Introduction
In many countries, governments have promoted behavioural measures, such as social distancing, avoiding crowded places, and hand hygiene measures to prevent the spread of COVID-19 and to protect vulnerable groups [1]. The effectiveness of these precautionary measures crucially depends on the compliance of the population. In the countries that imposed a lockdown, the rising number of hospital admissions and deaths has stagnated due to compliance with the precautionary measures [1]. However, as time passes people are less willing to comply with these measures [2]. As shown in previous virus outbreaks, perceived invulnerability, lack of motivation, and difficult situations or the perceived inability to carry out the behavioural measures can contribute to reduced compliance [3, 4]. Additionally, the amount of people going out in public areas was increasing due to the relaxation of the measures in some countries, the effects of which was shown in a second wave of infections [5]. In the Netherlands, the first COVID-19 patient was detected end of February 2020. An ‘intelligent’ lockdown was instigated as of March 12th to slow down the infection rate. This lockdown implicated confinements to limit gatherings including more than 3 people until June 1st 2020. Moreover, the population was urged to stay at home as much as possible, ‘non-vital’ shops had to close while others had to make sure to implement a social distancing policy with heavy fines for nonconformity [6]. Besides, restaurants, schools and children day-care had to close [7]. At the time of the study, in the Netherlands, the number of COVID-19 patients per day peaked in April 10th, and the number of deaths on March 31st.
A monitoring study among 50.291 Dutch citizens [8] pointed out that 91.4% of people would mind infecting someone with the virus. Furthermore, this study showed that most people support the measures, although the percentage of people who perceived the measures as effective has decreased between April 17th and June 17th. For social distancing, the decrease in perceived effectiveness was the largest: approximately 12% in these three months. This is in line with the increase of people having difficulties to comply with certain measures, such as washing hands regularly and keeping a distance of 1.5 meters. In addition, fewer people indicated to be willing to follow these measures if they persist for several months. The number of people who are inclined to maintain the social distancing measure until December 2020 decreased by 19.6%. During the first wave of COVID-19 people felt generally vulnerable, yet experienced difficulties to comply with some behavioral measures, amongst others because of not having anticipated on difficult situations.
Behavioural change strategies can be used to motivate and support people to protect themselves and/or vulnerable others, with the aim to promote long-term compliance with the behavioural measures, in the absence of a vaccine. Behaviour change strategies have shown to contribute to a range of lifestyle changes and adherence behaviour [e.g., 9, 10]. There is, however, still no evidence to support the application of specific strategies to improve compliance with the COVID-19 measures. Behavioural models, such as the COM-B model, suggest that behavioural adherence is the result of capability, opportunity and motivation to enact a behaviour. Protection motivation, according to theories is the result of perceiving oneself or others being at risk of an undesirable health outcome, perceiving the benefits of precautionary measures and feeling capable to carry out these measures. Previously, such models have shown to be able to predict precautionary measures against infectious diseases [e.g., 4, 11]. Capability or self-efficacy refers to once ability to carry out a behaviour. Finally, opportunity refers to environment that facilitates the desired behaviour. Behavioural change strategies to increase motivational factors, capability and opportunities are likely to contribute to improved compliance with the behavioural measures. There are considerable number of behavior change strategies. In the present study we selected behavior change strategies that targeted the most important determinants of behavior and could be used practically (following the APEASE criteria (acceptability, practicability, effectiveness, affordability, spill-over effects and equity) [12].
Present study
In the present study, we examined whether we could stimulate behavioural compliance with COVID-19 measures by implementing specific behaviour change strategies that can be used to target motivation, capabilities and opportunities, the key drivers of behaviour [13]. We carried out three experiments during the first lockdown between May and June 2020 in the Netherlands. In three experiments we tested three different behaviour change strategies. In the first experiment we examined whether a volitional help sheet could promote self-efficacy and behavioural compliance. Volitional help sheets, which are based on implementation intentions [14] have been shown to be effective in various health domains [15, 16]. They provide information on potentially difficult or challenging situations and state solutions for these situations (so-called IF-THEN planning). Thus, a volitional help sheet will help people to react in these specific situations and promote an automated response, in other words, to increase capability and opportunity for behaviour.
In the second experiment we examined whether behavioural journalism [17, 18] could improve motivation, self-efficacy (as a measure of capability) and behavioural compliance. Behavioural journalism is based on the theoretical idea of behavioural modelling [19] by means of authentic interviews with credible role models. Modelling is persuasive strategy in which people can learn from the behaviour and opinions of relevant others. The key idea is that by relating to the role models, who express their reason for protecting themselves from COVID-19, highlighting their means to do so, and showing positive outcomes, people would be able to identify with them. This could increase their motivation and feelings of ability to do so.
Lastly, we aspired to examine a behavioural method to increase prosocial motives to protect others. We aimed to examine whether empathy induction could promote salience of the need to protect vulnerable others, and increase the perceived responsibility to protect others [20]. Social psychologists have shown that helping behaviour is more likely when people: notice that something is going on, interpret the situation as being an emergency, feel a degree of responsibility, and implement the action choice. Particularly, the degree of responsibility can be enhanced by enhancing the feeling that a person is deserving of help [21]. In addition, studies on the promotion of helping behaviour have shown that promoting empathy/sympathy for a people in need of help can boost helping behaviour, but only when it is clear how helping behaviour can be realized [22]. Another aspect to promote social behaviour, is that people are motivated by their desire to get credit for their helping behaviour [23].
