1. Introduction
In the new era of the 21st century, the concept of education is changing, and the family structure and parenting style are changing at the same time. Although the material conditions and infrastructure have been improved, how to raise an excellent child has now become a new focus. Parenting responsibilities and pressures force parents to spend a lot of time and energy caring for and nurturing their children. In addition, the shortage of educational resources, the comparison between parents and peers, and the lack of parenting experience virtually increase their tension and anxiety.
From the perspective of family education and parenting methods in various countries, mothers are the most important caregivers in early childhood, for they have the longest contact with children and can exert the greatest influence on them. Studies have found that there is a correlation between mothers’ emotions and children’s emotions. However, previous studies have mainly explored the causes of mothers’ anxiety, mothers’ mental health status, and the direct or indirect effects of changes in parenting styles on children. For example, the results of a recent follow-up study showed that at different ages of children, there is a significant direct correlation between maternal anxiety and children’s internalizing problem behaviors, but there is no significant correlation between maternal anxiety and children’s externalizing problem behaviors [1]. Meanwhile, a cross-sectional study in Asia showed that maternal psychological control plays a significant mediating role between maternal anxiety and children’s problem behaviors [2]. There is a less in-depth exploration of the relationship between maternal anxiety and young children’s problem behaviors. Rees conducted a relatively systematic meta-analysis of related literature between 2002 and 2017 [3]. However, because the meta-analysis did not include the latest research results in the past ten years, and the quality of the literature included in the study was uneven, the reference to the meta-analysis results was limited. If we can analyze whether and in what way maternal anxiety can affect young children’s problem behaviors through further meta-analysis, and analyze the moderating effects of different characteristics, we can also expand the topics of the impact of maternal anxiety on children’s development. Additionally, we hope that the research content will point out the direction for future research. It is expected that in the future, more effective programs will be put forward to improve the impact of maternal anxiety symptoms on children’s development to ensure the development of children.
1.1. Maternal Anxiety
Anxiety is a complex but individual emotional response. Different scholars have different views on the definition of the specific concept of anxiety. Some scholars believe that anxiety is unique to people, while others believe that there are different types of anxiety. For example, Chinese scholar Bowei Luo defined anxiety as an emotional state in which self-esteem and self-confidence are frustrated, and feelings of failure and guilt, nervousness, and a sense of fear are increased due to the threat of inability to achieve goals or inability to overcome obstacles [4].
In terms of the development of children, maternal anxiety mainly includes a series of nervousness and oversensitivity to children’s diet, growth, development, behavior, appearance, clothing, temperament, peer, and parent-child relationships [5]. Compared with the normal sensitivity and responsiveness to children’s needs, anxious mothers are apprehensive about their children’s physical and mental development, fearing that their children will develop poorly or fall behind. In addition, in order to obtain a sense of parenting security, parents also strictly limit their children’s exploratory behavior. There are even some mothers who are always worried about the growth and development of their children. Over time, they will suffer from loss of appetite, poor sleep, nervousness, and restlessness. In the end, their maternal anxiety will fall into a vicious cycle.
1.2. Problem Behaviors of Preschool Children
Problem behaviors can also be called behavioral problems. In the 1920s, after Wickman researched problem behaviors, this field attracted the attention of domestic and foreign researchers. Up until now, fairly rich research results have been accumulated. However, there was no consensus on the exact definition of problem behaviors, which may be due to the different cultural backgrounds, positions, and research methods of domestic and foreign scholars. Wickman, an American scholar, believed that problem behaviors refer to the conflict between individual behavior and social behavioral norms. Furthermore, some scholars believe that problem behaviors are all problems that make people hard adapt to [6], and are a series of behaviors that are contrary to the behavioral standards, produced by children in the process of socialization [7]. Problem behaviors include emotion, attention deficit, and peer relationship problem behaviors [8].
The definition of problem behaviors by Chinese scholars tends to have the following viewpoints: all abnormal behaviors that hinder individual and social development are problem behaviors, including externalizing and internalizing problem behaviors [9]. In addition, Liping Chi and Ziqiang Xin believed that a series of behaviors, such as non-compliance with social norms, out of control emotional management, and ideology and morality against the norm, are all problem behaviors [10]. No matter which point of view is used to define problem behaviors, it is all discussed around the difficult adaptation behaviors that children and adolescents produce in the process of socialization.
1.3. The Relationship between Maternal Anxiety and Preschool Children’s Problem Behaviors
Based on social learning theory, maternal anxiety can influence children’s anxiety levels through genetics and parenting style [11]. Gete conducted a longitudinal tracking study on 396 mother–child pairs and found that maternal anxiety can be directly transmitted to children, which in turn has an impact on child development [12]. On this basis, Allen reviewed the literature and proposed that genetic susceptibility, environmental stress, parenting sensitivity, parental pressures, parenting experience, and the interaction among these factors can be used as the explanatory factors for the two significant characteristics of trait anxiety, intergenerational inheritance, and bidirectional regulation [13]. Furthermore, Cartwright-Hatton found that environmental factors were more likely to explain the intergenerational inheritance of anxiety than genetic factors [14]. Chinese scholar Lin studied 71 preschool children and found that preschool children’s emotional problems such as anxiety and depression were significantly correlated with their mothers’ emotional problems [15].
Some researchers believe that parental mood directly affects children’s behavioral problems. For instance, Maoz and others found that parents with high mood fluctuation were more likely to overestimate internalized and externalized behavioral problems of preschool or school-age children [16]. If mothers are in a negative mood such as with anxiety before or after childbirth, their children are more likely to have behavior problems during the preschool period [17]. Many children who have been exposed to maternal anxiety for a long time also show some similar symptoms of anxiety [18]. Moreover, living with anxious parents for a long time can also adversely affect children’s mental health and academic performance [19]. Other researchers, however, argue that parents’ moods are not directly related to children’s behavioral problems, but parents will affect children’s behavior problems through intermediate factors such as parenting behaviors and parent–child relationships. For example, parents in a negative mood state tend to show indifference, hyposensitivity, lack of patience, and other behaviors in parent–child interaction and do not show more joy in reuniting with children after separation [20]. Therefore, on the one hand, negative moods can weaken parents’ positive parenting behaviors. Compared with non-anxious mothers, parents in a long-term negative mood are more likely to evaluate their children negatively [21]. They are less involved in interactions with their children, and children are more likely to have negative moods and a decrease in activity [20]. On the other hand, a negative mood can lead to family dysfunction. Studies have found that parents in a negative mood for a long time have less emotional exchange and communication in the family, making the whole family in a depressing or conflicting atmosphere, which in turn affects children’s social behavior and results in more problem behaviors in children.
