Novel disease-modifying treatments have profound potential to transform the treatment landscape for neurodevelopmental diseases with strong genetic underpinnings.1–2 Detection of a treatment response currently relies on clinical measurements of neurodevelopmental outcome, which can be slow, unreliable and challenging in young and neurologically impaired populations. The electroencephalogram (EEG) provides a direct, objective measure of neuronal activity and offers a promising non-invasive tool to identify early and clinically meaningful biomarkers of neurological function for use in clinical trials.
Angelman syndrome (AS) is a genetic neurodevelopmental disorder occurring in 1 in 10,000 to 25,000 live births3–5 characterized by severe cognitive deficits, motor impairments and a high comorbidity with epilepsy.6–8 AS is caused by loss of function of the maternally inherited and paternally imprinted ubiquitin-protein ligase UBE3A gene in neurons.9,10 Targeted disease-modifying treatments to reinstate UBE3A expression in neurons are under development,11–18 and a robust, easily obtained and quantifiable biomarker that reliably assays disease severity is critical to an assessment of efficacy in clinical trials.
The baseline EEG is strikingly abnormal in AS, even in individuals without clinical seizures.19,20 Diffuse, frequent bursts of notched or polyphasic delta activity (2–4 Hz) are the most characteristic EEG feature of AS and are observed across all genotypic classes of disease.20–23 Quantitative EEG studies have shown increased delta power in individuals with AS compared to neurotypical control subjects24 and have also demonstrated that delta power varies with age25 and genotype26 in patients with AS. Increased delta power is observed in AS mouse models relative to wild-type mice,24 confirming preservation of this EEG phenotype across species. Highlighting the potential utility of delta power as a biomarker in clinical trials, decreased delta power was found to correlate with treatment in a recent open-label clinical trial.27 Although delta power is a promising biomarker for AS, whether this EEG measure correlates with neurological symptoms in this disease remains unknown.
We hypothesized that delta power would predict cognitive function in individuals with AS. Using data gathered in a large natural history study of individuals with AS including concurrent EEG recordings and neurodevelopmental testing, we found that increased delta power strongly correlated with decreased cognitive function in AS. We tested this relationship across states of consciousness, EEG recording modalities and data analysis approaches, and found that delta power provides a robust predictor of cognitive function in each circumstance. This work suggests that EEG delta power provides a powerful, objective, non-invasive, and reliable endpoint for treatment response in emerging clinical trials for AS.
Subjects and Methods Subject enrollmentData were obtained through the large, multicenter Angelman Syndrome Natural History Study (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT00296764) conducted as part of the Rare Diseases Clinical Research Network, Angelman, Rett and Prader-Willi syndrome consortium. Consent was obtained according to the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the institutional review boards of the participating sites. Subjects were recruited at six sites between the years of 2006–2017, and EEGs from two sites (Rady Children’s Hospital/University of Califoria San Diego and Boston Children’s Hospital) comprised the cohort used in this study. We excluded data where more than 3 months had elapsed between EEG and neurodevelopmental data collection.
Neurodevelopmental testingThe Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition, administered by psychologists with extensive testing experience in the AS population, was used to assess developmental functioning.28 The Bayley Scales are normed for typically developing children between 1 and 42 months of age, but have been used to assess patients with developmental disabilities of all ages, including AS.29 Five domains from the Bayley Scales (Cognitive, Expressive Communication, Receptive Communication, Fine Motor and Gross Motor) were assessed for each subject, where maximal scores possible for each test were: Cognitive (91), Receptive Communication (49), Expressive Communication (48), Fine Motor (66) and Gross Motor (72). Raw scores were used for analysis.
EEG data collection and processingEEG recordings were collected using the international 10–20 EEG system on either BioLogic or Xltek systems.30 At each session, 30 min of awake recording and 30 min of asleep recording were attempted.
The EEG data processing pipeline is shown in Figure 1. Data were collected at 200–512 Hz sampling rates. All data were bandpass filtered from 1 to 80 Hz and notch filtered at 60 Hz (using Hamming-windowed finite impulse response filters) for visual inspection. On visual review, sections of data containing at least one channel contaminated by muscle, ocular, movement, or electrical artifacts were manually identified. We choose to perform a manual review, rather than apply an automated method for artifact removal, to avoid the potential subtle impacts of data processing on the results, and carefully distinguish the delta frequency rhythm of interest from artifacts with overlapping spectral content (e.g., due to eye blinks).