Inducing empathy and the felt need to help vulnerable groups (helper effect) may increase the motivation of people to comply with the measures. Studies on the promotion of helping behaviour have shown that promoting empathy for people in need of help can boost helping behaviour [22].
Methods
Here we will provide information on the methods used for each of the different experiments (respectively study 1, 2 and 3). All experiments were approved by the TNO Institutional Review Board (2020–041).
Study 1: Volitional help sheet
Methods and participants.
In the present study it was hypothesized that the volitional help sheet would increase both self-efficacy and behavioural compliance with respect to the COVID-19 precautionary measures. The first study was carried out between May 10th and May 23rd 2020. During this period, the RIVM (National Institute for Public Health and the Environment) recommended complying with the following preventive measures: 1) washing one’s hands regularly (20 sec.) with water and soap, 2) sneezing in one’s elbow, 3) keeping 1.5m distance (except for people of the same household), 4) avoiding contact with vulnerable individuals (including people 70 years and older and/or with poor health), 5) staying at home as much as possible, 6) receiving as less visitors as possible, and 7) using paper towels. During data collection, preventive measures were supplemented with the advice to work from home as much as possible and avoid crowds. Incidences in the period of execution of the study ranged from 245 (10th of May) to 176 (May 23rd) new daily infections.
Eligible participants were adults aged 18 years or older who lived in the Netherlands and mastered the Dutch language. Participants were recruited via a Facebook advertisement stating that the survey would take approximately 10 to 15 minutes to complete and that participants could win a €25 voucher. The advertisement was tailored to reach individuals living in the Netherlands with an age span ranging from 18 to 65 years.
We used a randomized post-test only design, with two follow-up measures (directly after the intervention and at 1-week follow-up). Participants who provided informed consent were randomized to the control or experimental group by random number allocation. Participants in both groups were asked to complete a survey measuring sociodemographic variables. Subsequently, the experimental group completed the volitional help sheet. Both groups ended their session with a post-test survey measuring self-efficacy and intention to comply with the precautionary measures, perceived COVID-19 susceptibility, perceived severity, perceived vulnerability of others, subjective behavioural compliance, and response efficacy. At 1-week follow-up, participants received a survey link by e-mail assessing self-efficacy to comply with the precautionary measures and behavioural compliance. Among those who completed the study, € 25,- vouchers were raffled.
Experimental manipulation.
Participants allocated to the experimental group were provided with a volitional help sheet, including “if-then” statements, which comprised potential difficult situations to comply with the COVID-19 precautionary measures and corresponding possible solutions. The volitional help sheet presented 13 potential difficult situations and 20 different solutions (2–5 solutions per situation, with some solutions applying to various situations). These were previously gathered among the population via an online survey (n = 60).
These situations and solutions represented five COVID-19 preventive measures (e.g., washing your hands regularly (20 sec.) with water and soap). Difficult situations were for example, “If I can’t keep 1.5m distance while grocery shopping” or “If I do not have soap at home”, and solutions were e.g., “then I will move to a spot where I can keep enough distance” or “then I will put ’soap’ on my grocery shopping list”. Participants were asked to choose maximal three situations and one solution per situation. Participants then received a summary of their if-then plans. The volitional help sheet can be found in the supplementary material (see S1 Appendix). In total, 50 IF-THEN solutions were offered.
Measures.
Control variables. In order to examine differences between the participants in the control and experimental conditions we gathered information on demographic background: age, gender, country of birth, highest completed level of education and whether they worked in the health care sector. In addition, we examined self-reported COVID-19 infection of participants with one question with 5 response categories (“Are you, or have you been infected with the coronavirus?”, with answers ranging from “I don’t know”, “Yes, I was tested”, “I think so, but I was not tested”, “No, I don’t think so” to “No, I was negatively tested”). Furthermore, participants were asked whether they had been in contact with someone who was infected with the virus, to be answered with “Yes”, “No” or “I don’t know”. Finally we assessed Participation seriousness was assessed at post-test and follow-up with one item on a 5-point Likert scale (“How seriously did you fill out the survey?”, 1 = not at all, to 5 = totally).
Post-test and follow-up measures
Post-test measures included self-efficacy and intention to comply with COVID-19 precautionary measures, perceived susceptibility to get infected with COVID-19, perceived severity to get infected with COVID-19, perceived susceptibility of others to get infected with COVID-19, subjective behavioural compliance with COVID-19 measures and response efficacy to reduce the risk of infecting oneself and others. Items relating to self-efficacy, intention and behavioural compliance were based on the behavioral recommendations of the government. Constructs were assessed using a 5-point Likert scale. For each construct consisting of two or more items, the mean score was used in the regression analyses.
At 1 week follow-up, both self-efficacy to comply and behavioural compliance were assessed.
