The prevalence of overweight including obesity has increased rapidly amongst women in African countries, from 33·0 % in 2000 to 42·9 % in 2016(1). North African and Southern Sub-Saharan African women experience the second and third highest prevalence of overweight and obesity of the twenty one global burden of disease regions(2), which are higher than the global average(3).
Current literature suggests that there are underlying social and cultural factors related to body image, such as a preference for larger body sizes, contributing to the increasing prevalence of obesity in African females(4,5). Body size preference plays a role in healthy weight behaviours. It is defined as an individual’s perception of how acceptable their body is to themselves and society(6). This includes body image dissatisfaction that may contribute to attempting to achieve an ideal body image leading to a desire for weight loss or weight gain(7). Body dissatisfaction (for slimness) may be one of the key drivers of the obesity epidemic in African women and adolescent girls. A cultural preference for a heavier body size is thought to lead to greater body satisfaction of African women at larger body sizes(8). In addition, body dissatisfaction might promote weight gain in slimmer women trying to fit in with cultural norms. For example, Moroccan Sahraouian women actively engage in fattening behaviours to attain the desirable body size within their community(9). Fatness is a sign of femininity, fertility and being a nurturing mother. Women’s primary role in these societies is associated with motherhood, therefore, ‘being fat’ elevates females’ status by embodying their suitability for this role(10,11). Historically, fatness was viewed as a sign of wealth, signifying excess resources in settings where food shortages were common and also demonstrating that women were well taken care of by their husbands(11).
This preference for a larger body size in women can be explained by evolutionary benefits. Prior to the industrialisation of food production, food shortages were common in all societies; therefore, storing fat improved survival(10,12). This was particularly important for women of childbearing age, meaning fatter women would be more successful in pregnancy and childbearing(10,13). Notwithstanding, in certain African communities, weight gain during pregnancy is perceived negatively due to the fear of complications during delivery(14–16). The belief that fatness represents an advantage for pregnancy and childbearing might persevere in some African societies due to the persistence of undernutrition secondary to poverty, particularly in rural areas(17), potentially making fatness desirable, by setting one apart from the community and embodying excess in resource poor settings(10). This is supported by the positive association between socio-economic status (SES) and obesity in low-income African countries(18,19), whereas in high-income countries (HICs) the opposite is observed(20). In African middle-income countries, the association between SES and obesity is negative for women(20). Furthermore, Western body size preferences have shifted towards a slimmer body size, whilst the population’s mean BMI has increased, suggesting that body size preferences evolve to contradict societal norms(21,22).
An aversion to ‘thinness’ may also exist in low-income African countries because of stigmatisation associated with disease, for example, HIV and tuberculosis (TB)(23,24).
In addition to cultural factors contributing to body size preference, studies from HICs have reported that being overweight, media exposure and psychological factors contribute to higher body dissatisfaction(6,25–28). Younger females are more likely to want to change their body size to fit emerging societal norms valuing thinness(6). Therefore, access to ‘Western’ media in Africa might increase the value of slimmer bodies, particularly amongst younger African females. Adolescence signifies the onset of puberty, which increases awareness of body image directly and indirectly via a greater importance placed on peer perceptions(29). Indeed, studies have reported an increased ‘drive for thinness’ in Black South African adolescents when compared with their White South African counterparts. This contradicts studies of fattening practices in African women(9,30). This might be a result of transitioning cultural norms and generational differences.
In light of the increasing obesity epidemic in African women and the contradictory results reported from the studies discussed earlier(8,30–32), this paper presents the results of a mixed methods systematic review, to assess body size preferences for African women and adolescent girls living in Africa and the factors influencing these preferences.
Methods
Review typology
A mixed methods systematic review was chosen as it combines quantitative and qualitative evidence(33) generating a complete, and deeper understanding of women’s body size preferences and the factors influencing these (PROSPERO #: CRD42015020509).
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
Inclusion criteria were based on the SPIDER tool (Sample, Phenomenon of Interest, Design, Evaluation and Research Type) (Table 1). Studies conducted in any African country among female adolescents (10–19 years) and women (≥18 years) were included. The review focused exclusively on Black African or Arab females. All studies that assessed preferred body size of African females (adolescent girls and women) using narrative and/or pictorial measures, and those that elicited factors influencing these preferences were included. Furthermore, studies assessing African males’ preferences for African females’ body size were included. All non-randomised quantitative, qualitative and mixed methods studies were eligible for inclusion.
Table 1 SPIDER breakdown of research question and search terms
Category | Sample (S) | Phenomenon of interest (PofI) | Design (D) | Evaluation (E) | Research type (R) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Research question | African females and males, aged 10 and above residing in Africa | Preferred body size; desire to gain or lose weight. | Non-randomised quantitative studies; qualitative; mixed methods | Narrative or pictorial assessment; views; attitudes; opinions | Qualitative, quantitative and mixed methods |
Associated search terms | All African countries and regions listed separately; Africa; (keyword and MeSH term); | Initial search: Body image; thinness; body size | Not used in search | Body image; body perception; body esteem; body satisfaction; thinness OR associated MeSH terms | Not used in search |
Supplementary search. Body Image; body size; body weight; body esteem; weight control; body ideal; weight ideal; size ideal OR associated MeSH terms | |||||
Boolean operators used | OR | OR | N/A | OR | N/A |
Final Search Strategy: S AND PofI or E * NOT African-American AND *limited to humans.
Search strategy
The SPIDER tool was used to define search terms and eligibility criteria(34). The search was conducted on ASSIA, CINAHL, Medline, PsycINFO and Web of Science. Initial searches were conducted from 20/04/2015 until 15/05/2015. A supplementary search was then conducted, repeating the search strategies up to 31/08/2019. An example full search can be found in online Supplemental Material 1. Furthermore, the reference lists of studies included after full text screening were searched and topic experts consulted to identify additional studies.
Screening
Duplicates were removed in MS Excel before screening. All papers were first screened on titles and abstracts and then on full text in MS Excel (OO, EC, RP, MH, CBN). Reasons for exclusion were recorded at full-text stage (i.e. no measure of body size preferences or factors influencing these; data not stratified by gender, ethnic group or country). To ensure consistency in the application of inclusion criteria, two authors (HO and CBN) checked 10 % of all excluded documents screened. There was a good degree of concordance overall; discrepancies were resolved through discussion.
Data extraction
Data extraction of included studies was conducted in MS Excel by OO, EC, RP, MH, HO. Quantitative and qualitative data from mixed methods studies were extracted separately (see online Supplemental Material 2).
Data extracted from studies included general information (study ID, title, authors, date, study location (country, urban v. rural), study aim); study eligibility (participant selection, sample size, participant characteristics); method used to measure body image dimensions; ideal body size; body size self-assessment; body size self-satisfaction and factors influencing body size preferences. One author (RP) checked the full data extraction file to ensure accuracy and consistency across team members. Where data could not be extracted, authors were contacted to request relevant data, e.g. disaggregated data for pooled male and female samples.
Quality appraisal
The ‘Standard Quality Assessment Criteria for Evaluating Primary Research Papers from a Variety of Fields’ (QualSyst) was used to critically appraise both the quality of the studies and their reporting(35). This tool was chosen because it provided a standard checklist for all study designs(35). Each study included in the review was rated using a predefined list of criteria (n 14 for quantitative studies, n 10 for qualitative studies). The original tool was modified by replacing the score for each criterion (0, 1, 2) with a qualitative assessment of high quality/green (low risk of bias), medium quality/yellow or low quality/red (high risk of bias) as the Cochrane guidance advises against the use of scores(36). Five authors (OO, EC, RP, MH, HO) independently conducted quality appraisal and this was double-checked by RP and EC. Discrepancies in the rating were discussed until agreement was reached.
Data synthesis
A sequential-explanatory approach was used to integrate quantitative and qualitative evidence(37,38) (Fig. 1). In phase one, quantitative studies were synthesised to define African females’ body size preferences and the factors influencing these. Phase two involved summarising the evidence from qualitative studies on factors influencing body size preferences, and phase three aimed at integrating findings from both quantitative and qualitative studies. The qualitative data synthesis provided contextual and cultural understanding of the quantitative data. Phase four focused on generating an integrated (quantitative and qualitative evidence) visual map to represent factors associated with body size preferences.
Fig. 1
Visual model for mixed methods sequential explanatory design procedures
[Figure omitted. See PDF]
Phase 1: Summarising body size preferences quantitatively and factors influencing these
To identify and summarise body size preferences, we focused on three specific body image dimensions: ideal body size, body size self-assessment and body size self-satisfaction(39).
Ideal body size, assessed with various body image (i.e. figural/photographic rating) scales in several quantitative and mixed methods studies, was synthesised graphically where possible. To facilitate the comparison between studies using different scales, females’ ideal body size mean and males’ ideal body size mean for women and adolescent girls were converted into the four main BMI categories: underweight, normal weight, overweight and obesity, through the Silhouette Photographs scale presenting a convenient body image metric(40).
For scales with real BMI values(40–42), we matched BMI categories with each silhouette. For scales without real BMI values, we assigned a BMI category to each silhouette according to the BMI classification defined by authors of the selected articles below. For all articles using the Figure Rating Scale (FRS)(43), the Body Image Instrument(44), the Contour Drawing Rating Scale(45), the Body Image Assessment for Obesity(46), the Ideal Body Subscale(47), the Figural Stimuli(48), the Body Silhouette Chart(49) and the Body Size Silhouettes(50), we used respectively the BMI classification stated by Matoti-Mvalo and Puoane(24), Pulvers et al.
(44), Thompson and Gray(45), Ettarh et al.
(51), Cogan et al.
(52), Duda et al.
(48), Caradas et al.
(53) and Rguibi and Belahsen(8) (see online Supplemental Material 3). We extracted the BMI categories preferred by participants, defined from the ideal body size mean and its
Studies that measured ideal body size using a questionnaire were summarised narratively. The second (i.e. body size self-assessment) and third (i.e. body size self-satisfaction) dimensions of body image were also summarised narratively.
To identify factors influencing body size preferences quantitatively, we summarised the associations between a given factor and the outcome (i.e. either a preference for a slim(mer) or a large(r) body size).
Phase 2: Summarising qualitatively factors influencing body size preferences
Thematic synthesis was used to summarise qualitative studies(54,55). Themes, defined as concepts that occurred frequently across more than one article, were identified from the analysis of the extracted text.