However, other studies have found that maternal anxiety and depressive symptoms are significantly correlated with externalizing problem behaviors in girls, but are not associated with externalizing problem behaviors in boys [22]. At the same time, some scholars found that there was a negative correlation between maternal anxiety and the number of children’s health problems and temperament inhibition, but it was not associated with children’s internalizing problem behaviors, such as child anxiety [23]. Results of a long-term follow-up study showed that mothers experienced anxiety in their children at 3 months, 6 months, and 12 months, 10 years, and 12.5 years. Mother-reported child internalizing problem behaviors were significantly associated with maternal anxiety, but not for children of externalizing problem behaviors, and were not significantly associated with maternal anxiety or depressive symptoms [1]. As for the relationship between maternal anxiety and children’s externalizing problem behaviors, some scholars have also conducted related research. For example, a study by Zlomke found no correlation between maternal anxiety and children’s externalizing problem behaviors [24]. Although many empirical studies have proved that there is a significant correlation between maternal anxiety and children’s problem behaviors, as shown in the above studies, the correlation between these two is not clear, and some studies even have opposite conclusions that these two are not related. Because of this, Rees conducted a more systematic meta-analysis of the relationship between maternal anxiety and children’s internalizing problem behaviors [3]. However, because the study only included a total of 14 studies from 2002 to 2017, the number of studies was small and the quality was uneven, which severely limited the ability of this meta-analysis to draw clear conclusions. Besides, some of the included studies in this meta-analysis had problems such as lack of data and exclusion criteria, limited geographic coverage, and loss of researchers, which also meant that the generalizability of the meta-analysis results may be limited. Moreover, the focus of this meta-analysis was to distinguish the effects of maternal anxiety symptoms at different periods on children’s emotional problems, in which the age range of the children tested was very broad, which meant that the study could not focus on the effects of maternal anxiety on preschool children’s emotional problems. Finally, the meta-analysis concluded that maternal prenatal anxiety, pregnancy anxiety, and postpartum anxiety all showed a small effect on children’s emotional problems, and did not analyze the moderating effects of other factors in detail. Considering the limitations of many problems in this study to obtain valid conclusions, the true effect size of the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors, and what factors affect the effect size, the reference to this research question was extremely limited.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Literature Search
First, two reviewers respectively conducted a literature search in both Chinese and English. The search time period was set from the establishment of the database to 1 July 2022, and all the relevant literature including the latest literature was retrieved as far as possible. The search process was as follows: First, articles were retrieved from the CNKI Database, the China Science and Technology Journal Database, the Wanfang Database, the China Master’s Theses Full-text Database, and the China Doctoral Dissertations Full-text Database. Second, articles were retrieved from EBSCO, JSTOR, SAGE, Springer, Elsevier, and ProQuest Dissertation and Theses databases. We used the keywords ‘maternal anxiety, parenting anxiety’, ‘children, child’ and ‘problem behavior, externalizing problem behavior, internalizing problems, emotional problems, inattention, conduct problem, peer relationship, aggression, aggressive behavior, disciplinary offense, delinquent behavior’. At the same time, we also searched in Google Scholar. Any disagreements that arose were jointly resolved by a third reviewer for a final consensus.
2.2. Criteria for Literature Inclusion
For the relevant studies searched, we decided whether to include them in the subsequent meta-analysis according to the following criteria: (a) empirical studies with numerical results must be reported, while purely theoretical and review studies are excluded; (b) studies must examine the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors or any other indicators; (c) if the same sample data is used and published by different independent institutes, the literature providing more detailed sample information will be selected for coding; and (d) studies that did not report a full effect size are excluded. Finally, we obtained 34 papers that met the requirements for the meta-analysis. Among them, there were 26 studies in English and 8 studies in Chinese.
The flow chart of document retrieval is shown in Figure 1. A total of 3747 articles were found through a computer search and a manual search. After filtering out repeated articles and reviews, there were 2832 articles. Then, the titles and abstracts were manually searched, 2717 articles were excluded, and the remaining 115 full-text papers were screened. As a result, 81 articles were excluded, of which 46 were inconsistent in research methods and 35 were inconsistent in outcome indicators.
2.3. Coding
All articles were independently screened by two reviewers for the title, abstract, and full text based on predefined criteria, and disagreements were resolved by a third reviewer. The collected articles were coded for the following features: first author, year, region, sample size, children’s age, gender and its proportion, maternal age, maternal education level, follow-up time, variable types, and outcome indicators. In addition, for studies for which data couldn’t be extracted, we obtained data by contacting the authors, see Table 1.
For each independent sample, an effect size was obtained. At the same time, we took into consideration that some studies focused on multiple behavioral variables (internalizing problem behaviors and externalizing problem behaviors), reported the results of different sample groups (male and female), and reported the results of multiple measurements (follow-up study). We presented the multiple independent effect sizes contained in each study’s literature. Therefore, some research documents would contain multiple independent effect sizes. In the end, we had a total of 88 independent effect sizes.
2.4. Meta-Analytic Process
2.4.1. Effect Sizes
The data of this study were analyzed by the comprehensive meta-analysis software CMA2.2 and selected coefficient (r) as effect size. CMA2.2 converted various other effect values reported in the original independent study, such as the t-value, and Chi-square value, into correlation coefficients (r). If only the correlation coefficients of each dimension were reported, the correlation coefficient synthesis method (r-Fisher Z) was used to obtain the correlation coefficient between maternal anxiety and children’s problem behaviors in the total sample of subjects [25] for later analysis.
2.4.2. Model Selection
Meta-analysis generally adopts a fixed effects model and a random effect model. The fixed effects models assume that the true effect sizes of each independent study included in the meta-analysis are the same, and that differences in results between studies are caused by random error. A random effects model assumes that each independent study included in the meta-analysis has its true effect value, and the differences in results between different studies are caused by different true effect values and random errors [26].
Specifically, if the average effect value of the meta-analysis is only for the population included in the meta-analysis, and does not extend to other populations, a fixed effects model should be used. However, when the age of the subjects and the measurement indicators are different in independent studies, a random effects model should be used because these differences will have an impact on the final result [27]. In addition to exploring the main effect of maternal anxiety on problem behaviors, this study also explored possible moderating variables between these two, so a random effects model was more appropriate for this study. In the subsequent meta-analysis, heterogeneity tests were used to validate our model selection.
2.4.3. Publication Bias
Publication bias is considered to have occurred when the published research literature fails to represent the population of the research that has been completed in the field [28]. The result of publication bias is the incompleteness of the research literature in a particular field, which can seriously affect the results of the meta-analysis. Any meta-analysis should be concerned with publication bias [29]. To solve the problem of publication bias, we first obtained unpublished literature as much as possible in the literature search stage. In the subsequent meta-analysis process, we would use funnel plots, Rosenthal’s Fail-safe N, Egger’s test, and other methods to evaluate the publication bias of this meta-analysis.
Table 1Characteristics of the studies included in the study.