Items regarding self-efficacy and behavioural compliance were revised at follow-up due to changes in preventive measures recommended by the government (see Table 4).
Self-efficacy was assessed at post-test and follow-up with one item for each precautionary measure (e.g., “Do you feel able to wash your hands regularly (for 20 seconds)”; 1 = certainly not, to 5 = most certainly). The construct consisted of 7 items.
Behavioural compliance with the precautionary measures was assessed at follow-up with one item per precautionary measure (e.g., “In the last week I have sneezed or coughed in my elbow”, “In the last week I have avoided crowds”; 1 = never, to 5 = always). In total, behavioural compliance was assessed with 7 different items.
Intention to comply with the COVID-19 measures was determined at post-test with one item per precautionary measure (e.g., “In the next week, do you intend to keep 1.5 m away from other people?”; 1 = certainly not, to 5 = most certainly). The construct consisted of 7 items.
Perceived susceptibility to get infected with COVID-19 was assessed at post-test with two items (i.e., “What is the chance that you will become infected with the coronavirus the coming months?”, and “What is the chance you will get ill from the coronavirus the coming months?”; 1 = very small to 5 = very large).
Perceived severity was measured at post-test with 1 item (i.e., “How serious do you think it is to get infected with the coronavirus?”; 1 = not serious to 5 = very serious).
Perceived susceptibility of others towards getting infected with COVID-19 was measured at post-test with one item on a 5-point Likert scale (e.g., “How susceptible are [family members/ friends/ colleagues/ chronically ill/ neighbors] for the coronavirus?”, 1 = not at all susceptible, to 5 = very susceptible).
Subjective behavioural compliance was measured at post-test with one item on a 5-point Likert scale (“Do you comply with the precautionary measures in order to limit further spread of the coronavirus?”, 1 = certainly not, to 5 = most certainly).
Response efficacy was assessed at post-test with two items on a 5-point Likert scale (“The precautionary measures effectively reduce the risk of getting infected with the coronavirus” and “The precautionary measures reduce the risk of infecting others with the coronavirus”, 1 = certainly not, to 5 = most certainly).
A priori power analysis.
It was calculated that in order to detect a small-to-medium effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.40 [10]; power of 0.8 and significance level of 0.05 (alpha)), and to account for 20% dropout, 125 participants per intervention arm per experiment were needed.
Statistical analyses.
Differences in characteristics between the two groups were assessed using independent samples t-tests for continuous variables, Pearson’s chi-squared tests with Yates’ continuity correction for categorical variables, and Wilcoxon rank sum tests for ordered categorical variables. The differences in all psychological and behavioural constructs between the two groups were assessed using multiple linear regression models. The independent variables in these models were group, sex, age, and level of education. Effect sizes for linear regression models were calculated using Cohen’s f2statistic [24], , in which B is the variable of interest, i.e. the group variable, A is the set of all other variables, i.e., sex, age, and level of education, is the proportion of variance accounted for by A and B together, and is the proportion of variance accounted for by A. To assess the differences between the two groups on the separate precautionary measures, Wilcoxon rank sum tests with continuity correction were used. The internal consistency of psychological and behavioural constructs was assessed using Cronbach’s coefficient alpha [25]. Spearman’s rank correlations were calculated, when the psychological construct was only measured with two items (i.e., Perceived COVID-19 susceptibility). All analyses were performed using R (version 3.6.1) [26]. For all regression analyses the distribution of the residuals was checked with a QQ plot. The residuals were normally distributed. To counter effects of multi comparisons problem, we used Holm-Bonferroni correction.
Study 2: Behavioural journalism
In experiment two, again, participants were recruited via social media (similar to study 1) and randomly allocated to the experimental or control condition. Participants in the experimental group were offered the behavioural journalism manipulation based on four short films, in which “positive role models” explained how they dealt with the covid compliance measures during times of COVID-19. It was hypothesized that behavioral journalism strategy would increase feelings of capability (as measured by self-efficacy) and motivation (as measured by intention).
Method and participants.
We again used a randomized post-test only design with two post-tests (directly after the intervention and at 1-week follow-up). The second study took place between 15th of May and 7th of June 2020. Preventive measures at the time of execution comprised washing one’s hands regularly (20 sec.) with water and soap, sneezing in one’s elbow, keeping 1.5m distance (except for people of the same household), avoiding contact with vulnerable individuals (including people 70 years and older and/or with poor health), using paper towels, working from home as much as possible and avoiding crowds. The respective incidence during the time the study was executed ranged between 200 (15th of May) and 239 (7th of June 2020).
Experimental manipulation.
Participants in the behavioural journalism condition were offered four short films, ranging from 1:22 minutes to 1:40 minutes. The four films comprised: a male student, a young working couple, a pregnant woman and a healthcare worker. In all scenarios the depicted individuals shared the impact that COVID-19 has on their lives and how they dealt with it. This included taking precautionary measures when doing everyday errands and during daily activities. Furthermore, the role models explained why they believed it to be important to comply with the precautionary measures. Participants were instructed to watch at least one of the films while being allowed to watch as many of the role model stories as they felt seemed relevant or interesting to them.