Phase 3: Connecting the quantitative and qualitative phases
Summative content analysis(56) from quantitative data was integrated into qualitative data synthesis. Factors identified from the quantitative and qualitative synthesis were grouped under four main overarching categories: socio-demographic, health-related, psycho-social and socio-cultural. These categories were defined based on the findings that emerged.
Phase 4: Generating an integrated map of the factors influencing body size preferences
Evidence from all studies was summarised in a map with factors split into the four categories described above.
Results
Description of studies
Seventy three articles were included (Fig. 2 (57)). Studies were conducted in 21/54 African countries and clustered particularly within three regions: Southern Africa, West Africa and East Africa, with a predominance in South Africa (Fig. 3). More studies took place in urban (n 51) than rural (n 7) settings; and 15 studies included data from both settings.
Fig. 2
Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses flow diagram showing the selection of studies for the present systematic mixed-methods review
[Figure omitted. See PDF]
Fig. 3
Map displaying the African countries included in the review
[Figure omitted. See PDF]
Of the seventy-three articles, fifty were quantitative(8,9,27,28,40,41,48,51–53,58–97), fifteen were qualitative(11,23,98–110) and eight mixed methods(24,42,111–116) (Table 2). All quantitative studies utilised a cross-sectional design, with the exception of one longitudinal design(87). A total of 25 512 females and 2090 males aged ≥10 years from 17 African countries were included. Twenty-three qualitative studies from ten countries were included. Data from 828 participants and twenty households aged 10–70 years were synthesised (sample sizes between 10 and 193). Samples across studies were diverse in terms of age, SES, ethnicity, education and BMI.
Table 2 Description of included studies
Study ID | Country | Income level | Setting | Sample characteristics | Sample size | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | Age group* | Mean age ( |
|||||
Quantitative articles (n 50) | |||||||
Salokun & Toriola(58) | Nigeria | Low | Urban | Females | Adolescents | 12–15 | 140 |
Ford et al. (59) | Egypt | Medium | Urban | Females | Adolescents/Young adults | 19·5 (1·5) | 160 |
Walker et al. (60) | South Africa | Medium | Urban/Rural | Females | Adolescents | 16·5 (1·9) | 818 |
Furnham & Baguma(61) | Uganda | Low | Urban | Females | Adolescents/Young adults/Adults | 22·3 (3·9) | 51 |
Cogan et al. (52) | Ghana | Medium | Urban | Mixed | Young adults | 24·6 (no |
349 (♀: 184; ♂: 165) |
Toriola et al. (62) | Nigeria | Low | Urban | Females | Adolescents/Young adults/Adults | Students: 23·1 (3·3); Teachers: 35·0 (6·5) | 151 (103 students; 48 teachers) |
Caradas et al. (53) | South Africa | Medium | Urban | Females | Adolescents | 16·3 (1·1) | 60 |
Jackson et al. (63) | Egypt | Medium | Urban/Rural | Females | Adolescents | 14·4 (1·7) | 340 |
Holdsworth et al. (40) | Senegal | Low | Urban | Females | Young adults/Adults/Middle-aged | 20–50 | 301 |
Singh(64) | Guinea Bissau | Low | Rural | Females | Adults/Middle-aged | 20–55 | 37 |
Faber & Kruger(65) | South Africa | Medium | Rural | Females | Young adults/Adults | 33·5 (8·9) | 187 |
Mciza et al. (66) | South Africa | Medium | Urban | Females | Young adults/Adults/Middle-aged | 40 (10·7) | 63 |
Duda et al. (67) | Ghana | Medium | Urban | Females | All | 35·9 (19–74) | 305 |
Rguibi & Belahsen(8) | Morocco | Medium | Urban | Females | All | >15 | 249 |
Rguibi & Belahsen(9) | Morocco | Medium | Urban | Females | Adults/Middle-aged | 36·8 (11·8) | 249 |
Siervo et al. (68) | Gambia | Low | Urban | Females | Adolescents/Young adults/Adults | Adolescents: 18·6 (3·4); Adults: 42·5 (5·2) | 100 |
Szabo & Allwood(69) | South Africa | Medium | Urban/Rural | Females | Adolescents | 15·9 (2·1) | 939 |
Duda et al. (48) | Ghana | Medium | Urban | Females | All | 35·9 (19–74) | 305 |
Jumah & Duda(70) | Ghana | Medium | Urban | Mixed | Young adults/Adults | 33·0 (11·5) | 185 (♀: 62; ♂: 123) |
Frederick et al. (71) | Ghana | Medium | Rural | Mixed | Adolescents/Young adults/Adults | 27·8 (10·3) | 48 (♀: 26; ♂: 22) |
Lahmam et al. (72) | Morocco | Medium | Rural | Females | Adults/Middle-aged | 50·0 (16·3) | 271 |
Okoro & Oyejola(73) | Nigeria | Low | Urban | Mixed | Adults/Middle-aged | 55·7 (12·9) | 139 (♀: 84; ♂: 55) |
Mwaba & Roman(74) | South Africa | Medium | Urban | Females | Adolescents | 19·2 (no |
150 |
Swami et al. (27) | South Africa | Medium | Urban/Rural | Mixed | Adults/Middle-aged | 39·3 (10·7) | 205 (♀: 108; ♂: 97) |
Alwan et al. (75) | Seychelles | High | Urban/Rural | Females | Adolescents | 14·1 (1·5) | 476 |
Coetzee & Perrett(76) | South Africa | Medium | Urban | Females | Adolescents/Young adults/Adults | 18–30 | 53 |
Mchiza et al. (77) | South Africa | Medium | Urban | Females | Adolescents/Adults/Middle-aged | Daughters: 10·5 (0·9); Mothers: 38·5 (9·0) | 44 |
Benkeser et al. (78) | Ghana | Medium | Urban | Females | Adults/Middle-aged | 46·3 (18·2) | 2814 |
Maruf et al. (79) | Nigeria | Low | Urban | Females | Young adults | 21·8 (1·8) | 64 |
Swami et al.(41) | Zimbabwe | Low | Urban | Females | Adolescents/Young adults/Adults | 25·3 (6·9) | 140 |
Bhurtun & Jeewon(80) | Mauritius | High | Urban/Rural | Females | Adolescents | 13–18 | 90 |
Ettarh et al. (51) | Kenya | Medium | Urban slums | Females | Adults | 42·0 (no |
2265 |
Jafri et al.(81) | Morocco | Medium | Urban | Females | All | ≥ 18 | 425 |
El Ansari et al. (82) | Egypt | Medium | Urban | Females | Adolescents | 18·6 (1·2) | 1663 |
Gitau et al. (85) | South Africa | Medium | Urban | Mixed | Adolescents | 13 and 17 | 1302 (♀: 675; ♂: 627) |
Gitau et al. (83) | South Africa | Medium | Urban | Females | Adolescents | 15·0 (no |
183 |
Gitau et al.(84) | South Africa | Medium | Urban | Males | Adolescents | 14·9 (no |
179 |
Musaiger & Mannai(28) | Egypt | Medium | Urban | Females | Adolescents/Young adults/Adults | 17–32 | 210 |
Okoro et al. (73) | Nigeria | Low | Urban | Mixed | Adults/Middle-aged | 43·9 (17·2) | 524 (♀: 304; ♂: 220) |
Gradings et al. (87) | South Africa | Medium | Urban | Females | Adults/Middle-aged | Baseline: 41·1 (5·4); Follow-up: 49·3 (5·3) | 430 |
Amenyah & Michels(88) | Ghana | Medium | Urban | Mixed | Adolescents | 11–18 | 370 (♀: 194; ♂: 176) |
Caleyachetty et al. (89) | Mauritius | High | Urban/Rural | Females | Adults/Middle-aged | 45·8 (35·0–56·7) | 3022 |
Gualdi-Russo et al. (90) | Tunisia/Morocco | High/Medium | Urban | Females | All | Tunisians: 28·7 (11·5); Moroccans: 39·5 (13·1) | 228 (104 Tunisian; 124 Moroccan) |
Pedro et al. (91) | South Africa | Medium | Rural | Females | Adolescents | 13·6 (1·2) | 195 |
Yepes et al. (92) | Seychelles | High | Urban/Rural | Females | Young adults/Adults/Middle-aged | 24–64 | 709 |
Macia et al. (93) | Senegal | Low | Urban/Rural | Mixed | Young adults/Adults/Middle-aged | 36·2 (13·9) | 1500 |
Michels & Amenyah(94) | Ghana | Medium | Urban | Females | Adolescents | 11–18 | 194 |
Prioreschi et al. (95) | South Africa | Medium | Urban/Rural | Females | Adolescents/Young adults | 20·6 (1·1) | 1019 |
Naigaga et al.(96) | Algeria | High | Urban | Females | All | 18–45 and ≥ 46 | 180 |
Cohen et al.(97) | South Africa | Medium | Urban | Females | Adolescents/Adults/Middle-aged | Daughters:17·9 (0·4); Mothers: 41·4 (8·0) | 1230 (615 mothers; 615 daughters) |
Qualitative articles (n 15) | |||||||
Mvo et al. (98) | South Africa | Medium | Urban slums | Female | Adolescents/Young adults/Adults | 18–36 | 10 |
Treloar et al. (99) | Cameroon/Egypt | Medium | Urban/Rural | Mixed | Not reported | Not reported | 11 (♀: 6; ♂: 5) |
Kiawi et al. (100) | Cameroon | Medium | Urban | Female | All | ≥15 | 27 |
Dapi et al. (101) | Cameroon | Medium | Urban/Rural | Female | Adolescents | 12–15 | 15 |
Batnitzky(102) | Morocco | Medium | Urban | Female | Not reported | Not reported | 20 households |
Ezekiel et al.(23) | Tanzania | Low | Rural | Mixed | Adolescents/Young adults | 17·2 (no |
193 (♀: 84; ♂: 109) |
Batnitzky(11) | Morocco | Medium | Urban | Female | Not reported | Not reported | 20 |
Shaibu et al. (103) | Botswana | High | Urban | Mixed | Not reported | Not reported | 18 stakeholders |
Morris & Szabo(104) | South Africa | Medium | Urban | Female | Adolescents | 14–18 | 40 |
Ahmed & Saltus(105) | Sudan | Low | Urban | Female | Adolescents/Young adults | 16–25 | 19 |
Draper et al. (106) | South Africa | Medium | Urban slums | Female | Young adults/Adults/Middle-aged | 24–51 | 21 |
Pradeilles(107) | South Africa | Medium | Urban slums | Mixed | Young adults/Adults | 20–39 | 51 (♀: 19; ♂: 32) |
Okop et al. (108) | South Africa | Medium | Urban slums | Female | Adults/Middle-aged | 35–70 years | 36 |
Phillips et al.(109) | South Africa | Medium | Urban slums | Female | Young adults/Middle-aged | Daughters: 24·2; Mothers: 53·0 | 32 (17 daughters; 15 mothers) |
Tateyama et al. (110) | Zambia | Medium | Rural | Female | Adults/Middle-aged | ≥40 | 40 |
Mixed methods articles (n 8) | |||||||
Puoane et al.(111) | South Africa | Medium | Urban slums | Female | Adults/Middle-aged | 43·2 (7·2) | Quant: 44; Qual: 27 |
Bodiba et al. (112) | South Africa | Medium | Urban | Female | Adolescents | 16 (17–19) | Quant: 75; Qual: not reported |
Puoane et al.(113) | South Africa | Medium | Urban slums | Female | Adolescents | 10–18 | Quant: 240; Qual: 60 |
Matoti-Mvalo & Puoane(24) | South Africa | Medium | Urban slums | Female | All | 18–65 | Quant: 513; Qual: 20 |
Cohen et al. (42) | Cameroon | Medium | Urban | Mixed | All | Quant: 35·2 (13·5; Qual: 18–30 or 50–65 | Quant: 181 (♀: 98; ♂: 83); Qual: 16 (balanced sex ratio) |
Cohen et al. (114) | Cameroon | Medium | Urban/Rural | Mixed | All | Quant: 38·6 (13·6); Qual: 18–30 and 50–65 | Quant: 577 (♀: 339; ♂: 238); Qual: 24 (♀: 12; ♂: 12) |
Croffut et al.(115) | Malawi | Low | Urban/Rural | Female | Young adults/Adults | 27·0 (5·3) | Quant: 64; Qual: 64 |
Cohen et al. (116) | Senegal | Low | Urban/Rural | Mixed | Adolescents/Young adults/Middle-aged | Quant: 36·3 (15·6); Qual: <25 and >45 | Quant: 597 (♀: 313; ♂: 284); Qual: 84 (♀: 42; ♂: 42) |
♀: females; ♂: males.