Main Author | Year | Region | Sample Size | Age of Children | Gender (Male %) | Maternal Age | Maternal Education | Period | Variable Type | Correlation Coefficient |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ali [29] | 2019 | Americas | 182 | T | <50% | 30~35 | 60~80% | N | E | 0.300 |
Ali [29] | 2019 | Americas | 182 | T | <50% | 30~35 | 60~80% | N | I | 0.350 |
Arikan [30] | 2021 | Asia | 537 | T | 50~65% | 30~35 | <60% | N | I | 0.510 |
Arikan [30] | 2021 | Asia | 537 | T | 50~65% | 30~35 | <60% | N | E | 0.500 |
Antúnez [22] | 2018 | Europe | 311 | T | <50% | Not reported | Not reported | N | P | 0.130 |
Bangirana [31] | 2021 | Africa | 116 | T | <50% | Not reported | <60% | N | P | 0.163 |
Clavarino [32] | 2009 | Australia | 428 | C | 50~65% | <30 | >80% | N | E | 0.220 |
Clavarino [32] | 2009 | Australia | 400 | C | 50~65% | <30 | >80% | N | E | 0.238 |
Clavarino [32] | 2009 | Australia | 365 | C | 50~65% | <30 | >80% | N | E | 0.261 |
Clavarino [32] | 2009 | Australia | 608 | C | 50~65% | 30~35 | >80% | N | E | 0.204 |
Clavarino [32] | 2009 | Australia | 298 | C | 50~65% | 30~35 | >80% | N | E | 0.237 |
Clavarino [32] | 2009 | Australia | 387 | C | 50~65% | 30~35 | >80% | N | E | 0.256 |
Clavarino [32] | 2009 | Australia | 332 | C | 50~65% | 30~35 | >80% | N | E | 0.278 |
Ning [33] | 2021 | Asia | 12657 | Not reported | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | P | 0.112 |
Di Stefano [34] | 2014 | Americas | 993 | Not reported | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | E | 0.120 |
Di Stefano [34] | 2014 | Americas | 993 | Not reported | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | I | 0.270 |
Frigerio [35] | 2021 | Europe | 90 | T | 50~65% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | I | 0.470 |
Frigerio [35] | 2021 | Europe | 90 | T | 50~65% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | I | 0.520 |
Frigerio [35] | 2021 | Europe | 90 | T | 50~65% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | I | 0.380 |
Frigerio [35] | 2021 | Europe | 90 | T | 50~65% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | I | 0.460 |
Frigerio [35] | 2021 | Europe | 90 | T | 50~65% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | E | 0.250 |
Frigerio [35] | 2021 | Europe | 90 | T | 50~65% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | E | 0.310 |
Frigerio [35] | 2021 | Europe | 90 | T | 50~65% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | E | 0.330 |
Frigerio [35] | 2021 | Europe | 90 | T | 50~65% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | E | 0.400 |
Frigerio [36] | 2022 | Europe | 94 | T | 50~65% | 30~35 | >80% | E | I | 0.275 |
Frigerio [36] | 2022 | Europe | 88 | T | 50~65% | >35 | >80% | E | I | 0.115 |
Frigerio [36] | 2022 | Europe | 59 | C | 50~65% | >35 | >80% | E | I | 0.480 |
Frigerio [36] | 2022 | Europe | 94 | T | 50~65% | 30~35 | >80% | E | E | 0.285 |
Frigerio [36] | 2022 | Europe | 88 | T | 50~65% | >35 | >80% | E | E | 0.130 |
Frigerio [36] | 2022 | Europe | 59 | C | 50~65% | >35 | >80% | E | E | 0.365 |
Gagne [37] | 2019 | Americas | 198 | T | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | E | 0.160 |
Gagne [37] | 2019 | Americas | 198 | T | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | E | 0.230 |
Gagne [37] | 2019 | Americas | 1994 | C | Not reported | 30~35 | >80% | N | I | 0.220 |
Gagne [37] | 2019 | Americas | 1994 | C | Not reported | 30~35 | >80% | N | E | 0.230 |
Zilin [15] | 2019 | Asia | 71 | C | >65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | P | 0.544 |
Gjerde [38] | 2020 | Europe | 40457 | T | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | I | 0.150 |
Gjerde [38] | 2020 | Europe | 40457 | T | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | I | 0.140 |
Gjerde [38] | 2020 | Europe | 40457 | T | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | I | 0.170 |
Gjerde [38] | 2020 | Europe | 40457 | T | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | E | 0.110 |
Gjerde [38] | 2020 | Europe | 40457 | T | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | E | 0.110 |
Gjerde [38] | 2020 | Europe | 40457 | T | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | E | 0.150 |
HanetzGamliel [39] | 2021 | Asia | 141 | C | <50% | >35 | <60% | E | E | 0.260 |
HanetzGamliel [39] | 2021 | Asia | 141 | C | <50% | >35 | <60% | E | I | 0.370 |
Hautmann [40] | 2014 | Europe | 106 | C | >65% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | E | 0.340 |
Hautmann [40] | 2014 | Europe | 106 | C | >65% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | E | 0.360 |
Hautmann [40] | 2014 | Europe | 106 | C | >65% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | E | 0.400 |
Loomans [41] | 2011 | Europe | 3446 | C | 50~65% | 30~35 | <60% | N | P | 0.280 |
Marceau [42] | 2015 | Americas | 308 | Not reported | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | E | 0.020 |
Marceau [42] | 2015 | Americas | 308 | Not reported | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | E | 0.080 |
Marceau [42] | 2015 | Americas | 308 | Not reported | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | I | -0.003 |
Marceau [42] | 2015 | Americas | 308 | Not reported | 50~65% | Not reported | Not reported | N | I | 0.080 |
McFarlane [43] | 2014 | Americas | 300 | C | Not reported | 30~35 | Not reported | N | I | 0.611 |
McFarlane [43] | 2014 | Americas | 300 | C | Not reported | 30~35 | Not reported | N | E | 0.494 |
McFarlane [43] | 2014 | Americas | 300 | C | Not reported | 30~35 | Not reported | N | P | 0.666 |
Mount [23] | 2010 | Americas | 83 | T | >65% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | I | 0.110 |
Porter [44] | 2019 | Australia | 191 | T | Not reported | <30 | 60~80% | N | P | 0.300 |
Porter [44] | 2019 | Australia | 191 | T | Not reported | <30 | 60~80% | N | P | 0.180 |
Porter [44] | 2019 | Australia | 191 | T | Not reported | 30~35 | 60~80% | N | P | 0.280 |
Porter [44] | 2019 | Australia | 191 | T | Not reported | 30~35 | 60~80% | N | P | 0.350 |
Ying [2] | 2019 | Asia | 335 | C | 50~65% | 30~35 | <60% | N | E | 0.320 |
Ying [2] | 2019 | Asia | 335 | C | 50~65% | 30~35 | <60% | N | E | 0.140 |
Huihui [45] | 2016 | Asia | 3653 | Not reported | 50~65% | Not reported | <60% | N | P | 0.078 |
Huihui [45] | 2016 | Asia | 3375 | Not reported | 50~65% | Not reported | <60% | N | P | 0.091 |
Teramoto [46] | 2005 | Asia | 670 | T | Not reported | Not reported | Not reported | N | P | 0.400 |
Teramoto [46] | 2005 | Asia | 670 | T | Not reported | Not reported | Not reported | N | E | 0.400 |
Teramoto [46] | 2005 | Asia | 670 | T | Not reported | Not reported | Not reported | N | I | 0.330 |
Thorsteinsdottir [47] | 2018 | Europe | 623 | C | <50% | >35 | 60~80% | N | P | 0.562 |
Tichelman [48] | 2021 | Europe | 1528 | Not reported | Not reported | 30~35 | <60% | N | E | 0.247 |
Tichelman [48] | 2021 | Europe | 1528 | Not reported | Not reported | 30~35 | <60% | N | I | 0.214 |
wai [49] | 2017 | Asia | 61 | T | <50% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | I | 0.670 |
wai [49] | 2017 | Asia | 61 | T | <50% | 30~35 | Not reported | N | E | 0.520 |
Yueyuan [50] | 2020 | Asia | 506 | C | 50~65% | Not reported | <60% | N | I | 0.208 |
Wiggins [51] | 2019 | Americas | 672 | Not reported | >65% | 30~35 | >80% | N | P | 0.127 |
Yoldaş [52] | 2019 | Asia | 40 | T | <50% | 30~35 | <60% | N | I | 0.430 |
Yoldaş [52] | 2019 | Asia | 40 | T | <50% | 30~35 | <60% | N | E | 0.512 |
Yoldaş [52] | 2019 | Asia | 40 | T | <50% | 30~35 | <60% | N | P | 0.488 |
Yoldaş [52] | 2019 | Asia | 40 | T | <50% | 30~35 | 60~80% | N | I | 0.412 |
Yoldaş [52] | 2019 | Asia | 40 | T | <50% | 30~35 | 60~80% | N | I | 0.403 |
Yurdusen [53] | 2013 | Asia | 204 | C | 50~65% | 30~35 | 60~80% | N | I | 0.420 |
Yurdusen [53] | 2013 | Asia | 204 | C | 50~65% | 30~35 | 60~80% | N | E | 0.330 |
Yurdusen [53] | 2013 | Asia | 204 | C | 50~65% | 30~35 | 60~80% | N | P | 0.430 |
Yan [54] | 2011 | Asia | 308 | Not reported | <50% | Not reported | 60~80% | N | I | 0.418 |
Zhang [55] | 2022 | Americas | 831 | T | 50~65% | <30 | Not reported | N | P | 0.280 |
Hao [56] | 2021 | Asia | 3097 | Not reported | 50~65% | Not reported | <60% | E | P | 0.278 |
Yang [57] | 2014 | Asia | 355 | Not reported | 50~65% | Not reported | 60~80% | N | I | 0.351 |
Zlomke [24] | 2020 | Americas | 64 | C | >65% | 30~35 | 60~80% | N | P | 0.045 |
Zlomke [24] | 2020 | Americas | 64 | C | >65% | 30~35 | 60~80% | N | P | 0.153 |
Zlomke [24] | 2020 | Americas | 64 | C | >65% | 30~35 | 60~80% | N | P | 0.097 |
Note: T indicates that the study samples are infants and toddlers (aged 0–3). C indicates that the study samples are children between the ages of 3–6, and <50% indicates that the proportion of male children in the study sample is less than 50%. 50~65% indicates that the proportion of male children in the study sample is between 50% and 65%. >65% indicates that the proportion of male children in the study is more than 65%. <60% indicates that less than 60 percent of the mothers in the study sample had higher education. 60%~80% indicates that the proportion of mothers with higher education in the sample is between 60% and 80%. >80% more than 80% of the mothers in the study sample had higher education. N indicates that the study was conducted during a non-epidemic period. E indicates that the study was carried out during the epidemic period. I indicates that the result variable is internalizing problem behavior. E indicates that the result variable is externalizing problem behavior. P indicates that the result variable is an overall problem behavior.
3. Results
3.1. Heterogeneity Test
The heterogeneity test was carried out for the problem behaviors of preschool children, and the results were shown in Table 2. According to the results in Table 2, the Q-test for problem behaviors was significant, indicating that the effect sizes of the study in the meta-analysis were heterogeneous. In addition, according to the interpretation of I2 by Borenstein [26], the I2 of the meta-analysis of moral behaviors was 1452.493, which indicated that 97.01% observed variation in the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behavior was caused by real differences in the relationship. σ2 represented the variance of the true effect size. σ2 showed that the true effect size had some variation. The results of the heterogeneity test showed that the random effects model we selected for the meta-analysis was accurate.
3.2. Publication Bias Test
First, the publication bias of this meta-analysis was examined by using a funnel plot, shown in Figure 2 for the two types of moral behavior. From the funnel plot, the research literature involving preschool children’s problem behavior was not evenly distributed on both sides of the total effect size, and most studies were located on the right side of the total effect size. This distribution suggested that there may be publication bias in the studies on preschool children’s problem behaviors. Since the funnel plot can preliminarily examine the publication bias from a subjective point of view, Rosenthal’s Failsafe N and Egger’s tests were followed to more accurately examine the publication bias, and the results are shown in Table 3.
According to the results of Egger’s test, there was a certain publication bias in the studies involving preschool children’s problem behaviors. According to Rosenthal’s N value, it is necessary to include 5 K + 10 (K is the value of effect size) in the research literature involving these behaviors to make the total effect size insignificant, indicating that there was no serious publication bias in this study.
Among the three publication bias tests mentioned above, one result (Rosenthal’s N) showed that there was no publication bias in the meta-analysis of preschool children’s problem behaviors, and the results were not recognized by all three tests. However, according to Borenstein, the purpose of the publication bias test should be to determine which of the following types of meta-analysis results belong to: 1, the effect of deviation is negligible; 2, the effect of bias cannot be ignored, but the results are still valid; and 3, the results may be problematic [26]. Therefore, further analysis was needed. We used the Trim and Fill method proposed by Duval and Tweedie to test the impact of publication bias on the results of the meta-analysis [58]. The results showed that the total effect of the random effects model on preschool children’s problem behaviors was still significant after cutting and sticking the literature. In conclusion, although there may be slight publication bias in the meta-analysis of this study, the main conclusions of the meta-analysis were still valid.
3.3. Main Effect Analysis
The relationship between mother’s anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors was examined as a whole, and the results were shown in Table 4. The results showed that there were 88 independent effect sizes of maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors, with a total of 295,032 subjects. The overall correlation coefficient between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors was 0.273, as shown in Figure 3.
3.4. Moderation Effect Test
In Figure 3, it can be seen that the effect size of each study is distributed on the left and right sides of the total effect size (diamond in the figure), and that there is great variation among the effect sizes of each study. To analyze the variation, we respectively tested the regions (America, Asia, Europe, Australia, and Africa), age (children and infants) and gender, mother’s age, education level, and whether the research was in the epidemic period for the moderating effect of the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors. The results are shown in Table 5. From the results of the moderating effect analysis, the region of the subjects can affect the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors (Qb = 15.551, p = 0.004). Children’s age (Qb = 4.957, p = 0.026), the gender of children (Qb = 12.522, p = 0.002), maternal age (Qb = 11.713, p = 0.003), and mother’s education level (Qb = 10.124, p = 0.006) can also influence the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors.
4. Discussion
This study used a meta-analysis to integrate previously related research results in the field of the development of children’s problem behaviors to explore the impact of maternal anxiety on children’s problem behaviors. Specifically, we integrated previous studies on the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors and explored the factors influencing this relationship. The results of the meta-analysis supported the conclusion that maternal anxiety increased children’s problem behaviors [45]. Moreover, the results of the moderating effect analysis showed that the region of subjects, the age and gender of children, the mother’s age, and education level all affected the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors.
4.1. Maternal Anxiety and Preschool Children’s Problem Behaviors
In this study, a random effects model was used to conduct a meta-analysis of empirical studies on the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors. The results showed that there was a significant positive correlation between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors with an effect size of 0.273 (k = 88, N = 295,032), which was consistent with the meta-analysis of Rees [3]. Therefore, the main effect analysis results of this study proved that maternal anxiety had a significant impact on preschool children’s problem behaviors.
First of all, maternal anxiety was significantly correlated with preschool children’s problem behaviors, that is, the more preschool children were exposed to maternal anxiety, the more problem behaviors children would exhibit. Although maternal anxiety has now been widely studied as a risk factor for the development of anxiety in children, the possible role of the transmission process and genes in the association between parental and child anxiety prevalence showed that 50% of anxiety tendencies were thought to be inherited [59]. However, anxious mothers caring for their children also influenced the development of children’s anxiety [60,61], which explained why only 30% of anxiety-prone children developed anxiety disorders [61].
Second, maternal anxiety can significantly predict preschool children’s problem behaviors, that is, the more children are exposed to maternal anxiety, the more externalizing problem behaviors children exhibit. Social learning theory holds that children’s problem behaviors are learned through parenting behaviors [62,63]. And when mothers are extremely anxious, they are more likely to adopt inappropriate parenting behaviors or have a high level of control, beating and scolding. Then this process will be learned by the child, making the child’s level of problem behavior further developed. As result, preschool children unconsciously think that this kind of violence is acceptable, and may further imitate these behaviors [64], leading to externalizing behaviors such as aggressive behaviors, violent behaviors, and so on.
Finally, the more anxiety children perceive from their mothers, the more problem behaviors children will exhibit. In a growing environment, when children are frequently exposed to maternal anxiety, they may perceive that it is difficult to cope effectively with the situation, and this process will be accompanied by a strong negative emotional response. This leads to the individual adopting avoidance or destructive negative coping mechanisms and will finally lead to the emergence of problem behaviors. The cognition background theory holds that the subjective meanings of children from their mothers’ emotions and attitudes have a significant impact on their social adaptation. At the same time, some studies have proved that mothers’ emotional problems such as anxiety have a greater impact on children’s problem behaviors than parents’ attitudes [53]. Specifically, in the mothers’ anxiety environment, on the one hand, children may normalize negative emotions, and imitate and learn negative emotional expressions from their mothers, which increases the possibility of preschool children’s problem behaviors. On the other hand, when children perceive the threat from their mothers’ anxiety and inappropriate parenting behaviors, they will perceive that it is difficult for them to cope with such a negative environment, leading to maladaptive problem behaviors in children [65]. Besides, long-term exposure to negative emotions also increases the likelihood that children will develop problem behaviors.
4.2. Influencing Factors
4.2.1. Cultural Background
The moderating effect analysis showed that the region of the subjects could influence the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors. First, regarding the impact of the subject’s region on the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors, the results showed that in the research on maternal anxiety in Asia, the average correlation coefficient between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors is 0.356; in the studies of America, the average correlation coefficient was 0.239; in European studies, the average correlation coefficient was 0.230; in the studies of Australia, the average correlation coefficient was 0.247; and in studies of Africa, the average correlation was 0.163. The significant moderating effect of the region of the subjects indicated that maternal anxiety was more strongly associated with preschool children’s problem behaviors in Asia than that in America, Europe, Australia, and Africa. The possible reason for this moderating effect is that the cultural background of preschool education has a certain influence on it. Transmission between different regions or different types of cultures will have an impact on children’s development and education. Therefore, it is an important basis for the operation of children’s families, childcare institutions, and communities, and the study of culture should be emphasized [66]. Second, most EU member states consider preschool education as an important part of lifelong learning strategies, and vigorously support the development of preschool education from the perspective of national policies, which has greatly reduced the pressure on mothers to raise children [67]. Finally, a large number of family support policies in European countries and America have fully alleviated the contradiction between family life and occupation, and have greatly reduced the pressure on professional women to take care of their children, thereby improving the negative emotions of mothers to prevent them from affecting the development of children [68].
The results of the moderating effect of the tested regions showed that it was more meaningful to research the internal mechanism and intervention plan of maternal anxiety affecting preschool children’s problem behaviors in Asia, which also pointed out the direction for follow-up research. The reasons are as follows: first, in terms of policy and social support, Asia does not have a relatively complete system like Europe and America. Therefore, it may be more effective to intervene in the path of maternal anxiety affecting children’s problem behaviors through other methods. Because compared with large-scale policies or the establishment of social public service systems, the promotion and implementation of small-scale intervention programs will be greatly improved. Second, children in Asia are significantly more affected by maternal anxiety than those in Europe and America, which to a certain extent affirms the theoretical and practical significance of this study or related research. Also, it can promote more effective advice on relevant educational institutions or social organizations in related fields to improve the mental health of mothers and preschool children around the world.
4.2.2. Age and Gender of Children
The moderating effect analysis showed that children’s age could affect the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors, and the correlation coefficient between maternal anxiety and problem behaviors of children aged 3–6 was 0.333, while the correlation coefficient of children aged 0–3 was 0.270. The significant moderating effect of children’s age indicated that compared with children aged 0–3, the relationship between maternal anxiety and problem behaviors of children aged 3–6 years was stronger. The possible reason for this moderating effect is that children enter kindergarten when they are 3 to 6 years old and they begin to enter the socialization stage and contact individuals other than their mothers. Although the social influence such as classmates and friends is very limited, children’s coping styles are not perfect [69], and children aged 3–6 are still more exposed to negative emotional influences than infants and toddlers. At the same time, because mothers get more time to work when their children enter kindergarten, children aged 3 to 6 are at higher risk of exposure to negative emotions from their mothers and other sources than infants and toddlers [70]. Therefore, children aged 3–6 are more likely to be affected by maternal anxiety and exhibit more problem behaviors than children aged 0–3.
At the same time, foreign longitudinal studies have shown that the cognitive ability of preschool children to trigger their own anxiety will continue to improve with age, so the level of anxiety in various dimensions will gradually decline with the increase of children’s age [71]. However, the reason for the inconsistency of the results of this meta-analysis may be due to the following reasons: on the one hand, the problems encountered by preschool children will gradually become more frequent and complex, and the children’s expressive ability will continue to strengthen with age, so parents can more directly and accurately receive information on children’s anxiety and assess their anxiety mood. This is two-sided. When children’s cognition of dangerous stimuli deepens with age and cognitive ability, it will also lead to an increase in problem behaviors [71]. On the other hand, 3 to 6-year-old children will face the kindergarten stage, so their mothers will have stricter requirements for them. However, such strict and more diverse requirements will easily lead to children’s resistance psychology, which will easily lead to various problem behaviors [72].
Moreover, the moderating effect analysis showed that the child’s gender also affected the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors. And with the increase in the proportion of male subjects, the effect size between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors gradually decreased. This result suggested that the link between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors is stronger in girls than in boys. The possible reason for this moderating effect is: first, due to completely different physiological structures, boys and girls will have different psychological responses and attribution methods to the same problem [73]. Comparatively speaking, girls are more delicate in their minds and are more sensitive to dangerous situations. And they may even concretize and exaggerate dangerous stimuli, and are prone to adopt a more negative attribution method, which leads to various problem behaviors [73]. Second, based on traditional culture and social consensus, girls’ physical strength and athletic ability are relatively weak, so they are more likely to suffer physical injuries, and a more restrained personality is more likely to lead to problem behaviors such as anxiety [74].
4.2.3. Mother’s Age and Education Level
Results showed that maternal age could affect the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors, and with the increase of maternal age, the effect size between these two gradually increased. The significant moderating effect of maternal age indicated that the relationship between anxiety states and preschool children’s problem behaviors was stronger in mothers over 30 years old than in mothers under 30 years old. The possible reason for this moderating effect is that older mothers have greater parenting pressure, and are more easily affected by their emotions when dealing with children’s various problems, making young children more prone to behavioral problems [75]. Since mothers of children under 30 are mostly new mothers, they will pay more attention to making rules for their children to avoid mistakes, will be more sensitive to children’s negative performance, and will more focus on the adverse effects of their own negative emotions on children [75].
Besides, moderating effect analysis showed that the maternal education level could also affect the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors, and as the proportion of mothers with higher education increased, the effect size between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors decreased gradually, suggesting that mothers without higher education were more strongly associated with preschool children’s problem behaviors than mothers with higher education. The possible reason is that the higher the mother’s education level, the lower the incidence of children’s behavioral problems. This may be because when mothers’ emotional problems have adverse effects on children, mothers with lower education levels cannot adopt more scientific and reasonable parenting methods to guide children to face them correctly [56].
Second, mothers with higher education levels will be more objective and accurate in their cognitive assessment of dangerous stimuli. Individuals with a higher education level will be more truly aware of dangerous stimuli, more rationally choose defense mechanisms, and will be more likely to have a positive self-evaluation of themselves [76]. And they will be more appropriate to deal with psychological stress within the individual. Moreover, they will pay more attention to and comprehensively deal with negative emotions. For example, they will learn more about psychological theories to help themselves relieve anxiety, and they will also take more diverse methods such as listening to music and doing sports to overcome anxiety, thereby reducing the impact of their anxiety on children’s development [77]. They also will pay more attention to the physical and mental health of preschool children, and face life with a more optimistic attitude. On the contrary, mothers with lower education may not be sensitive to the perception of negative emotions in preschool children, and cannot accurately and properly perceive the negative emotions of preschool children, thus affecting the development of children [78].
4.3. Limitations and Future Research
Limitations of the study are as follows. (1) The possible influence of the family’s socioeconomic status on the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors was not considered. In the field of research on maternal anxiety, family socioeconomic status is often considered an important factor. Since almost none of the original studies finally included in the meta-analysis reported the effect size of the family’s socioeconomic status, its role could not be analyzed. (2) The small sample size and uneven distribution in the moderating effect analysis can affect the results of the analysis. (3) Some of the original studies included in this meta-analysis did not report eigenvalues such as maternal age.
As an important research topic in child development psychology, it is of great theoretical and practical significance to explore the influencing factors of maternal anxiety on preschool children’s problem behaviors. Future research could focus on the following. (1) When discussing the role of rational factors in children’s problem behaviors, the role of irrational factors should also be considered, and the impact of maternal anxiety on it should be comprehensively explored from multiple perspectives. (2) Related research should also be carried out on the relationship between the measurement tools of children’s problem behaviors. (3) With the increase of age, the cognitive ability of the individual gradually matures. At different ages, the relationship between maternal anxiety and children’s problem behaviors may be different, and the strength of maternal factors and children’s cognitive factors may be different. So future research should focus on this aspect.
5. Conclusions
This meta-analysis found that there was a relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors. The region of the subjects, children’s age and gender, mother’s age, and education level all affected the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors.
J.H. and Z.S. led the research design, data analysis, and drafted this manuscript. T.Q. guided the research design and revised the manuscript substantially. J.Y. and X.Z. revised the section of the analysis and discussion and corrected the entire manuscript. D.L. analyzed and verified the data in this manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Figure 3. The forest map of the effect of problem behavior [2,15,22,23,24,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57].
Effect heterogeneity test results.
Outcome | Q | df | p | I 2 | σ 2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Problem Behavior | 1452.493 | 87 | <0.001 | 94.010 | 0.005 |
Results of publication bias test.
Outcome | Rosenthal’s N | Egger’s Intercept | SE | LL | UL | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Problem Behavior | 6737 | 2.94487 | 0.41627 | 2.11736 | 3.77238 | <0.001 |
Note: LL, UL represents the lower limit and upper limit of 95% confidence interval of Egger’s intercept.
Analysis results of random effect model.
Outcome | N | k | r | LL | UL | z | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Problem Behavior | 295,032 | 88 | 0.273 | 0.255 | 0.292 | 27.277 | <0.001 |
Note: N represents the sample size, k represents the number of studies, LL and UL represent the lower and upper limits of the 95% confidence interval of r.
Results of adjustment effect analysis.
Outcome Variable | Regulated Variable | Category | k | r | LL | UL | Qb | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Problem Behavior | Region | Americas | 21 | 0.239 | 0.167 | 0.309 | 15.551 | 0.004 |
Asia | 27 | 0.356 | 0.297 | 0.411 | ||||
Europe | 28 | 0.230 | 0.205 | 0.254 | ||||
Australia | 11 | 0.247 | 0.205 | 0.254 | ||||
Africa | 1 | 0.163 | -0.020 | 0.335 | ||||
Age of Children | Child | 31 | 0.333 | 0.282 | 0.382 | 4.957 | 0.026 | |
Toddler | 42 | 0.270 | 0.246 | 0.293 | ||||
Gender (Male%) | <50% | 15 | 0.398 | 0.303 | 0.484 | 12.522 | 0.002 | |
50~65% | 50 | 0.220 | 0.201 | 0.239 | ||||
>65% | 9 | 0.250 | 0.135 | 0.358 | ||||
Maternal Age | >35 | 7 | 0.340 | 0.173 | 0.488 | 11.713 | 0.003 | |
30~35 | 48 | 0.348 | 0.308 | 0.387 | ||||
<30 | 6 | 0.253 | 0.216 | 0.291 | ||||
Maternal Education | <60% | 17 | 0.285 | 0.219 | 0.348 | 10.124 | 0.006 | |
60~80% | 17 | 0.334 | 0.265 | 0.400 | ||||
>80% | 16 | 0.225 | 0.199 | 0.250 | ||||
Period | Non-epidemic period | 79 | 0.272 | 0.253 | 0.291 | 0.081 | 0.776 | |
Epidemic period | 9 | 0.280 | 0.229 | 0.330 |
References
1. Missler, M.A.; van Straten, A.; Denissen, J.J.A.; Donker, T.; de Weerth, C.; Beijers, R. The first 12.5 years of parenthood: A latent trait-state occasion model of the longitudinal association between maternal distress and child internalizing and externalizing problems. Dev. Psychol.; 2021; 57, pp. 1124-1135. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0001203] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34435827]
2. Ying, S.; Fei, G.; Zhiyan, C. Relationship between Parental Co-parenting and Children’s Be-havior Problems: A Multiple Mediation Model of Maternal Anxiety and Maternal Psycho-logical Control. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol.; 2019; 27, pp. 1210-1219.
3. Rees, S.; Channon, S.; Waters, C.S. The impact of maternal prenatal and postnatal anxiety on children’s emotional problems: A systematic review. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry; 2018; 28, pp. 257-280. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1173-5]
4. Bowei, L. A Preliminary Study on the Causes of Female High Anxiety Tendency. Psychol. Sci.; 1997; 20, pp. 378-379.
5. Sang, B. Parental consciousness, parenting anxiety and innate temperament-a new hotspot in the study of the relationship between the only child and the parent-child. Contemp. Youth Stud.; 1998; 2, pp. 6-10.
6. Achenbach, T.M. Manual for the Youth Self-Report and 1991 Profile; University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry: Burlington, VT, USA, 1991; 1. ISBN 9780938565093
7. Onyskiw, J.E. Domestic Violence and Children’s Adjustment: A review of research. J. Emot. Abus.; 2003; 3, pp. 11-45. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J135v03n01_02]
8. Durant, R.; Knight, J.; Goodman, E. Factors associated with aggressive and delinquent behaviors among patients attending an adolescent medicine clinic. J. Adolesc. Health; 1997; 21, pp. 303-308. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1054-139X(97)00159-6]
9. Chongde, L.; Zhiliang, Y.; Xiting, H. The Comprehensive Dictionary of Psychology; Shanghai Education Press: Shanghai, China, 2003; ISBN 9787532093182
10. Chi, L.; Xin, Z. Family Function and its related factors. New Perspect. Psychol.; 2001; 3, pp. 55–60+64. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3969/j.issn.1003-5184.2001.03.012]
11. Puka, K.; Widjaja, E.; Smith, M.L. The influence of patient, caregiver, and family factors on symptoms of anxiety and depression in children and adolescents with intractable epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav.; 2017; 67, pp. 45-50. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.12.011]
12. Gete, S.; Calderon-Margalit, R.; Grotto, I.; Ornoy, A. A comparison of the effects of maternal anxiety and depression on child development. Eur. J. Public Health; 2019; 29, 275. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckz185.275]
13. Allen, J.L.; Sandberg, S.; Chhoa, C.Y.; Fearn, T.; Rapee, R.M. Parent-dependent stressors and the onset of anxiety disorders in children: Links with parental psychopathology. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry; 2017; 27, pp. 221-231. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00787-017-1038-3] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28791523]
14. Cartwright-Hatton, S.; Ewing, D.; Dash, S.; Hughes, Z.; Thompson, E.J.; Hazell, C.M.; Field, A.P.; Startup, H. Preventing family transmission of anxiety: Feasibility RCT of a brief intervention for parents. Br. J. Clin. Psychol.; 2018; 57, pp. 351-366. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12177] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29575043]
15. Lin, G.Z.; Jing, L.; Fang, H.X.; Li, Q.; Jing, S.U.; Shan, Z.K. Analysis of the correlation be-tween emotional and behavior problems in preschool children with autism and maternal emotional problems. Chin. J. Child Health Care; 2019; 27, pp. 473-476. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.11852/zgetbjzz2018-1592]
16. Maoz, H.; Goldstein, T.; Goldstein, B.I.; Axelson, D.A.; Fan, J.; Hickey, M.B.; Monk, K.; Sakolsky, D.; Diler, R.S.; Brent, D. et al. The Effects of Parental Mood on Reports of Their Children’s Psychopathology. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry; 2014; 53, pp. 1111-1122.e5. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.07.005]
17. Santos, I.S.; Matijasevich, A.; Barros, A.J.; Barros, F.C. Antenatal and postnatal maternal mood symptoms and psychiatric disorders in pre-school children from the 2004 Pelotas Birth Cohort. J. Affect. Disord.; 2014; 164, pp. 112-117. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.04.033]
18. Elgar, F.J.; McGrath, P.; Waschbusch, D.; Stewart, S.H.; Curtis, L.J. Mutual influences on maternal depression and child adjustment problems. Clin. Psychol. Rev.; 2004; 24, pp. 441-459. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2004.02.002]
19. Downey, G.; Coyne, J.C. Children of depressed parents: An integrative review. Psychol. Bull.; 1990; 108, pp. 50-76. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.108.1.50]
20. Edhborg, M.; Lundh, W.; Seimyr, L.; Widström, A. The parent-child relationship in the context of maternal depressive mood. Arch. Women’s Ment. Health; 2003; 6, pp. 211-216. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00737-003-0020-x]
21. Conrad, M.; Hammen, C. Role of maternal depression in perceptions of child maladjustment. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.; 1989; 57, pp. 663-667. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.57.5.663]
22. Antúnez, Z.; De La Osa, N.; Granero, R.; Ezpeleta, L. Reciprocity Between Parental Psychopathology and Oppositional Symptoms From Preschool to Middle Childhood. J. Clin. Psychol.; 2017; 74, pp. 489-504. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22504]
23. Mount, K.S.; Crockenberg, S.C.; Jó, P.S.B.; Wagar, J.-L. Maternal and child correlates of anxiety in 2½-year-old children. Infant Behav. Dev.; 2010; 33, pp. 567-578. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.07.008] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20723999]
24. Zlomke, K.; Rossetti, K.; Murphy, J.; Mallicoat, K.; Swingle, H. Feeding Problems and Maternal Anxiety in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Matern. Child Health J.; 2020; 24, pp. 1278-1287. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10995-020-02966-8] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32596794]
25. Houcan, Z.; Jianping, X. Modern Psychology and Educational Statistics; Beijing Normal University Press: Beijing, China, 2004; ISBN 9787303000395
26. Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.; Higgins, J.; Rothstein, H. Introduction to Mate-Analysis; Wiley: West Sussex, UK, 2009; ISBN 9780470057247
27. Wu, P.; Liu, H. Association between Moral Reasoning and Moral Behavior: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Acta Psychol. Sin.; 2014; 46, 01192. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.01192]
28. Rothstein, H.R.; Sutton, A.J.; Borenstein, M. Publication Bias in Meta-Analysis: Prevention, Assessment and Adjustments; Rothstein, H.R.; Sutton, A.J.; Borenstein, M. Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2006; ISBN 978-0-470-87014-3
29. Ali, E.; Letourneau, N.; Benzies, K.; Ntanda, H.; Dewey, D.; Campbell, T.; Giesbrecht, G. Maternal Prenatal Anxiety and Children’s Externalizing and Internalizing Behavioral Problems: The Moderating Roles of Maternal-Child Attachment Security and Child Sex. Can. J. Nurs. Res.; 2019; 52, pp. 88-99. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0844562119894184] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31856590]
30. Arikan, G.; Kumru, A. Patterns of Associations between Maternal Symptoms and Child Problem Behaviors: The Mediating Role of Mentalization, Negative Intentionality, and Unsupportive Emotion Socialization. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev.; 2020; 52, pp. 640-653. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10578-020-01046-w]
31. Bangirana, P.; Birabwa, A.; Nyakato, M.; Nakitende, A.J.; Kroupina, M.; Ssenkusu, J.M.; Nakasujja, N.; Musisi, S.; John, C.C.; Idro, R. Use of the creating opportunities for parent empowerment programme to decrease mental health problems in Ugandan children surviving severe malaria: A randomized controlled trial. Malar. J.; 2021; 20, 267. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12936-021-03795-y]
32. Clavarino, A.M.; Mamun, A.; O’Callaghan, M.; Aird, R.; Bor, W.; O’Callaghan, F.; Williams, G.; Marrington, S.; Najman, J.; Alati, R. Maternal Anxiety and Attention Problems in Children at 5 and 14 Years. J. Atten. Disord.; 2009; 13, pp. 658-667. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1087054709347203]
33. Ning, D.; Hui, X.; Feihua, S.; Chuanfu, S.; Zhong, X.; Liping, B.; Juan, C. Prevalence and influencing factors of autism spectrum disorders in preschool children in Wuhu. South China J. Prev. Med.; 2021; 47, pp. 1384-1387. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.12183/j.scjpm.2021.1384]
34. Di Stefano, G.; Cyr, F. Child Adjustment Following Parental Separation: The Role of Maternal Well-Being, Parenting Quality, and Household Income. J. Child Custody; 2014; 11, pp. 5-24. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15379418.2014.892802]
35. Frigerio, A.; Nazzari, S. Antenatal maternal anxiety, maternal sensitivity and toddlers’ behavioral problems: An investigation of possible pathways. Early Hum. Dev.; 2021; 157, 105364. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2021.105364]
36. Frigerio, A.; Nettuno, F.; Nazzari, S. Maternal mood moderates the trajectory of emotional and behavioural problems from pre- to during the COVID-19 lockdown in preschool children. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry; 2022; pp. 1-11. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00787-021-01925-0] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35001205]
37. Gagne, J.R.; Chang, C.-N.; Fang, H.; Spann, C.; Kwok, O.-M. A multimethod study of inhibitory control and behavioural problems in preschoolers. Infant Child Dev.; 2018; 28, e2115. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1002/icd.2115]
38. Gjerde, L.C.; Eilertsen, E.M.; Eley, T.C.; McAdams, T.A.; Reichborn-Kjennerud, T.; Røysamb, E.; Ystrom, E. Maternal Perinatal and Concurrent Anxiety and Mental Health Problems in Early Childhood: A Sibling-Comparison Study. Child Dev.; 2018; 91, pp. 456-470. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13192] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30536534]
39. Hanetz-Gamliel, K.; Levy, S.; Dollberg, D.G. Mediation of Mothers’ Anxiety and Parenting in Children’s Behavior Problems during COVID-19. J. Child Fam. Stud.; 2021; 30, pp. 2732-2743. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10826-021-02115-x] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34584394]
40. Hautmann, C.; Eichelberger, I.; Hanisch, C.; Plück, J.; Walter, D.; Döpfner, M. Association between parental emotional symptoms and child antisocial behaviour: What is specific and is it mediated by parenting?. Int. J. Behav. Dev.; 2014; 39, pp. 43-52. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025414535120]
41. Loomans, E.; van der Stelt, O.; van Eijsden, M.; Gemke, R.; Vrijkotte, T.; Bergh, B.V.D. Antenatal maternal anxiety is associated with problem behaviour at age five. Early Hum. Dev.; 2011; 87, pp. 565-570. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.04.014]
42. Marceau, K.; Laurent, H.K.; Neiderhiser, J.M.; Reiss, D.; Shaw, D.S.; Natsuaki, M.N.; Fisher, P.A.; Leve, L.D. Combined Influences of Genes, Prenatal Environment, Cortisol, and Parenting on the Development of Children’s Internalizing Versus Externalizing Problems. Behav. Genet.; 2014; 45, pp. 268-282. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10519-014-9689-z]
43. McFarlane, J.; Symes, L.; Binder, B.K.; Maddoux, J.; Paulson, R. Maternal-Child Dyads of Functioning: The Intergenerational Impact of Violence Against Women on Children. Matern. Child Health J.; 2014; 18, pp. 2236-2243. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10995-014-1473-4]
44. Porter, E.; Lewis, A.J.; Watson, S.J.; Galbally, M. Perinatal maternal mental health and infant socio-emotional development: A growth curve analysis using the MPEWS cohort. Infant Behav. Dev.; 2019; 57, 101336. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101336]
45. Tao, H.; Shao, T.; Ni, L.; Sun, Y.; Yan, S.; Gu, C.; Tong, S. The Relationship between Maternal Emotional Symptoms During Pregnancy and Emotional and Behavior Problems in Preschool Children: A Birth Cohort Study. Chin. J. Prev. Med.; 2016; 50, pp. 129-135. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3760/cma.j.issn.0253-9624.2016.02.006]
46. Teramoto, S.; Soeda, A.; Hayashi, Y.; Saito, K.; Urashima, M. Problematic behaviours of 3-year-old children in Japan: Relationship with socioeconomic and family backgrounds. Early Hum. Dev.; 2005; 81, pp. 563-569. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.12.011] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15935934]
47. Thorsteinsdottir, S.; Gunnarsdottir, T.; Boles, R.E.; Njardvik, U. Weight status and disordered sleep in preschool children, parents’ negative mood states and marital status. Child. Health Care; 2017; 47, pp. 432-451. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02739615.2017.1392305]
48. Tichelman, E.; Warmink-Perdijk, W.; Henrichs, J.; Peters, L.; Schellevis, F.G.; Berger, M.Y.; Burger, H. Intrapartum synthetic oxytocin, behavioral and emotional problems in children, and the role of postnatal depressive symptoms, postnatal anxiety and mother-to-infant bonding: A Dutch prospective cohort study. Midwifery; 2021; 100, 103045. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.midw.2021.103045] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34077815]
49. Tse, W.S.; Siu, A.F.; Wong, T.K. How does maternal oxytocin influence children’s mental health problem and maternal mental health problem?. Psychiatry Res.; 2017; 258, pp. 124-129. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.068]
50. Wang, Y. The Effect of Maternal Trait Anxiety on Preschool Children’s Anxiety: The Mediating Roles of Maternal Psychological Control and Parent-Child Attachment; Harbin Engineering University: Harbin, China, 2020; [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.27060/d.cnki.ghbcu.2020.000526]
51. Wiggins, L.D.; Rubenstein, E.; Daniels, J.; DiGuiseppi, C.; Yeargin-Allsopp, M.; Schieve, L.A.; Tian, L.H.; Sabourin, K.; Moody, E.; Pinto-Martin, J. et al. A Phenotype of Childhood Autism Is Associated with Preexisting Maternal Anxiety and Depression. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol.; 2018; 47, pp. 731-740. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0469-8]
52. Yoldaş, T.; Günbey, C.; Değerliyurt, A.; Erol, N.; Özmert, E.; Yalnızoğlu, D. Behavioral problems of preschool children with new-onset epilepsy and one-year follow-up—A prospective study. Epilepsy Behav.; 2019; 92, pp. 171-175. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.12.025]
53. Yurduşen, S.; Erol, N.; Gençöz, T. The Effects of Parental Attitudes and Mothers’ Psychological Well-Being on the Emotional and Behavioral Problems of Their Preschool Children. Matern. Child Health J.; 2012; 17, pp. 68-75. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10995-012-0946-6]
54. Yan, Z. Research on the Relationship Between the Mother’s Family Conflict, Mental Health and Children’s Anxiety; Capital Normal University: Beijing, China, 2011.
55. Zhang, L.; Mersky, J.P.; Lee, C.P. Intergenerational Pathways Linking Mothers’ Adverse Childhood Experiences and Children’s Social-Emotional Problems. Child Maltreatment; 2022; 10775595211067212. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/10775595211067212]
56. Hao, Z.; Haiyan, T.; Lingchao, D.; Huimin, Z.; Heng, M. Behavioral Problems and Its Association with Family Factors of Young Children in Home Quarantine during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Chin. J. Soc. Med.; 2021; 38, pp. 263-267.
57. Yang, Z. Preschool’s Anxiety and Its Relationship to Mother’s Anxiety and Parenting Styles; Shandong Normal University: Jinan, China, 2014; [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.7666/d.Y1287993]
58. Duval, S.; Tweedie, R. Trim and Fill: A Simple Funnel-Plot-Based Method of Testing and Adjusting for Publication Bias in Meta-Analysis. Biometrics; 2000; 56, pp. 455-463. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x]
59. Bögels, S.M.; Brechman-Toussaint, M.L. Family issues in child anxiety: Attachment, family functioning, parental rearing and beliefs. Clin. Psychol. Rev.; 2006; 26, pp. 834-856. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2005.08.001] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16473441]
60. Nicol-Harper, R.; Harvey, A.G.; Stein, A. Interactions between mothers and infants: Impact of maternal anxiety. Infant Behav. Dev.; 2007; 30, pp. 161-167. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.08.005] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17292789]
61. Shamir-Essakow, G.; Ungerer, J.A.; Rapee, R. Attachment, Behavioral Inhibition, and Anxiety in Preschool Children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol.; 2005; 33, pp. 131-143. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-1822-2] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15839492]
62. Corvo, K.; Johnson, P. Sharpening Ockham’s Razor: The role of psychopathology and neuropsychopathology in the perpetration of domestic violence. Aggress. Violent Behav.; 2013; 18, pp. 175-182. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2012.11.017]
63. Weldon, S.; Gilchrist, E. Implicit Theories in Intimate Partner Violence Offenders. J. Fam. Violence; 2012; 27, pp. 761-772. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10896-012-9465-x]
64. McDonald, R.; Jouriles, E.N.; Tart, C.D.; Minze, L.C. Children’s adjustment problems in families characterized by men’s severe violence toward women: Does other family violence matter?. Child Abus. Negl.; 2009; 33, pp. 94-101. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.03.005]
65. Hungerford, A.; Wait, S.K.; Fritz, A.M.; Clements, C.M. Exposure to intimate partner violence and children’s psychological adjustment, cognitive functioning, and social competence: A review. Aggress. Violent Behav.; 2012; 17, pp. 373-382. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2012.04.002]
66. Xiaoqian, P. Research on Regional Culture of Chinese Preschool Education in the Context of Globalization: A Cultural Appropriate Perspective; East China Normal University: Shanghai, China, 2010.
67. Qianqian, X. New Trends of EU Early Childhood Education and Care Policies. Glob. Educ.; 2016; 45, pp. 71-80.
68. Jianhua, H. Family Policy in France and Its Intend to Balance Women’s Work and Life. Collect. Women’s Stud.; 2008; 6, pp. 70-76.
69. Vu, N.L.; Jouriles, E.N.; McDonald, R.; Rosenfield, D. Children’s exposure to intimate partner violence: A meta-analysis of longitudinal associations with child adjustment problems. Clin. Psychol. Rev.; 2016; 46, pp. 25-33. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.04.003]
70. Fantuzzo, J.; Boruch, R.; Beriama, A.; Atkins, M.; Marcus, S. Domestic Violence and Children: Prevalence and Risk in Five Major U.S. Cities. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry; 1997; 36, pp. 116-122. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199701000-00025]
71. Caspi, A.; Henry, B.; McGee, R.O.; Moffitt, T.E.; Silva, P.A. Temperamental Origins of Child and Adolescent Behavior Problems: From Age Three to Age Fifteen. Child Dev.; 1995; 66, pp. 55-68. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131190] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7497829]
72. Stormont, M.; Beckner, R.; Mitchell, B.; Richter, M. Supporting Successful Transition to Kindergarten: General Challenges and Specific Implications for Students with Problem Behavior. Psychol. Sch.; 2005; 42, pp. 765-778. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pits.20111]
73. Paulus, F.W.; Backes, A.; Sander, C.S.; Weber, M.; Von Gontard, A. Anxiety Disorders and Behavioral Inhibition in Preschool Children: A Population-Based Study. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev.; 2014; 46, pp. 150-157. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10578-014-0460-8] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24659133]
74. Wang, M.; Zhang, Y.; Yu, J.; Xing, X. Relationship among Preschoolers’ Anxiety, Temperament, and Family Environment. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol.; 2012; 20, pp. 371–373+397. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2012.03.022]
75. Hooper, E.; Feng, X.; Christian, L.; Slesnick, N. Emotion Expression, Emotionality, Depressive Symptoms, and Stress: Maternal Profiles Related to Child Outcomes. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol.; 2015; 43, pp. 1319-1331. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0019-6] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25894387]
76. Holloway, S.D.; Suzuki, S.; Yamamoto, Y.; Behrens, K.Y. Parenting Self-Efficacy among Japanese Mothers. J. Comp. Fam. Stud.; 2005; 36, pp. 61-76. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3138/jcfs.36.1.61]
77. Field, T. Postnatal anxiety prevalence, predictors and effects on development: A narrative review. Infant Behav. Dev.; 2018; 51, pp. 24-32. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.02.005]
78. Augustine, J.M.; Negraia, D.V.; Iceland, J.D.; Matthews, S.A.; Hook, J. Can Increased Educational Attainment Among Lower-Educated Mothers Reduce Inequalities in Children’s Skill Development?. Demography; 2017; 55, pp. 59-82. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s13524-017-0637-4]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Although numerous studies have found that maternal anxiety is a risk factor for the development of children’s problem behaviors, and there is a possible role of genes in the association between the two. And anxious mothers caring for their children can also affect the development of children’s problem behaviors. However, there is also considerable evidence from studies that refute this view. This study used a meta-analysis to explore the relationship between maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problem behaviors. Through literature retrieval and selection, in terms of the criteria for inclusion in the meta-analysis, 88 independent effect sizes (34 studies, 295,032 participants) were picked out as meta-analysis units. The test for heterogeneity illustrated that there was significant heterogeneity in 88 independent effect sizes, while the random effects model was an appropriate model for the subsequent meta-analysis. The publication bias test indicated that the impact of publication bias was modest but the major findings remained valid. In addition, in terms of the tentative review analysis and research hypotheses, the random effects model was used as a meta-analysis model. The research revealed that maternal anxiety was significantly positively correlated with preschool children’s internalizing problem behaviors, externalizing problem behaviors, and overall problem behaviors. The moderating effect analysis showed that region and gender of the child affected the relationship between maternal anxiety and children’s internalizing problem behaviors and externalizing problem behaviors, and region, child’s age and gender, mother’s age, and education level affected maternal anxiety and preschool children’s problems behavioral relationship. Hence, these results affirmed the role of maternal anxiety and emphasized the need to pay attention to the demographic characteristics and cultural background of the subjects during the research process and consider the generalizability of the conclusions under different circumstances.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
1 Institute of International Education, Wenzhou University, Wenzhou 325035, China
2 School of Education, Wenzhou University, Wenzhou 325035, China