Scenario 1 (1:25 minutes). In the first scenario, a young male student (21 years) reports how he accommodates to his new lifestyle in times of COVID-19: Meeting his friends online for pub quizzes and attending online lectures. Furthermore, the student tells that he is currently unable to visit his grandparents. Yet, he states his problems not to be as big as he ‘only’ needs to stay at home whereas others are suffering far more from the impact COVID-19 has on their lives, highlighting the need to comply with the precautionary measures. Besides emphasizing the importance of staying healthy, the young man ends on a positive note, elaborating that the current situation can be seen as a chance to do and learn new things from home (Fig 1). (The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Depicting student Maarten, 21 years, “meeting his friends online”.
Scenario 2 (1:40 minutes). The second scenario depicts a young couple who reveal what their everyday life looks like under the current circumstances: While adapting their lives in accordance with the precautionary measures and missing out on social contacts seemed to be hard at first, the couple reports changes to their daily rhythm including running errands alone, working from home and eventually being in touch/connected with others even more than before. They explain that they comply with the regulations to prevent infecting vulnerable others such as their (grand)parents, thereby advising on being creative to find something that is energizing and one enjoys like trying new recipes.
Scenario 3 (1:35 minutes). The third scenario is about a pregnant woman. The young mother talks about how she and her partner now work from home and take care of their son together. Apart from doing pregnancy yoga via zoom in her living room, she talks about getting to know their neighbourhood in a different way through taking a lot of walks. The role model reports that she and her family comply with the regulations to not infect vulnerable others. Also, she reports that working from home provides her with peace of mind and that her son’s development enormously benefitted from the prolonged time at home as the toddler now talks more than before and immensely enjoyed the time spent with his parents.
Scenario 4 (1:22 minutes). In the last scenario, the viewer accompanies a young woman working as specialist in geriatric medicine to her workplace, where she helps vulnerable elderly suffering from physical and mental health problems such as dementia. Whilst one of the greatest changes in her daily work life is the lack of visitors her patients would normally receive, she talks about seeing her patients less often and spending time in the office more frequently than she normally would. Before working with infected patients, she has to get changed and is very alert not to infect any other patient, which is one of her fears. Simultaneously, to persevere, the health care worker reports to remind herself that she is complying with the regulations for her patients.
Measures.
Similar demographic variables as in the first experiment were included as part of a baseline questionnaire. Items regarding self-efficacy, intention to comply with precautionary measures and behavioural compliance thereof were adapted to the most current behavioural measures due to governmental revisions of precautionary measures. Behavioural compliance was therefore assessed with 7 items. All other constructs were identical to study 1 (see Table 2).
Manipulation check. In addition, the films were evaluated in the experimental group, by asking participants at post-test how many of the films they watched, and whether they perceived the role model stories as relevant, recognizable and helpful (1 = not at all, to 5 = very relevant/recognizable/helpful).
Post-test and follow-up measures
Measures were similar to those of study 1, except that the items regarding self-efficacy, intention to comply with precautionary measures and behavioural compliance were adapted to comply to the most current behavioural measures due to governmental revisions of precautionary measures. Behavioural compliance was therefore assessed with 7 items. Like study 1 we assessed all measures at post-test, and self-efficacy, intention and behaviours at 1-week follow-up.
Study 3: Empathy induction
Method and participants.
In this experiment we assessed whether empathy induction could promote salience of the need to protect vulnerable others and increase the perceived responsibility to protect others. We used a randomized post-test only design with two post-test measures (directly and at 1-week follow-up). In this experiment, participants were randomized to the experimental condition, which received the empathy induction or the control group.
The third study took place between 16th of May and 7th of June 2020. Preventive measures at the time of execution comprised washing one’s hands regularly (20 sec.) with water and soap, sneezing in one’s elbow, keeping 1.5m distance (except for people of the same household), avoiding contact with vulnerable individuals (including people 70 years and older and/or with poor health), using paper towels, working from home as much as possible and avoiding crowds. The respective incidence during the time the study was executed ranged between 189 (16th of May) and 239 (7th of June 2020).
The recruitment of participants and procedure were identical to those of study 1, except that the advertisement was set to reach individuals ranging from 18 to 40 years. This change was made in order to reach members of the younger generation, who were expected to protect the more vulnerable members of society, including elderly citizens.
Experiment: Empathy induction.
Participants allocated to the experimental group were asked to watch a short film (1:42 min.) depicting a 70-year old woman who explains why she belongs to the at-risk population, due to her age and having asthma (Fig 2) (The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details). In the video, the woman points out that, while she is following the precautionary measures herself to keep safe from COVID-19, she is still dependent on others to follow the measures to be protected. Participants who indicated their readiness to protect others were offered a gift as credit for wanting to do so. The gift also served as a reminder for taking precautionary measures and comprised a blue silicone band stating “Door mij coronavrij!” [Corona-free through me!”].
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Showing Anne-Marie, a 70-year old woman during activities in times of corona.
Measures.