* Age group: adolescents: 10–19; young adults: 20–25; adults: 25–44 and middle-aged: 45+.
Quality appraisal
The quality assessment (see online Supplemental Material 4) revealed that quantitative and qualitative studies separately scored highly on criteria such as question/objective sufficiently described, evident and appropriate study design and conclusions. Quantitative studies scored highly on well-defined outcome measures; however, some studies reported poorly on subject selection and characteristics, estimate of variance and control for confounding. Similarly, in almost all qualitative studies, authors failed to show reflexivity and/or to report verification of the procedure to establish credibility.
Data synthesis
Body size preferences for African women and adolescent girls
Dimension one: ideal body size
The majority of quantitative and mixed methods studies (46/58 (79·3 %)) assessed African women’s body size ideals (see online Supplemental Material 5). Most studies used body image scales whilst the remaining studies used questionnaires to capture the relationship between specific attributes (e.g. healthy, wealthy, dignified, respected) and different body sizes.
For studies using body image scales and for which a BMI category could be extracted, we found that, overall, participants in most studies preferred normal weight to overweight (Fig. 4). We observed a positive relationship between age category and ideal body size, with valorisation of underweight in some adolescent females(63,85) and valorisation of overweight in middle-aged women(27,73,77,78,86,97). The rural–urban comparison showed that the ideal BMI category was higher in rural than urban areas in South Africa(27,69), Senegal(93,116) and Cameroon(114).
Fig. 4
Body size ideals for African women and adolescent girls
[Figure omitted. See PDF]
Few studies explored attributes associated with different body sizes. One study(66) observed differences between urban black and white South African women, especially in middle-aged women, regarding the ‘normal’ and ‘fat’ attributes; and observed that black girls and their mothers were less likely to associate ‘fatness’ with being unhappy in comparison with the white and mixed ancestry group. Likewise, another study(65) found that more than 80 % of black South African rural women disagreed that ‘fat people eat more than thin people’. Moreover, 25 % of these women associated overweight with a lack of financial problems, and more women with a normal weight associated overweight/obesity with food intake, compared with those overweight and obese. In a black township of Cape Town, 74 % of black females considered that being fat ‘made you dignified’ and 43 % believed that this weight status led one to ‘feel better about yourself’(111). Indeed, a woman who was overweight was perceived as ‘well-liked’ (100 %), ‘proud of her movements’ (100 %), ‘healthy’ (100 %) and ‘happy’ (94 %). However, a woman who was ‘thin’ was perceived by 33 %, 58 % and 60 % of the sample as ‘sick’, a ‘woman with worries’ and ‘not treated well by her husband’, respectively.
Dimension two: body size self-assessment
In total, 19/58 (32·7 %) studies included information on body size self-assessment (see online Supplemental Material 5). Using body image scales, one study(51) found that 34·6 % of Kenyan women living in urban Nairobi underestimated their body weight; with 28·8 % of women who underestimated their weight classified as obese, a pattern also observed in Cameroonian urban women(42). The same was also observed with overweight/obesity in Nigerian students and South African black urban women, as well as in black schoolgirls, Algerian Saharawi refugees, urban Tunisian, Moroccan and Malawian women(24,53,79,87,90,96,97,115). Only 37·1 % of Ghanaian women living in Accra perceived their current body size as overweight or obese, even though 49·9 % were in these weight categories(70), a pattern also observed by others in the same city(48,78).
Using questionnaire items in rural Morocco, almost all (99·2 %) women who were overweight/obese underestimated their body weight status, which increased with age(72). Similarly, 89 % of middle-aged South African black women living in Cape Town were happy with their weight, whereas most of them were overweight or obese(66). Likewise, approximately two-thirds of Black women with overweight/obesity in Cape Town did not perceive themselves as such(111). Finally, in urban Cameroon, 37·5 % of women considering themselves normal weight were overweight or obese(42).
Using body image scales, most adolescent Egyptian schoolgirls estimated their body weight accurately along the spectrum of BMI categories(63). Similarly, in the Seychelles, 24 % of adolescent girls of normal weight considered themselves overweight,(75) and in Mauritius most adolescent girls who classified themselves as overweight were in fact normal weight(80).
Dimension three: body size self-satisfaction
Overall, 44/58 (75·9 %) studies included information on body size self-satisfaction (see online Supplemental Material 5).
Of the studies that assessed satisfaction using scales (n 24), seventeen found a positive Feel minus Ideal Discrepancy (FID) (i.e. current > ideal), meaning that women and/or adolescent girls wanted to lose weight(27,42,48,53,59,66,69,71,73,79,87,90,91,95,97,114,116). A further five studies found a negative FID (i.e. current < ideal), meaning that women and or/adolescent girls wanted to gain weight(41,52,62,86,115) and two found mixed results. The two studies reporting mixed results included Mchiza et al. (77) with a positive FID among mothers and null FID (i.e. satisfied with current weight) among black girls, and Siervo et al. (68) with a positive FID among middle-aged women and a negative FID among young women.
Of the studies that assessed satisfaction using questionnaires (n 30), ten found that women who were overweight or obese were satisfied with their current body size(8,40,51,60,65,75,78,80,88,97). The percentage of participants with a BMI ≥ 25 kg/m2 who were satisfied with their current weight ranged from 10·5 % (obese women only)(40) to 95·0 % (overweight women only)(65). For all papers reporting prevalence of body satisfaction among women who were overweight or obese separately, we found that the percentage of satisfaction was higher among women who were overweight, indicating that they were overall more satisfied with their body size in comparison with women who were obese(8,40,51,65,78). One study conducted in South Africa(97) found that the level of body satisfaction was slightly higher among mothers (28·0 %) when compared with their daughters (23·1 %) (both had a BMI ≥ 25 kg/m2), and another study in South Africa(60) showed that body satisfaction was higher among women who were overweight or obese in rural areas (61·4 %) compared with those in urban areas (32·3 %). Eleven studies reported information on the proportion of participants with a BMI ≥ 25 kg/m2 wanting to be larger(8,40,51,65,72,75,78,80,88,96,97). We found that this phenomenon ranged from none in the Seychelles(75), Mauritius(80) and Algeria(96) to 45·5 % in Morocco(72).
Of the studies that assessed satisfaction using questionnaires (n 30), eight found underweight participants (BMI < 18·5 kg/m2) were satisfied with their current body weight(8,40,51,60,78,80,88,97). The proportion of participants satisfied ranged from 2·3 % amongst women in urban Ghana(78) to 95·5 % amongst adolescent girls in rural South Africa(60). A study comparing body satisfaction among mothers and daughters who had a BMI < 18·5 kg/m2 showed that daughters were more satisfied with their body size (45·9 %) in comparison with mothers (22·2 %), meaning that younger generations might have a preference for slimmer bodies(97). Six studies reported information on the proportion of participants with a BMI < 18·5 kg/m2 willing to lose weight(8,9,40,78,80,97). We found that the prevalence of women who were underweight and wanted to be slimmer ranged from none in Morocco(8,9) to 33·3 % in Mauritius(80).
Factors influencing body size preferences: quantitative and qualitative evidence
Twenty-nine quantitative studies and twenty-three qualitative studies (including eight mixed-methods studies) reported factors associated with body size preferences (see online Supplemental Material 5). For both types of studies, factors were grouped into four overarching themes (socio-demographic, health-related, psycho-social and socio-cultural), and their influence on preferred body size (i.e. preference for a slim(mer) or a large(r) body size) indicated (Tables 3 and 4). Eleven analytical themes emerged from qualitative studies (Table 5). The integrated results are discussed below and summarised in Fig. 5.
