of meditation have the ability to activate a submodule of the DMN in Yoga practitioners [133]. Recently, it has been suggested that the DMN has three subnets, or operational modules: two symmetrical occipito-parieto-temporal subnets and one frontal subnet [134, 135]. An interesting aspect of this subdivision is that the frontal operational module is related to a sense of agency (the perceiver, the subject or witness of the experience) or a first-person perspective, and the other two modules are related to the continuity of the “I” embodied and localized within a bodily space [133–135]. Fingelkurts et al. compare the function of the DMN of healthy fully conscious subjects versus that of patients in vegetative and minimally conscious states, and they show that in vegetative patients all operative modules of the DMN are almost completely attenuated, while in patients with a minimal conscious state only the frontal subnet exhibits activity. Finally, in healthy and fully conscious subjects the three subnets function normally [134]. Interestingly, following long-term practice of three types of yogic meditation (such as focus on the breath, body scan, and mantra repetition), activity in the frontal operation module of the DMN is enhanced, while the other two modules are deactivated [133]. As in yogic meditation, it is always encouraged to avoid mind wandering and it is possible that the activation of the DMN following long-term practice is due to the constant self-referential process, and not due to mind wandering. This constant self-awareness could allow moving a safe distance away from the experience, which in turn could promote self-regulated behaviors, improve mental health [75], and foster a healthy lifestyle [108, 129].
Moreover, meditation is able to modulate another network that could be fundamental to self-referential processes: the paralimbic network that involves the anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortices [136, 137]. In experienced practitioners, Yoga nidra meditation, a type of body scan meditation, is accompanied by a strong increase in sensory awareness [136] and significant differences in the brain network activity as compared to the resting state [136, 137]. In contrast, sustained activity in the paralimbic network constituted by the midline frontal regions is observed in both meditative and resting states. This finding has led to the hypothesis that the paralimbic network contributes to the regulation of a common reference of self-perspective [136, 137]. Taken together, these data could indicate that attention, awareness, and emotion are integrated by a paralimbic network, and the activity of this region, together with the DMN, could be modulated by meditation techniques derived from Yoga. This kind of regulation could be helpful to efficiently allocate brain resources to optimize behavior and well-being.
Also, the characterization of each of the subnetworks of the DMN, and its relation with other networks (including the paralimbic network) in various contemplative practices, such as Yoga in experimented practitioners, could be a novel methodological approach to better understand the role of the DMN in meditation. At the same time, the study of the DMN and meditation may provide greater insights into the operational module involved in the regulation of the DMN, and its relationship with several neuropathologies related (or not) with aging.
5.2. Mindfulness
Mindfulness has its origin in the Buddhist tradition [2, 138], and it may be understood as a mental state characterized by “paying attention on purpose, in the present moment, and nonjudgmentally to the unfolding of experience, moment by moment” [86]. It may be considered a key skill developed through contemplative practices such as meditation and Yoga [77, 139]. The benefit delivered by this practice lies on the notion that attention is a core function to be developed [140, 141]. Its underlying neural mechanisms would be related to the salience network [141, 142], the executive control network, and the orienting network [141, 143].
Several authors have proposed that mindfulness training may help older adults in managing the cognitive, emotional, and psychological challenges of aging [144–146]. This has been corroborated by a meta-analysis of 47 trials that demonstrated a positive impact of mindfulness meditation programs over anxiety- and depression-related outcomes, independent of age [147]. Indeed, Prakash et al. showed that mindfulness disposition, defined as the ability to focus on the present and disengage from DMN-related thinking, is associated to enhanced connectivity in the DMN in older adults [148]. Gard et al. assessed mindfulness using the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire, across all participants, in a cross-sectional study including meditation (insight meditation from the deconstructive family) and Yoga practitioners (Kripalu Yoga, a traditional Raja Yoga school). They found that this dimension related positively to fluid intelligence, network resilience, and integration [127]. This suggests a mechanistic role of meditation in the preservation of intelligence and functional brain network architecture [127]. In addition, a randomized controlled trial, conducted by Wells et al., corroborated that a mindfulness-based stress reduction program, which applied OM techniques (weekly 2-hour group sessions over 8 weeks, with suggested 30-minute daily home practice), resulted in an improved connectivity in the posterior cingulate cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, and left hippocampus in participants who suffered from mild cognitive impairment, as compared to controls who did not receive this intervention [149]. This practice demonstrated an enhancement in executive function and greater leftward frontal alpha asymmetry in elderly practitioners [150]. Other findings supporting mindfulness meditation have verified that a daily practice, escalating from 5 to 20 minutes over 6 weeks, is linked to grey matter changes (i.e., increases in the right precuneus and decreases in the left prefrontal cortex, right hippocampus, right thalamus, and right parietal cortex [151]). Similarly, Laneri et al. analysed the effects of long-term mindfulness meditation on the brain’s white matter microstructure and its aging by employing diffusion tensor imaging in 33 meditators with 5-38 years of experience from Buddhist and Zen centres in Germany (all meditators practiced mindfulness meditation styles such as Vipassana, of the deconstructive family, Shamatha, and Zazen), and in 31 healthy nonmeditators [109]. The authors concluded that mindfulness meditation might contribute to the preservation of the integrity of white matter, diminishing age-related white matter degeneration.
Following the model of attention that proposes three main components (alerting, orienting, and conflict resolution, or an executive component) [74], elderly randomized to mindfulness training (10 minutes, 5 times a week for 8 weeks) exhibited improved response latency in the Stroop test and in electrophysiological measures, indicating improved maintenance of goal-directed attention in this visuospatial task [141]. However, other authors found no improvement in measures of executive control and emotional regulation [12]. On the other hand, a decline of efficiency, specifically in the executive component of attention, was observed in elderly meditators compared to meditation-naïve, young adults [80]. This effect was not observed in age-matched long-term meditators, with similar education level and cognitive function (
These data collectively suggest that the use of meditation could increase the optimal function of the brain in the elderly through voluntary regulation of brain networks, such as the DMN and TPN. This is evidenced not only by functional changes, but also structural ones, which could facilitate optimal aging and prevent neurological and neuropsychiatric diseases. A greater part of these studies, developed using standardized techniques, highlight the impact of mindfulness OM techniques as a promising practice to enhance cognitive functioning during aging.
6. Perspectives
While more clinical and basic research is needed to establish the modulation of the DMN and TPN through meditation, and to understand the impact of modulation on aging and mental disease, the data indicate that meditation may influence different cognitive processes, thus increasing attentional focus and cognitive flexibility [154]. Meditation is also suggested to modulate several other physiological processes, including enhancing relaxation through the decrease of sympathetic nervous system activity, controlling the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in stress, decreasing heart rate variability, and diminishing emotional reactivity [145, 155–157]. All these processes are mediated through what Dorjee calls “the metacognitive self-regulatory” capacity of the mind, an ability based on meta-awareness and sustained attention [78] (Figure 2). Furthermore, these findings suggest that it is plausible to develop new strategies to facilitate optimal aging.