Constructs were the similar to those used in Study 2 (see Table 2). In addition, empathy induction and attitude to protect others were measured using a 5-point Likert scale.
Empathy induction was assessed at post-test after the intervention with five items (“Which feelings did watching the short film with Anne-Marie evoke?” with respect to “Sympathy”, “Compassion”, “Involvement”, “Empathy” and “Warmth”, 1 = not at all, to 5 = absolutely).
Attitude towards protecting others was assessed at post-test with three items (“We should protect vulnerable people from corona”, 1 = totally disagree, to 5 = totally agree, “How important do you think it is to prevent vulnerable people to get infected with corona?”, 1 = totally unimportant, to 5 = totally important, and “To what extent are you willing to protect another by following the precautionary measures?”, 1 = not at all, to 5 = totally). Individuals reporting a certain extent to which they are willing to protect another were offered to receive a small gift to get credit for their behaviour.
Manipulation check. Participants in the experimental group were asked to indicate whether they had watched the film along with feelings evoked by watching it, as well as their attitude towards, and willingness to protect others. In addition, participants in the experimental group were asked to indicate whether they had requested, received and worn the gift.
Results
Study 1: Volitional help sheet
In total, 424 of 482 participants consented to participate, who were allocated to the intervention (n = 181) or control (n = 243) group. Data of 339 participants were analysed (n = 149 intervention, n = 190 control). Dropout was 21%, mainly due to non-response or unreachable e-mail addresses.
The characteristics of participants are presented in Table 1. Participants in the intervention and control group were similar in socio-demographical characteristics, with only a difference in level of education between the two groups. Most participants were female, were born in the Netherlands, did not work in healthcare and had someone in their environment with an increased risk of becoming ill from COVID-19.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
A manipulation check was performed for the VHS. Participants selected situations that they perceived to be challenging. In total, 58.0% of the participants perceived it as challenging ‘when there is not enough space to pass someone in the supermarket’. Most participants reported that they would either wait until there is enough space or to step aside to create enough space as solution. Furthermore 56.4% of the participants declared to experience it as challenging ‘when family members feel lonely and ask if I come by’. Most common solutions to this situation were to visit them, but only outside and with enough distance (60.8%) or to videocall those family members (32.4%). For the full list, see S1 Appendix.
Tables 2 and 3 present the means, standard deviations, and internal consistency of the constructs and the results of the regression analyses with the uncorrected p-values. The regression analyses (with condition as predictor, controlled for sex, age, and level of education) indicated that participants who received the volitional help sheet showed more favourable results on most measures. At post-test, participants in the intervention group had a higher self-efficacy and intention to comply with the precautionary measures compared to the control group. In addition, participants in the intervention group had a higher perceived compliance, a higher perceived susceptibility towards themselves and a higher perceived vulnerability of others to become infected with COVID-19. Lastly, participants in the intervention group reported a higher perceived severity of becoming infected with COVID-19.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
When we adjusted for multiple testing by means of the Holm-Bonferroni correction, the differences in perceived severity and perceived susceptibility of others remained significant.
At follow-up, participants in the intervention group more often complied with several individual precautionary measures compared to the control group. Participants reported to comply more often to sneezing and coughing in the elbow, staying at home as much as possible, receiving as little visitors as possible and avoiding crowds compared to the control group (see Table 4). No significant differences were found between conditions on the behavioural compliance index and self-efficacy at follow-up. When adjusting for multiple comparison by means of the Holm-Bonferroni method, only the effects for receiving as little visitors as possible, and avoiding crowds remain.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Study 2: Behavioural journalism
In total, 593 of 638 participants consented to participate and were randomly allocated to the behavioural journalism condition (n = 290) or control (n = 303) group. In total, data of 449 participants were analysed (n = 212 intervention, n = 235 control). Dropout among the participants was 25%; this was due to non-response or unreachable e-mail addresses.
Characteristics of participants are presented in Table 1. Socio-demographics were similar for the intervention and control group. There was, however, a small difference between the two groups with regard to sex. Most participants were female, were born in the Netherlands, finished higher education, did not work in healthcare and had someone in their environment with and increased risk of becoming ill from COVID-19.
The manipulation check showed that 37.6% of the participants watched one video, 43.1% two or more videos and 19.3% indicated not to have watched any video. The videos from the male student and the young woman working as specialist in geriatric medicine were most frequently watched (47.6% and 43.1%). Most of the participants reported that the videos were relevant (55.3%) and recognizable (59.3%). Nevertheless, only 10% perceived the stories as helpful.
Tables 2 and 3 show the results of means, standard deviations, and internal consistency of social psychological measures and the results of the regression analyses. We hypothesised that the engagement with behavioural journalism would contribute to an increase in intention and self-efficacy to comply with preventive measures. However, no significant differences were found between the intervention and control group on any of the measures at both post-test and follow-up. Furthermore, there were no differences in compliance with the behavioural measures (see Table 4).
Study 3: Empathy induction
In total, 578 of 623 participants consented to participate, which were then allocated to the intervention (n = 261) or control (n = 317) group. In total, data of 428 participants were analysed (n = 196 intervention, n = 232 control). Of the participants, 26% dropped out due to non-response or unreachable e-mail addresses.