Table 3 Factors associated with a preference for a slim(mer) body size among African women and adolescent girls from the quantitative evidence synthesis
Table 4 Factors associated with a preference for a large(r) body size among African women and adolescent girls from the quantitative evidence synthesis
Table 5 Factors influencing body size preferences for African women and adolescent girls from the qualitative evidence synthesis
Theme | Child theme | Grand-child theme | Studies |
---|---|---|---|
Sociodemographic factors | |||
Place of residence | Integration into urban lifestyle | Urban lifestyle and weight gain/valorisation of overweight/stoutness | 116 |
Rural residency | Rural and limited corpulence valued (high fatness not adapted to rural labour) | 116 | |
Rural and valorisation of stoutness | 114 | ||
Socio-economic status | SES | Higher SES have thin body ideal | 11,102 |
Age | Young | Young people have thin(ner) body ideal | 107,109,116 |
Old | Middle-aged and older women tend to value overweight/heavier body sizes | ||
Health-related factors Health | Fatness and health | Fatness and NR-NCDs (e.g. hypertension, diabetes)/poor physical health | 24,100,103,106,107,108 *,109,110,111,113,115 |
Psychosocial factors | |||
Appearance and attractiveness (to men and/or society) | Perception of a large/thin body | Stoutness and beauty (pre-marriage) | 114 |
Fatness and having a figure/good shape | 23,24,42,101,106,107,113,105 | ||
Full-figured (medium-sized) women who were neither slim nor fat were seen as attractive by society. | 105 | ||
A full-figured body was considered to be sexually attractive, enhancing sexual gravitation | 105 | ||
Larger body sizes/fatness, chubbiness and big body size as signs of health | 107,108 †,115 | ||
Obesity unattractive | 100,105,112 | ||
Thinness and beauty | 100,104,106,107,108,110 | ||
Thinness and good shape | 107,108 * | ||
Thinness and intelligence | 108 | ||
Thinness – fashionable clothes | 104,106,112,113 | ||
Thinness and vulnerability/proneness to diseases | 105,109 | ||
Thinness and healthiness | 109 | ||
Thinness unattractive | 107 | ||
Clothing | Clothing fitting properly – looking nicer on full-figured women | 105,114,115 | |
Thinness – fashionable clothes | 114,106,112,113 | ||
Clothing fitting properly – looking nicer on a thinner body size | 98,100,104,108,111 | ||
Thinness and looking smart | 100,108 | ||
Personal well-being | Personal well-being | Stoutness and well-being | 114 |
Full-figured and social esteem | 105 | ||
Fatness and happiness | 107,108,109 | ||
Obesity limitations – difficulty performing exercise and daily activities, shortness of breath | 11,98,100,102,108,110,112,113 | ||
Thinness – feeling better about oneself | 106,111,112 | ||
Thinness and happiness | 108 * | ||
Thinness and physically active | 107 | ||
Thinness and popularity | 111,112 | ||
Thinness and unhappiness | 108 ‡ | ||
Thinness and stress/depression | 108 | ||
Thinness and laziness | 110 | ||
Fatalistic attitudes toward body size | Aetiology of thinness/fatness | Body size is God given | 11,100,102 |
Body size is driven by genetic | 100,108,113 | ||
Fatness caused by pregnancy | 11,102,110 | ||
Fatness caused by medication (e.g. ART treatment) | 98,110 | ||
Overweight caused by inability to afford good quality meat | 111 | ||
Obesity attributed to laziness, sluggishness, stigma and tiredness | 108 | ||
Fatness and unhealthy eating (both in quality and quantity) | 98,99,106,108, 111,113 | ||
Weight gain from stress | 98,99,111 | ||
Thinness and eating too little or not healthily | 107,108 | ||
Weight loss from stressful times (e.g. stress, financial problems, social problems, physical abuse, drug abuse) | 98,104,108 | ||
Socio-cultural factors | |||
Negative treatment by the community | Thinness and misfortune | Thinness and poverty/failure in life | 42,100,106 |
Thinness and perception that family cannot afford food | 105 | ||
Stigma | Thinness/weight loss associated with sickness, e.g. HIV/AIDS, TB, cancer, anorexia | 23,24,100,103,104,106,107,108,110,111,113 | |
Social discrimination against overweight | 98,103,104,112,113 | ||
Acculturative stress | Media | Thinness and modelling | 101,104,113 |
Media promotion of thin and thinness | 104,112,116 | ||
Ethnicity | Pressure from other ethnic groups to be thin | 98,104 | |
Happy to accept African heritage | Cultural norms/identify | Fatness as norm/African heritage - Full figured preservation of identity | 42,104,105,106,107,108,110,112 |
Thinness classed as ‘un-African’ | 107 | ||
Social Standing | Respectability in community | Fatness and doing well in life | 23,24,42,99,100,103,111,113,114 |
Fatness and increased chances of getting married | 109 | ||
Body size after marriage/fatness or weight gain after marriage symbol of the capacity of the husband to look after his family | 99,100,111,114 | ||
Stoutness/fatness and fertility | 107,114 | ||
Fatness and ability to take good care of family | 24,99,110,114 | ||
Fatness and dignity/respect for a woman and her family | 107,108 | ||
Fatness as a symbol of wealth and prosperity/full figured and economic standing | 105,108,110,114,116 | ||
Fatness and capability/strength | 23,24,42,101, 107,111,113 | ||
Thinness and strength | 107 | ||
Thinness and affluence | 11,102 |
* Found for normal weight women only.
† Found in overweight/obese women.
‡ Found in obese women.
Fig. 5
Integrated map of the factors influencing body size preferences for African women and adolescent girls
[Figure omitted. See PDF]
Socio-demographic factors
The quantitative evidence showed that increased SES(40,48,92), non-manual occupations(92) and living in urban areas(27,63,69) were associated with slimmer body size preferences. Rural residency was associated with a greater preference for a larger body size,(27,63,69,116) whilst marriage was associated with a lower preference for a larger body size(8,78). Younger participants had a greater preference for slimmer body sizes in Senegal(40), Ghana(67,78), Gambia(68), South Africa(27) and the Seychelles(92), but a greater preference for a larger body size in three studies conducted in Morocco(8,72,81). Increased years of formal education was associated with a greater preference for both slimmer body size ideals in Ghana(48), Cameroon(41) and Seychelles(92) and larger body size ideals in Morocco(8,81). However, one study(78) in Ghana found that increased years of formal education was associated with a lower preference for larger body size but this was NS.
The qualitative evidence revealed that higher SES(11,102) and younger participants(107,109,116) had slimmer body size preferences. Valorisation of stoutness was found in both urban Senegal(116) and rural Cameroon(114), especially in middle-aged and older women. However, high fatness was not valued by younger and also older women in rural Senegal, as it was deemed ill-adapted to rural labour and hence, not valued(116).
‘If you are too rey [stout] in the village, you will not be able to work or cultivate…’ (middle-aged woman, urban, Senegal)(116)
Health-related factors
BMI
Individual BMI was associated with body size preference, although mixed results were found.
Overall, increased BMI in women was positively associated with a greater preference for slimmer body sizes(69,78), with the exception of one study(91), and negatively associated with a preference for a large(r) body size(8,69). One study also found a negative association between men’s individual BMI and a preference for slimmer female body sizes(27).
Women who were overweight or obese had a predominantly greater preference for a slimmer body size(8,40,67,79,87,95,97). Similarly, a greater waist and hip circumference, central and peripheral adiposity and total fat and fat free soft tissue mass were positively associated with slimmer body size preferences or participants desire for weight loss(87). Being overweight or obese was also significantly associated with a lower preference for a larger body size(72,95), with the exception of one study in South Africa, for which the association was positive(91).
Women who were underweight or normal weight had either a greater preference for a slimmer body size/lower preference for a larger body size(80,81,88) or a greater preference for a large(r) body size(40,95).
Concern about health
Two quantitative studies found that participants who reported being concerned about their overall health status or the possibility of developing nutrition-related noncommunicable diseases had a greater preference for a slimmer body size(67,70). The relationship between overweight/fatness and nutrition-related noncommunicable diseases was reported in eleven qualitative studies(24,100,103,106–111,113,115). As a result, some participants wanted to lose weight for health reasons.
‘I heard that when a person is fat there are a lot of fats and oils in the body, which may result in some diseases, like hypertension and diabetes’…’I would like to have medium [overweight] body, because sometimes when you are too fat, it becomes difficult to walk’ (adult women, urban/rural, Malawi)(115)
Biological factors
Being nulliparous(48) was associated with a greater preference for a slimmer body size whilst pubertal status (i.e. mid to post-pubertal girls)(91) was associated with a preference for a larger body size.
Psycho-social factors
Appearance and attractiveness
Quantitative studies reported a positive association between appearance or a desire to look better and a preference for a slimmer body size(80,82). Likewise, clothing (i.e. clothes too tight) was positively associated with a desire for a slimmer body size(69,83).
Appearance and attractiveness to men and/or society was a theme that emerged in most of the qualitative studies. An important aspect of appearance was the way clothing fell on different body sizes. Participants were motivated to lose weight to fit into their old clothes, to look smart or because they felt that clothes on thinner body sizes looked nicer(98,100,104,108,111).
‘Being overweight does not look nice, people often think that you are pregnant’… ‘Bigger clothes are more expensive and less pretty’ (adult/middle-aged women, urban, South Africa)(111)
Four studies reported that fashionable clothes only came in smaller sizes and would therefore only fit slim people(104,106,112,113). However, the importance of filling out clothes to showcase one’s figure was also reported in three studies(105,114,115).
Body size was associated with physical attractiveness, which in turn equated to a better chance of ‘getting a boyfriend or husband’. In six studies, thinness was associated with beauty and good shape(100,104,106–108,111), and three studies reported ‘fatness’ as unattractive and diminishing one’s chances of securing a boyfriend/husband(100,105,112). On the other hand, one study associated stoutness with beauty in women pre-marriage(114) and seven studies associated ‘being fat’, particularly having large hips, with ‘having a figure’ or a ‘good shape’(23,24,41,101,106,107,113).
‘Our culture says that we are supposed to be fat. You must have structure, you must be beautiful. In other words, our culture does not allow women to be thin. They say that someone who is fat is sexier’. (adult/middle-aged woman, urban, South Africa)(106)
To have a good figure, it was seen as important that women gained weight proportionally and did not get too fat(42). It was seen as particularly important that women were not physically restricted by their body size, otherwise their fatness shifted into the realm of being perceived as a result of greed or laziness. In another study, full-figured women (defined as women of a ‘medium size’, neither slim nor fat by investigators) were seen as attractive by society and were considered to be sexually attractive, enhancing sexual gravitation(105). Body size was also associated with intelligence (thin women were perceived as smart)(108) and health, but the evidence was mixed for the latter. In two studies, we found that thinness was associated with good health(105,109) whilst in three other studies, we found that being overweight was a sign of perceived good health(107,108,115).