FIGURE 1. EEG data processing procedure. (A) Data were referenced to three standard montages for review and analysis: the linked-ear, bipolar, and average references. Electrodes used in computing the reference are highlighted in blue. (B) EEG data were manually reviewed and staged for periods of sleep and wakefulness. (C) Data were visually inspected and manually cleaned to remove non-cerebral artifacts (highlighted here in yellow). An example “clean” dataset is shown (bottom row) with breakpoints identified by vertical black lines. (D) Power spectral densities (PSDs) were calculated for each EEG recording. The delta [alpha] frequency range is indicated with a blue [red] rectangle on the plotted PSD. An example EEG trace (grey) showing filtered delta [alpha] activity in blue [red] in a sample EEG trace is shown in the inset.
Data were analyzed referenced to the common average reference, longitudinal bipolar reference and the linked-ear reference. In addition, regional clusters of electrodes were group-average referenced to assess the impact of subsampling channels on model quality.
EEG data were first manually staged for wake and sleep states by an experienced clinical neurophysiologist (CJC). As children with AS lack normal wake and sleep EEG architecture, here, all data with a blink, movement, or eyes open documented were categorized as awake. All data without a blink or movement artifacts in addition to either documentation of closed eyes or the presence of increased slowing at the vertex and frontocentral regions or well-characterized sleep architecture (e.g., sleep spindles and K-complexes) were classified as sleep. Data without sufficient evidence to categorize them as awake or asleep were excluded from the analysis.
Power spectral density estimates were derived using custom MATLAB scripts. Sliding, non-overlapping 1-sec windows were used to calculate delta power (2–4 Hz), theta power (5–7 Hz), alpha power (8–12 Hz), and beta power (13–30 Hz) using the fast Fourier Transform with a Hamming window taper. For relative power estimates, within each 1-sec sample, power spectral densities were divided by the summed broad-spectrum power (1–50 Hz) in the same 1-sec sample, resulting in a unitless estimate of delta power.
Statistical modelingTo evaluate the relationship between cognitive function and delta power, we modeled cognitive score as a function of delta power and a subject-specific intercept to account for multiple observations from some subjects. We included age and genotype:age as fixed effects in each model. To determine the partial R2 for each predictor, we computed the full model sum of squared error (SSE) and then the reduced model SSE excluding the predictor of interest using the formula:[Image Omitted. See PDF]
In our primary analysis, relative power in the delta band was computed from wake-state EEG data from all 19 channels in the common average reference, excluding all epochs manually marked as containing artifact.
We subsequently evaluated the relationship between delta power and Bayley Cognitive Score in which delta power was estimated from varying states of consciousness (wake, sleep, or mixed states); montages (bipolar longitudinal, common average, or linked-ear reference); pre-processing steps (different subsamplings of electrodes, and cleaned or raw data); and spectral measures (absolute or relative delta power, and relative theta, alpha, or beta band power). To compare across nested models, we used the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC). Using the AIC, the degree of penalty can be approximately understood in terms of the number of parameters, where if comparing two models, the model with the lower AIC would still be preferable even if it had an additional ΔAIC/2 parameters.31 We interpret a change in AIC (ΔAIC) of less than 2 as model equivalency, and ΔAIC ≥ 4 as model superiority.32 To allow direct comparison of AIC scores between models, datasets of equal sizes were compared.
In additional post hoc analyses, we used spectral power in frequency bands outside the 2–4 Hz delta range, as well as raw delta power, to predict cognitive function. All relative power values were normalized to global power in the 1–50 Hz frequency band. To allow direct comparison of coefficient estimates amongst spectral bands with different power value ranges, spectral power values were z-scored, and the standardized coefficient estimates (β ± SE) based on these z-scores are reported. To assess the relationship of delta power to other developmental domains assessed by the Bayley, we evaluated the ability of delta power to predict dependent variables: Fine Motor (N = 131), Gross Motor (N = 133), Receptive Communication (N = 131), and Expressive Communication raw scores (N = 131).
Data availabilityDerived data supporting the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author on request.
Results Subject and EEG data characteristicsEighty-two unique participants with AS (6.52 ± 4.41 years, range 1.08–28.16 years; 32F) with 133 coordinated EEG recordings (including 82 first visits, 27 sec visits, 15 third visits, 6 fourth visits, 2 fifth visits, and 1 sixth visit) and Bayley Scale assessments were analyzed. All EEGs captured the awake state and 54 captured non-rapid eye movement sleep. The average recording length was 27.08 ± 12.36 min (mean ± SD) for awake data, and 17.85 ± 10.19 min for sleep data. After manual removal of artifacts, an average of 7.32 ± 5.80 min of clean awake data and 17.53 ± 10.20 min of clean sleep data were available for analysis.
Fifty-eight subjects (70.73%) had a deletion genotype, of which 20 subjects (24.39%) had a class 1 deletion, 33 (40.24%) had a class 2 deletion, and a further five subjects (6.10%) had an atypical or unspecified deletion. Twenty-four subjects (29.27%) had a non-deletion genotype, of which 10 (12.20%) had a UBE3A mutation, 5 (6.10%) had an imprinting defect, 6 (7.32%) had uniparental paternal disomy, and 3 (3.66%) had abnormal DNA methylation with a negative fluorescent in situ hybridization test, ruling out deletion. Consistent with prior literature,24,26 on our visual inspection, Bayley Cognitive Scores reliably clustered between deletion and non-deletion AS groups, but visual inspection did not suggest any differences in Bayley Cognitive Scores across subclassifications within deletion and non-deletion groups. However, our sample sizes for some subgroups were small, limiting the power to detect a difference amongst deletion and non-deletion genotype subclassifications.
The mean performances on the Bayley Scales domains were Cognitive 50.6 (range 18–83), Fine Motor 31.37 (range 8–52), Gross Motor 44.52 (range 13–65), Receptive Communication 17.07 (range 5–38), and Expressive Communication 11.08 (range 4–29). The median duration of time between assessment for the Bayley Scales and EEG recording was 0 days (range 0–72).
Delta power is a reliable biomarker of cognitive function in Angelman syndromeTo test for a relationship between delta power and cognitive performance, we first assessed for variation based on age and genotype. Consistent with prior studies, age and genotype both predicted cognitive function (Fig. 2A),29,33,34 and model testing showed that an interaction term including both genotype and age outperformed genotype alone (ΔAIC > 5). We, therefore, included this interaction term (genotype:age) as a predictor. Among subjects with longitudinal data available, the change in delta predicted the change in the Bayley Cognitive Score (Fig. 2B), however, subjects were found to have different baseline values (Fig. 2C). We, therefore, included a subject-specific intercept in the model to account for repeat visits by several subjects. Our final mixed effects model was:[Image Omitted. See PDF]where the dependent variable BCS represents the Bayley Cognitive Score. Fixed effects include: (1) log10(age), representing the log10-transformed age in years; (2) Igenotype, an indicator variable representing deletion or non-deletion genotype; and, (3) spectral power, here, the relative delta power during wakefulness. A random effect on intercept is included for subject ID to account for repeat subject visits.
FIGURE 2. Delta power predicts cognitive function. (A) Both log10(age) (P [less than] 10−31) and genotype:log10(age) interaction (P [less than] 10−12) predict cognitive function. Each circle indicates a subject visit. The solid [dashed] curves indicate model fit [95% confidence intervals]. Subjects with deletion [non-deletion] genotype are indicated in red [blue]. (B) Amongst longitudinal subjects (N = 27 subjects, N = 51 pairs), change in delta power predicts change in the Bayley Cognitive Score (R2 = 0.0814, P = 0.0386, β = −13.40 [SE 6.30]). (C) Subjects had similar cognitive scores across longitudinal assessments, with different baseline values (e.g., y-intercepts). Connected points in color indicate longitudinal data from the same subject. Points in gray indicate subjects with only one visit. (D) The observed Bayley Cognitive Scores and the Bayley Cognitive Scores predicted by the model were highly correlated (R2 = 0.9374), indicating a good model fit. Each circle indicates a real and predicted subject score. The dashed line indicates identical values between the two scores. (E) There is a linear relationship between delta power and the Bayley Cognitive Score after controlling for age, age: genotype interaction, and repeat visits using the mixed-effects model shown at the top of the panel (R2 = 0.9374, delta power P [less than] 10−5, log10(age) P [less than] 10−24, genotype:log10(age) P [less than] 10−11, repeat subjects P [less than] 10−8). The solid black line shows the model fit and the gray shaded region indicates the 95% confidence intervals. Red circles indicate combined EEG and cognitive score visits. Red lines indicate repeat visits by the same subject. The insert shows the standard 10–20 EEG channels used to estimate delta power. *The Bayley Cognitive Score has been adjusted for a fixed age and genotype for visualization.
Fitting this model to the data, we find a strong negative relationship between delta power and cognitive function, wherein the model explains 93.74% of the variance in the Bayley Cognitive Score (Fig. 2D–E; R2 = 0.9374, delta power P < 10−5, log10(age) P < 10−24, genotype:log10(age) P < 10−11, repeat subjects P < 10−8). For each 0.1 increase in relative delta power, the Bayley Cognitive Score decreased by 2.00 points (95% CI [−2.81, −1.19]). Within this model, we find that age explains most of the variance (partial R2 = 0.7169), followed by delta power (partial R2 = 0.2075), and then age:genotypic interaction (partial R2 = 0.1027). Full model criteria and output statistics are shown in Table 1. We note that the majority of individuals in our sample were above the upper age limit for the norm-referenced Bayley Scales standard scores. Age-equivalent scores have been adapted for the AS population35 but our model performed best using raw scores and including age as a continuous variable (using age-equivalent Bayley Cognitive Scores in our model, R2 = 0.7001).
Table 1 Delta power correlates with cognitive function in AS.
Predictor | Coefficient estimate | P | Partial R2 |
Delta power | −20.01 ± 4.08 | 2.76e-06 | 0.2075 |
log10(age) | 29.73 ± 2.28 | 3.07e-25 | 0.7169 |
Genotype:log10(age) | −13.57 ± 1.71 | 1.00e-12 | 0.1027 |
Coefficient estimates (±SE), P-values, and partial R2 values for the fixed effects model predictors.
We assessed model accuracy and goodness-of-fit using a leave-one-out cross-validation procedure, adapted for mixed-effects models, where the number of iterations is equal to the number of subjects. Here, all data points from a single subject were excluded and model criteria recomputed and used to predict the Bayley Cognitive score for the excluded subject. This process was repeated for all subjects. We found that the residuals from this procedure are randomly distributed (Fig. 3A) and the model performs accurately on test data (Fig. 3B; R2 = 0.9455), indicating good model generalizability. We conclude that delta power is a powerful predictor of cognitive function in AS and that the model generalizes across this large and varied AS dataset.
FIGURE 3. Model validation. (A) The residuals are normally distributed, with a low root mean squared prediction error (RMSPE = 2.50). (B) there is a strong correlation (R2 = 0.9455) between the actual and predicted Bayley Cognitive Score values for each excluded subject, indicating a good model fit. Each circle indicates a subject visit and colors represent the same subject in (A) and (B).
To test whether state of consciousness impacted the predictive performance of EEG delta power as a biomarker for cognitive function in AS, we compared delta power derived from a subset of wake, sleep, and mixed state EEG segments available from the same cohort of subjects (N = 54). We found similar relationships between cognitive score and delta power using delta power in sleep (R2 = 0.9613, P = 0.0048, coefficient estimate β = −22.01 [SE 7.45], AIC = 340.52), wake (R2 = 0.9691, P = 0.0021, β = −18.97 [SE 5.84], AIC = 339.41), and mixed states of consciousness (50% wake and 50% sleep; R2 = 0.9690, P = 0.0013, β = −22.57 [SE 6.60], AIC = 338.39; Fig. S1A–C). Delta power predicts cognitive function in each case (P < 0.008, R2 values 0.94–0.95, and ΔAIC < 2 between models). To supplement this finding, we modeled (by linear regression) delta power in the awake state as a function of delta power in the sleep state in the 54 EEGs with both states available and found a strong correlation (R2 = 0.5450; Fig. S1D). We conclude that the state of consciousness during the EEG recording does not impact the performance of delta power in predicting cognitive function in AS.
Delta power as a biomarker is robust to different referencing proceduresTo determine the generalizability of delta power as a biomarker across recording configurations, we compared the performance of delta power estimated from EEG data referenced to the bipolar reference and linked-ear reference to the results using the common average reference. We found that models with delta power computed from the common average reference (R2 = 0.9372, AIC = 797.39) and the bipolar reference (R2 = 0.9353, AIC = 798.06) outperformed the model with delta power computed from the linked-ear reference (R2 = 0.9354, AIC = 803.68, 5 < ΔAIC < 7). We found no evidence of a difference (ΔAIC < 1) in model performance between the common average reference and bipolar reference. The coefficient estimate for delta power was significant and similar for all montages (average reference: P < 10−5, β = −19.27 [SE 4.12]; bipolar reference: P < 10−4, β = −19.06 [SE 4.16]; linked-ear reference: P < 10−3, β = −16.44 [SE 4.28]). We conclude that delta power estimated from the common average and bipolar references work equivalently well to predict cognitive function, and both slightly outperform the linked-ear reference (Table S1).
Models of cognitive function improve with delta power estimated from posterior EEG channelsDelta power abnormalities have been observed to occur across the entire scalp recording in people with AS.24 To test the relationship between delta power and cognitive score at different scalp locations, we evaluated the performance of delta power estimates obtained from posterior subsamples of electrodes. Six clusters of interest were defined referenced to a local common average reference. Most clusters excluded frontal channels, as they are most impacted by ocular artifacts and muscle artifacts, and focused on posterior channels least impacted by these artifacts (Fig. 4, Row 1–2).
FIGURE 4. Sub-sampled electrode clusters and raw EEG data predict cognitive function in AS. Sub-sampled clusters of electrodes are shown in the first row (blue = channel included in montage), with the groupwise abbreviation and the names of channels included listed in the second row. The full average reference is shown in the second column in gray for comparison, and cleaned data in the full average reference, to which all other cells are compared, is shown in dark gray. For each of the sub-sampled channel montages, and the full average reference, relative delta power was computed using cleaned, raw, and raw data with balanced duration to cleaned data, and used to predict the Bayley Cognitive Score. Cells with AIC scores superior (ΔAIC > 4) to those from the model using cleaned data from all electrodes in the full average reference are highlighted in green.
We found that the exclusion of the frontal electrodes improved model performance (ΔAIC > 4) when compared to using data from all channels (Fig. 4, Row 3). In particular, electrode subsets that only included occipital and parietal channels (OP, R2 = 0.9503, delta power P < 10−7), occipital, parietal and temporal channels (OPT, R2 = 0.9493, P < 10−7), occipital, parietal, and central channels (OPC, R2 = 0.9498, P < 10−7), and occipital, parietal, central, and temporal channels (OPCT, R2 = 0.9457, P < 10−6) yielded models with superior performance compared to the model utilizing the full EEG recording montage (5 < ΔAIC < 8). See Figure 4, Row 1–2 for graphic depictions of these electrode subsets tested.
Models of cognitive function using raw EEG data perform wellManual removal of ocular, motor, and muscle artifacts required approximately 1–2 h per 30 min of wake EEG data. The greatest sources of noise were muscle and blink artifact, which contaminated at least one channel in greater than 50% of the awake EEG recordings. To determine the necessity of this time-consuming manual step, we evaluated model performance with delta power estimated from the raw EEG data, without removing artifacts. We found that delta estimates from raw data using the OP (R2 = 0.9517, delta power P < 10−8), OPT (R2 = 0.9493, P < 10−8), OPC (R2 = 0.9526, P < 10−8), and OPCT (R2 = 0.9467, P < 10−8) montages outperformed the original model with delta power estimated from cleaned data using the full average reference montage (ΔAIC > 4, Fig. 4, Row 3–4).
Improved model performance with raw data could be explained by the longer total duration of EEG data included in the analyses. To avoid this confounder, we compared model performance using delta power estimated from raw EEG data of the same duration as cleaned data segments for each subject (mean ± STD, 7.32 ± 5.80 min). Even with equal data quantities, the raw data analyzed using the channel subsets excluding frontal electrodes showed slightly improved model fits compared to the cleaned data from the full EEG montage (1 < ΔAIC < 3; Fig. 4, Row 5). We conclude that with frontal electrodes removed, delta power estimates from raw data outperform a similar measure computed from manually cleaned data in predicting cognitive function in AS (Fig. 5; full model output statistics are shown in Table 2).
FIGURE 5. Delta power calculated from raw data in the OP montage predicts cognitive function. There is a linear relationship between delta power, estimated from raw data in the OP montage (inset), and the Bayley Cognitive Score after controlling for age, age:genotype interaction, and random effects for repeat subjects (R2 = 0.9517, delta power P [less than] 10−8, log10(age) P [less than] 10−22, genotype:log10(age) P [less than] 10−12, repeat subjects P [less than] 10−8). The solid black line indicates the linear model fit, and the gray shaded region indicates the 95% confidence interval. Longitudinal same-subject data are connected by red dotted lines. *The Bayley Cognitive Score has been adjusted for the impacts of age and genotype and the recast values plotted.
Table 2 Delta power, estimated from raw EEG data in the OP montage, correlates with cognitive function in AS.
Predictor | Coefficient estimate | P | Partial R2 |
Delta power | −26.94 ± 4.21 | 2.77e-09 | 0.3806 |
log10(age) | 27.87 ± 2.25 | 1.59e-23 | 0.5902 |
Genotype : log10(age) | −13.89 ± 1.67 | 1.12e-13 | 0.0516 |
Coefficient estimates (±SE), P-values, and partial R2 values for the fixed effects model predictors.
Models of cognitive function with relative delta power outperform other canonical frequency bandsRelative measures of delta power can be impacted by prominent activities in other frequencies bands, such as the posterior dominant alpha rhythm. However, absolute power measures can be impacted by non-cortical features, such as skull thickness.36 Given these competing limitations, we evaluated whether absolute or relative delta power provided a better biomarker for cognitive function in AS. We found a significant negative relationship between absolute delta power and Bayley Cognitive Score (P < 10−3). However, the model using relative delta power outperformed the model with absolute delta power (ΔAIC = 11.22).
To evaluate the specificity of the model to the delta band, we evaluated the relationship between each of the canonical frequency bands (alpha, theta, beta) and cognitive function. We found a positive relationship between Bayley Cognitive Score and relative theta power (P = 0.0011), the log10-transform of relative alpha power (P < 10−4), and the log10-transform of relative beta power (P < 10−5; Table S2). Including both delta power and either relative theta power, relative log10 alpha power, or relative log10 beta power in the model, only relative delta power remained a significant predictor of cognitive function (Table S3), suggesting that relative power in frequency bands other than delta may simply provide a poor surrogate measure of relative delta power. We conclude that relative delta power is the optimal spectral band to predict cognitive function in AS.
Delta power predicts motor and language functionIn addition to cognitive performance, we tested delta power as a predictor of performance in other developmental domains using the Fine and Gross Motor and Expressive and Receptive Communication Scores of the Bayley Scales. We note that although these measures are independently tested and scored in each subject, the Bayley Scales were highly collinear across developmental domains (Fig. S2). The strongest correlations were observed between the Cognitive, Fine Motor, and Receptive Communication Scores (R2 > 0.7).
We found a negative correlation between delta power estimated from the OP montage on raw data and receptive communication (Fig. 6A; R2 = 0.8718, P < 10−3, β = −10.77 [SE 3.12]), expressive communication (Fig. 6B; R2 = 0.6801, P = 0.0209, β = −6.16 [SE 2.63]), and fine motor skills (Fig. 6C; R2 = 0.9074, P < 10−4, β = −14.84 [SE 3.42]). We did not find evidence of a relationship between delta power estimated from the OP montage and gross motor skills (Fig. 6D; R2 = 0.9382, P = 0.36, β = −4.02 [SE 4.34]). Using the cleaned data in the full-channel montage, we found a negative relationship between delta power and fine motor skills (R2 = 0.9026, P < 10−3, β = −11.91 [SE 3.15]) and receptive communication (R2 = 0.8559, P = 0.0302, β = −6.36 [SE 2.90]), and a negative trend between delta power and expressive communication (R2 = 0.6823, P = 0.0585, β = −4.58 [SE 2.40]). We did not find evidence of a relationship between delta power estimated from the full-channel montage and gross motor skills (P > 0.2), though gross motor scores were noted to have a limited range of scores across AS patients, limiting the power to detect a relationship. We conclude that delta power predicts not only cognitive function, but additional domains of neurodevelopment—motor and language function—in AS, findings that are most evident using the OP montage.
FIGURE 6. Motor and language function are predicted by delta power with reduced data collection criteria. Delta power estimated using the OP regional reference montage (insets), and raw EEG data was used to predict the (A) Bayley Receptive Communication Score, (B) Bayley Expressive Communication Score, (C) Bayley Fine Motor Score, and (D) Bayley Gross Motor Score. For all plots, the solid black line shows the linear fit, and the gray shaded region indicates the 95% confidence interval. Longitudinal same-subject data are connected by red dotted lines. The R2 value given by the model and the P value for delta power are shown in bolded text. *Scores have been adjusted for a fixed age and genotype for visualization.
Using a large natural history database of individuals with combined short EEG recordings and cognitive assessments, we found that delta power provides a robust biomarker for cognitive function in Angelman syndrome. This relationship was evident after controlling for age and genotype and was generalizable across a large cohort of subjects with AS. These results support the viability of delta power as a promising surrogate biomarker for clinical trials to test emerging disease-modifying treatments in this population.
EEG recordings provide a direct measure of neuronal activity, and delta activity has been proposed to reflect the dynamics of intrinsic membrane and synaptic conductances, with local generators in the thalamus37,38 and the cortex.39,40 The relationship between increased delta power with decreased cognitive function has been observed across many disease states.41–44 The acquisition of cognitive skills also coincides with decreased delta power and increased prevalence of faster oscillations across normal development.45 Hypersynchronization in the delta frequency band and decreased signal complexity observed in patients with AS have been inferred as a state of diminished consciousness.46 Excessive delta activity in AS at least in part replaces or disrupts typically present rhythms, such as alpha band activity (8–12 Hz), which has known correlations with cognitive function, particularly in relation to the ability to orient and sustain attention, process information, and store and retrieve semantic memory.47–51 Here, we demonstrated that increased delta power corresponds to decreased cognitive, motor, and language performance in AS, validating the utility of this biomarker as a surrogate for clinical disease severity. Given this strong relationship and the impact that these abnormal rhythms have on normal cognitive function, treatments that improve these abnormal brain rhythms would be expected to correlate with improvement in AS.
We found that the strong relationship between delta power and cognitive function in AS persists across different states of consciousness. This finding is of practical importance because disrupted sleep and the lack of well-characterized sleep architecture in most patients with AS makes confident estimates of sleep states challenging in this population. Furthermore, we found that manual cleaning of the EEG data is unnecessary to reliably measure this high amplitude and diffuse signal. Finally, we found that the use of just five electrodes, placed in the posterior regions of the head to avoid the most common recording artifacts, provides optimal estimates of delta power to reliably predict cognitive function. Because the application of EEG electrodes is both time-consuming and can be poorly tolerated in young children or those with severe developmental delay, fewer electrodes may improve the tolerance of an EEG recording session. The robustness of this biomarker to different brain regions, states of consciousness, and high-artifact recordings support its practical deployment in clinical populations.
Although delta power abnormalities have historically been targeted as the canonical EEG feature in AS, abnormalities in other frequency bands have also been reported in AS. Theta power has been shown to be increased in approximately half of the AS population.21,23,25,52 Decreased beta power has been found in patients with deletion-positive AS,26 in which the genes GABRB3, GABRA5, and GABRG3, encoding subunits of the GABAA receptor (involved in the production of healthy beta oscillations53), are deleted in addition to UBE3A. Consistent with prior reports, we found relationships between cognitive function and relative theta and beta band activity, but these relationships did not persist after controlling for delta band activity. Thus, this work finds that delta power is the optimal frequency band to predict cognitive function in AS.
At present, there are no specific disease-modifying therapies available for AS, but transformative treatments are on the horizon. Current supportive therapies include physical and occupational therapies, augmentative communication strategies, and medication for comorbid seizures, attention issues, sleep disturbances, and gastrointestinal issues,54 as well as dietary adjustments to help reduce seizures.55–57 Emerging therapies which restore neural UBE3A expression have the potential to directly modify the pathogenic disease process in AS.12 In rodent models of AS,11 successes in restoring neural UBE3A expression using antisense oligonucleotides13 and topoisomerase inhibitors14,15 have been achieved, and have fast-approaching potential for transition to clinical therapies. Direct measures of UBE3A expression in these models were demonstrated through direct assays of neuronal tissue; behavioral measures provided a less robust response. A rapid, non-invasive biomarker for target engagement and treatment response will be useful as these potential therapies are translated to human patients. Delta power in short EEG recordings may provide a non-invasive measure to detect treatment response and a clinically meaningful reduction in disease severity.
AS results in severe functional impairments and, currently, supportive therapies are the only means of treatment. As new disease-modifying treatments emerge and are transitioned to clinical trials, quantitative biomarkers are required for rapid and accurate assessment of intervention efficacy. Here we show that delta power correlates with cognitive function in AS, and that this correlation persists with changes in the state of consciousness, variable electrode configuration, and non-neuronal artifacts in the data. Furthermore, the simplicity of the model is an attractive feature for clinical application; in an era of ever-expanding machine learning models and complex approaches to computational analysis, the identification of a biomarker with a simple, direct relationship to outcome, without specific parameter requirements, increases usability and model interpretation.58 Here, a simple and interpretable mixed-effect linear model achieved near-perfect prediction of cognitive function. We, therefore, propose that delta power provides a robust, easily obtained, and practical biomarker of clinical disease severity in AS. This work also demonstrates the utility of EEG to identify reliable biomarkers for neurological function in neurodevelopmental disease.
AcknowledgementsM.A.K. acknowledges support by the National Science Foundation DMS Award #1451384. E.R.S. acknowledges support by the National Science Foundation GRFP. C.J.C. acknowledges support by the National Institutes of Health K23 Award NS092923. L.B. received support from the NIH to obtain the data used in this project.
Conflict of InterestC.J.C. and M.A.K. have provided consulting services for Biogen Inc. R.K. is employed at Biogen Inc. R.T. receives clinical support from the Angelman Foundation and consults for Ovid Pharmaceuticals and Roche Pharma.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2021. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Objective
Angelman syndrome (AS) is a severe neurodevelopmental disorder caused by loss of function of the maternally inherited UBE3A gene in neurons. Promising disease-modifying treatments to reinstate UBE3A expression are under development and an early measure of treatment response is critical to their deployment in clinical trials. Increased delta power in EEG recordings, reflecting abnormal neuronal synchrony, occurs in AS across species and correlates with genotype. Whether delta power provides a reliable biomarker for clinical symptoms remains unknown.
Methods
We analyzed combined EEG recordings and developmental assessments in a large cohort of individuals with AS (N = 82 subjects, 133 combined EEG and cognitive assessments, 1.08–28.16 years; 32F) and evaluated delta power as a biomarker for cognitive function, as measured by the Bayley Cognitive Score. We examined the robustness of this biomarker to varying states of consciousness, recording techniques and analysis procedures.
Results
Delta power predicted the Bayley Scale cognitive score (P < 10−5, R2 = 0.9374) after controlling for age (P < 10−24), genotype:age (P < 10−11), and repeat assessments (P < 10−8), with the excellent fit on cross validation (R2 = 0.95). There were no differences in model performance across states of consciousness or bipolar versus average montages (ΔAIC < 2). Models using raw data excluding frontal channels outperformed other models (ΔAIC > 4) and predicted performance in expressive (P = 0.0209) and receptive communication (P < 10−3) and fine motor skills (P < 10−4).
Interpretation
Delta power is a simple, direct measure of neuronal activity that reliably correlates with cognitive function in AS. This electrophysiological biomarker offers an objective, clinically relevant endpoint for treatment response in emerging clinical trials.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details

1 Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts, USA; School of Medicine, University of California, San Diego, California, USA
2 Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts, USA; Department of Mathematics and Statistics, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
3 Department of Pediatrics, University of California, San Diego, California, USA
4 Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts, USA; Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
5 Division of Clinical Development, Biogen Inc, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA
6 Department of Mathematics and Statistics, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts, USA