The characteristics of participants are displayed in Table 1. Both the experimental and the control group were comparable based on socio-demographical characteristics. There was, however, a small difference in age between the two groups, which was corrected for in the analyses. Most participants were Dutch females who finished higher education.
The manipulation check showed that most participants in the intervention group reported to have established feelings of sympathy (72.0%), compassion (71.2%), involvement (56.9%), understanding (76.6%) and warmth (52.7%) when watching the video of the vulnerable woman. Additionally, 82.7% reported to find it important to protect (vulnerable) others and 67.7% reported that they were willing to protect (vulnerable) others by complying to the precautionary measures after watching the video (see S2 Appendix).
When examining the uncorrected tests, participants in the empathy induction group had a higher intention to adhere to the precautionary measures compared to participants in the control group at post-test. Additionally, participants in the empathy induction group perceived an infection with the virus to be more severe and perceived others to be more vulnerable to become infected with COVID-19 compared to participants in the control group. At follow-up, no significant differences were found between participants in the intervention and control group on any of the social psychological determinants or behavioural compliance at follow-up. After adjusting for multiple comparison by means of the Holm-Bonferroni correction, only the effects for perceived susceptibility of others remained.
Discussion
In the present study we examined strategies to enhance compliance with the COVID-19 behavioural measures.
Based on the three experiments we showed that people exposed to the volitional help sheets were more likely follow-up behavioural measures to avoid other people (limiting visitors at home and avoiding crowds). In addition, people who received the volitional help sheets were more likely to acknowledge the risk of COVID-19 for themselves and for vulnerable others. The empathy induction manipulation enhanced the perception of risk of vulnerable others. Generally, based on conservative analyses adjusted for multiple testing, we were not able to confirm our hypotheses to improve behavioural compliance or its determinants motivation, self-efficacy and intention by means of the three strategies volitional help sheet, behavioural journalism and empathy induction, except for a confirmation of effects on individual behavioural compliance measures by means of the volitional help sheet. However, analyses unadjusted for multiple testing suggested that the volitional help sheet and empathy induction may have potential to improve behavioural compliance: First indications for effects were found for empathy induction on intention to comply with behavioural compliance measures and for the volitional help sheet on self-efficacy, intention and behavioural compliance. Although these effects were small, and short-term, they could be clinically relevant from a public health perspective [27]. Even changing the behaviour of a small percentage of the population could reduce the transmission of COVID-19 and save lives [28]. Furthermore, our findings comply with some other recent findings. For instance, also Pfattheicher et al. [29] showed that highlighting vulnerable persons can encourage physical distancing. Unfortunately, the behavioural journalism strategy did not improve motivation or compliance.
Thus far, a lot of effort has been invested in conveying advice to comply with behavioural measures e.g. [30–32]. Nevertheless, people do not always experience when, where, how and why to comply with the behavioural norms. Especially, when measures change over time. We have shown that behaviour change techniques, derived from health psychology, can be helpful in communicating towards people and supporting people to maintain the behavioural measures.
We did show that two out of three strategies may be effective means to promote (determinants of) compliance to the behavioural measures. We also showed that experimental pilot tests to examine the usefulness of interventions can be piloted rapidly and effectively, which could improve evidence-based advice towards the public, prior to large-scale implementation.
Before discussing further practical implications, we should acknowledge some limitations of our study. Generally, our sample comprised of a convenience sample (recruited by Facebook). Besides, the sample was generally higher educated, the findings may thus not translate to the general public.
Second, we used a post-test control group design with a follow-up at one week. Although, we did not see any demographic differences between the experimental and control group, a pre-post-test control group design with a baseline assessment of the psychosocial determinants and behaviour would have ruled out potential differences, which would provide a stronger case for causation between the intervention and the outcomes.
Another potential limitation may be the lack of validated measures—despite good reliability. Nevertheless, the measures were operationalized in accordance with guidelines [e.g., 33]. Similar measures have been used to also understand behavior and determinants in the context of similar type of infectious diseases [e.g., 4].
Finally, the follow-up at one week does not allow to draw conclusions regarding any longer-term effects on compliance over time. Yet, to minimise participant burden and attrition, we opted for the present design.
In terms of practical implications, we think that both the empathy induction strategy and volitional help sheet could contribute to compliance of preventive COVID-19 measures. A suggestion would be to combine the strategies as the empathy induction might enhance (prosocial) protection motivation: motivation is generally a requirement for the effectiveness of implementation intentions or the VHS [34]. On the other hand, implementation intentions have shown to decrease intention-behaviour gaps, especially for more complex behaviours. Hence, this approach might both motivate and strengthen people to maintain the behavioural measures, by highlighting the importance to protect vulnerable people, and providing the ways to overcome difficult situations. We believe these strategies may be potential means for use during the phase of an outbreak and beyond. Moreover, our study shows that health psychology theory and intervention strategies may provide an important contribution to the more medical or epidemiological science devoted to minimizing the repercussions of the COVID-19 outbreak.