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]Nevertheless, the mechanisms by which meditation modulates the DMN and TPN, and how this influences physiological processes, remain elusive. At the neural level, meditation has been shown to both increase and decrease specific brain networks. In general, meditation decreases connectivity between the DMN and salience, frontoparietal, somatomotor, and visual networks, while increasing connectivity in the posterior cingulate cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, and left hippocampus. It is important not to think that these differences of direction in connectivity modulation imply an inconsistent result: given that both networks are involved in different processes, this opposed connectivity could shed light on the mechanisms involved in the effects of meditation on cognitive processes. Further research is needed in order to elucidate what is the role of these specific networks in the specific cognitive processes shown to be modulated by meditation.
But, indeed, this active cognitive process which is able to modulate several brain networks has been suggested as a therapeutic approach for neuropsychiatric and even neurodegenerative disorders, important for an optimal aging. Interestingly, and as suggested by this article, many of these symptoms could be reduced directly through the modulation of DMN activity that could interfere with goal-directed tasks [158, 159]. We also have shown that within the current classification of meditation, the attentional family has a greater capacity for the modulation of the DMN. Also, the greater the hours and frequency of practice (experience), the greater is the decrease in the activity of the DMN during the task [160]. This agrees with the general idea that during goal-directed tasks, there could be an anticorrelation between the attentional networks and the DMN. In this context, FA meditation has shown several positive results, while OM meditation has divergent results regarding control of the DMN in the literature.
At present, the divergence of the results about this type of meditation is an open question. However, it is necessary to consider an important factor about the nature of the OM meditation. Unlike FA meditation, which is directly associated with an object of attention (either an internal or external goal-directed task), to achieve a state of effortless awareness, OM meditation increases the aperture of awareness to all mental objects and sensory inputs that spontaneously appear and disappear from the mental field, to allow a meta-awareness of one’s own mental habits [160]. This practice therefore requires a greater skill on the part of the practitioners, due to its similarity with a state of mental wandering. As we previously described in this article, OM meditation demonstrates similarities and possibly an interaction with certain nodes of the DMN, and possibly with the wandering of the mind [160].
Thus, it is possible that the activation of the DMN during the practice could be related to the constant self-referential process, and not due to mind wandering, as we previously mention in the Yoga section. To see in detail the difference between the state of OM and mind wandering, refer to Vago and Zeidan [160].
Also, another possible explanation to the divergence in the results of this type of meditation may be due to some extent in the complexity of the proposed task and the inability of practitioners, especially novices, to achieve this skill. This is especially relevant when OM meditation is instructed as a primary technique, without the achievement of previous stages (such as focused attention on an object), going in an opposite direction to the progressive path in which the eastern schools of meditation have taught for centuries [77, 160].
In this context, Yoga as a meditation school seems to have a more integral approach because of the particular dedication that Yoga puts into the gradual progression of meditative practice. One of the distinguishing characteristics of classical—or Raja—Yoga is the combination of practices like postures, breath control, and several forms of meditation that could modulate the autonomous nervous system, principally by postures and breathing exercises. This modulation is a door to the regulation of other important systems such as immune, digestive, cardiovascular, or muscular systems. Unlike other practices that integrate movement, attention, and breathing, and that could promote similar effects (for example, Tai-Chi or Qi gong, which have also been the focus of scientific interest [121, 161]), Yoga could also modulate cognitive functions like attention, memory, and mind wandering, due to the inclusion of specific techniques of sustained attention/meditation. In this context, Yoga meditation also includes techniques that could be classified as FA and OM meditation, which in turn promote the activation of TPNs and also modulation of the DMN [101, 133]. However, more research and a broader classification is required to include all the meditation techniques present in Yoga, because the current classification of meditations is eminently directed for those belonging to the school of mindfulness.
In this context, another possibility could be to organize these types of disciplines under a general concept that may include most of the practices that are in the current investigation. These practices must accomplish basic characteristics, such as those proposed by Dorjee to be the metacognitive self-regulatory capacity of the mind, based on meta-awareness and sustained attention [78] (Figure 2). This allows (i) integrating different methods, (ii) providing a better characterization of the techniques, interventions, and results, and (iii) avoiding control over concepts by one or two schools at the exclusion of others. While several techniques or schools that could be characterized as contemplatives have different approaches (two of the best known are Yoga and mindfulness), there are many others, including the relaxation response, the Liverpool model of mindfulness, or other meditative moment techniques such as Tai Chi [72, 121].
So, it is fundamental to increase and systematize the research in contemplative practices, covering the different aspects of each practice that could causally explain its cognitive and neurophysiological effects, and how this practice could promote well-being at different stages of life (crucially, in aging), as well as how it could be used in the prevention and treatment of several types of diseases.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
[1] United Nations, World Population, Ageing, 2015.
[2] R. Marciniak, K. Sheardova, P. Čermáková, D. Hudeček, R. Šumec, J. Hort, "Effect of meditation on cognitive functions in context of aging and neurodegenerative diseases," Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 8,DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00017, 2014.
[3] K. J. Schlesinger, B. O. Turner, B. A. Lopez, M. B. Miller, J. M. Carlson, "Age-dependent changes in task-based modular organization of the human brain," NeuroImage, vol. 146, pp. 741-762, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.001, 2017.
[4] V. La Corte, M. Sperduti, C. Malherbe, F. Vialatte, S. Lion, T. Gallarda, C. Oppenheim, P. Piolino, "Cognitive decline and reorganization of functional connectivity in healthy aging: the pivotal role of the salience network in the prediction of age and cognitive performances," Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, vol. 8,DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00204, 2016.
[5] F. A. Mansouri, K. Tanaka, M. J. Buckley, "Conflict-induced behavioural adjustment: a clue to the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex," Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 10 no. 2, pp. 141-152, DOI: 10.1038/nrn2538, 2009.
[6] R. L. Buckner, "Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate," Neuron, vol. 44 no. 1, pp. 195-208, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006, 2004.
[7] D. L. Schacter, A. W. Kaszniak, J. F. Kihlstrom, M. Valdiserri, "The relation between source memory and aging," Psychology and Aging, vol. 6 no. 4, pp. 559-568, DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.6.4.559, 1991.
[8] T. Hedden, J. D. E. Gabrieli, "Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience," Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 5 no. 2, pp. 87-96, DOI: 10.1038/nrn1323, 2004.
[9] K. A. Tsvetanov, R. N. A. Henson, L. K. Tyler, A. Razi, L. Geerligs, T. E. Ham, J. B. Rowe, Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience, "Extrinsic and intrinsic brain network connectivity maintains cognition across the lifespan despite accelerated decay of regional brain activation," The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 36 no. 11, pp. 3115-3126, DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2733-15.2016, 2016.
[10] W. A. Chaovalitwongse, D. Won, O. Seref, P. Borghesani, M. K. Askren, S. Willis, T. J. Grabowski, "Network optimization of functional connectivity within default mode network regions to detect cognitive decline," IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering, vol. 25 no. 7, pp. 1079-1089, DOI: 10.1109/TNSRE.2017.2679056, 2017.
[11] M. Corbetta, G. Patel, G. L. Shulman, "The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind," Neuron, vol. 58 no. 3, pp. 306-324, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017, 2008.
[12] A. Raz, J. Buhle, "Typologies of attentional networks," Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 7 no. 5, pp. 367-379, DOI: 10.1038/nrn1903, 2006.
[13] M. Corbetta, G. L. Shulman, "Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain," Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 3 no. 3, pp. 201-215, DOI: 10.1038/nrn755, 2002.
[14] K. Onoda, M. Ishihara, S. Yamaguchi, "Decreased functional connectivity by aging is associated with cognitive decline," Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 24 no. 11, pp. 2186-2198, DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00269, 2012.
[15] J. Song, R. M. Birn, M. Boly, T. B. Meier, V. A. Nair, M. E. Meyerand, V. Prabhakaran, "Age-related reorganizational changes in modularity and functional connectivity of human brain networks," Brain Connectivity, vol. 4 no. 9, pp. 662-676, DOI: 10.1089/brain.2014.0286, 2014.
[16] D. Tomasi, N. D. Volkow, "Aging and functional brain networks," Molecular Psychiatry, vol. 17 no. 5, pp. 549-558, DOI: 10.1038/mp.2011.81, 2012.
[17] J. Gomez-Ramirez, Y. Li, Q. Wu, J. Wu, "A quantitative study of network robustness in resting-state fMRI in young and elder adults," Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, vol. 7,DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00256, 2016.
[18] T. Ngandu, J. Lehtisalo, E. Levälahti, T. Laatikainen, J. Lindström, M. Peltonen, A. Solomon, S. Ahtiluoto, R. Antikainen, T. Hänninen, A. Jula, F. Mangialasche, T. Paajanen, S. Pajala, R. Rauramaa, T. Strandberg, J. Tuomilehto, H. Soininen, M. Kivipelto, "Recruitment and baseline characteristics of participants in the Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability (FINGER)—a randomized controlled lifestyle trial," International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, vol. 11 no. 9, pp. 9345-9360, DOI: 10.3390/ijerph110909345, 2014.
[19] B. Imtiaz, A.-M. Tolppanen, M. Kivipelto, H. Soininen, "Future directions in Alzheimer’s disease from risk factors to prevention," Biochemical Pharmacology, vol. 88 no. 4, pp. 661-670, DOI: 10.1016/j.bcp.2014.01.003, 2014.
[20] T. Ngandu, J. Lehtisalo, A. Solomon, E. Levälahti, S. Ahtiluoto, R. Antikainen, L. Bäckman, T. Hänninen, A. Jula, T. Laatikainen, J. Lindström, F. Mangialasche, T. Paajanen, S. Pajala, M. Peltonen, R. Rauramaa, A. Stigsdotter-Neely, T. Strandberg, J. Tuomilehto, H. Soininen, M. Kivipelto, "A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial," The Lancet, vol. 385 no. 9984, pp. 2255-2263, DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)60461-5, 2015.
[21] B. Bandelow, S. Michaelis, "Epidemiology of anxiety disorders in the 21st century," Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, vol. 17 no. 3, pp. 327-335, 2015.
[22] G. M. Slavich, M. R. Irwin, "From stress to inflammation and major depressive disorder: a social signal transduction theory of depression," Psychiatric Bulletin, vol. 140 no. 3, pp. 774-815, DOI: 10.1037/a0035302, 2014.
[23] A. Sood, D. T. Jones, "On mind wandering, attention, brain networks, and meditation," Explore, vol. 9 no. 3, pp. 136-141, DOI: 10.1016/j.explore.2013.02.005, 2013.
[24] D. S. Khalsa, "Stress, meditation, and Alzheimer’s disease prevention: where the evidence stands," Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, vol. 48 no. 1,DOI: 10.3233/JAD-142766, 2015.
[25] M. D. Greicius, B. Krasnow, A. L. Reiss, V. Menon, "Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 100 no. 1, pp. 253-258, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100, 2003.
[26] J. S. Damoiseaux, S. A. R. B. Rombouts, F. Barkhof, P. Scheltens, C. J. Stam, S. M. Smith, C. F. Beckmann, "Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 103 no. 37, pp. 13848-13853, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103, 2006.
[27] M. E. Raichle, A. M. MacLeod, A. Z. Snyder, W. J. Powers, D. A. Gusnard, G. L. Shulman, "A default mode of brain function," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 98 no. 2, pp. 676-682, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676, 2001.
[28] M. E. Raichle, "The brain’s default mode network," Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 38 no. 1, pp. 433-447, DOI: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030, 2015.
[29] M. E. Raichle, "Two views of brain function," Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 14 no. 4, pp. 180-190, DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.008, 2010.
[30] L. Sokoloff, R. Mangold, R. L. Wechsler, C. Kennedy, S. S. Kety, "The effect of mental arithmetic on cerebral circulation and metabolism," The Journal of Clinical Investigation, vol. 34, 7, Part 1, pp. 1101-1108, DOI: 10.1172/JCI103159, 1955.
[31] R. G. Shulman, D. L. Rothman, K. L. Behar, F. Hyder, "Energetic basis of brain activity: implications for neuroimaging," Trends in Neurosciences, vol. 27 no. 8, pp. 489-495, DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.06.005, 2004.
[32] M. D. Greicius, G. Srivastava, A. L. Reiss, V. Menon, "Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 101 no. 13, pp. 4637-4642, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0308627101, 2004.
[33] R. L. Bluhm, J. Miller, R. A. Lanius, E. A. Osuch, K. Boksman, R. Neufeld, J. Theberge, B. Schaefer, P. Williamson, "Spontaneous low-frequency fluctuations in the BOLD signal in schizophrenic patients: anomalies in the default network," Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 33 no. 4, pp. 1004-1012, DOI: 10.1093/schbul/sbm052, 2007.
[34] P. Delaveau, P. Salgado-Pineda, P. Fossati, T. Witjas, J. P. Azulay, O. Blin, "Dopaminergic modulation of the default mode network in Parkinson’s disease," European Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 20 no. 11, pp. 784-792, DOI: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.07.001, 2010.
[35] M. Quarantelli, E. Salvatore, S. M. D. A. Giorgio, A. Filla, A. Cervo, C. V. Russo, S. Cocozza, M. Massarelli, A. Brunetti, G. de Michele, "Default-mode network changes in Huntington’s disease: an integrated MRI study of functional connectivity and morphometry," PLoS One, vol. 8 no. 8, article e72159,DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0072159, 2013.
[36] I. Neuner, C. J. Werner, J. Arrubla, T. Stöcker, C. Ehlen, H. P. Wegener, F. Schneider, N. J. Shah, "Imaging the where and when of tic generation and resting state networks in adult Tourette patients," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 8,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00362, 2014.
[37] J. S. Damoiseaux, C. F. Beckmann, E. J. S. Arigita, F. Barkhof, P. Scheltens, C. J. Stam, S. M. Smith, S. A. R. B. Rombouts, "Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging," Cerebral Cortex, vol. 18 no. 8, pp. 1856-1864, DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhm207, 2008.
[38] C. Sorg, V. Riedl, M. Muhlau, V. D. Calhoun, T. Eichele, L. Laer, A. Drzezga, H. Forstl, A. Kurz, C. Zimmer, A. M. Wohlschlager, "Selective changes of resting-state networks in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 104 no. 47, pp. 18760-18765, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0708803104, 2007.
[39] R. M. Hutchison, S. M. Mirsattari, C. K. Jones, J. S. Gati, L. S. Leung, "Functional networks in the anesthetized rat brain revealed by independent component analysis of resting-state fMRI," Journal of Neurophysiology, vol. 103 no. 6, pp. 3398-3406, DOI: 10.1152/jn.00141.2010, 2010.
[40] Y. I. Sheline, D. M. Barch, J. L. Price, M. M. Rundle, S. N. Vaishnavi, A. Z. Snyder, M. A. Mintun, S. Wang, R. S. Coalson, M. E. Raichle, "The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 106 no. 6, pp. 1942-1947, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0812686106, 2009.
[41] R. Simon, M. Engström, "The default mode network as a biomarker for monitoring the therapeutic effects of meditation," Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 6,DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00776, 2015.
[42] J. A. Brewer, P. D. Worhunsky, J. R. Gray, Y. Y. Tang, J. Weber, H. Kober, "Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 108 no. 50, pp. 20254-20259, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1112029108, 2011.
[43] D. A. Gusnard, M. E. Raichle, M. E. Raichle, "Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain," Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 2 no. 10, pp. 685-694, DOI: 10.1038/35094500, 2001.
[44] M. D. Fox, A. Z. Snyder, J. L. Vincent, M. Corbetta, D. C. van Essen, M. E. Raichle, "The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 102 no. 27, pp. 9673-9678, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102, 2005.
[45] M. Hampson, N. R. Driesen, P. Skudlarski, J. C. Gore, R. T. Constable, "Brain connectivity related to working memory performance," The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 26 no. 51, pp. 13338-13343, DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3408-06.2006, 2006.
[46] R. Leech, S. Kamourieh, C. F. Beckmann, D. J. Sharp, "Fractionating the default mode network: distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control," The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 31 no. 9, pp. 3217-3224, DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5626-10.2011, 2011.
[47] M. A. O’Connell, C. Basak, "Effects of task complexity and age-differences on task-related functional connectivity of attentional networks," Neuropsychologia, vol. 114, pp. 50-64, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.013, 2018.
[48] P. Fransson, "How default is the default mode of brain function? Further evidence from intrinsic BOLD signal fluctuations," Neuropsychologia, vol. 44 no. 14, pp. 2836-2845, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.017, 2006.
[49] M. D. Greicius, V. Menon, "Default-mode activity during a passive sensory task: uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation," Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 16 no. 9, pp. 1484-1492, DOI: 10.1162/0898929042568532, 2004.
[50] D. S. Margulies, S. S. Ghosh, A. Goulas, M. Falkiewicz, J. M. Huntenburg, G. Langs, G. Bezgin, S. B. Eickhoff, F. X. Castellanos, M. Petrides, E. Jefferies, J. Smallwood, "Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 113 no. 44, pp. 12574-12579, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1608282113, 2016.
[51] D. A. Gusnard, E. Akbudak, G. L. Shulman, M. E. Raichle, "Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 98 no. 7, pp. 4259-4264, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.071043098, 2001.
[52] R. L. Buckner, J. R. Andrews-Hanna, D. L. Schacter, "The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease," Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 1124 no. 1,DOI: 10.1196/annals.1440.011, 2008.
[53] M. F. Mason, M. I. Norton, J. D. van Horn, D. M. Wegner, S. T. Grafton, C. N. Macrae, "Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought," Science, vol. 315 no. 5810, pp. 393-395, DOI: 10.1126/science.1131295, 2007.
[54] D. H. Weissman, K. C. Roberts, K. M. Visscher, M. G. Woldorff, "The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention," Nature Neuroscience, vol. 9 no. 7, pp. 971-978, DOI: 10.1038/nn1727, 2006.
[55] R. A. Henríquez, A. B. Chica, P. Billeke, P. Bartolomeo, "Fluctuating minds: spontaneous psychophysical variability during mind-wandering," PLoS One, vol. 11 no. 2, article e0147174,DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0147174, 2016.
[56] B. Haatveit, J. Jensen, D. Alnæs, T. Kaufmann, C. L. Brandt, C. Thoresen, O. A. Andreassen, I. Melle, T. Ueland, L. T. Westlye, "Reduced load-dependent default mode network deactivation across executive tasks in schizophrenia spectrum disorders," NeuroImage: Clinical, vol. 12, pp. 389-396, DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.08.012, 2016.
[57] A. G. Garrity, G. D. Pearlson, K. McKiernan, D. Lloyd, K. A. Kiehl, V. D. Calhoun, "Aberrant “default mode” functional connectivity in schizophrenia," The American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 164 no. 3, pp. 450-457, DOI: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.3.450, 2007.
[58] E. Pomarol-Clotet, R. Salvador, S. Sarró, J. Gomar, F. Vila, A. Martínez, A. Guerrero, J. Ortiz-Gil, B. Sans-Sansa, A. Capdevila, J. M. Cebamanos, P. McKenna, "Failure to deactivate in the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia: dysfunction of the default mode network?," Psychological Medicine, vol. 38 no. 8, pp. 1185-1193, DOI: 10.1017/S0033291708003565, 2008.
[59] W. Liao, Z. Zhang, Z. Pan, D. Mantini, J. Ding, X. Duan, C. Luo, Z. Wang, Q. Tan, G. Lu, H. Chen, "Default mode network abnormalities in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy: a study combining fMRI and DTI," Human Brain Mapping, vol. 32 no. 6, pp. 883-895, DOI: 10.1002/hbm.21076, 2011.
[60] A. J. Bastos-Leite, G. R. Ridgway, C. Silveira, A. Norton, S. Reis, K. J. Friston, "Dysconnectivity within the default mode in first-episode schizophrenia: a stochastic dynamic causal modeling study with functional magnetic resonance imaging," Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 41 no. 1, pp. 144-153, DOI: 10.1093/schbul/sbu080, 2015.
[61] S. Whitfield-Gabrieli, J. M. Ford, "Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology," Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, vol. 8 no. 1, pp. 49-76, DOI: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143049, 2012.
[62] A. Anticevic, M. W. Cole, J. D. Murray, P. R. Corlett, X. J. Wang, J. H. Krystal, "The role of default network deactivation in cognition and disease," Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 16 no. 12, pp. 584-592, DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.008, 2012.
[63] C. L. Grady, M. V. Springer, D. Hongwanishkul, A. R. McIntosh, G. Winocur, "Age-related changes in brain activity across the adult lifespan," Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 18 no. 2, pp. 227-241, DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.2.227, 2006.
[64] J. Persson, C. Lustig, J. K. Nelson, P. A. Reuter-Lorenz, "Age differences in deactivation: a link to cognitive control?," Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 19 no. 6, pp. 1021-1032, DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.1021, 2007.
[65] C. Lustig, A. Z. Snyder, M. Bhakta, K. C. O'Brien, M. McAvoy, M. E. Raichle, J. C. Morris, R. L. Buckner, "Functional deactivations: change with age and dementia of the Alzheimer type," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 100 no. 24, pp. 14504-14509, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2235925100, 2003.
[66] K. Christoff, A. M. Gordon, J. Smallwood, R. Smith, J. W. Schooler, "Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 106 no. 21, pp. 8719-8724, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0900234106, 2009.
[67] W. Hasenkamp, C. D. Wilson-Mendenhall, E. Duncan, L. W. Barsalou, "Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: a fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states," NeuroImage, vol. 59 no. 1, pp. 750-760, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.008, 2012.
[68] A. M. C. Kelly, L. Q. Uddin, B. B. Biswal, F. X. Castellanos, M. P. Milham, "Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability," NeuroImage, vol. 39 no. 1, pp. 527-537, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.008, 2008.
[69] W. W. Seeley, V. Menon, A. F. Schatzberg, J. Keller, G. H. Glover, H. Kenna, A. L. Reiss, M. D. Greicius, "Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control," The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 27 no. 9, pp. 2349-2356, DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007, 2007.
[70] M. I. Posner, S. E. Petersen, "The attention system of the human brain," Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 13 no. 1, pp. 25-42, DOI: 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325, 1990.
[71] S. E. Petersen, M. I. Posner, "The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after," Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 35 no. 1, pp. 73-89, DOI: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525, 2012.
[72] P. Malinowski, "Neural mechanisms of attentional control in mindfulness meditation," Frontiers in Neuroscience, vol. 7,DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00008, 2013.
[73] M. I. Posner, M. K. Rothbart, "Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science," Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 58 no. 1,DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516, 2007.
[74] Y. Y. Tang, B. K. Hölzel, M. I. Posner, "The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation," Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 16 no. 4, pp. 213-225, DOI: 10.1038/nrn3916, 2015.
[75] C. J. Dahl, A. Lutz, R. J. Davidson, "Reconstructing and deconstructing the self: cognitive mechanisms in meditation practice," Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 19 no. 9, pp. 515-523, DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.001, 2015.
[76] J. W. Schooler, J. Smallwood, K. Christoff, T. C. Handy, E. D. Reichle, M. A. Sayette, "Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind," Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 15 no. 7, pp. 319-326, DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.006, 2011.
[77] T. Gard, J. J. Noggle, C. L. Park, D. R. Vago, A. Wilson, "Potential self-regulatory mechanisms of yoga for psychological health," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 8,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00770, 2014.
[78] D. Dorjee, "Defining contemplative science: the metacognitive self-regulatory capacity of the mind, context of meditation practice and modes of existential awareness," Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 7,DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01788, 2016.
[79] A. Lutz, H. A. Slagter, J. D. Dunne, R. J. Davidson, "Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation," Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 12 no. 4, pp. 163-169, DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005, 2008.
[80] M. Sperduti, D. Makowski, P. Piolino, "The protective role of long-term meditation on the decline of the executive component of attention in aging: a preliminary cross-sectional study," Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, vol. 23 no. 6, pp. 691-702, DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2016.1159652, 2016.
[81] K. A. Garrison, T. A. Zeffiro, D. Scheinost, R. T. Constable, J. A. Brewer, "Meditation leads to reduced default mode network activity beyond an active task," Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 15 no. 3, pp. 712-720, DOI: 10.3758/s13415-015-0358-3, 2015.
[82] A. Manna, A. Raffone, M. G. Perrucci, D. Nardo, A. Ferretti, A. Tartaro, A. Londei, C. del Gratta, M. O. Belardinelli, G. L. Romani, "Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation," Brain Research Bulletin, vol. 82 no. 1-2, pp. 46-56, DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.03.001, 2010.
[83] E. H. Kozasa, J. R. Sato, S. S. Lacerda, M. A. M. Barreiros, J. Radvany, T. A. Russell, L. G. Sanches, L. E. A. M. Mello, E. Amaro, "Meditation training increases brain efficiency in an attention task," NeuroImage, vol. 59 no. 1, pp. 745-749, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.088, 2012.
[84] E. H. Kozasa, J. B. Balardin, J. R. Sato, K. T. Chaim, S. S. Lacerda, J. Radvany, L. E. A. M. Mello, E. Amaro, "Effects of a 7-day meditation retreat on the brain function of meditators and non-meditators during an attention task," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 12,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00222, 2018.
[85] M. S. Worden, J. J. Foxe, N. Wang, G. V. Simpson, "Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by retinotopically specific α -bank electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex," The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 20 no. 6, article RC63,DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-06-j0002.2000, 2000.
[86] G. Thut, A. Nietzel, S. A. Brandt, A. Pascual-Leone, "α -Band electroencephalographic activity over occipital cortex indexes visuospatial attention bias and predicts visual target detection," The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 26 no. 37, pp. 9494-9502, DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0875-06.2006, 2006.
[87] S. P. Kelly, M. Gomez-Ramirez, J. J. Foxe, "The strength of anticipatory spatial biasing predicts target discrimination at attended locations: a high-density EEG study," The European Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 30 no. 11, pp. 2224-2234, DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06980.x, 2009.
[88] C. E. Kerr, S. R. Jones, Q. Wan, D. L. Pritchett, R. H. Wasserman, A. Wexler, J. J. Villanueva, J. R. Shaw, S. W. Lazar, T. J. Kaptchuk, R. Littenberg, M. S. Hämäläinen, C. I. Moore, "Effects of mindfulness meditation training on anticipatory alpha modulation in primary somatosensory cortex," Brain Research Bulletin, vol. 85 no. 3-4, pp. 96-103, DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.03.026, 2011.
[89] C. E. Kerr, M. D. Sacchet, S. W. Lazar, C. I. Moore, S. R. Jones, "Mindfulness starts with the body: somatosensory attention and top-down modulation of cortical alpha rhythms in mindfulness meditation," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 7,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00012, 2013.
[90] K. C. R. Fox, S. Nijeboer, M. L. Dixon, J. L. Floman, M. Ellamil, S. P. Rumak, P. Sedlmeier, K. Christoff, "Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners," Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, vol. 43, pp. 48-73, DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.016, 2014.
[91] D.-H. Kang, H. J. Jo, W. H. Jung, S. H. Kim, Y. H. Jung, C. H. Choi, U. S. Lee, S. C. An, J. H. Jang, J. S. Kwon, "The effect of meditation on brain structure: cortical thickness mapping and diffusion tensor imaging," Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, vol. 8 no. 1, pp. 27-33, DOI: 10.1093/scan/nss056, 2013.
[92] J. S. Beer, O. P. John, D. Scabini, R. T. Knight, "Orbitofrontal cortex and social behavior: integrating self-monitoring and emotion-cognition interactions," Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 18 no. 6, pp. 871-879, DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.871, 2006.
[93] G. Bush, P. Luu, M. I. Posner, "Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex," Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 4 no. 6, pp. 215-222, DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2, 2000.
[94] M. I. Posner, M. K. Rothbart, B. E. Sheese, Y. Tang, "The anterior cingulate gyrus and the mechanism of self-regulation," Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 7 no. 4, pp. 391-395, DOI: 10.3758/CABN.7.4.391, 2007.
[95] B. A. Vogt, "Pain and emotion interactions in subregions of the cingulate gyrus," Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 6 no. 7, pp. 533-544, DOI: 10.1038/nrn1704, 2005.
[96] S. W. Lazar, C. E. Kerr, R. H. Wasserman, J. R. Gray, D. N. Greve, M. T. Treadway, M. McGarvey, B. T. Quinn, J. A. Dusek, H. Benson, S. L. Rauch, C. I. Moore, B. Fischl, "Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness," NeuroReport, vol. 16 no. 17, pp. 1893-1897, DOI: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000186598.66243.19, 2005.
[97] K. Vogeley, G. R. Fink, "Neural correlates of the first-person-perspective," Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 7 no. 1, pp. 38-42, DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00003-7, 2003.
[98] A. E. Cavanna, M. R. Trimble, "The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates," Brain, vol. 129 no. 3, pp. 564-583, DOI: 10.1093/brain/awl004, 2006.
[99] R. Kanai, G. Rees, "The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition," Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 12 no. 4, pp. 231-242, DOI: 10.1038/nrn3000, 2011.
[100] H. Takeuchi, Y. Taki, H. Hashizume, Y. Sassa, T. Nagase, R. Nouchi, R. Kawashima, "Effects of training of processing speed on neural systems," The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 31 no. 34, pp. 12139-12148, DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2948-11.2011, 2011.
[101] B. P. Acevedo, S. Pospos, H. Lavretsky, "The neural mechanisms of meditative practices: novel approaches for healthy aging," Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, vol. 3 no. 4, pp. 328-339, DOI: 10.1007/s40473-016-0098-x, 2016.
[102] S. Norton, F. E. Matthews, D. E. Barnes, K. Yaffe, C. Brayne, "Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based data," The Lancet Neurology, vol. 13 no. 8, pp. 788-794, DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70136-X, 2014.
[103] V. Zufferey, A. Donati, J. Popp, R. Meuli, J. Rossier, R. Frackowiak, B. Draganski, A. von Gunten, F. Kherif, "Neuroticism, depression, and anxiety traits exacerbate the state of cognitive impairment and hippocampal vulnerability to Alzheimer’s disease," Alzheimer's & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, vol. 7, pp. 107-114, DOI: 10.1016/j.dadm.2017.05.002, 2017.
[104] R. S. Wilson, L. L. Barnes, C. F. Mendes de Leon, N. T. Aggarwal, J. S. Schneider, J. Bach, J. Pilat, L. A. Beckett, S. E. Arnold, D. A. Evans, D. A. Bennett, "Depressive symptoms, cognitive decline, and risk of AD in older persons," Neurology, vol. 59 no. 3, pp. 364-370, DOI: 10.1212/WNL.59.3.364, 2002.
[105] D. E. Barnes, K. Yaffe, "The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence," The Lancet Neurology, vol. 10 no. 9, pp. 819-828, DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70072-2, 2011.
[106] K. E. Innes, T. K. Selfe, "Meditation as a therapeutic intervention for adults at risk for Alzheimer’s disease—potential benefits and underlying mechanisms," Frontiers in Psychiatry, vol. 5,DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00040, 2014.
[107] G. Chételat, A. Lutz, E. Arenaza-Urquijo, F. Collette, O. Klimecki, N. Marchant, "Why could meditation practice help promote mental health and well-being in aging?," Alzheimer's Research & Therapy, vol. 10 no. 1,DOI: 10.1186/s13195-018-0388-5, 2018.
[108] F. A. Cotier, R. Zhang, T. M. C. Lee, "A longitudinal study of the effect of short-term meditation training on functional network organization of the aging brain," Scientific Reports, vol. 7 no. 1,DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-00678-8, 2017.
[109] D. Laneri, V. Schuster, B. Dietsche, A. Jansen, U. Ott, J. Sommer, "Effects of long-term mindfulness meditation on brain’s white matter microstructure and its aging," Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, vol. 7,DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00254, 2016.
[110] B. M. Shah, E. R. Hajjar, "Polypharmacy, adverse drug reactions, and geriatric syndromes," Clinics in Geriatric Medicine, vol. 28 no. 2, pp. 173-186, DOI: 10.1016/j.cger.2012.01.002, 2012.
[111] K. V. Naveen, R. N. H. R. Nagendra, S. Telles, "Yoga breathing through a particular nostril increases spatial memory scores without lateralized effects," Psychological Reports, vol. 81 no. 2, pp. 555-561, DOI: 10.2466/pr0.1997.81.2.555, 1997.
[112] S. Telles, P. Raghuraj, S. Maharana, H. R. Nagendra, "Immediate effect of three yoga breathing techniques on performance on a letter-cancellation task," Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol. 104, pp. 1289-1296, DOI: 10.2466/pms.104.4.1289-1296, 2007.
[113] S. E. Sauer-Zavala, E. C. Walsh, T. A. Eisenlohr-Moul, E. L. B. Lykins, "Comparing mindfulness-based intervention strategies: differential effects of sitting meditation, body scan, and mindful yoga," Mindfulness, vol. 4 no. 4, pp. 383-388, DOI: 10.1007/s12671-012-0139-9, 2013.
[114] E. L. Garland, S. A. Gaylord, B. L. Fredrickson, "Positive reappraisal mediates the stress-reductive effects of mindfulness: an upward spiral process," Mindfulness, vol. 2 no. 1, pp. 59-67, DOI: 10.1007/s12671-011-0043-8, 2011.
[115] S. Bryan, G. Pinto Zipp, R. Parasher, "The effects of yoga on psychosocial variables and exercise adherence: a randomized, controlled pilot study," Alternative Therapies in Health & Medicine, vol. 18 no. 5, pp. 50-59, 2012.
[116] B. M. Cappo, D. S. Holmes, "The utility of prolonged respiratory exhalation for reducing physiological and psychological arousal in non-threatening and threatening situations," Journal of Psychosomatic Research, vol. 28 no. 4, pp. 265-273, DOI: 10.1016/0022-3999(84)90048-5, 1984.
[117] T. Field, "Yoga clinical research review," Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice, vol. 17 no. 1,DOI: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2010.09.007, 2011.
[118] S.-H. Park, K. S. Han, "Blood pressure response to meditation and yoga: a systematic review and meta-analysis," The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 23 no. 9, pp. 685-695, DOI: 10.1089/acm.2016.0234, 2017.
[119] L. Nivethitha, A. Mooventhan, N. Manjunath, "Effects of various Prāṇāyāma on cardiovascular and autonomic variables," Ancient Science of Life, vol. 36 no. 2, pp. 72-77, DOI: 10.4103/asl.ASL_178_16, 2016.