Body size perception
An overestimation of body size or perception of self as overweight/obese was significantly associated with a preference for a slimmer body size(75,80), whereas women who perceived themselves as underweight had a greater preference for a larger body size(75). In one study(95), an EAT-26 score >20, which indicates a high level of concern about dieting, body weight or problematic eating behaviours, was associated with either a greater preference for a slimmer body size (urban residents) or larger body size (rural residents).
Avoidance of HIV stigma
One quantitative study found a positive association between avoiding HIV stigma and a preference for a large(r) body size(115).
Personal well-being
Perceptions attached to body size and personal well-being were discussed by participants in most of the qualitative studies. In five studies, fatness or a full-figured body size was associated with well-being(114), social esteem(105) and happiness(107–111).
‘If a person is fat [overweight], we usually assume she is happy and has a lot of money. It’s evident that he/she eats nicely’ (middle-aged woman, urban, South Africa)(108)
However, the difficulties associated with obesity (i.e. difficulty performing exercise and daily activities and shortness of breath) were also highlighted(11,98,100,102,108,111–113).
‘I think there is nothing good about being fat because you are constantly sick, and you constantly have pains’ (adolescent girl, urban, South Africa)(113)
The results on thinness were mixed with studies highlighting an association between thinness and markers of personal well-being, such as self-esteem(106,111,112), happiness(108), being physically active(107) and popularity(111,112) and other studies providing evidence of an association between thinness and unhappiness(108), stress/depression(108) and laziness(110).
Fatalistic attitudes towards body size
Various reasons were proposed as ‘causing’ obesity, many of which are founded in scientific evidence, ranging from unhealthy eating in terms of quantity and quality(98,99,106,108,111,113), stress(98,99,111), medication (e.g. anti-retroviral treatment)(98,110), genetics(100,108,113), pregnancy(11,102,111) and laziness(108).
‘There are people who are born fat even though they try to lose weight’ (adolescent girl, urban, South Africa)(113)
However, the belief that obesity was ‘God-given’ also emerged(11,100,102):
‘I used to be thin before I had children. Then slowly after each child I became bigger. This is what Allah intends for a mother to look like’ (woman, urban, Morocco)(11)
This fatalistic belief can lead to participants accepting their body size and being less motivated to lose weight(11,100). Factors influencing thinness included stress(98,104,108) and eating too little(107,108).
Socio-cultural factors
Family influence
From the quantitative evidence, males’ or husbands’ opinions(42,65,67,71,86,88,116) and mothers’ opinions(97) influenced women’s body size preferences. This was a positive influence for a preference for a slimmer body and mixed for a preference for a large(r) body size.
Negative treatment by the community
Thinness and/or weight loss were associated with misfortune in four studies. A person who was thin was seen as being too poor to afford food, or having failed in life(42,100,105,106).
‘My parents also put pressure on me, always commenting on my weight and why I look so thin. They think that being thin is a stigma as people might think that the family cannot afford to buy food’ (adolescent girl, urban, Sudan)(105)
Activities associated with trying to lose weight, such as walking, were also associated with poverty(100,106).
Being thin or losing weight was also strongly associated with illness, particularly with HIV and/or TB(23,24,100,103,104,106–108,110,111,113). Participants preferred not to embody these factors – poverty, HIV and TB – as they are associated with being gossiped about and shunned. Even participants who were aware of the health risks of being overweight and acknowledged that it was impossible to tell if someone has HIV just from their body size still preferred to gain weight or remain overweight to avoid being ostracised(106).
‘She would not be happy [to lose weight] because in our community it would be perceived that there is something wrong with her. She would think that the community is looking at her, so she would feel insecure. They would judge her because they say the person is sick and now wants to cover it up by dieting and exercise’ (adult/middle-aged woman, urban, South Africa) (106)
However, five studies, mainly involving secondary school pupils, reported bullying and exclusion of individuals who were overweight(98,103,104,112,113).
Cultural norms, identity and acculturative stress
Two quantitative studies provided evidence for a positive association between exposure to media and a preference for a slimmer body size(27,28). One Egyptian study(28) found that the risk that pictures in female magazines would influence girls’ ideas of a perfect body shape was almost three times higher among female students who were exposed to magazines v. not or less frequently exposed. An association between television exposure and female students’ ideas of a perfect body shape was also observed.
If having a large body size was seen as a traditional African ideal or heritage(42,104,105–108,110,112), the media’s promotion of thinness was viewed as the antithesis to this tradition(104,112,116). Participants reported feeling pressure and disappointment from the media’s constant promotion of the thin ideal(104,112).
‘I hate the media’s emphasis on weight loss because we cannot all be the same. Some people are born thin and others fat. This emphasis makes people think that fat is unacceptable and ugly’ (adolescent girl, urban, South Africa)(112)
Some participants wanted to be thin like models because they aspired to a modelling career, or because it was fashionable(101,104,113). Only secondary school pupils reported these views. Black South African participants also reported negative comments from White and Mixed-Ancestry friends/colleagues as influencing their perceptions of body size ideals and causing insecurity in their African heritage beliefs(98,104).
Social standing
Body size was viewed as an embodiment of community respectability. Fatness in women pre-marriage was important as it increased their chances of marriage(109). A woman who gained weight soon after marriage was viewed with pride by her in-laws and respected by the community, as her weight gain symbolised being well cared for by her husband and more capable of being fertile and caring for her family(24,99,100,107,108,110,111,114).
‘For a woman, being overweight suggests that her husband takes good care of her, that he is comfortable, he has money’ (adult woman, urban, Cameroon)(114)
A large body size was also associated with general capability, strength, doing well in life(23,24,42,99–101,103,107,111,113) and also represented a symbol of wealth and prosperity(105,108,110,113,116). On the other hand, three studies reported that thinness was associated with affluence(11,102) and strength(107).
Discussion
Summary of key findings
The evidence we synthesised on body size preferences from seventy three quantitative, qualitative and mixed methods studies in twenty-one African countries suggests that normal or overweight body sizes are mainly preferred for African females. This echoes earlier findings from authors who conducted research amongst African Americans over 20 years ago(117). This systematic review found evidence that obesity was a preferred body size in only a small number of studies (n 4), contrary to widely held assumptions concerning the African region.
We observed differences between rural and urban settings within the countries in terms of body size preferences. Furthermore, we noted that countries with the highest economic development, such as South Africa(83–85), Ghana(94) and Egypt(28,59,63,82), presented several studies showing a preference for thinness, particularly in young women.
Socio-demographic, health-related, psycho-social and socio-cultural factors were found to influence African females’ body size preferences. Factors such as age, SES, place of residence, concern about nutrition-related noncommunicable diseases, clothing and media were found to influence body size preferences in both quantitative and qualitative studies.
Younger participants in Morocco had a higher preference for larger body size whilst in West Africa (Ghana, Gambia, Senegal), East Africa (Seychelles) and Southern Africa (South Africa), they had a greater preference for slimmer body sizes. The effect of age on body size preferences may act via two main pathways. First, lay norms revealed that being overweight was associated with marital and economic status, which could represent proxies for responsibilities associated with age(102). Indeed, age has both a biological and socio-cultural component. In traditional societies, age represents wisdom, responsibility and maturity, especially when associated with moderate overweight for married women(118). Second, young people living in urban areas are more exposed to external media and more likely to question the benefits of overweight(27,68,69,101). Media exposure may influence body size preferences more in adolescent females than in adults. Although older females with a higher BMI tended to desire a slimmer body size, there was some evidence that they become desensitised over time to external media(119), especially as they still valued overweight more, as a symbol of prosperity, fertility and peacefulness in the household(8,39,97).
Both quantitative(40,48,92) and qualitative evidence(11,102) showed that increased SES or better employment was associated with slimmer body size preference. The effect of wealth on body size preferences may act by modulating exposure to factors such as external media, tighter ‘fashionable’ clothing, knowledge of the health risks associated with obesity and better access to food resources in urban environments.
Some themes that emerged from the qualitative synthesis corresponded well with factors reported in the quantitative studies in our review, and in a previously published qualitative review(120). As part of their review, Ozodiegwu and colleagues(120) explored factors in the literature that influence overweight or obesity in women of reproductive age living in four different Sub-Saharan African countries (i.e. Ghana, Kenya, South Africa and Botswana); and therefore, we present some overlapping results. Nevertheless, our mixed-methods review included papers from a broader time range (1985 to August 2019), as well as from twenty-one countries in Africa. In our study, appearance played a role in women’s satisfaction with their body size, as they wanted to look attractive to men and peers. Thinness and fatness were both associated with attractiveness, albeit in different studies. Fatness was associated with having a nice figure; a preference for larger hips was reported by African men and women is in accordance with historical observations from Africa and preferences reported by African Americans(25,117,120). This preference has been explained by evolutionary theory whereby large hips are perceived as indicative of a woman’s suitability for childbearing, and therefore her attractiveness and worth within that society, as historically women’s roles equated to being a wife and mother(25). Thinness, as an embodiment of beauty or looking smart, may highlight women’s changing roles in these societies, or perhaps the encroaching influence of ‘Western’ media and ideals on these traditional views(22). It appears these influences may be more prominent amongst younger participants, as evidenced by the fact that acculturative stress was only reported by younger women. Pressure from peers to look slimmer was also reported among adolescent girls(120). The risk of nutrition-related noncommunicable diseases associated with obesity was found to influence women’s preferred body size. Knowledge of the associated health risks of obesity was not universally reported across studies. Women who were knowledgeable about health were still reluctant to lose weight to avoid the stigma associated with HIV and/or TB, as reported in a previous review(120). This risk of facing ridicule also overshadowed weight loss motivations driven by the prospect of improving personal well-being. This emphasises the importance of community acceptance and social standing on African women’s autonomy. It also highlights how the power of social norms can disempower individuals who might want to manage their weight for personal or health reasons. However, it appears that the HIV and/or TB stigma associated with a phobia for thinness seems to progressively disappear in the most educated young women, while the least educated still persist in valuing stoutness, their socio-cultural environment usually linking thinness with poor healthy status, such as being HIV-positive(85). Another factor that contributed to disempowering women from managing their weight was their understanding of the aetiology of fatness/thinness. The aetiologies cited were generally viewed as being out of the individual’s control and therefore not worth trying to change.
Strengths and limitations
The main strength of this review is the use of a mixedmethods’ approach to review a large body of evidence. This allowed a holistic understanding of body size preferences for African women and adolescent girls and the factors driving these. Furthermore, all included studies were of medium or high quality. Even if the FRS(42), used in several studies, was initially developed for Caucasian populations, it was subsequently adapted to African populations(121), and a black adolescent female version was also developed and validated(66). These African versions of the FRS were used in some studies included in this review(73,77,86), showing that the FRS has been contextualised to a certain degree to assess body size perceptions of Africans. Whilst the FRS has limitations as a figural scale, its widespread use allows contextual interpretation of the findings across studies. Another strength is that the prevalence of overweight/obesity among the countries included in our study ranged from 21·3 % in Senegal to 72·0 % in Seychelles(122), which can be considered capturing a range of countries at different stages of the nutrition transition in the African continent.
Limitations of the review included generalisability to the African continent, as Africa is a large, economically and ethnically diverse continent. Most of the quantitative studies were conducted in urban settings, limiting their applicability outside urban settings. Studies were conducted in 21/54 African countries and clustered particularly within Southern Africa, West Africa and East Africa, capturing differences between traditional and modern perceptions of female body size. We noted that findings from Morocco appeared quite different from other countries in terms of body size preferences and factors influencing these. Although factors that play a role in influencing body size preferences were identified in the quantitative studies, it was not possible to establish causality, as almost all included studies were cross-sectional.
Studies included in our review were appraised for quality using the QualSyst tool, which assessed both the quality of studies and their reporting. To our knowledge, this was the most appropriate published tool for the range of study designs included in our review. However, we acknowledge the inherent limitations of such tools, which are reliant on what is reported by authors and that are also subjective in nature(123). As such, there is a possibility that study quality may have been misclassified. A specific risk of bias tool may have provided a more objective measure of study quality by focusing on the actual design and conduct of a study. However, no such tool currently exists for the range of study designs included in this review. Furthermore, given the lack of consensus on the issue of excluding studies based on quality(123) and evidence that shows that excluding lower quality studies does not significantly impact on the synthesis of results(124,125), we decided to include all studies regardless of quality in our review as we wanted to capture a fuller account of the topic.
Policy implications
A preference for overweight body sizes by some African women need not be viewed as a barrier to addressing the rising challenge of overweight and obesity in Africa. A set of interventions, as outlined in the Behaviour Change Wheel(126), could be proposed. For example, emphasis could be placed on education (i.e. increasing knowledge or understanding), persuasion (using communication to stimulate action) and enablement (i.e. increasing means and/or reducing barriers to increase capability or opportunity) interventions. These could include the following actions: (i) providing information to promote a healthy weight through diet and physical activity; (ii) educating the whole community about the gravity of the health risks of obesity and (iii) addressing the underlying attitudes that cause stigmatisation of people with HIV/AIDS and weight loss in general. In addition, conducting mass media campaigns could be used as a vehicle for changing attitudes and risks associated with obesity. Furthermore, younger participants have already expressed unwanted pressure to be slim from the media, as well as some ethnic groups in some African countries; therefore, care must be taken to ensure that educational campaigns do not shift body ideals too far in the other direction and precipitate eating disorders. Qualitative and mixed methods studies focusing on individuals’ perceptions of weight management and education programmes should also be conducted, so that their cultural acceptability and barriers to their use can be determined and used to design culturally acceptable programmes.
Conclusion
Obesity is an important public health issue with a high prevalence amongst African women. It is generally assumed that larger body sizes are preferred for African females. Overall, the evidence synthesised in this review found a preference for a normal and overweight (not obese) body sizes amongst African females. Factors influencing body size preferences were wide ranging. Whilst middle-aged and elderly women tended to value overweight, young women who are more exposed to norms in ‘Western’ media tended to value thinness and wanted to lose weight to improve health. Nevertheless, these perceived health risks were overshadowed by the community’s negative perceptions towards weight loss and thinness, which are associated with HIV and subsequently negative treatment by the community. A preference for overweight (not obese) body sizes among some African females means that traditional cultural norms may still be an obstacle for preventing overweight/obesity. The findings of this review highlight the need for interventions that account for the array of modifiable factors that maintain these preferences (e.g. education, SES, cultural norms, peer pressure and media) and for interventions to be tailored to different stages of the life course. Emphasis needs to be placed on education to prevent all forms of malnutrition such as promoting a healthy diet to help maintain a healthy body weight and raising awareness on the health consequences of obesity. The widespread preference for normal weight is positive in public health terms; however, the relative valorisation of underweight in adolescents and young women may lead to an increase in body dissatisfaction and eating disorders.
Acknowledgements
Acknowledgements: None. Financial support: This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. Conflicts of interest: There are no conflicts of interest. Authorship: M.H. designed the research question with the help of R.P., O.O. and H.O. conducted the searches. M.H., R.P., E.C. and C.B.N. conducted the title, abstract and full text screening. H.O. and C.B.N. checked 10 % of all excluded records. R.P., O.O., E.C., M.H. and H.O. conducted data extraction and quality assessment. R.P. and E.C. led the synthesis with the help of A.I., O.O. and R.P. led the writing up of the manuscript with the help of E.C., M.H., A.I. and O.O. All authors took responsibility for the final editing, reviewing and approval of the manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: Not applicable.
Supplementary material
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980021000768
References1. Development Initiatives (2020) Global Nutrition Report 2020: Action to Equity to End Malnutrition. Bristol, UK: Development Initiatives.
2. Kengne AP, Bentham J, Zhou B et al. (2017) Trends in obesity and diabetes across Africa from 1980 to 2014: an analysis of pooled population-based studies. Int J Epidemiol 46, 1421–1432.
3. Ng M, Fleming T, Robinson M et al. (2014) Global, regional, and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults during 1980–2013: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2013. Lancet 384, 766–781.
4. Prentice AM (2006) The emerging epidemic of obesity in developing countries. Int J Epidemiol 35, 93–99. 10.1093/ije/dyi272
5. Gatineau M & Mathrani S (2011) Ethnicity and obesity in the UK. Perspect Public Health 131, 159–160. 10.1177/1757913911412478
6. Burrowes N (2013) Body image-a rapid evidence assessment of the literature. https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/202946/120715_RAE_on_body_image_final.pdf (accessed September 2020).
7. Sarwer DB, Thompson JK & Cash TF (2005) Body image and obesity in adulthood. Psychiatr Clin N Am 28, 69–87.
8. Rguibi M & Belahsen R (2006) Body size preferences and sociocultural influences on attitudes towards obesity among Moroccan Sahraoui women. Body Image 3, 395–400. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.07.007
9. Rguibi M & Belahsen R (2006) Fattening practices among Moroccan Saharawi women. East Mediterr Heal J 12, 619–624.
10. Brown PJ & Konner M (1987) An anthropological perspective on obesity. Ann N Y Acad Sci 499, 29–46.
11. Batnitzky AK (2011) Cultural constructions of “obesity”: understanding body size, social class and gender in Morocco. Health Place 17, 345–352. 10.1016/j.healthplace.2010.11.012
12. Caballero B (2007) The global epidemic of obesity: an overview. Epidemiol Rev 29, 1–5. 10.1093/epirev/mxm012
13. Lain KY & Catalano PM (2007) Metabolic changes in pregnancy. Clin Obstet Gynecol 50, 938–948. 10.1097/GRF.0b013e31815a5494
14. Perumal N, Cole DC, Ouédraogo HZ et al. (2013) Health and nutrition knowledge, attitudes and practices of pregnant women attending and not-attending ANC clinics in Western Kenya: a cross-sectional analysis. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 13, 146. 10.1186/1471-2393-13-146
15. Aubel J, Touré I & Diagne M (2004) Senegalese grandmothers promote improved maternal and child nutrition practices: the guardians of tradition are not averse to change. Soc Sci Med 59, 945–959. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.11.044
16. Huybregts LF, Roberfroid DA, Kolsteren PW et al. (2009) Dietary behaviour, food and nutrient intake of pregnant women in a rural community in Burkina Faso. Matern Child Nutr 5, 211–222.
17. Bain LE, Awah PK, Geraldine N et al. (2013) Malnutrition in Sub-Saharan Africa: burden, causes and prospects. Pan Afr Med J 15, 1937–8688. 10.11604/pamj.2013.15.120.2535
18. Cohen AK, Rai M, Rehkopf DH et al. (2013) Educational attainment and obesity: a systematic review. Obes Rev 14, 989–1005. 10.1111/obr.12062
19. Ziraba AK, Fotso JC & Ochako R (2009) Overweight and obesity in urban Africa: a problem of the rich or the poor? BMC Public Health 9, 465. 10.1186/1471-2458-9-465
20. Dinsa GD, Goryakin Y, Fumagalli E et al. (2012) Obesity and socioeconomic status in developing countries: a systematic review. Obes Rev 13, 1067–1079. 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2012.01017.x
21. Bakhshi S (2011) Women’s body image and the role of culture: a review of the literature. Eur J Psychol 7, 374–394. 10.5964/ejop.v7i2.135
22. Swami V (2015) Cultural influences on body size ideals. Eur Psychol 20, 44–51. 10.1027/1016-9040/a000150
23. Ezekiel MJ, Talle A, Juma JM et al. (2009) “When in the body, it makes you look fat and HIV negative”: the constitution of antiretroviral therapy in local discourse among youth in Kahe, Tanzania. Soc Sci Med 68, 957–964. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.12.016
24. Matoti-Mvalo T & Puoane T (2011) Perceptions of body size and its association with HIV/AIDS. South African J Clin Nutr 24, 40–45.
25. Tunaley JR, Walsh S & Nicolson P (1999) “I’m not bad for my age”: the meaning of body size and eating in the lives of older women. Ageing Soc 19, 741–759.
26. Spurgas AK (2005) Body image and cultural background. Sociol Inq 75, 297–316. 10.1111/j.1475-682X.2005.00124.x
27. Swami V, Frederick DA, Aavik T et al. (2010) The attractive female body weight and female body dissatisfaction in 26 countries across 10 world regions: results of the international body project I. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 36, 309–325. 10.1177/0146167209359702
28. Musaiger AO & Al-Mannai M (2014) Association between exposure to media and body weight concern among female university students in five Arab countries: a preliminary cross-cultural study. J Biosoc Sci 46, 240–247. 10.1017/S0021932013000278
29. Jones DC & Smolak L (2011) Body image during adolescence: a developmental perspective. In Encyclopedia of Adolescence, vol 1, pp. 77–86 [ RGJ Levesque, editor]. New York, USA: Elsevier Inc.
30. Brink PJ (1989) The fattening room among the Annang of Nigeria. Med Anthropol 12, 131–143. 10.1080/01459740.1989.9966016
31. Wassenaar D, le Grange D, Winship J et al. (2000) The prevalence of eating disorder pathology in a cross-ethnic population of female students in South Africa. Eur Eat Disord Rev 8, 225–236. 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0968(200005)8:3<225::AID-ERV324>3.0.CO;2-P
32. Carney T & Louw J (2006) Eating disordered behaviors and media exposure. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 41, 957–966. 10.1007/s00127-006-0120-9
33. Grant MJ & Booth A (2009) A typology of reviews: an analysis of 14 review types and associated methodologies. Health Info Libr J 26, 91–108. 10.1111/j.1471-1842.2009.00848.x
34. Cooke A, Smith D & Booth A (2012) Beyond PICO: the SPIDER tool for qualitative evidence synthesis. Qual Health Res 22, 1435–1443. 10.1177/1049732312452938
35. Kmet L, Lee R & Cook L (2004) Standard Quality Assessment Criteria for Evaluating Primary Research Papers from a Variety of Fields. Alberta Heritage Foundation for Medical Research (AHFMR) HTA Initiative #13. https://www.ihe.ca/download/standard_quality_assessment_criteria_for_evaluating_primary_research_papers_from_a_variety_of_fields.pdf (accessed September 2020).
36. Higgins J & Green S (2006) Assessment of study quality. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions 4.2.6 Section 6. The Cochrane Library. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
37. Pluye P & Hong QN (2014) Combining the power of stories and the power of numbers: mixed methods research and mixed studies reviews. Annu Rev Public Health 35, 29–45. 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032013-182440
38. Ivankova NV, Creswell JW & Stick SL (2006) Using mixed-methods sequential explanatory design: from theory to practive. Field Methods 18, 3–20.
39. Gradidge PJ, Golele P & Cohen E (2020) Body weight perceptions and obesity amongst university employed South African women. Women Health 60, 851–862.
40. Holdsworth M, Gartner A, Landais E et al. (2004) Perceptions of healthy and desirable body size in urban Senegalese women. Int J Obes 28, 1561–1568.
41. Swami V, Mada R & Tovée MJ (2012) Weight discrepancy and body appreciation of Zimbabwean women in Zimbabwe and Britain. Body Image 9, 559–562. 10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.05.006
42. Cohen E, Boetsch G, Palstra FP et al. (2013) Social valorisation of stoutness as a determinant of obesity in the context of nutritional transition in Cameroon: the Bamiléké case. Soc Sci Med 96, 24–32.
43. Stunkard AJ, Sørensen T & Schulsinger F (1983) Use of the Danish Adoption Register for the study of obesity and thinness. Res Publ Assoc Res Nerv Ment Dis 60, 115–120.
44. Pulvers KM, Lee RE, Kaur H et al. (2004) Development of a culturally relevant body image instrument among urban African Americans. Obes Res 12, 1641–1651.
45. Thompson MA & Gray JJ (1995) Development and validation of a new body-image assessment scale. J Pers Assess 64, 258–269.
46. Williamson DA, Womble LG, Zucker NL et al. (2000) Body image assessment for obesity (BIA-O): development of a new procedure. Int J Obes 24, 1326–1332. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801363
47. Furnham A & Alibhai N (1983) Cross-cultural differences in the perception of female body shapes. Psychol Med 13, 829–837.
48. Duda RB, Jumah NA, Hill AG et al. (2007) Assessment of the ideal body image of women in Accra, Ghana. Trop Doct 37, 241–244.
49. Bell C, Kirkpatrick SW & Rinn RC (1986) Body image of anorexic, obese, and normal females. J Clin Psychol 42, 431–439. 10.1002/1097-4679(198605)42:3<431::AID-JCLP2270420305>3.0.CO;2-I
50. Liburd LC, Anderson LA, Edgar T et al. (1999) Body size and body shape: perceptions of black women with diabetes. Diabetes Educ 25, 382–388.
51. Ettarh R, Van de Vijver S, Oti S et al. (2013) Overweight, obesity, and perception of body image among slum residents in Nairobi, Kenya, 2008–2009. Prev Chronic Dis 10, E212. 10.5888/pcd10.130198
52. Cogan JC, Bhalla SK, Sefa-Dedeh A et al. (1996) A comparison study of United States and African students on perceptions of obesity and thinness. J Cross Cult Psychol 27, 98–113.
53. Caradas AA, Lambert EV & Charlton KE (2001) An ethnic comparison of eating attitudes and associated body image concerns in adolescent South African schoolgirls. J Hum Nutr Diet 14, 111–120.
54. Thomas J & Harden A (2008) Methods for the thematic synthesis of qualitative research in systematic reviews. BMC Med Res Methodol 8, 45.
55. Barnett-Page E & Thomas J (2009) Methods for the synthesis of qualitative research: a critical review. BMC Med Res Methodol 9, 59. 10.1186/1471-2288-9-59
56. Hsieh HF & Shannon SE (2005) Three approaches to qualitative content analysis. Qual Health Res 15, 1277–1288. 10.1177/1049732305276687
57. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J et al. (2009) Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. BMJ 309, e1000097.
58. Salokun SO & Toriola AL (1985) Perceived somatotype and stereotypes of physique among nigerian schoolchildren. J Psychol Interdiscip Appl 119, 587–594.
59. Ford KA, Dolan BM & Evans C (1990) Cultural factors in the eating disorders: a study of body shape preferences of Arab students. J Psychosom Res 34, 501–507.
60. Walker ARP, Walker BF, Locke MM et al. (1991) Body image and eating behaviour in interethnic adolescent girls. J R Soc Health 111, 12–16.
61. Furnham A & Baguma P (1994) Cross-cultural differences in the evaluation of male and female body shapes. Int J Eat Disord 15, 81–89.
62. Toriola AL, Dolan BM, Evans C et al. (1996) Weight satisfaction of nigerian women in nigeria and britain: inter-generational and cross-cultural influences. Eur Eat Disord Rev 4, 84–94.
63. Jackson R, Rashed M & Saad-Eldin R (2003) Rural urban differences in weight, body image, and dieting behavior among adolescent Egyptian schoolgirls. Int J Food Sci Nutr 54, 1–11.
64. Singh D (2004) Mating strategies of young women: role of physical attractiveness. J Sex Res 41, 43–54.
65. Faber M & Kruger HS (2005) Dietary intake, perceptions regarding body weight, and attitudes toward weight control of normal weight, overweight, and obese black females in a rural village in South Africa. Ethn Dis 15, 238–245.
66. Mciza Z, Goedecke JH, Steyn NP et al. (2005) Development and validation of instruments measuring body image and body weight dissatisfaction in South African mothers and their daughters. Public Health Nutr 8, 509–519. 10.1079/PHN2005814
67. Duda RB, Jumah NA, Hill AG et al. (2006) Interest in healthy living outweighs presumed cultural norms for obesity for Ghanaian women. Health Qual Life Outcomes 4, 44. 10.1186/1477-7525-4-44
68. Siervo M, Grey P, Nyan OA et al. A pilot study on body image, attractiveness and body size in Gambians living in an urban community. Eat Weight Disord 11, 100–109.
69. Szabo CP & Allwood CW (2006) Body figure preferences in South African adolescent females: a cross cultural study. Afr Health Sci 6, 201–206.
70. Jumah NA & Duda RB (2007) Comparison of the perception of ideal body images of Ghanaian men and women. Afr J Health Sci 14, 54–60.
71. Frederick D, Forbes GB, Berezovskaya A et al. (2008) Female body dissatisfaction and perceptions of the attractive female body in Ghana, the Ukraine, and the United States. Psychol Top 17, 203–219.
72. Lahmam A, Baali A, Hilali MK et al. (2008) Obesity, overweight and body-weight perception in a High Atlas Moroccan population. Obes Rev 9, 93–99. 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2007.00413.x
73. Okoro E & Oyejola B (2008) Body image preference among Nigerians with type 2 diabetes. Pract Diabetes Int 25, 228–231. 10.1002/pdi.1259
74. Mwaba K & Roman NV (2009) Body image satisfaction among a sample of black female South African students. Soc Behav Personal Int J 37, 905–909.
75. Alwan H, Viswanathan B, Paccaud F et al. (2011) Is accurate perception of body image associated with appropriate weight-control behavior among adolescents of the Seychelles. J Obes 2011, 817242. 10.1155/2011/817242
76. Coetzee V & Perrett DI (2011) African and Caucasian body ideals in South Africa and the United States. Eat Behav 12, 72–74.
77. McHiza ZJ, Goedecke JH & Lambert E V (2011) Intra-familial and ethnic effects on attitudinal and perceptual body image: a cohort of South African mother-daughter dyads. BMC Public Health 11, 433. 10.1186/1471-2458-11-433
78. Benkeser RM, Biritwum R & Hill AG (2012) Prevalence of overweight and obesity and perception of healthy and desirable body size in urban, Ghanaian women. Ghana Med J 46, 66–75.
79. Maruf FA, Akinpelu AO & Nwankwo MJ (2012) Perceived body image and weight: discrepancies and gender differences among university undergraduates. Afr Health Sci 12, 464–472.
80. Bhurtun DD & Jeewon R (2013) Body weight perception and weight control practices among teenagers. ISRN Nutr 2013, 395125.
81. Jafri A, Jabari M, Dahhak M et al. (2013) Obesity and its related factors among women from popular neighborhoods in Casablanca, Morocco. Ethn Dis 23, 369–373.
82. El Ansari W, Dibba E, Labeeb S et al. (2014) Body image concern and its correlates among male and female undergraduate students at Assuit University in Egypt. Glob J Health Sci 6, 105–117. 10.5539/gjhs.v6n5p105
83. Gitau TM, Micklesfield LK, Pettifor JM et al. (2014) Ethnic differences in eating attitudes, body image and self-esteem among adolescent females living in urban South Africa. African J Psychiatry 17, 468–474.
84. Gitau TM, Micklesfield LK, Pettifor JM et al. (2014) Eating attitudes, body image satisfaction and self-esteem of South African Black and White male adolescents and their perception of female body silhouettes. J Child Adolesc Ment Health 26, 193–205.
85. Gitau TM, Micklesfield LK, Pettifor JM et al. (2014) Changes in eating attitudes, body esteem and weight control behaviours during adolescence in a South African cohort. PLoS One 9, 109709.
86. Okoro EO, Oyejola BA, Etebu EN et al. (2014) Body size preference among Yoruba in three Nigerian communities. Eat Weight Disord 19, 77–88.
87. Gradidge PJL, Norris SA, Micklesfield LK et al. (2015) The role of lifestyle and psycho-social factors in predicting changes in body composition in black South African women. PLoS One 10, e0132914. 10.1371/journal.pone.0132914
88. Amenyah SD & Michels N (2016) Body size ideals, beliefs and dissatisfaction in Ghanaian adolescents: sociodemographic determinants and intercorrelations. Public Health 139, 112–120.
89. Caleyachetty R, Kengne AP, Muennig P et al. Misperception of body weight among overweight or obese adults in Mauritius. Obes Res Clin Pract 10, 216–219.
90. Gualdi-Russo E, Rinaldo N, Khyatti M et al. (2016) Weight status, fatness and body image perception of North African immigrant women in Italy. Public Health Nutr 19, 2743–2751. 10.1017/S1368980016000872
91. Pedro TM, Micklesfield LK, Kahn K et al. (2016) Body image satisfaction, eating attitudes and perceptions of female body silhouettes in rural South African adolescents. PLoS One 11, e0154784. 10.1371/journal.pone.0154784
92. Yepes M, Maurer J, Stringhini S et al. (2016) Ideal body size as a mediator for the gender-specific association between socioeconomic status and body mass index: evidence from an upper-middle–income country in the African region. Heal Educ Behav 43, 56S–63S. 10.1177/1090198116630527
93. Macia E, Cohen E, Gueye L et al. (2017) Prevalence of obesity and body size perceptions in urban and rural Senegal: new insight on the epidemiological transition in West Africa. Cardiovasc J Afr 28, 324–330. 10.5830/CVJA-2017-034
94. Michels N & Amenyah SD (2017) Body size ideals and dissatisfaction in Ghanaian adolescents: role of media, lifestyle and well-being. Public Health 146, 65–74. 10.1016/j.puhe.2017.01.006
95. Prioreschi A, Wrottesley SV, Cohen E et al. (2017) Examining the relationships between body image, eating attitudes, BMI, and physical activity in rural and urban South African young adult females using structural equation modeling. PLoS One 12, e0187508. 10.1371/journal.pone.0187508
96. Naigaga DA, Jahanlu D, Claudius HM et al. (2018) Body size perceptions and preferences favor overweight in adult Saharawi refugees. Nutr J 17, 17. 10.1186/s12937-018-0330-5
97. Cohen E, Gradidge PJL, Micklesfield LK et al. (2019) Relationship between body mass index and body image disturbances among South African mothers and their daughters living in Soweto, Johannesburg. Fam Community Heal 42, 140–149.
98. Mvo Z, Dick J & Steyn K (1999) Perceptions of overweight African women about acceptable body size of women and children. Curationis 222, 27–31.
99. Treloar C, Porteous J, Hassan F et al. (1999) The cross cultural context of obesity: an INCLEN multicentre collaborative study. Health Place 5, 279–286.
100. Kiawi E, Edwards R, Shu J et al. (2006) Knowledge, attitudes, and behavior relating to diabetes and its main risk factors among urban residents in Cameroon: a qualitative survey. Ethn Dis 16, 503–509.
101. Dapi LN, Omoloko C, Janlert U et al. (2007) “I eat to be happy, to be strong, and to live.” perceptions of rural and urban adolescents in Cameroon, Africa. J Nutr Educ Behav 39, 320–326.
102. Batnitzky A (2008) Obesity and household roles: gender and social class in Morocco. Sociol Heal Illn 30, 445–462.
103. Shaibu S, Holsten JE, Stettler N et al. (2012) Adolescent obesity prevention in Botswana. J Sch Nurs 28, 220–229.
104. Morris PF & Szabo CP (2013) Meanings of thinness and dysfunctional eating in black South African females: a qualitative study. African J Psychiatry 16, 338–342.
105. Ahmed I & Saltus R (2015) A thematic analysis of female university students’ perceptions of idealised body image in Sudan, and their experiences of performing common beauty practices. Divers Equal Heal Care 12, 40–47.
106. Draper CE, Davidowitz KJ & Goedecke JH (2016) Perceptions relating to body size, weight loss and weight-loss interventions in black South African women: a qualitative study. Public Health Nutr 19, 548–556.
107. Pradeilles R (2015) Neighbourhood and household socio-economic influences on diet and anthropometric status in urban South African adolescents. PhD Thesis, Loughborough University.
108. Okop KJ, Mukumbang FC, Mathole T et al. (2016) Perceptions of body size, obesity threat and the willingness to lose weight among black South African adults: a qualitative study. BMC Public Health 16, 365.
109. Phillips EA, Comeau DL, Pisa PT et al. (2016) Perceptions of diet, physical activity, and obesity-related health among black daughter-mother pairs in Soweto, South Africa: a qualitative study. BMC Public Health 16, 750.
110. Tateyama Y, Musumari PM, Techasrivichien T et al. (2019) Dietary habits, body image, and health service access related to cardiovascular diseases in rural Zambia: a qualitative study. PLoS One 14, e0212739.
111. Puoane T, Fourie JM, Shapiro M et al. (2005) “Big is beautiful”–an exploration with urban black community health workers in a South African township. South African J Clin Nutr 18, 6–15. 10.1080/16070658.2005.11734033
112. Bodiba P, Madu SN, Ezeokana JO et al. (2008) The relationship between body mass index and self-concept among adolescent black female university students. Curationis 31, 77–84. 10.4102/curationis.v31i1.917
113. Puoane T, Lungiswa D & Steyn N (2010) Perceptions about body image and sizes among black African girls living in Cape Town. Ethn Dis 20, 29–34.
114. Cohen E, Amougou N, Ponty A et al. (2017) Nutrition transition and biocultural determinants of obesity among Cameroonian migrants in urban Cameroon and France. Int J Environ Res Public Health 14, 696.
115. Croffut SE, Hamela G, Mofolo I et al. (2018) HIV-positive Malawian women with young children prefer overweight body sizes and link underweight body size with inability to exclusively breastfeed. Matern Child Nutr 14, e12446.
116. Cohen E, Gradidge PJL, Ndao A et al. (2019) Biocultural determinants of overweight and obesity in the context of nutrition transition in Senegal: A holistic anthropological approach. J Biosoc Sci 51, 469–490.
117. Flynn KJ & Fitzgibbon M (1998) Body images and obesity risk among black females: a review of the literature. Ann Behav Med 20, 13–24.
118. De Garine I & Pollock NJ (1996) Social aspects of obesity. Am J Epidemiol 144, 712–714.
119. Cusumano DL & Thompson JK (1997) Body image and body shape ideals in magazines: exposure, awareness, and internalization. Sex Roles 37, 701–721. 10.1007/BF02936336
120. Ozodiegwu ID, Littleton MA, Nwabueze C et al. (2019) A qualitative research synthesis of contextual factors contributing to female overweight and obesity over the life course in sub-Saharan Africa. PLoS One 14, e0224612.
121. Becker DN, Yanek LR, Koftman CM et al. (1999) Body image preferences among urban African Americans and White from low income communities. Ethn Dis 9, 377–386.
122. World Obesity (2021) Global obesity observatory. https://data.worldobesity.org/ (accessed January 2021).
123. Verhage A & Boels D (2017) Critical appraisal of mixed methods research studies in a systematic scoping review on plural policing: assessing the impact of excluding inadequately reported studies by means of a sensitivity analysis. Qual Quant 51, 1449–1468.
124. Carroll C, Booth A & Lloyd-Jones M (2012) Should we exclude inadequately reported studies from qualitative systematic reviews? An evaluation of sensitivity analyses in two case study reviews. Qual Health Res 22, 1425–1434. 10.1177/1049732312452937
125. Carroll C & Booth A (2015) Quality assessment of qualitative evidence for systematic review and synthesis: is it meaningful, and if so, how should it be performed? Res Synth Methods 6, 149–154.
126. Michie S, Stralen MM & West R (2011) The behaviour change wheel: a new method for characterising and designing behaviour change interventions. Implement Sci 6, 42.
* Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society. This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (
Abstract
Objective: To synthesise evidence on body size preferences for females living in Africa and the factors influencing these. Design: Mixed-methods systematic review including searches on Medline, CINHAL, ASSIA, Web of Science and PsycINFO (PROSPERO CRD42015020509). A sequential-explanatory approach was used to integrate quantitative and qualitative findings. Setting: Urban and rural Africa. Participants: Studies of both sexes providing data on body size preferences for adolescent girls and women aged ≥10 years. Results: Seventy-three articles from twenty-one countries were included: fifty quantitative, fifteen qualitative and eight mixed methods. Most studies reported a preference for normal or overweight body sizes. Some studies of adolescent girls/young women indicated a preference for underweight. Factors influencing preferences for large(r) body sizes included: socio-demographic (e.g. education, rural residency), health-related (e.g. current BMI, pubertal status), psycho-social (e.g. avoiding HIV stigma) and socio-cultural factors (e.g. spouse’s preference, social standing, cultural norms). Factors influencing preferences for slim(mer) body sizes included: socio-demographic (e.g. higher socioeconomic status, urban residency, younger age), health-related (e.g. health knowledge, being nulliparous), psycho-social (e.g. appearance, body size perception as overweight/obese) and socio-cultural factors (e.g. peer pressure, media). Conclusions: Preference for overweight (not obese) body sizes among some African females means that interventions need to account for the array of factors that maintain these preferences. The widespread preference for normal weight is positive in public health terms, but the valorisation of underweight in adolescent girls/young women may lead to an increase in body dissatisfaction. Emphasis needs to be placed on education to prevent all forms of malnutrition.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
1 School of Sport, Exercise and Health Sciences (SSEHS), Loughborough University, Loughborough LE11 3TU, UK
2 UMR MoISA (Montpellier Interdisciplinary Centre on Sustainable Agri-food Systems), (Univ Montpellier, CIRAD, CIHEAM-IAMM, INRAE, Institut Agro, IRD), Montpellier, France
3 University Hospitals of Leicester NHS trust, Leicester, UK
4 Warwick Medical School, University of Warwick, Coventry, UK
5 Department of Geography, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK
6 MRC/Wits Developmental Pathways for Health Research Unit, Department of Paediatrics, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa
7 MRC/Wits Developmental Pathways for Health Research Unit, Department of Paediatrics, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa; UMR CNRS-MNHN 7206 « Eco-anthropologie », Musée de l’Homme, Paris, France