The Behavioural Journalism strategy did not result in favourable changes. Nevertheless, we are hesitant to advice not to use the strategy. Clearly, the manipulation check showed we were able to ensure people could relate to the situations. We did not find a difference between older and younger participants (data not shown), which we checked in order to assess whether the younger aged role models mattered. Nevertheless, the manipulation check also revealed the information gathered was not novel; in addition, one in five people reported that they did not watch any video. Future studies should examine which scenarios would be novel, acceptable and adoptable.
WHO has acknowledged the importance of behavioural sciences in combatting the pandemic [32]. We hope to have demonstrated that health psychology may contribute to preventing the spread of COVID-19. Moreover, we add to the different behavioural monitors being conducted in the Netherlands [31] and elsewhere, by providing means to combat declines in motivation. These strategies can be used by governmental agencies to promote compliance, be it the national government or the local public health centres, when facing outbreaks. The insights at hand remain relevant in times of vaccination as people need to remain compliant with the preventive measures after vaccination.
Conclusion
In the present study, we showed that health behavioural strategies may have the potential to contribute to prolonged compliance with the behavioural measures. This information could be valuable for both governments, governmental organisations and public health centres in further combating COVID-19, also in times of increased vaccination uptake. Experimental pretesting of behavioural interventions to improve behavioural compliance is useful to ensure more evidence-based communication.
Supporting information
S1 Appendix. Volitional help sheet.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0272001.s001
(DOCX)
S2 Appendix. Manipulation check.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0272001.s002
(DOCX)
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Iris Eekhout for preparing the statistical analyses for publication.
Citation: van Empelen P, Preuhs K, Bakker LA, Buursma P, Andree R, Anraad C, et al. (2022) Improving behavioural compliance with the COVID-19 precautionary measures by means of innovative communication strategies: Social experimental studies. PLoS ONE 17(7): e0272001. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0272001
About the Authors:
Pepijn van Empelen
Roles: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: TNO, Department of Child Health, Leiden, The Netherlands
Katharina Preuhs
Roles: Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: TNO, Department of Child Health, Leiden, The Netherlands
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3570-9509
Leonhard A. Bakker
Roles: Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft
Affiliation: TNO, Department of Child Health, Leiden, The Netherlands
Petra Buursma
Roles: Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft
Affiliation: TNO, Department of Child Health, Leiden, The Netherlands
Rosa Andree
Roles: Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft
Affiliation: TNO, Department of Child Health, Leiden, The Netherlands
Charlotte Anraad
Roles: Investigation, Methodology
Affiliations TNO, Department of Child Health, Leiden, The Netherlands, Department of Work & Social Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University Maastricht, the Netherlands
Hilde M. van Keulen
Roles: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: TNO, Department of Child Health, Leiden, The Netherlands
1. Zheng Q, Jones FK, Leavitt SV, et al. HIT-COVID, a global database tracking public health interventions to COVID-19. Sci Data. 2020;7(286). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-020-00610-2 pmid:32855428
2. RIVM. Summary of results from study on behaviour and well-being rounds 1 through 5. 2020. Available: Summary of results from study on behaviour and well-being rounds 1 through 5 | RIVM
3. Bults M, Beaujean DJMA, De Zwart O, Kok G, Van Empelen P, Van Steenbergen JE, et al. Mexican flu: Risk perception among the population, personal actions and trust in public information. Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd. 2010;154: 897–901.
4. Bults M, Beaujean DJMA, De Zwart O, Kok G, Van Empelen P, Van Steenbergen JE, et al. Perceived risk, anxiety, and behavioural responses of the general public during the early phase of the Influenza A (H1N1) pandemic in the Netherlands: Results of three consecutive online surveys. BMC Public Health. 2011;11.
5. Xu S, Li Y. Beware of the second wave of COVID-19. Lancet. 2020;395: 1321–1322. pmid:32277876
6. Trouw. Een ‘intelligente lockdown’. Dit zijn de nieuwe maatregelen tegen het Coronavirus.
7. Rijkoverheid.nl. Maart 2020: Maatregelen tegen verspreiding coronavirus, intelligente lockdown. 2020. Available: https://www.rijksoverheid.nl/onderwerpen/coronavirus-tijdlijn/maart-2020-maatregelen-tegen-verspreiding-coronavirus;.
8. WHO. Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) advice for the public. 2020. Available: www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019/advice-for-public.
9. Kahwati L, Viswanathan M, Golin CE, Kane H, Lewis M, Jacobs S. Identifying configurations of behavior change techniques in effective medication adherence interventions: a qualitative comparative analysis. Systematic reviews. 2016;5(1): 1–9. pmid:27209092
10. Webb T, Joseph J, Yardley L, Michie S. Using the internet to promote health behavior change: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the impact of theoretical basis, use of behavior change techniques, and mode of delivery on efficacy. Journal of medical Internet research. 2010;12(1):e1376.
11. Bish A, Michie S. Demographic and attitudinal determinants of protective behaviours during a pandemic: A review. British journal of health psychology. 2010;15(4):797–824. pmid:20109274
12. West R, Michie S, Rubin GJ, et al. Applying principles of behaviour change to reduce SARS-CoV-2 transmission. Nat Hum Behav. 2020;4:451–459. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0887-9 pmid:32377018
13. Michie S, van Stralen MM, West R. The behaviour change wheel: A new method for characterising and designing behaviour change interventions. Implement Sci. 2011;6.
14. Gollwitzer PM. Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. Am Psychol. 1999;54: 493–503.
15. Armitage CJ. A Volitional Help Sheet to Encourage Smoking Cessation: A Randomized Exploratory Trial. Health Psychol. 2008;27: 557–566. pmid:18823182
16. Armitage CJ, Alganem S, Norman P. Randomized Controlled Trial of a Volitional Help Sheet to Encourage Weight Loss in the Middle East. Prev Sci. 2017;18: 976–983. pmid:28643134
17. Corby NH, Enguídanos SM, Kay LS. Development and use of role model stories in a community level HIV risk reduction intervention. Public Health Rep. 1996;111: 54–58.
18. McAlister A. Behavioral Journalism in Health and Risk Messaging. In: Anonymous Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication.: Oxford University Press; 2017.
19. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman; 2012.
20. Batson CD, Chang J, Orr R, Rowland J. Empathy, attitudes, and action: Can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group motivate one to help the group? Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2002;28: 1656–1666.
21. Darley JM, Latane B. Norms and normative behavior: Field studies of social interdependence. Altrusim and helping behvior, 1970.
22. Baldner C, Pierro A, Kruglanski AW. Moving Toward Helping Behavior: The Roles of Sympathy, Helping Goal Attainability, and Locomotion Orientation. Basic Appl Soc Psychol. 2020;42: 133–149.
23. Touré-Tillery M, Fishbach A. Too far to help: The effect of perceived distance on the expected impact and likelihood of charitable action. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2017;112(6):860. pmid:28253000
24. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York (NY): Routledge; 2013.
25. Cronbach LJ. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika. 1951;16: 297–334.
26. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. 2019. Available: www.R-project.org/.
27. Funder David C., Ozer Daniel J.,. Evaluating Effect Size in Psychological Research: Sense and Nonsense. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science. 2019;2: 156–168.
28. Everett JAC, Colombatto C, Chituc V, Brady WJ, Crockett M. The effectiveness of moral messages on public health behavioral intentions during the COVID-19 pandemic. PsyArXiv. 2020.
29. Pfattheicher S, Nockur L, Böhm R, Sassenrath C, Petersen MB. The Emotional Path to Action: Empathy Promotes Physical Distancing and Wearing of Face Masks During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Psychol Sci. 2020;31: 1363–1373. pmid:32993455
30. CDC. Protect Yourself. 2020. Available: https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/prevent-getting-sick/prevention.html?CDC_AA_refVal=https%3A%2F%2Fwww.cdc.gov%2Fcoronavirus%2F2019-ncov%2Fprepare%2Fprevention.html.
31. RIVM. Applying behavioural science to COVID-19. 2020. Available: www.rivm.nl/en/novel-coronaviruscovid-19/research/behaviour.
32. WHO. New WHO/Europe tool for behavioural insights: critical to inform COVID-19 response. 2020. Available: www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/health-emergencies/coronavirus-covid-19/news/news/2020/4/new-whoeurope-tool-for-behavioural-insights-critical-to-inform-covid-19-response.
33. Conner M, Norman P. Predicting Health Behaviour: Research and Practice with Social Cognition Models. Glasgow: Open University Press, 2005.
34. Sheeran P, Milnes S, Webb TL, et al. Implementation intentions and health behaviour. In: Conner M, Norman P, editors. Predicting and changing health behaviour: research and practice with social cognition models.; 2015.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2022 van Empelen et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Objectives
Behavioural measures such as social distancing are crucial to prevent the spread of COVID-19. People sometimes have difficulty to comply with these behavioural measures for various reasons. Insight is needed into evidence-based strategies that can promote compliance. In the present study, we examined whether behaviour change techniques (volitional help sheet (VHS), behavioural journalism (BJ) and empathy inductions (EI)) could promote behavioural compliance with the COVID-19 measures.
Methods
Three online experiments were carried out (N = 424–593) among Dutch adult citizens, using a randomized 2-group post-test and 1-week follow-up design. In each experiment, a control group was compared with the experimental condition (respectively VHS, BJ or EI condition).
Results
Two out of the three different strategies did result in favourable changes with regard to the compliance-related measures. The VHS contributed to changes in perceived susceptibility of others (t = -2,78; f**2 = 0,019), perceived severity (t = -3,65; f**2 = 0,032) and individual behavioural compliance measures. People exposed to the VHS were more likely to receive less visitors (w = 16638; p = 0.003)and avoid crowds (w = 16631; p = 0.003). EI increased the perceived vulnerability of others. Video-based role model stories, based on BJ did not result in any changes.
Conclusions
Behaviour change strategies may contribute to promoting behavioural compliance and could be used in public health communication. The empathy induction can be used to enhance other protection motives, while the volitional help sheet effectively can help people to overcome compliance barriers. Behavioural science can add to fighting the COVID-19 pandemic.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer