About the Authors:
Helena Rawsthorne
Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: School of Biological Sciences, Highfield Campus, University of Southampton, Southampton, United Kingdom
ORCID logo https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2284-8112
Fernando Calahorro
Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: School of Biological Sciences, Highfield Campus, University of Southampton, Southampton, United Kingdom
Lindy Holden-Dye
Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: School of Biological Sciences, Highfield Campus, University of Southampton, Southampton, United Kingdom
ORCID logo https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9704-1217
Vincent O’ Connor
Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: School of Biological Sciences, Highfield Campus, University of Southampton, Southampton, United Kingdom
James Dillon
Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: School of Biological Sciences, Highfield Campus, University of Southampton, Southampton, United Kingdom
ORCID logo https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3244-7483
Introduction
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a pervasive neurodevelopmental behavioural disorder. ASD is characterised by a triad of behavioural impairments, these being repetitive behaviours and impairment to verbal and social communication [1]. Neuro-atypical individuals have been shown to produce altered behavioural outputs in response to a range of sensory cues [2], including chemosensory cues that drive social behaviours [3]. Impairment within the integration of sensory stimuli is thought to underlie the altered perception of such cues [4]. This highlights the importance of neural circuits in the processing of sensory information to coordinate a behavioural output in the social domain [5].
It is well established that there is a strong genetic contribution in autism [6]. The genetic architecture of ASD is complex with hundreds of genes of varying penetrance implicated in its aetiology [7]. This is complicated further by the interplay between genetic variants in the form of rare, highly penetrant, and common low penetrant variants [8,9]. Common variants attribute polygenic risk in ASD with mutations to multiple loci having additive effects on a given phenotype [8]. The burden of common variants in an individual’s genetic background can influence the degree of risk a rare variant can impose [9]. The combinatorial effect of rare and common variants contributes to the spectrum of phenotypes displayed across autism cases [10].
ASD associated genes span across a range of biological functions, for example synaptic, cell signalling and epigenetic modification [11]. Evidence suggests that although ASD genes are functionally diverse they are connected through protein interaction networks [12] and control processes such as neuronal morphology and synaptic function that modulate the activity state of neural networks [8,13,14]. This means that the consequence of even a single genetic variant can be widespread through inter-connecting gene networks and have emergent effects on neural circuits [8]. For many ASD associated genes it is still unclear how they function in neural networks which underpin behavioural phenotypes [15], such as disrupted social and motor behaviour. However, investigating determinants of defined neural circuits underpinning autism associated neuro-atypical behaviour is providing traction for discrete investigation of complex traits. Study of distinct behaviours in mice has begun to unpick the effect of genetic disruption on molecular circuits and synapse function [16–18]. Additionally, the impact of genetic variation on a number of synaptic genes has been extensively studied in various animal models [19–24].
Animal models highlight the value of using orthologues to understand the function of ASD associated genes in behavioural domains associated with autism [25–28]. C. elegans provides a tractable system that allows for the high throughput of genetic determinants to be investigated in a simple nervous system [29]. Conservation of genes involved in synapse function and the use of integrative neurons highlights the utility of C. elegans neuronal function and how it co-ordinates complex sensory integrative behaviours that model the disruptions that are expressed through genetic mutations associated with autism [30,31]. The genetic homology between the C. elegans and mammalian genome [32], and the fact that mutant strains are widely accessible, means that the C. elegans model lends itself to the systems level analysis of disease associated genes. This has led to a plethora of studies using single gene analysis to investigate the impact of genetic mutation to ASD associated gene orthologues on behavioural output [33]. As well as this, C. elegans have been utilised in multiple high-throughput screens which have largely used morphological and locomotory readouts to screen for behavioural deficit [34–36].
Behavioural output in response to integration of sensory cues can be assayed in C. elegans by way of food leaving behaviour. The propensity of a worm to leave a lawn of bacterial food can be modulated by multiple sensory cues [37]. It has been shown that in the presence of increasing numbers of progeny, adult worms will leave an otherwise replete food lawn in a dose-dependent manor. This progeny-dependent food leaving behaviour is the result of inter-organismal communication and is thought to be underpinned by a novel social circuit [38]. The utility of this social paradigm to probe autism related dysfunction was demonstrated by showing that when a penetrant mutation of human neuroligin is introduced into the worm orthologue, nlg-1, it results in disrupted progeny induced food leaving behaviour [39].
We have used this bona fide social paradigm to investigate genetic determinants associated with human ASD. Investigation of C. elegans orthologues in a subset of candidate genes identified a number that disrupt a social behavioural paradigm in the worm. Furthermore, we show that whilst a large proportion of mutants displayed behavioural deficit in the social domain, there was limited disruption to the other phenotypes investigated suggesting a selective behavioural deficit. Identification of novel candidate genes in this way has also highlighted key biological functional domains that appear to play an important role in social behaviour, therefore shedding light on the functional contribution ASD associated genes may have on the disrupted phenotypes associated with this disorder.
Materials and methods
Prioritising ASD associated genes for study in C. elegans
To identify genes associated with ASD we used SFARI Gene Archive (http://gene-archive.sfari.org/, version 3.0). Within this database the Human Gene Module (https://gene-archive.sfari.org/database/human-gene/) ranks genes from 1–6 based on the evidence supporting the gene’s association with ASD. Genes in category 1-High confidence and category 2-Strong candidate were selected for analysis due to the fact that data implicating those genes in ASD reach genome wide significance and there is evidence for the variant having a functional effect in humans. Orthologous genes in C. elegans were identified by searching the human gene name in WormBase (https://wormbase.org/, version WS264) and using the human gene Ensembl ID in OrthoList (http://www.greenwaldlab.org/ortholist/). C. elegans strains available for order from the Caenorhabditis Genetics Centre (CGC) and/or the National BioResource Project (NBRP) were prioritised for investigation. Using information gathered from WormBase, CGC, NBRP and a literature review, mutants were excluded if they were lethal, sterile or uncoordinated. Thus, we filtered for candidates best suited to investigation in the food lawn based assay. The prioritised C. elegans mutant strains for study can be found in Table 1. Genes were ascribed to one of five functional categories: synaptic, neuronal, cell signalling, epigenetic modifiers and phospholipid metabolism based on their function described by UniProtKB (https://www.uniprot.org/uniprot/). Genes described as having a role in synaptic transmission, structure, activity or plasticity were categorised as ‘synaptic’. Genes with a role in neuronal excitability or adhesion were categorised as ‘neuronal’. Genes described as having a predominant role in cell signalling pathways were categorised as ‘cell signalling’. Genes with a role in transcriptional regulation or chromatin remodelling were categorised as ‘epigenetic modifier’. The gene MBOAT7 is described as functioning in phospholipid metabolism and so was categorised as ‘phospholipid metabolism’.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. Summary of human genes prioritised for study in C. elegans mutant strains.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243121.t001
C. elegans culturing and strains used
All C. elegans strains were maintained using standard conditions [80]. C. elegans were age synchronised by picking L4 hermaphrodites onto a new plate 18 hours prior to behavioural assays. Bristol N2 were used as wild-type control. All other strains used can be found in Table 1. Strains were obtained from either the Caenorhabditis Genetics Center or National BioResource Project.
Food leaving assay
5cm NGM (nematode growth medium) plates were prepared using a standard protocol [80]. 50μl of OP50 E.coli at OD600 of 0.8 was gently spotted on the middle of an unseeded plate. Approximately 18 hours following this, seven L4+1 day old hermaphrodites were picked onto the centre of the bacterial lawn. Plates were then incubated at 20°C for 24 hours, during this time the seven assay worms lay eggs which produces a progeny laden lawn. In all food leaving assays the number of food leaving events were counted manually during a 30 minute observation period using a binocular dissecting microscope (Nikon SMZ800; X10). A food leaving event was defined as when the whole of the worm’s body came off the bacterial food lawn, as previously described [39]. Following each food leaving assay the % proportion of eggs, L1 and L2 progeny on the plate was calculated. For all food leaving assays N2 and nlg-1(ok259) animals were analysed in parallel to other mutant cohorts. Investigators were blind to the genotypes being observed.
Pre-conditioned food leaving assay
NGM plates were prepared and seeded as described above. 18 hours after seeding assay plates, half were pre-conditioned with progeny using the protocol described previously [38] and the remaining plates were used as matched unconditioned controls. In the preconditioned plates 10 gravid adults were picked onto the centre of the bacterial lawn and left to lay 150–200 eggs before being picked off. 18 hours after this, for each mutant under investigation, seven L4+1 day old hermaphrodites were picked onto the centre of a naïve bacterial food lawn. This acts as a matched unconditioned control. Another seven L4+1 day old hermaphrodites were picked onto a pre-conditioned bacterial food lawn in which 150–200 eggs had developed for 18 hours. The plates were then incubated at 20°C for 2 hours, this time course and pre-conditioning is equivalent to a 24 hour naive incubation. Leaving events were then observed for 30 minutes as described above.
Pharyngeal pumping
Following the measurement of food leaving at the 24 hour time point, feeding behaviour was quantified by counting the pharyngeal pumping of three of the seven worms. The worms selected for these measurements were on food for the observation period. One pharyngeal pump was defined as one cycle of contraction-relaxation of the terminal bulb of the pharyngeal muscle. This behaviour was measured for 1 minute using a binocular dissecting microscope (Nikon SMZ800; X63) and the pumps per minute for each worm recorded from a single observation [81].
Thrashing
Thrashing analysis was performed on the C. elegans mutants that were investigated in the pre-conditioned food leaving assay. Using a 24 well plate, 6–7 N2 or mutant worms were picked per well containing 500μl of M9 with 0.1% bovine serum albumin and left for 5–10 minutes before thrashing was observed. For each worm thrashing was counted for 30 seconds. Each thrash was defined as a complete movement through the midpoint of the worms body and back. For each mutant under investigation N2 control worms were analysed in parallel and at least two separate assays were performed. Investigators were blind to the genotypes being investigated.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was performed using GraphPad Prism 8 software. Data are expressed as mean or mean ±SEM as indicated in the figure legend. Statistical tests and post-hoc analysis is indicated in the figure legends. Significance level was set to P<0.05.
Results
Selection of human ASD associated genes for study using C. elegans social behaviour
The genetic architecture of autism is complex with over 1,000 genes currently implicated in the disorder [11]. Furthermore, the functional contribution that many of these genes make to the behavioural domains implicated in ASD remains unclear. We have created a pipeline (Fig 1) to select C. elegans orthologues of human ASD associated genes and that can be investigated in a paradigm of social behaviour in the worm.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 1. Prioritisation and categorization of the C. elegans orthologues of prioritised human ASD associated genes.
High confidence ASD associated genes in category 1 and 2 in SFARI Gene Archive were input. The pipeline selects human genes which have an orthologue in C. elegans which can be studied in an available mutant strain which is neither lethal, sterile or uncoordinated. In brackets are the resources used for analysis. CGC–Caenorhabditis Genetics Center. NBRP–National BioResource Project. The number of genes analysed using SFARI Gene Archive (https://gene-archive.sfari.org/, accessed October 2018) are stated. The pie chart indicates the percent of the 21 human genes that were placed into five functional groupings.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243121.g001
We used SFARI Gene, a growing database which categorises ASD risk genes based on the strength of evidence supporting the association. We prioritised 91 genes ranked by SFARI Gene Archive (accessed October 2018) as category 1-high confidence and category 2-strong candidate. Of these 91 genes, 84% (76/91) had at least one orthologue in C. elegans. A mutant strain was available for 84% (64/76) of the orthologous genes using the criteria that the mutant strain was available from the CGC and/or NBRP. Of these, 43 genes had available mutants that were either lethal, sterile or uncoordinated (Fig 1). We considered that such phenotypes rendered these mutants unsuitable for investigation in the social behaviour assay. On this basis we selected 40 C. elegans mutants spanning 21 human ASD associated genes for further investigation (Table 1). The human ASD associated genes were each assigned to a group based upon the functional description of the encoded protein in UniProtKB database. The functional groupings were: synaptic, neuronal, cell signalling, epigenetic modifiers and phospholipid metabolism. This led to a distribution of candidates highlighting 43% as synaptic genes, 33% as epigenetic modifiers, 10% as cell signalling, 9% neuronal and 5% phospholipid metabolism (Fig 1).
Screening mutants using food leaving behaviour identifies ASD associated genetic determinants of social behaviour
To investigate food leaving behaviour, mutants were picked onto the centre of a bacterial lawn and food leaving events were measured after 24 hours. During the 24 hour incubation period the adult worms lay eggs which hatch into C. elegans progeny. It has been previously shown that progeny-derived social cues mediate a progeny-dependent increase in adult food leaving behaviour [38]. In accordance with previous findings we observed that N2 worms left the food lawn after 24 hours at a rate of approximately 0.088 leaving events/worm/minute (Fig 2). We had previously established a blunted food leaving response for the nlg-1(ok259) mutant [39] and this was used as an internal measure in the current assays (Fig 2).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 2. Food leaving behaviour of C. elegans mutants after 24 hours on food to investigate human ASD associated genes.
A food leaving assay was performed with N2, nlg-1(ok259) and 39 other C. elegans mutants. Genes are categorised and colour coded into different functional domains. The black line indicates the number of leaving events/worm/minute for N2 control. The red line indicates the food leaving rate of nlg-1(ok259) control. N2 and nlg-1(ok259) n = 19. All other mutants n = 3–4, where n refers to the number of replicates of an individual experiment. Strains were screened in batches across different days. Each batch consisted of 4–6 mutant strains and a paired wild-type control. Data plotted includes all wild-type controls. All data shown as mean ±SEM. Statistical analysis performed using a one-way ANOVA and Dunnetts’s multiple comparison test; ns, p>0.05; *, p<0.05; **, p p≤0.01; ***, p≤0.001; ****, p≤0.0001. All significance relates to a comparison with N2 control.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243121.g002
Against this backdrop, N2 and nlg-1(ok259) were investigated alongside the selected mutants we filtered through following initial selection from the SAFRI Gene data base. This comparison showed that 23 of the 39 C. elegans mutants showed a mean food leaving rate lower than that of nlg-1(ok259) suggesting food leaving impairment (Fig 2). Mutants with a reduced food leaving phenotype were distributed across the five functional categories we defined suggesting genetic disruption within a range of molecular determinants from distinct biological domains may contribute to the emergence of C. elegans social behaviour.
As part of the investigation, where possible, we analysed two or more mutant alleles for a single gene (Fig 2). For some mutants, for example for gap-2 and rig-6 mutants, the two mutants phenocopied one another and showed a food leaving rate similar to that of N2. Interestingly, we found two loss-of-function nmr-2 mutants which also phenocopied one another but showed significant food leaving impairment (Fig 2). In contrast, there were also instances where mutant alleles did not phenocopy each other. For example nrx-1 and chd-7 mutants showed one mutant allele with impaired food leaving and one with a behavioural response to progeny similar to N2 (Fig 2).
Impaired social behaviour of mutants is likely a selective response to progeny derived social cues
Previous work has identified the value of investigating additional behaviours that can be scored in the observational arena [36]. In this respect the food leaving assay allows for multi-tracking phenotypic analysis including pharyngeal pumping, development and egg laying. In the case of pharyngeal pumping and egg laying, this reflects the output of a defined neuromodulation and the possible consequence progeny exposure might have on this. In the case of development, this provides insight into whether the mutations perturb gross development, a useful consideration in a neurodevelopmental disorder.
After each food leaving assay we quantified the pharyngeal pump rate of the mutants. Pharyngeal pumping is modulated via external sensory cues such as food [82]. Therefore we wanted to test whether another sensory regulated behaviour was affected in these mutants. 87% of mutants showed no pumping phenotype (Fig 3). In fact the majority of mutants with impaired social behaviour (Fig 2) had a pumping rate similar to N2 (Fig 3). This shows that most mutants with reduced food leaving behaviour are largely capable of responding to food-dependent sensory cues and co-ordinating normal feeding behaviour. In addition, the cca-1(ad1650) mutant which showed the most deficient pumping phenotype (Fig 3) did not show a food leaving phenotype (Fig 2), further suggesting that deficits in feeding behaviour are unlikely to explain differences in food leaving behaviour.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 3. Pharyngeal pump rate for C. elegans mutants.
After a food leaving assay at 24 hours, three worms were chosen at random and their pharyngeal pump rate was counted per minute. N2 and nlg-1(ok259) n = 57. All other mutants n = 9–12, where n refers to the number of individual animals investigated. Strains were screened in batches across different days. Each batch consisted of 4–6 mutant strains and a paired wild-type control. Data plotted includes all wild-type controls. The red line indicates pumps per minute for N2 control. All data shown as mean ±SEM. Statistical analysis performed using a one-way ANOVA and Dunnetts’s multiple comparison test; ns, p>0.05; *, p<0.05; **, p≤0.01; ***, p≤0.001; ****, p≤0.0001. All significance relates to a comparison with N2 control.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243121.g003
Next, we measured early development by quantifying the proportion of total progeny that were eggs, L1 and L2 progeny 24 hours after introducing 7 L4+1. We used % proportion to normalise for observed variation in the total number of eggs laid. 75% of mutants developed at a similar rate to N2 showing that there is no gross early developmental delay (Fig 4). Interestingly, whilst early development seems to be largely unaffected we noted a larger variation in the egg laying of distinct mutants when compared to N2 controls (S1 Fig). The number of eggs laid by a mutant is an important consideration for this assay because the density of progeny populating a food lawn is known to influence the food leaving rate of adult worms [38]. We plotted the relationship between the number of progeny produced by a mutant and the food leaving behaviour and showed that the two were correlated (Fig 5). Interestingly, this applies to the nrx-1 and chd-7 mutants for which the two alleles tested resulted in distinct social phenotypes (Fig 2). In each case the mutant that showed impaired social behaviour (Fig 2) also produced fewer progeny (S1 Fig). Producing fewer progeny means adult worms were exposed to fewer progeny-derived social cues [38] and could explain the low food leaving rate seen. The correlation between social behaviour and progeny exposure, and the limited disruption seen to the other phenotypes tested, implies that progeny-derived social cues selectively effect social behaviour and therefore progeny exposure is an important consideration in this type of investigation.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 4. Percent total eggs and progeny produced by C. elegans mutants at 24 hours.
After a food leaving assay from naïve food lawns occupied by 7 L4+1, the percent total offspring that were eggs, L1 and L2 progeny were quantified. N2 and nlg-1(ok259) n = 19. All other mutants n = 3–4, where n refers to the number of replicates of an individual experiment. Strains were screened in batches across different days. Each batch consisted of 4–6 mutant strains and a paired wild-type control. Data plotted includes all wild-type controls. The black lines indicate % total eggs, % total L1 progeny and % total L2 progeny for N2 control. Pink asterisks indicate statistical difference between mutant and N2 for % total eggs. Blue asterisks indicate statistical difference between mutant and N2 for % total L1 progeny. Purple asterisks indicate statistical difference between mutant and N2 for % total L2 progeny. All data shown as mean ±SEM. Statistical analysis performed using a two-way ANOVA and Tukey’s multiple comparison test; ns, p>0.05; *, p<0.05; **, p≤0.01; ***, p≤0.001; ****, p≤0.0001.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243121.g004
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 5. Correlation between food leaving behaviour of C. elegans mutants and progeny production during food leaving assay.
The percent food leaving rate and progeny produced for each C. elegans mutant was calculated in comparison to N2. N2 and nlg-1(ok259) n = 19. All other mutants n = 3–4, where n refers to the number of replicates of an individual experiment. All data shown as mean. Statistical analysis performed using Pearson correlation coefficient.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243121.g005
Exposure to N2 progeny selectively modulates social behaviour in a number of mutants
Those strains which produced few progeny confound the assessment of the reduced food leaving behaviour as the response is dependent on the density of progeny populating the food lawn [38]. This was addressed by testing the food leaving behaviour of 30 mutants in response to an experimentally controlled number of N2 progeny. This used a pre-conditioning approach in which N2 progeny pre-populate the lawn and precondition them by mimicking the progeny population that emerge in the first food leaving assay. These assays allow the acute effect of progeny exposure on food leaving behaviour to be investigated. This secondary screen focussed on mutants that showed a mean food leaving rate lower than that of nlg-1(ok259) in at least one allele tested (Fig 2). Thus we directly tested the veracity of mutants that emerge from the first screen and explicitly address the potential confound of reduced progeny number. For each mutant we performed a paired experiment in which mutant food leaving was measured on a naïve, unmatched control, plate containing OP50 and a preconditioned plate that incubated 140–200 eggs for 24 hours before introducing 7 L4+1 adults. In accordance with previous findings, N2 adults showed enhanced food leaving in response to progeny and this response was blunted in the nlg-1(ok259) adults exposed to pre-loaded N2 progeny (Fig 6).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 6. Food leaving behaviour of C. elegans mutants in the absence of progeny and exposure to N2 progeny.
A food leaving assay was performed with N2, nlg-1(ok259) and 30 other C. elegans mutants on naïve and pre-conditioned food lawns. A naïve lawn contains no progeny whereas a pre-conditioned food lawn contains ~150–200 N2 progeny. The red line indicates the food leaving rate of N2 naïve control. The black line indicates the food leaving rate of the N2 pre-loaded control. Data shown as mean ±SEM. N2 and nlg-1(ok259) n = 16. All other mutants n = 3–4, where n refers to the number of replicates of an individual experiment. Strains were screened in batches across different days. Each batch consisted of 4–6 mutant strains and a paired wild-type control. Data plotted includes all wild-type controls. Statistical analysis performed using a two-way ANOVA and Sidak’s multiple comparison test; ns, p>0.05; p ≤0.001****.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243121.g006
Analysis of mutants in response to pre-loaded N2 progeny revealed a number of mutants which left infrequently on both naïve and pre-conditioned food lawns, showing little progeny-enhanced food leaving (Fig 6). We reasoned that the low food leaving rate of these mutants could be explained by locomotory deficits. To address this we performed a thrashing assay to assess the innate movement ability of the mutants. 12 of the 31 mutants tested showed minor disruption to thrashing behaviour (Fig 7A). For the majority of mutants thrashing did not predict food leaving phenotype. For example, nlg-1(ok259) and set-4 mutants showed deficits in food leaving behaviour (Fig 6) without any impairment to thrashing (Fig 7A). Furthermore, four mutants of the mboa-7 gene all showed impaired progeny-induced food leaving behaviour with only one of these mutants having reduced thrashing (Fig 7A). The bioinformatic pipeline filtered out mutant strains deficient in gross motility. In addition, we investigated the association between thrashing and food leaving behaviour and identified that there is a correlation between these behaviours (Fig 7B). This suggests that, whilst there are examples of mutants that have impaired food leaving in the absence of a thrashing phenotype, we cannot discount more subtle motility defects contributing to the summed food leaving response. The social impairment we observed in mutants suggests that a variety of genes may act as molecular determinants of social behaviour. Interestingly, these genes were part of synaptic, cell signalling, epigenetic modifier and phospholipid metabolism categories. This highlights that molecular determinants from these biological domains may be important for the emergence of social behaviour.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 7. Thrashing behaviour of C. elegans mutants compared to N2.
A 5–10 minutes after being picked into liquid medium, C. elegans thrashing behaviour was measured for 30 seconds per worm. The black line indicates the thrashes/30s of N2 control. All data shown as mean ±SEM. Statistical analysis performed using a one-way ANOVA and Dunnetts’s multiple comparison test; ns, p>0.05; *, p<0.05; ***, p≤0.001; ****, p≤0.0001. All significance relates to a comparison with N2 control. B Correlation between thrashing (Fig 7A) and food leaving behaviour on N2 pre-loaded food lawns (Fig 6). The percent thrashing rate and food leaving rate on N2 pre-loaded food lawns for C. elegans mutants was calculated in comparison to N2. N2 is indicated by the datapoint coloured in blue. All data shown as mean. Statistical analysis performed using Pearson correlation coefficient. For thrashing experiments N2 n = 88. All other mutants n = 10–13, where n refers to the number of individual animals investigated. Strains were screened in batches across different days. Each batch consisted of 3–8 mutant strains and a paired wild-type control. For food leaving assays N2 and nlg-1(ok259) n = 16. All other mutants n = 3–4, where n refers to the number of replicates of an individual experiment. Strains were screened in batches across different days. Each batch consisted of 4–6 mutant strains and a paired wild-type control.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243121.g007
The comparison of behaviour on naïve and pre-conditioned lawns also allowed for the analysis of egg laying behaviour in response to progeny. We quantified the number of eggs laid by each mutant on naive and pre-conditioned food lawns after each food leaving assay. Interestingly, all mutants laid the same number of eggs on naïve and pre-conditioned food lawns (S2 Fig). This shows that progeny exposure modulates food leaving behaviour and not egg laying. This therefore suggests that the circuit which integrates progeny cues to sculpt food leaving motility is independent of egg laying behaviour which is modulated by other environmental cues.
Overall, starting with 91 human ASD associated genes we used criteria based filtering to define 21 candidate genes for analysis using a C. elegans social paradigm. An initial screen of social behaviour in response to progeny produced over 24 hours indicated 23 mutants with a reduced food leaving phenotype. We then confirmed the veracity of this phenotype using a progeny pre-conditioned food leaving approach and identified mutants that showed a socially impaired phenotype. The limited disruption in other phenotypes tested for these mutants suggests that reduced food leaving is a selective social impairment in response to progeny-derived social cues. Identification of these mutants highlights genetic determinants that appear to play a role in social behaviour and also suggests that a number of biological domains (synaptic, cell signalling, epigenetic modification and phospholipid metabolism) are important for the coordination of social behaviour.
Discussion
ASD is characterised by a triad of behavioural impairments including neuro-atypical behaviour in the social domain [1]. Individuals with ASD have also been shown to produce altered behavioural responses to a range of chemosensory cues such as olfactory, tactile and gustatory cues [5,83]. Multi-sensory processing deficits identified in ASD highlights the importance of sensory integration at a circuit level [84] however, it is still unclear how disruption within neural circuits evoke a modified behavioural output. Recent experiments have highlighted the value of investigating molecular determinants of ASD in the context of defined integrative circuits to try and understand more precisely how disruption within these circuits underpins the phenotypes associated with ASD [16,17,36,47]. Approaches, such as these, that better resolve the underlying mechanisms should facilitate pharmacological treatment of ASD and other neuropsychiatric disorders [85].
ASD is known to have a complex genetic architecture, with hundreds of genes with varying penetrance implicated in its aetiology [7]. Although the genetic basis is well documented the functional contribution which many of the genes make to the behavioural domains associated with ASD is unclear. Animal models have begun to understand genetic contribution in autism [26]. The analysis of single, high penetrant, variants is becoming increasingly well refined with the use of animal social behaviours. Use of social behaviours underpinned by discrete neural circuits has helped establish the role of some ASD associated genes in the social domain [39,86]. However, the analysis of common, low penetrant, variants is more complex. Additive effects from polygenic interaction of multiple common variants contributes to wide spread disruption at distinct levels of the biological system which is expressed as an emergent behaviour [8].
C. elegans have been used in targeted single gene approaches and in screens of ASD associated genes to provide valuable insight into the role of some genes in sensory processing, development and learning phenotypes [36,87]. Recently we have shown the utility of using a social behavioural paradigm in C. elegans to investigate a single ASD associated gene [39]. This paradigm is based on inter-organismal signalling by use of chemosensory social cues which results in a progeny-induced food leaving phenotype [38]. In this study we have used this social paradigm in a screen of ASD associated genes and identified gene candidates with a role in C. elegans social behaviour.
We created a pipeline to prioritise human genes for investigation using C. elegans social behaviour. The pipeline identified 84% of the ASD genes ranked in categories 1 and 2 by SFARI have an orthologue in C. elegans. In general, the rate of conservation between the human and C. elegans genomes is 53% [88] and suggests an enriched conservation of ASD genes between humans and C. elegans. A large proportion of the orthologous genes had known developmental phenotypes and were not selected for further analysis in this study. The analysis of such genes has the potential to provide broader insight into other neuropsychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia given the overlap of risk genes between this disorder and ASD and the shared neurodevelopmental component of these two conditions. We selected 21 human genes for investigation using 40 C. elegans mutant orthologues. Similarities between our prioritisation strategy and those used in previous C. elegans studies resulted in the iterative selection of some well-studied ASD associated genes such as neuroligin and neurexin [34,36]. Our study is distinct from others because we biased our gene filtering approach to select for C. elegans mutants that were appropriate for analysis of social behaviour using a pre-conditioned food leaving approach. We were selective in choosing mutants appropriate for our behavioural analysis, for example the exclusion of overt locomotory mutants due to their possible confounding effect on food leaving motility.
We used a single point analysis focused on progeny induced food leaving from which we also analysed pharyngeal pumping, early development and egg laying capabilities in response to progeny derived social cues. We identified a number of mutants with an altered behavioural response to progeny populating a food lawn providing evidence that C. elegans are capable of modelling disruption to an emergent behaviour in response to mutation to an ASD associated gene. Where possible more than a single variant for a particular gene of interest was tested. Whilst in the majority of cases there was concordance in the behavioural output of variants, there were some exceptions and this may reflect differences in the number of times strains were outcrossed and potential differences in genetic backgrounds. Refined parametrization of the sub-behaviour that underpins the food leaving behaviour, combined with precise genetic lesion will facilitate the systematic testing of alleles with equivalence to human mutations [39].
Movement in liquid has been used in other studies to screen ASD associated genes [34]. Our analysis of thrashing in mutants identified that this type of locomotory assay does not accurately predict an impaired food leaving behaviour and does not serve as a surrogate for the more complex integrative progeny-induced social behaviour phenotype. This highlights our behavioural screen as a unique platform which is selectively tuned to identify genetic determinants with a role in social circuits, that when disrupted could appear phenotypically normal in thrashing behaviour.
The candidates that we identified as having a role in social behaviour are orthologues of human genes that range in function including synaptic, cell signalling and epigenetic modification. Genes disrupted in each of these domains are known to contribute to ASD [89,90]. Therefore, the genes that emerge from our screen are representative of the main functional domains disrupted in autism. Our screen has therefore produced a diverse list of candidate genes that can be used to interrogate the systems level disruption that evokes modified behavioural output in ASD. Previous work has focused largely on locomotory and morphological readouts of altered behavioural phenotypes [34–36] whereas our approach facilitates the identification of candidate genes with a role in a more complex, sensory regulated, emergent behaviour which more closely resembles the social domain disrupted in autism. Identification of candidates using this approach therefore provides a benchmark from which the social circuit can be further dissected [91].
We identified five synaptic genes, nlg-1, nrx-1, shn-1, glr-1 and nmr-2, with a role in coordinating progeny-induced social behaviour. In the mammalian nervous system, NLGN, NRXN and SHANK’s interaction at the synapse is well established and dysfunction to all three genes has been widely implicated in ASD [92]. Synaptic scaffolds including SHANK are known to interact with receptors such as AMPA and NMDA to help regulate the ion channel composition at the synapse [93]. This provides evidence that this assay for social interaction identifies behavioural disruption in orthologues of genes that function together at mammalian synapses. The role of these mammalian genes in nervous system function and/or synaptic transmission are functionally conserved in C. elegans [40,49,51,94–96]. This means that we can resolve singular determinants with the potential to unpick genes that encode dysfunctional interactions. This raises the opportunity to model the polygenic nature of ASD [8,36,97–99].
Previous scaled use of assays to investigate ASD associated genes in C. elegans used strategies to prioritise genes before behavioural analysis [34–36]. The outcome of these studies resulted in an incomplete overlap of some genes investigated in our study. We made a comparison between C. elegans mutants that emerged from our study with an impaired social phenotype to mutants that have emerged from previous studies as having impaired movement and habituation phenotypes [34,36]. However, the vast majority of mutants that we identified with behavioural impairment are unique to this study. For example, shn-1(gk181) and set-9(n4949) show impaired social behaviour in response to progeny, whilst appearing grossly wild-type for the other phenotypes we tested. In addition these mutants do not show a behavioural phenotype in movement or habituation behaviour when investigated in previous studies [34,36]. This highlights that the emergent behaviour we have used reveals genes that are missed when they emerge from the bioinformatic pipeline. This makes the case that applying a lower throughput observer based assay will refine previous efforts to model the functional impact of genes implicated in ASD.
The emergent behaviour that we have used is a complex, sensory integrative behaviour. Habituation learning is another complex behaviour in C. elegans that has been investigated in a previous screen of ASD associated genes [36]. Therefore, we wanted to identify whether there was overlap in mutants with behavioural impairment in two distinct complex behavioural phenotypes. We made a comparison of mutants that we had identified as having a social impairment to mutants that have been shown to have a habituation phenotype [36]. We identified four synaptic mutants, nlg-1(ok259), nrx-1(ds1), glr-1(n2461) and nmr-2(ok3324), which have impaired social behaviour and have also been shown to have a habituation phenotype. This suggests that these genes may have a role in coordinating more than one complex sensory-regulated behaviour in C. elegans. This may also suggest that a key role of synaptic genes is in coordinating higher behaviours in C. elegans. With this in mind it would be interesting to extend the analysis of mutants with habituation impairment and screen them for social deficits. Our approach lends itself to the identification of complex behavioural deficits and so would be valuable in this analysis to further understand if there is an over-representation of synaptic genes in complex sensory integrative phenotypes.
In addition to highlighting the important contribution of synaptic, cell signalling and epigenetic genes, we identify a gene involved in phospholipid metabolism that appears to play a role in progeny-induced social behaviour. mboa-7 is a lysoPI acetyltransferase which is important in the regulation of phospholipid membranes [100] and cell signalling [78] in C. elegans. In mammals, the regulation of membrane composition is important for cellular processes, signalling and nervous system function [101,102]. Studies of the MBOAT7 ortholog in mice suggest it may function in brain development [103], however this gene is comparatively less well studied than other ASD associated genes for its functional contribution to the disorder. Therefore, the identification of this gene with a role in progeny-induced social behaviour highlights how this study enriches the understanding of the molecular determinants of social behaviour from underrepresented genes in autism.
In conclusion, investigation of ASD associated orthologues in C. elegans identified genes from a number of candidates implicated in ASD that disrupt social behaviour in the worm. Identification of these genes highlights how this assay might be used in quantitative approaches that can probe the single [39] and polygenic nature of ASD and its underpinning genetic architecture [36]. The robust nature of this assay provokes a better detailing of the cellular and circuit dependence of this social interaction. Guided by the cellular determinants of behaviour, investigation can extend to probe the polygenic nature of ASD and take a similar approach in other psychiatric diseases that have significant consequences for behavioural traits in the social domain [104,105].
Supporting information
S1 Fig. Proportion of eggs and progeny produced by C. elegans mutants after 24 hours.
After a food leaving assay the number of eggs and progeny produced after 24 hours was counted. N2 and nlg-1(ok259) n = 19. All other mutants n = 3–4, where n refers to the number of replicates of an individual experiment. Strains were screened in batches across different days. Each batch consisted of 4–6 mutant strains and a paired wild-type control. Data plotted includes all wild-type controls. The black line indicates the total number of eggs and progeny produced by N2 control. All data shown as mean ±SEM. Statistical analysis performed using a two-way ANOVA and Dunnetts’s multiple comparison test; ns, p>0.05; *, p<0.05; **, p≤0.01; ***, p≤0.001; ****, p≤0.0001. All significance relates to the total number of eggs and progeny produced in comparison with N2 control.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243121.s001
(TIF)
S2 Fig. The number of eggs laid is unchanged in the presence of progeny.
After a food leaving assay the number of eggs laid was quantified. The black line indicates the number of eggs laid by N2 control. All data shown as mean ±SEM. N2 and nlg-1(ok259) n = 16. All other mutants n = 3–4, where n refers to the number of replicates of an individual experiment. Strains were screened in batches across different days. Each batch consisted of 4–6 mutant strains and a paired wild-type control. Data plotted includes all wild-type controls. Statistical analysis performed using a two-way ANOVA and sidak’s multiple comparison test. No significance was identified when comparing eggs laid on a naïve and pre-loaded food lawn for each strain.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243121.s002
(TIF)
Acknowledgments
Some strains were provided by the CGC. Some strains were provided by the National BioResource Project (NBRP) Japan.
Citation: Rawsthorne H, Calahorro F, Holden-Dye L, O’ Connor V, Dillon J (2021) Investigating autism associated genes in C. elegans reveals candidates with a role in social behaviour. PLoS ONE 16(5): e0243121. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243121
1. Faras H, Al Ateeqi N, Tidmarsh L. Autism spectrum disorders. Ann Saudi Med. 2010;30(4):295–300. pmid:20622347
2. Balasco L, Provenzano G, Bozzi Y. Sensory Abnormalities in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Focus on the Tactile Domain, From Genetic Mouse Models to the Clinic. Front Psychiatry. 2020;10:1016. pmid:32047448
3. Endevelt-Shapira Y, Perl O, Ravia A, Amir D, Eisen A, Bezalel V, et al. Altered responses to social chemosignals in autism spectrum disorder. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21(1):111–119. pmid:29180748
4. Stevenson RA, Siemann JK, Schneider BC, Eberly HE, Woynaroski TG, Camarata SM, et al. Multisensory Temporal Integration in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Neurosci. 2014;34(3):691–697. pmid:24431427
5. Marco EJ, Hinkley LBN, Hill SS, Nagarajan SS. Sensory processing in autism: a review of neurophysiologic findings. Pediatr Res. 2011;69(5 Pt 2):48R–54R. pmid:21289533
6. Huguet G, Ey E, Bourgeron T. The Genetic Landscapes of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2013;14:191–213. pmid:23875794
7. De Rubeis S, Buxbaum JD. Genetics and genomics of autism spectrum disorder: embracing complexity. Hum Mol Genet. 2015;24:R24–R31. pmid:26188008
8. Iakoucheva LM, Muotri AR, Sebat J. Getting to the Cores of Autism. Cell. 2019;178(6):1287–1298. pmid:31491383
9. Bourgeron T. From the genetic architecture to synaptic plasticity in autism spectrum disorder. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(9):551–563. pmid:26289574
10. Weiner DJ, Wigdor EM, Ripke S, Walters RK, Kosmicki JA, Grove J, et al. Polygenic transmission disequilibrium confirms that common and rare variation act additively to create risk for autism spectrum disorders. Nat Genet. 2017;49(7):978–985. pmid:28504703
11. De Rubeis S, He X, Goldberg AP, Poultney CS, Samocha K, Cicek AE, et al. Synaptic, transcriptional and chromatin genes disrupted in autism. Nature. 2014;515(7526):209–215. pmid:25363760
12. Corominas R, Yang XP, Lin GN, Kang SL, Shen Y, Ghamsari L, et al. Protein interaction network of alternatively spliced isoforms from brain links genetic risk factors for autism. Nat Commun. 2014;5:3650. pmid:24722188
13. Krumm N, O’Roak BJ, Shendure J, Eichler EE. A de novo convergence of autism genetics and molecular neuroscience. Trends Neurosci. 2014;37(2):95–105. pmid:24387789
14. Aguirre-Chen C, Stec N, Ramos OM, Kim N, Kramer M, McCarthy S, et al. A Caenorhabditis elegans Model for Integrating the Functions of Neuropsychiatric Risk Genes Identifies Components Required for Normal Dendritic Morphology. G3. 2020;10(5):1617–1628. pmid:32132169
15. Geschwind DH. Genetics of autism spectrum disorders. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(9):409–416. pmid:21855394
16. Kim H, Lim CS, Kaang BK. Neuronal mechanisms and circuits underlying repetitive behaviors in mouse models of autism spectrum disorder. Behav Brain Funct. 2016;12:3. pmid:26790724
17. Golden CEM, Buxbaum JD, Rubeis S. Disrupted circuits in mouse models of autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2018;48:106–112. pmid:29222989
18. Lee E, Lee J, Kim E. Excitation/Inhibition Imbalance in Animal Models of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;81(10):838–847. pmid:27450033
19. Yoo J, Bakes J, Bradley C, Collingridge GL, Kaang BK. Shank mutant mice as an animal model of autism. Philos Trans R Soc B-Biol Sci. 2014;369(1633):20130143. pmid:24298145
20. Verma V, Paul A, Vishwanath AA, Vaidya B, Clement JP. Understanding intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorders from common mouse models: synapses to behaviour. Open Biol. 2019;9(6):180265. pmid:31185809
21. Rabaneda LG, Robles-Lanuza E, Nieto-Gonzalez JL, Scholl FG. Neurexin Dysfunction in Adult Neurons Results in Autistic-like Behavior in Mice. Cell Rep. 2014;8(2):337–345. pmid:25017069
22. Hunter JW, Mullen GP, McManus JR, Heatherly JM, Duke A, Rand JB. Neuroligin-deficient mutants of C. elegans have sensory processing deficits and are hypersensitive to oxidative stress and mercury toxicity. Dis Model Mech. 2010;3(5–6):366–376. pmid:20083577
23. Calahorro F, Ruiz-Rubio M. Functional Phenotypic Rescue of Caenorhabditis elegans Neuroligin-Deficient Mutants by the Human and Rat NLGN1 Genes. PLoS One. 2012;7(6):e39277. pmid:22723984
24. Calahorro F, Ruiz-Rubio M. Human alpha- and beta-NRXN1 isoforms rescue behavioral impairments of Caenorhabditis elegans neurexin-deficient mutants. Genes Brain Behav. 2013;12(4):453–464. pmid:23638761
25. Schroeder JC, Reim D, Boeckers TM, Schmeisser MJ. Genetic Animal Models for Autism Spectrum Disorder. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2017;30:311–324. pmid:26602248
26. Crawley JN. Translational animal models of autism and neurodevelopmental disorders. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2012;14(3):293–305. pmid:23226954
27. Ueoka I, Pham HTN, Matsumoto K, Yamaguchi M. Autism Spectrum Disorder-Related Syndromes: Modeling with Drosophila and Rodents. Int J Mol Sci. 2019;20(17):4071. pmid:31438473
28. Tang W, Davidson JD, Zhang G, Conen KE, Fang J, Serluca F, et al. Genetic Control of Collective Behavior in Zebrafish. iScience. 2020;23(3):100942. pmid:32179471
29. Sengupta P, Samuel ADT. Caenorhabditis elegans: a model system for systems neuroscience. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2009;19(6):637–643. pmid:19896359
30. Bargmann CI. Neurobiology of the Caenorhabditis elegans genome. Science. 1998;282(5396):2028–2033. pmid:9851919
31. Metaxakis A, Petratou D, Tavernarakis N. Multimodal sensory processing in Caenorhabditis elegans. Open Biol. 2018;8(6):180049. pmid:29925633
32. Lai CH, Chou CY, Ch’ang LY, Liu CS, Lin WC. Identification of novel human genes evolutionarily conserved in Caenorhabditis elegans by comparative proteomics. Genome Res. 2000;10(5):703–713. pmid:10810093
33. Schmeisser K, Parker JA. Worms on the spectrum—C-elegans models in autism research. Exp Neurol. 2018;299:199–206. pmid:28434869
34. Schmeisser K, Fardghassemi Y, Parker JA. A rapid chemical-genetic screen utilizing impaired movement phenotypes in C.elegans: Input into genetics of neurodevelopmental disorders. Exp Neurol. 2017;293:101–114. pmid:28373024
35. Wong WR, Brugman KI, Maher S, Oh JY, Howe K, Kato M, et al. Autism-associated missense genetic variants impact locomotion and neurodevelopment in Caenorhabditis elegans. Hum Mol Genet. 2019;28(13):2271–2281. pmid:31220273
36. McDiarmid TA, Belmadani M, Liang J, Meili F, Mathews EA, Mullen GP, et al. Systematic phenomics analysis of autism-associated genes reveals parallel networks underlying reversible impairments in habituation. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2019:201912049. pmid:31754030
37. Shtonda BB, Avery L. Dietary choice behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Exp Biol. 2006;209(1):89–102. pmid:16354781
38. Scott E, Hudson A, Feist E, Calahorro F, Dillon J, de Freitas R, et al. An oxytocin-dependent social interaction between larvae and adult C. elegans. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):10122. pmid:28860630
39. Rawsthorne H, Calahorro F, Feist E, Holden-Dye L, O’Connor V, Dillon J. Neuroligin dependence of social behaviour in C. elegans provides a model to investigate an autism associated gene. Hum Mol Genet. 2020;21(29):3546–3553. pmid:33206170
40. Hart AC, Sims S, Kaplan JM. SYNAPTIC CODE FOR SENSORY MODALITIES REVEALED BY C-ELEGANS GLR-1 GLUTAMATE-RECEPTOR. Nature. 1995;378(6552):82–85. pmid:7477294
41. Chalasani SH, Chronis N, Tsunozaki M, Gray JM, Ramot D, Goodman MB, et al. Correction: Corrigendum: Dissecting a circuit for olfactory behaviour in Caenorhabditis elegans Nature. 2016;533(7601):130. pmid:26789243
42. http://www.wormbase.org,release WS264, date 19.01.18.
43. Hukema RK, Rademakers S, Jansen G. Gustatory plasticity in C-elegans involves integration of negative cues and NaCl taste mediated by serotonin, dopamine, and glutamate. Learn Mem. 2008;15(11):829–836. pmid:18984564
44. Brockie PJ, Madsen DM, Zheng Y, Mellem J, Maricq AV. Differential expression of glutamate receptor subunits in the nervous system of Caenorhabditis elegans and their regulation by the homeodomain protein UNC-42. J Neurosci. 2001;21(5):1510–1522. pmid:11222641
45. Lemieux GA, Cunningham KA, Lin L, Mayer F, Werb Z, Ashrafi K. Kynurenic Acid Is a Nutritional Cue that Enables Behavioral Plasticity. Cell. 2015;160(1–2):119–131. pmid:25594177
46. Vohra M, Lemieux GA, Lin L, Ashrafi K. The beneficial effects of dietary restriction on learning are distinct from its effects on longevity and mediated by depletion of a neuroinhibitory metabolite. PLoS Biol. 2017;15(8):e2002032. pmid:28763436
47. Calahorro F, Keefe F, Dillon J, Holden-Dye L, O’Connor V. Neuroligin tuning of pharyngeal pumping reveals extrapharyngeal modulation of feeding in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Exp Biol. 2019;222(3):jeb189423. pmid:30559302
48. Philbrook A, Ramachandran S, Lambert CM, Oliver D, Florman J, Alkema MJ, et al. Neurexin directs partner-specific synaptic connectivity in C-elegans. eLife. 2018;7:e35692. pmid:30039797
49. Maro GS, Gao SB, Olechwier AM, Hung WL, Liu M, Ozkan E, et al. MADD-4/Punctin and Neurexin Organize C. elegans GABAergic Postsynapses through Neuroligin. Neuron. 2015;86(6):1420–1432. pmid:26028574
50. Haklai-Topper L, Soutschek J, Sabanay H, Scheel J, Hobert O, Peles E. The neurexin superfamily of Caenorhabditis elegans. Gene Expr Patterns. 2011;11(1–2):144–150. pmid:21055481
51. Oh WC, Song HO, Cho JH, Park BJ. ANK repeat-domain of SHN-1 Is indispensable for in vivo SHN-1 function in C. elegans. Mol Cells. 2011;31(1):79–84. pmid:21191812
52. Jee C, Lee J, Lee JI, Lee WH, Park BJ, Yu JR, et al. SHN-1, a Shank homologue in C-elegans, affects defecation rhythm via the inositol-1,4,5-trisphosphate receptor. FEBS Lett. 2004;561(1–3):29–36. pmid:15013747
53. Mullen GP, Mathews EA, Saxena P, Fields SD, McManus JR, Moulder G, et al. The Caenorhabditis elegans snf-11 gene encodes a sodium-dependent GABA transporter required for clearance of synaptic GABA. Mol Biol Cell. 2006;17(7):3021–3030. pmid:16641366
54. Gendrel M, Atlas EG, Hobert O. A cellular and regulatory map of the GABAergic nervous system of C. elegans. eLife. 2016;5:e17686. pmid:27740909
55. Gyurko MD, Csermely P, Soti C, Stetak A. Distinct roles of the RasGAP family proteins in C. elegans associative learning and memory. Sci Rep. 2015;5:15084. pmid:26469632
56. Steger KA, Shtonda BB, Thacker C, Snutch TP, Avery L. The C. elegans T-type calcium channel CCA-1 boosts neuromuscular transmission. J Exp Biol. 2005;208(11):2191–2203. pmid:15914662
57. Katidou M, Tavernarakis N, Karagogeos D. The contactin RIG-6 mediates neuronal and non-neuronal cell migration in Caenorhabditis elegans. Dev Biol. 2013;373(1):184–195. pmid:23123963
58. Schwarz V, Pan J, Voltmer-Irsch S, Hutter H. IgCAMs redundantly control axon navigation in Caenorhabditis elegans. Neural Dev. 2009;4:13. pmid:19341471
59. Das R, Melo JA, Thondamal M, Morton EA, Cornwell AB, Crick B, et al. The homeodomain-interacting protein kinase HPK-1 preserves protein homeostasis and longevity through master regulatory control of the HSF-1 chaperone network and TORC1restricted autophagy in Caenorhabditis elegans. PLoS Genet. 2017;13(10):e1007038. pmid:29036198
60. Mack HID, Zhang PC, Fonslow BR, Yates JR. The protein kinase MBK-1 contributes to lifespan extension in daf-2 mutant and germline-deficient Caenorhabditis elegans. Aging-US. 2017;9(5):1414–1432. pmid:28562327
61. Raich WB, Moorman C, Lacefield CO, Lehrer J, Bartsch D, Plasterk RHA, et al. Characterization of Caenorhabditis elegans homologs of the Down syndrome candidate gene DYRK1A. Genetics. 2003;163(2):571–580. pmid:12618396
62. Ogg S, Ruvkun G. The C-elegans PTEN homolog, DAF-18, acts in the insulin receptor-like metabolic signaling pathway. Mol Cell. 1998;2(6):887–893. pmid:9885576
63. Adachi T, Kunitomo H, Tomioka M, Ohno H, Okochi Y, Mori I, et al. Reversal of Salt Preference Is Directed by the Insulin/PI3K and G(q)/PKC Signaling in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 2010;186(4):1309–1319. pmid:20837997
64. Masse I, Molin L, Billaud M, Solari F. Lifespan and dauer regulation by tissue-specific activities of Caenorhabditis elegans DAF-18. Dev Biol. 2005;286(1):91–101. pmid:16153634
65. Brisbin S, Liu J, Boudreau J, Peng J, Evangelista M, Chin-Sang I. A Role for C. elegans Eph RTK Signaling in PTEN Regulation. Dev Cell. 2009;17(4):459–469. pmid:19853560
66. Fukuyama M, Rougvie AE, Rothman JH. C-elegans DAF-18/PTEN mediates nutrient-dependent arrest of cell cycle and growth in the germline. Curr Biol. 2006;16(8):773–779. pmid:16631584
67. Feng HY, Craig HL, Hope IA. Expression Pattern Analysis of Regulatory Transcription Factors in Caenorhabditis elegans. Methods Mol Biol. 2012;786:21–50. pmid:21938618
68. De Arras L, Seng A, Lackford B, Keikhaee MR, Bowerman B, Freedman JH, et al. An Evolutionarily Conserved Innate Immunity Protein Interaction Network. J Biol Chem. 2013;288(3):1967–1978. pmid:23209288
69. Zuryn S, Ahier A, Portoso M, White ER, Morin MC, Margueron R, et al. Sequential histone-modifying activities determine the robustness of transdifferentiation. Science. 2014;345(6198):826–829. pmid:25124442
70. Labbadia J, Morimoto RI. Repression of the Heat Shock Response Is a Programmed Event at the Onset of Reproduction. Mol Cell. 2015;59(4):639–650. pmid:26212459
71. Delaney CE, Chen AT, Graniel JV, Dumas KJ, Hu PJ. A histone H4 lysine 20 methyltransferase couples environmental cues to sensory neuron control of developmental plasticity. Development. 2017;144(7):1273–1282. pmid:28209779
72. Kreher J, Takasaki T, Cockrum C, Sidoli S, Garcia BA, Jensen ON, et al. Distinct Roles of Two Histone Methyltransferases in Transmitting H3K36me3-Based Epigenetic Memory Across Generations in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 2018;210(3):969–982. pmid:30217796
73. Pu MT, Ni ZY, Wang MH, Wang XJ, Wood JG, Helfand SL, et al. Trimethylation of Lys36 on H3 restricts gene expression change during aging and impacts life span. Genes Dev. 2015;29(7):718–731. pmid:25838541
74. Engert CG, Droste R, van Oudenaarden A, Horvitz HR. A C. elegans protein with a PRDM9-like SET domain localizes to chromatin-associated foci and promotes spermatocyte gene expression, sperm production and fertility. PLoS Genet. 2018;14(4):e1007295. pmid:29702639
75. Greer EL, Beese-Sims SE, Brookes E, Spadafora R, Zhu Y, Rothbart SB, et al. A Histone Methylation Network Regulates Transgenerational Epigenetic Memory in C-elegans. Cell Rep. 2014;7(1):113–126. pmid:24685137
76. Wang WK, Chaturbedi A, Wang MH, An S, Velayudhan SS, Lee SS. SET-9 and SET-26 are H3K4me3 readers and play critical roles in germline development and longevity. eLife. 2018;7:e34970. pmid:29714684
77. Andersen EC, Horvitz HR. Two C-elegans histone methyltransferases repress lin-3 EGF transcription to inhibit vulval development. Development. 2007;134(16):2991–2999. pmid:17634190
78. Lee HC, Kubo T, Kono N, Kage-Nakadai E, Gengyo-Ando K, Mitani S, et al. Depletion of mboa-7, an enzyme that incorporates polyunsaturated fatty acids into phosphatidylinositol (PI), impairs PI 3-phosphate signaling in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genes Cells. 2012;17(9):748–757. pmid:22862955
79. Lee HC, Inoue T, Imae R, Kono N, Shirae S, Matsuda S, et al. Caenorhabditis elegans mboa-7, a member of the MBOAT family, is required for selective incorporation of polyunsaturated fatty acids into phosphatidylinositol. Mol Biol Cell. 2008;19(3):1174–1184. pmid:18094042
80. Brenner S. The genetics of Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 1974;77(1):71–94. pmid:4366476
81. Dallière N, Bhatla N, Luedtke Z, Ma DK, Woolman J, Walker RJ, et al. Multiple excitatory and inhibitory neural signals converge to fine-tune Caenorhabditis elegans feeding to food availability. FASEB J. 2016;30(2):836–848. pmid:26514165
82. Li ZY, Li YD, Yi YL, Huang WM, Yang S, Niu WP, et al. Dissecting a central flip-flop circuit that integrates contradictory sensory cues in C. elegans feeding regulation. Nat Commun. 2012;3:776. pmid:22491324
83. Bennetto L, Kuschner ES, Hyman SL. Olfaction and taste processing in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(9):1015–1021. pmid:17572391
84. Stevenson RA, Siemann JK, Woynaroski TG, Schneider BC, Eberly HE, Camarata SM, et al. Evidence for diminished multisensory integration in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(12):3161–3167. pmid:25022248
85. Sahin M, Sur M. Genes, circuits, and precision therapies for autism and related neurodevelopmental disorders. Science. 2015;350(6263):10.1126. pmid:26472761
86. Silverman JL, Yang M, Lord C, Crawley JN. Behavioural phenotyping assays for mouse models of autism. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11(7):490–502. pmid:20559336
87. Calahorro F, Alejandre E, Ruiz-Rubio M. Osmotic avoidance in Caenorhabditis elegans: synaptic function of two genes, orthologues of human NRXN1 and NLGN1, as candidates for autism. J Vis Exp. 2009;(34):1616. pmid:20010541
88. Shaye DD, Greenwald I. OrthoList: A Compendium of C. elegans Genes with Human Orthologs. PLoS One. 2011;6(5):e20085. pmid:21647448
89. Guang S, Pang N, Deng X, Yang L, He F, Wu L, et al. Synaptopathology Involved in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front Cell Neurosci. 2018;12:470. pmid:30627085
90. Rylaarsdam L, Guemez-Gamboa A. Genetic Causes and Modifiers of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front Cell Neurosci. 2019;13:385. pmid:31481879
91. Macosko EZ, Pokala N, Feinberg EH, Chalasani SH, Butcher RA, Clardy J, et al. A hub-and-spoke circuit drives pheromone attraction and social behaviour in C-elegans. Nature. 2009;458(7242):1171–1175. pmid:19349961
92. Sudhof TC. Neuroligins and neurexins link synaptic function to cognitive disease. Nature. 2008;455(7215):903–911. pmid:18923512
93. Sheng M, Kim E. The Postsynaptic Organization of Synapses. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol. 2011;3(12):a005678. pmid:22046028
94. Tong XJ, Lopez-Soto EJ, Li L, Liu HW, Nedelcu D, Lipscombe D, et al. Retrograde Synaptic Inhibition Is Mediated by alpha-Neurexin Binding to the alpha 2 delta Subunits of N-Type Calcium Channels. Neuron. 2017;95(2):326–340. pmid:28669545
95. Maricq AV, Peckol E, Driscoll M, Bargmann CI. Mechanosensory signalling in C. elegans mediated by the GLR-1 glutamate receptor. Nature. 1995;378(6552):78–81. pmid:7477293
96. Kano T, Brockie PJ, Sassa T, Fujimoto H, Kawahara Y, Iino Y, et al. Memory in Caenorhabditis elegans is mediated by NMDA-type ionotropic glutamate receptors. Curr Biol. 2008;18(13):1010–1015. pmid:18583134
97. Buddell T, Friedman V, Drozd CJ, Quinn CC. An autism-causing calcium channel variant functions with selective autophagy to alter axon targeting and behavior. PLoS Genet. 2019;15(12):e1008488. pmid:31805042
98. Genç Ö, An J-Y, Fetter RD, Kulik Y, Zunino G, Sanders SJ, et al. Homeostatic plasticity fails at the intersection of autism-gene mutations and a novel class of common genetic modifiers. eLife. 2020;9:e55775. pmid:32609087
99. Sledziowska M, Kalbassi S, Baudouin SJ. Complex Interactions between Genes and Social Environment Cause Phenotypes Associated with Autism Spectrum Disorders in Mice. eNeuro. 2020;7(4):ENEURO.0124-20.2020. pmid:32669345
100. Lee H-C, Inoue T, Imae R, Kono N, Shirae S, Matsuda S, et al. Caenorhabditis elegans mboa-7, a member of the MBOAT family, is required for selective incorporation of polyunsaturated fatty acids into phosphatidylinositol. Mol Biol Cell. 2008;19(3):1174–1184. pmid:18094042
101. Volpatti JR, Al-Maawali A, Smith L, Al-Hashim A, Brill JA, Dowling JJ. The expanding spectrum of neurological disorders of phosphoinositide metabolism. Dis Model Mech. 2019;12(8):dmm038174. pmid:31413155
102. Raghu P, Joseph A, Krishnan H, Singh P, Saha S. Phosphoinositides: Regulators of Nervous System Function in Health and Disease. Front Mol Neurosci. 2019;12:208. pmid:31507376
103. Lee H-C, Inoue T, Sasaki J, Kubo T, Matsuda S, Nakasaki Y, et al. LPIAT1 regulates arachidonic acid content in phosphatidylinositol and is required for cortical lamination in mice. Mol Biol Cell. 2012;23(24):4689–4700. pmid:23097495
104. St Pourcain B, Robinson EB, Anttila V, Sullivan BB, Maller J, Golding J, et al. ASD and schizophrenia show distinct developmental profiles in common genetic overlap with population-based social communication difficulties. Mol Psychiatr. 2018;23(2):263–270. pmid:28044064
105. Barak B, Feng G. Neurobiology of social behavior abnormalities in autism and Williams syndrome. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19(6):647–655. pmid:29323671
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2021 Rawsthorne et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.