[120] A. Tyagi, M. Cohen, "Yoga and heart rate variability: a comprehensive review of the literature," International Journal of Yoga, vol. 9 no. 2, pp. 97-113, DOI: 10.4103/0973-6131.183712, 2016.
[121] P. Payne, M. A. Crane-Godreau, "Meditative movement for depression and anxiety," Frontiers in Psychiatry, vol. 4,DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00071, 2013.
[122] R. R. Guddeti, G. Dang, M. A. Williams, V. M. Alla, "Role of yoga in cardiac disease and rehabilitation," Journal of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation and Prevention, vol. 1,DOI: 10.1097/HCR.0000000000000372, 2018.
[123] L. Nivethitha, N. Manjunath, A. Mooventhan, "Heart rate variability changes during and after the practice of Bhramari Pranayama," International Journal of Yoga, vol. 10 no. 2, pp. 99-102, DOI: 10.4103/0973-6131.205518, 2017.
[124] A. Zaccaro, A. Piarulli, M. Laurino, E. Garbella, D. Menicucci, B. Neri, A. Gemignani, "How breath-control can change your life: a systematic review on psycho-physiological correlates of slow breathing," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 12,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00353, 2018.
[125] F. J. Schell, B. Allolio, O. W. Schonecke, "Physiological and psychological effects of Hatha-Yoga exercise in healthy women," International Journal of Psychosomatics, vol. 41 no. 1-4, pp. 46-52, 1994.
[126] R. F. Afonso, J. B. Balardin, S. Lazar, J. R. Sato, N. Igarashi, D. F. Santaella, S. S. Lacerda, E. Amaro Jr, E. H. Kozasa, "Greater cortical thickness in elderly female yoga practitioners—a cross-sectional study," Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, vol. 9,DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00201, 2017.
[127] T. Gard, M. Taquet, R. Dixit, B. K. Hölzel, Y. A. de Montjoye, N. Brach, D. H. Salat, B. C. Dickerson, J. R. Gray, S. W. Lazar, "Fluid intelligence and brain functional organization in aging yoga and meditation practitioners," Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, vol. 6,DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00076, 2014.
[128] A. Vasudev, A. Arena, A. M. Burhan, E. Ionson, H. Hirjee, P. Maldeniya, S. Wetmore, R. I. Newman, "A training programme involving automatic self-transcending meditation in late-life depression: preliminary analysis of an ongoing randomised controlled trial," BJPsych Open, vol. 2 no. 02, pp. 195-198, DOI: 10.1192/bjpo.bp.115.002394, 2016.
[129] R. Shao, K. Keuper, X. Geng, T. M. C. Lee, "Pons to posterior cingulate functional projections predict affective processing changes in the elderly following eight weeks of meditation training," eBioMedicine, vol. 10, pp. 236-248, DOI: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2016.06.018, 2016.
[130] H. Lavretsky, E. S. Epel, P. Siddarth, N. Nazarian, N. S. Cyr, D. S. Khalsa, J. Lin, E. Blackburn, M. R. Irwin, "A pilot study of yogic meditation for family dementia caregivers with depressive symptoms: effects on mental health, cognition, and telomerase activity," International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, vol. 28 no. 1, pp. 57-65, DOI: 10.1002/gps.3790, 2013.
[131] K. S. Carter, R. C. III, "Breath-based meditation: a mechanism to restore the physiological and cognitive reserves for optimal human performance," World Journal of Clinical Cases, vol. 4 no. 4, pp. 99-102, DOI: 10.12998/wjcc.v4.i4.99, 2016.
[132] J. Thomas, G. Jamieson, M. Cohen, "Low and then high frequency oscillations of distinct right cortical networks are progressively enhanced by medium and long term Satyananda Yoga meditation practice," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 8,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00197, 2014.
[133] A. A. Fingelkurts, A. A. Fingelkurts, T. Kallio-Tamminen, "Long-term meditation training induced changes in the operational synchrony of default mode network modules during a resting state," Cognitive Processing, vol. 17 no. 1, pp. 27-37, DOI: 10.1007/s10339-015-0743-4, 2016.
[134] A. A. Fingelkurts, A. A. Fingelkurts, S. Bagnato, C. Boccagni, G. Galardi, "DMN operational synchrony relates to self-consciousness: evidence from patients in vegetative and minimally conscious states," The Open Neuroimaging Journal, vol. 6, pp. 55-68, DOI: 10.2174/1874440001206010055, 2012.
[135] A. A. Fingelkurts, A. A. Fingelkurts, S. Bagnato, C. Boccagni, G. Galardi, "The chief role of frontal operational module of the brain default mode network in the potential recovery of consciousness from the vegetative state: a preliminary comparison of three case reports," Open Neuroimaging Journal, vol. 10, pp. 41-51, DOI: 10.2174/1874440001610010041, 2016.
[136] H. C. Lou, T. W. Kjaer, L. Friberg, G. Wildschiodtz, S. Holm, M. Nowak, "A 15 O‐H 2 O PET study of meditation and the resting state of normal consciousness," Human Brain Mapping, vol. 7 no. 2, pp. 98-105, DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)7:2<98::AID-HBM3>3.0.CO;2-M, 1999.
[137] H. C. Lou, M. Joensson, M. L. Kringelbach, "Yoga lessons for consciousness research: a paralimbic network balancing brain resource allocation," Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 2,DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00366, 2011.
[138] R. J. Davidson, A. W. Kaszniak, "Conceptual and methodological issues in research on mindfulness and meditation," American Psychologist, vol. 70 no. 7, pp. 581-592, DOI: 10.1037/a0039512, 2015.
[139] M. Barinaga, "Buddhism and neuroscience: studying the well-trained mind," Science, vol. 302 no. 5642, pp. 44-46, DOI: 10.1126/science.302.5642.44, 2003.
[140] B. K. Hölzel, S. W. Lazar, T. Gard, Z. Schuman-Olivier, D. R. Vago, U. Ott, "How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective," Perspectives on Psychological Science, vol. 6 no. 6, pp. 537-559, DOI: 10.1177/1745691611419671, 2011.
[141] P. Malinowski, A. W. Moore, B. R. Mead, T. Gruber, "Mindful aging: the effects of regular brief mindfulness practice on electrophysiological markers of cognitive and affective processing in older adults," Mindfulness, vol. 8 no. 1, pp. 78-94, DOI: 10.1007/s12671-015-0482-8, 2017.
[142] D. Sridharan, D. J. Levitin, V. Menon, "A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 105 no. 34, pp. 12569-12574, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0800005105, 2008.
[143] M. I. Posner, "Imaging attention networks," NeuroImage, vol. 61 no. 2, pp. 450-456, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.040, 2012.
[144] A. Chiesa, P. Malinowski, "Mindfulness-based approaches: are they all the same?," Journal of Clinical Psychology, vol. 67 no. 4, pp. 404-424, DOI: 10.1002/jclp.20776, 2011.
[145] P. R. Goldin, J. J. Gross, "Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder," Emotion, vol. 10 no. 1, pp. 83-91, DOI: 10.1037/a0018441, 2010.
[146] J. M. Sorrell, "Meditation for older adults: a new look at an ancient intervention for mental health," Journal of Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health Services, vol. 53 no. 5, pp. 15-19, DOI: 10.3928/02793695-20150330-01, 2015.
[147] M. Goyal, S. Singh, E. M. S. Sibinga, N. F. Gould, A. Rowland-Seymour, R. Sharma, Z. Berger, D. Sleicher, D. D. Maron, H. M. Shihab, P. D. Ranasinghe, S. Linn, S. Saha, E. B. Bass, J. A. Haythornthwaite, "Meditation programs for psychological stress and well-being: a systematic review and meta-analysis," JAMA Internal Medicine, vol. 174 no. 3, pp. 357-368, DOI: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.13018, 2014.
[148] R. Shaurya Prakash, A. A. de Leon, M. Klatt, W. Malarkey, B. Patterson, "Mindfulness disposition and default-mode network connectivity in older adults," Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, vol. 8 no. 1, pp. 112-117, DOI: 10.1093/scan/nss115, 2013.
[149] R. E. Wells, G. Y. Yeh, C. E. Kerr, J. Wolkin, R. B. Davis, Y. Tan, R. Spaeth, R. B. Wall, J. Walsh, T. J. Kaptchuk, D. Press, R. S. Phillips, J. Kong, "Meditation’s impact on default mode network and hippocampus in mild cognitive impairment: a pilot study," Neuroscience Letters, vol. 556, pp. 15-19, DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.10.001, 2013.
[150] J. A. Moynihan, B. P. Chapman, R. Klorman, M. S. Krasner, P. R. Duberstein, K. W. Brown, N. L. Talbot, "Mindfulness-based stress reduction for older adults: effects on executive function, frontal alpha asymmetry and immune function," Neuropsychobiology, vol. 68 no. 1, pp. 34-43, DOI: 10.1159/000350949, 2013.
[151] D. S. Black, F. Kurth, E. Luders, B. Wu, "Brain gray matter changes associated with mindfulness meditation in older adults: an exploratory pilot study using voxelbased morphometry," Neuro - Open Journal, vol. 1 no. 1, pp. 23-26, DOI: 10.17140/NOJ-1-106, 2014.
[152] B. S. Oken, H. Wahbeh, E. Goodrich, D. Klee, T. Memmott, M. Miller, R. Fu, "Meditation in stressed older adults: improvements in self-rated mental health not paralleled by improvements in cognitive function or physiological measures," Mindfulness, vol. 8 no. 3, pp. 627-638, DOI: 10.1007/s12671-016-0640-7, 2017.
[153] H. Wahbeh, E. Goodrich, B. S. Oken, "Internet-based mindfulness meditation for cognition and mood in older adults: a pilot study," Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, vol. 22 no. 2, pp. 44-53, 2016.
[154] A. Moore, P. Malinowski, "Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility," Consciousness and Cognition, vol. 18 no. 1, pp. 176-186, DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.12.008, 2009.
[155] N. A. S. Farb, A. K. Anderson, H. Mayberg, J. Bean, D. McKeon, Z. V. Segal, "Minding one’s emotions: mindfulness training alters the neural expression of sadness," Emotion, vol. 10 no. 1, pp. 25-33, DOI: 10.1037/a0017151, 2010.
[156] J. R. Krygier, J. A. J. Heathers, S. Shahrestani, M. Abbott, J. J. Gross, A. H. Kemp, "Mindfulness meditation, well-being, and heart rate variability: a preliminary investigation into the impact of intensive Vipassana meditation," International Journal of Psychophysiology, vol. 89 no. 3, pp. 305-313, DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.017, 2013.
[157] S. Brand, E. Holsboer-Trachsler, J. R. Naranjo, S. Schmidt, "Influence of mindfulness practice on cortisol and sleep in long-term and short-term meditators," Neuropsychobiology, vol. 65 no. 3, pp. 109-118, DOI: 10.1159/000330362, 2012.
[158] P. Goldin, W. Ramel, J. Gross, "Mindfulness meditation training and self-referential processing in social anxiety disorder: behavioral and neural effects," Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, vol. 23 no. 3, pp. 242-257, DOI: 10.1891/0889-8391.23.3.242, 2009.
[159] N. A. S. Farb, Z. V. Segal, H. Mayberg, J. Bean, D. McKeon, Z. Fatima, A. K. Anderson, "Attending to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference," Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, vol. 2 no. 4, pp. 313-322, DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsm030, 2007.
[160] D. R. Vago, F. Zeidan, "The brain on silent: mind wandering, mindful awareness, and states of mental tranquility," Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 1373 no. 1, pp. 96-113, DOI: 10.1111/nyas.13171, 2016.
[161] L. Schmalzl, C. E. Kerr, "Editorial: neural mechanisms underlying movement-based embodied contemplative practices," Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 10,DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00169, 2016.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright © 2019 Ricardo Ramírez-Barrantes et al. This work is licensed under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Aging is a physiological process accompanied by cognitive decline, principally in memory and executive functions. Alterations in the connectivity of the default mode network (DMN) have been found to participate in cognitive decline, as well as in several neurocognitive disorders. The DMN has antisynchronic activity with attentional networks (task-positive networks (TPN)), which are critical to executive function and memory. Findings pointing to the regulation of the DMN via activation of TPN suggest that it can be used as a strategy for neuroprotection. Meditation is a noninvasive and nonpharmacological technique proven to increase meta-awareness, a cognitive ability which involves the control of both networks. In this review, we discuss the possibility of facilitating healthy aging through the regulation of networks through meditation. We propose that by practicing specific types of meditation, cognitive decline could be slowed, promoting a healthy lifestyle, which may enhance the quality of life for the elderly.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details





1 Escuela de Tecnología Médica, Universidad Andrés Bello, Quillota 980, 2531015 Viña del Mar, Chile
2 Interdisciplinary Centre for Health Studies (CIESAL), Universidad de Valparaíso, Angamos 655, 2540064 Viña del Mar, Chile; Biomedical Research Centre (CIB), Universidad de Valparaíso, Angamos 655, 2540064 Viña del Mar, Chile; School of Medicine, Universidad de Valparaíso, Angamos 655, 2540064 Viña del Mar, Chile
3 Interdisciplinary Centre for Health Studies (CIESAL), Universidad de Valparaíso, Angamos 655, 2540064 Viña del Mar, Chile
4 División de Neurociencias (NeuroCICS), Centro de Investigación en Complejidad Social (CICS), Facultad de Gobierno, Universidad del Desarrollo, Santiago, Chile
5 Laboratorio de Estructura y Función Celular, Escuela de Medicina, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Valparaíso, Hontaneda 2664, 2341386 Valparaíso, Chile
6 Laboratorio de Neurociencia Cognitiva y Social, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Diego Portales, Chile; Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Interdisciplinary Center for Neuroscience, School of Medicine, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile