Introduction
Animals routinely interrupt their current behaviour to scan their environment for potential threats, particularly from predators. Grouping helps to reduce the costs of vigilance to detect predators [1–4] but increases demand on monitoring conspecifics (social vigilance; [3, 5–8]). However, there is increasing evidence that individuals in a group differ in their vigilance due to differences in state, sex or age (e.g., [2, 9, 10]).
Vigilance is often measured in a foraging context to investigate trade-offs between foraging and vigilance [11] by assessing the frequency and duration of heads-up (e.g., [4, 9, 11, 12]). More recently, primarily horizontal head movements (in contrast to primarily vertical head movements) have been used to measure vigilance. This method allows measuring vigilance in any context and provides more information about the strategies used when scanning the environment [13]. Every horizontal movement of the head brings the eyes of an individual in contact with a different part of the environment [13], hence the frequency of head movements indicates how much of its environment an animal covers within a given time [13]. A high frequency of head movements means that different areas of the environment are scanned in fast succession with a short duration of looking in a particular direction. This has been linked to a visual search strategy that allows detecting any threats quickly [14] and often occurs in more dangerous situations (e.g., novel environments, at the periphery of a group, in small groups; [15–17]). In contrast, a low frequency of head movements indicates a longer dwelling on one aspect of the environment consistent with a visual tracking strategy that allows collecting information about distance, identity and movement of a target (e.g., predators or conspecifics; [14]). Both methods (heads-up and horizontal head movements) are consistent in that a higher frequency of vigilance (more heads-up or more horizontal head movements) usually occurs in more dangerous situations and results in a faster detection of predators [13].
Differences in vigilance between individuals in a group can be linked to their vulnerability; females in size dimorphic species are more vigilant than males [10] as are individuals at the periphery of groups [18]. Whereas age seems to follow an inverse U-shape curve with juveniles [9, 19, 20] and old adults [4, 21] being less vigilant, potentially reflecting the need for higher food intake in juveniles, whereas bigger size or senescence may be the reason in adults.
Another factor differently affecting individual’s vigilance is group composition. Vigilance in groups with young is higher than without young [18], particularly in females with young [5, 8, 10, 20]. Moreover, in ibex (Capra sibirica) female groups and mixed sex groups were more vigilant than male groups reflecting the higher predation in female groups and social vigilance directed towards harassing males in mixed groups [4]. Higher vigilance has also been observed in colour polymorphic species when in mixed morph groups as compared to monomorphic groups again indicating social vigilance [22].
More recently, vigilance has been linked to consistent individual differences (personality). While there is considerable plasticity in vigilance across situations (higher vigilance in novel situations or under higher predation risk [10, 18, 23, 24]), habitats [25] and season [2–4], individuals differ consistently in their vigilance over time [19, 26–29]. Moreover, a few studies found vigilance to be consistent within [19] and across contexts [24, 29].
The above findings suggest that vigilance is a personality trait on its own. Whereas other studies have treated vigilance more as a (cognitive) trait that may be affected by personality (measured as boldness, aggression etc) of an individual [30, 31]. In brief, personalities may vary along a risk-reward continuum with bold, aggressive individuals prioritising fast and high rewards over risk, whereas shy, non-aggressive individuals prioritise safety over rewards [32]. Consequently, bold individuals would be expected to be less vigilant. This has been confirmed in a range of species from birds to mammals including humans [26, 28, 30, 33], whereas in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) vigilance differed consistently between individuals but was unrelated to boldness [31]. In Eurasian siskins (Carduelis spinus) bolder individuals had a higher scan rate indicating higher vigilance compensating for their riskier behaviour [34]. In the human literature another personality trait–extraversion/ introversion has been weakly but consistently linked to vigilance with extroverts being less vigilant [33, 35, 36]. Finally, Risko et al. [37] found that in humans, perceptual curiosity (the desire to touch something novel as part of the openness dimension) was positively linked to vigilance.
Here we investigate vigilance in the colour-polymorphic Gouldian finch (Chloebia gouldiae). The species occurs in two main head colours, black and red, in the wild with about 70% black-headed birds and 30% red-headed birds and less than 1% yellow-headed birds in most populations [38]. They are highly social and inhabit tropical savannah grassland in North Australia [39]. Their personality is linked to their head colour with red-headed birds being more aggressive but less explorative towards a novel object and risk-averse in potentially dangerous situations than black-headed birds [40]. While the two main morphs do not differ in their vigilance, group composition matters. Whenever red-headed birds are present (mixed or pure red-headed groups), vigilance is heightened compared to pure black-headed groups [22]. Moreover, vigilance was highly plastic across situations with fewer head movements the more novel a situation was [22].
The aim of the current study was to investigate
1. Whether vigilance is a personality trait (contextual and temporal consistency).
2. Whether vigilance correlates with other personality traits.
3. How vigilance changes in relation to situation, age, sex, head colour and head colour composition.
Material and methods
Twenty-two Gouldian finches took part in the experiment with 12 females (6 black-headed, 6 red-headed) and 10 males (4 black-headed, 6 red-headed). Ages ranged from 1 to 8 years. The birds originated from 10 different bird breeders acquired over several years with all birds residing in the Animal Facility at Liverpool John Moors University since at least a year.
Birds were kept in mixed age, sex and head colour groups of about 6 birds, each. Dimensions of the holding cages were 120 x 80 x 100 cm (length x depth x height) and consisted of three closed walls with a wire mesh front and ceiling. The interior consisted of natural twigs and perches with food provided in feeders at the front. Food consisted of a mixture of Blattner Amadine Zucht Spezial (Gouldamadine), Blattner Astrilden Spezial and Blattner rote Mannahirse (Blattner Heimtierfutter, Ermengerst, Germany). Blattner bird grit was provided separately, as were eggshells. Cages contained water dispensers and a bath. The light regime was 13 h light to 11 h dark.
Experimental procedure
Experiments were conducted in an adjacent room containing six cages (120 x 70 x 100 cm) arranged back-to-back in two rows. Cages consisted of three wooden walls with a wire mesh front and ceiling facilitating that birds could not see but hear each other. Two perches were available left and right running perpendicular to the front. Food and water were provided at the front wire. A camera was permanently mounted on a tripod one metre away from the front of each cage.
For the current experiment only four of the six cages were used due to logistical reasons. Experiments were conducted over a seven-day period testing four groups at the same time (= one batch) with the next batch of birds being moved into the experimental cages the day after the preceding group had finished. Cages were thoroughly cleaned between groups. Gouldian finches are highly social; hence, birds were tested in same sex pairs either in same or mixed head colour pairs (4 black-headed, 4 red-headed, 4 mixed head colour pairs balanced across sexes). As there were only 22 birds, two black-headed males were used a second time to provide a partner for two red-headed males. Vigilance data of the re-used birds were not included in the analysis as they were already represented in the data set with their first testing. Head colour combinations and sexes were balanced within and across batches.
The following experiments were conducted: Vigilance was investigated in four different situations. (1) novel environment (when released into the experimental cage), (2) familiar environment (day 5 in experimental cage), (3) changed environment 1 (day 6) and (4) changed environment 2 (day 7). A change in the environment consisted of either a novel object (metal star, 10.5 x 4.5 cm, height x width) placed at a neutral location in the cage (over a perch but away from food and water) or a novel object (Christmas stocking sock, 11.5 x 5.5 x 2.5 cm; HxWxDepth) placed over the feeder. The order of presentation (day 6 or 7) was balanced across the objects. The two changed environments represented different levels of risk. The star at the neutral location could be ignored when wanted, causing a lower level of risk, whereas the sock over the feeder required interaction and approach when the birds wanted to feed. All experiments started at 10:00am and lasted for one hour. Data were recorded with digital video cameras using the GeoVision 1480 software (GeoVision Inc., Taipei, Taiwan) for later analysis.
Sixteen of the 22 birds had taken part in a very similar vigilance experiment a year earlier with exactly the same procedure for the novel and familiar situation but a slightly different protocol for the changed situation. Instead of placing a novel object inside the cage, a novel object (toy penguin or panda bear, 15–17 cm height, respectively) mounted on a tripod was placed 40 cm away from the front wire outside the cage [22]. Experiences with the cages a year ago were unlikely to have affected responses as reactions to unfamiliar situations usually fully recover after non-exposure of three to four weeks [41, 42].
Birds were released into the cage with video recording starting immediately to collect vigilance data in the novel environment (situation 1). After one hour, video recording was stopped, and the birds had until day 5 to habituate to the cage. On day 5, vigilance was recorded again in the now familiar environment (situation 2). The next day, either the star was positioned over the neutral perch or the stocking sock over the feeder (changed environment one and two–situation 3 and 4). In case of the stocking sock, birds were food deprived for one hour prior to the start of the experiment to have similar hunger levels. On day 7, the object not used on day 6 was introduced. On both days recording lasted for one hour after which the objects were removed. Birds were moved back into their holding cages after the experiment had finished on day 7.
Data analysis
Vigilance was measured as horizontal head movements [13] and defined as any visible movement of the head. Birds rarely looked up or down and these head movements were not considered. Frequency of head movements was extracted for each individual and situation. This measure is inversely related to the duration of looking into a particular direction [17].
All analyses were conducted with SPSS v. 26. The full data set is available in the S1 Table. To answer the first question whether vigilance is a personality trait, I first tested for contextual consistency of vigilance across the four situations in all birds (n = 22) using Pearson correlation tests. In a second step, I tested for contextual and temporal consistency across a year and three situations (based on the outcome of the correlation analysis) using the birds that were tested twice one year apart (n = 16). Vigilance in the novel, familiar and changed situation in the two years was compared. The changed situation presented a change that could be ignored (away from food and water) and differed slightly between years. In year two it was the star at a neutral location, whereas in year one an object was positioned outside the cage. Two objects were used in year one and the mean of the vigilance shown across the two objects was used in the analysis [22]. This analysis was done with One-Way ANOVA. One-Way ANOVAs compare within group variation against between group variation. Here intra-individual variation in vigilance across different situations and years was compared against the between-individual variation in vigilance. Each individual contributed six data points (96 data points in total).
The second question was about whether vigilance correlates with other personality traits. Data from the birds in year two (n = 22) in the two changed situations were used for this analysis. The star positioned at a neutral location is a standard measure for object exploration [43]. The novel object elicits both, approach (neophilia) and avoidance (neophobia) reactions. Approach occurs when the motivation to approach is higher than the motivation to avoid the novel object [43]. Object exploration has been shown to be consistent over time and a personality trait in the Gouldian finch [40]. Three latencies were recorded: the time between introducing the object over the perch and a) landing on the perch with the novel object, b) being in reach (two body lengths marked with a black line on the perch) of the object and c) touching the object. Only the latency to land on the perch was used for further analysis as all but two birds landed on the perch (20/22) providing the most variation, whereas 12 approached the object in reach and nine touched the object. Latency to perch was positively related with latency in reach (Spearman correlation: n = 22, corr. coef. = 0.58, p = 0.005) and latency in reach with latency to touch (corr. coef. = 0.75, p < 0.001).
The stocking sock positioned above the feeder is a standard measure for object neophobia [43, 44]. Here the motivation to feed and the motivation to avoid the novel object are in conflict with each other and approach times are a good representation of the fear involved [43]. Object neophobia is a consistent trait and positively correlated with object exploration in the Gouldian finches [45]. Again, three latencies were recorded: the time between positioning the novel object on top of the feeder and a) being in reach of the feeder, b) landing on the feeder and c) feeding. Only the latency to be in reach of the feeder was used for further analysis as ten out of twenty-two birds approached the feeder, whereas nine landed on the feeder and one fed. The latency to be in reach of the feeder was highly correlated with the latency to land on the feeder (Spearman Correlation: n = 22, corr. coef. = 0.96, p < 0.001) and close to significance with the latency to feed (corr. coef. = 0.39, p = 0.069). The latency to land on the perch with the object (object exploration) and the latency to be in reach of the feeder (object neophobia), respectively, were correlated with the vigilance measure for each situation using Spearman correlations as data deviated from normality.
To address the third question about how situation, age, sex, head colour and head colour composition affect vigilance, I ran General Linear Mixed Models (GLMM). Analyses were run on the individual level with situation as repeated measure and individual as random factor to account for repeated testing. The sample size was n = 22 with each bird contributing four data points, one for each situation, resulting in 88 data points in total. The dependent variable was frequencies of head movements with an identity link function. A series of models were built to test for effects of different variables and interactions. The basic model consisted of four fixed factors (situation, head colour morph, sex and age class), one covariate (partner head colour morph) and two interaction terms (head colour morph x partner head colour morph and head colour morph x sex), which is the same basic model used for analysis in the vigilance study the year before [22] except for sex being a fixed factor now rather than a covariate. Using the same basic model allows direct comparison with the new situations used in the current study. Age classes consisted of one- to two-year-old birds (n = 5), three to four years of age (n = 8) and older than four years (n = 9). More complex models included interaction terms with situations and combinations of two interactions. Akaike criterion was used to select the best model.
Ethical note
Experiments were conducted in accordance with The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour (ASAB) ethical guidelines [46] and were non-invasive. Experiments were approved by the University Ethics Committee.
Results
Question 1 investigated consistency of vigilance across the four situations. Head movements in the familiar environment were positively correlated with head movements in one of the changed (star at a neutral location) and the novel environment situation (Pearson correlation: n = 22, familiar vs changed (star at neutral location) corr. coef. = 0.68, p < 0.001, familiar vs novel corr. coef. = 0.50, p = 0.017). Head movements in both changed environments showed a weak trend to be positively correlated with head movements in the novel environment (star: corr. coef. = 0.37, 0.092, sock: corr. coef. = 0.37, p = 0.089).
Furthermore, temporal and contextual consistency was investigated in a subset of birds that were tested in this study and the year before considering vigilance in the familiar situation, the novel situation and one changed situation (novel object at a neutral location or outside the cage in year one). Within individual variation did not differ significantly from the between individual variation (One-Way ANOVA: n = 16, F = 1.365, p = 0.185).
Question 2 was concerned about a link between vigilance and other personality traits. Head movements in the changed situation (sock above feeder) correlated positively with the latency to land on the perch with the novel object (corr. coef. = 0.53, p = 0.011) but no other correlations were identified (all p > 0.2).
Question 3 addressed how vigilance in the different situations was affected by head colour morph, age, sex and head colour morph composition. The best model retaining the most information was the basic model with the interactions age class x situation, sex x situation, head colour morph x situation and partner head colour morph x situation (Table 1). The following variables and interactions were significant (Table 2). Vigilance differed across situations (F3,62 = 44.421, p < 0.001; Fig 1) with the fewest head movements in the novel environment (novel vs familiar t21 = 3.712, p < 0.001, novel vs changed (neutral location) t21 = 2.038, p = 0.046, novel vs changed (above feeder) t21 = 5.010, p < 0.001). Head movements were intermediate in the familiar environment and were significantly higher than in the novel environment (see above), but significantly lower than in the changed environment with the object above the feeder (statistics are from an additional paired t-test as the GLMM only provided outputs in relation to the novel situation: t21 = -4.750, p < 0.001). Head movements in the familiar and changed environment with the object at a neutral location were similar (additional paired t-test: t21 = 0.926, p = 0.365). Finally, head movements were highest in the changed environment with the object above the feeder and differed significantly from all other situations (additional paired t-test: changed (object above feeder) vs changed (object at neutral location) t21 = -5.062, p < 0.001, for all other comparisons see above).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Mean and standard error of frequency of head movements per hour (n = 22) in four situations: familiar environment, two changed environments (star over neutral perch, sock above feeder) and novel environment. * P < 0.05; *** P < 0.001; [***] P < 0.001 from additional paired t-tests.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Age class was the second main factor with a significant effect (F2,62 = 5.339, p = 0.007; Fig 2). Overall, head movements decreased with age, particularly in the oldest age class. However, posthoc tests were not significant (1–2 years vs > 4 years: t = 1.141, p = 0.258, 3–4 years vs > 4 years: t = 1.425, p = 0.159).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Mean and standard error of frequency of head movements per hour (n = 22) across age classes.
The main factor sex had a significant effect (F1,62 = 5.012, p = 0.029) with males making fewer head movements (1012 ± 48) than females (1132 ± 41). Moreover, the interaction head colour morph x partner head colour morph was significant (F2,62 = 16.164, p < 0.001; Fig 3). Mixed head colour morphs made significantly more head movements than pure head colour morphs (pure black-headed vs mixed head colour: t = -2.540, p = 0.014, pure red-headed vs mixed head colour: t = 2.277, p = 0.026). No other variables and interactions were significant. The random effect of individual was not significant (z = 0.077, p = 0.939).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Mean and standard error of frequency of head movements per hour (n = 22) for black-headed (BH) and red-headed (RH) birds when in pure black-headed (black bar), red headed (red bars) or mixed head colour (hatched) morph pairings. * P < 0.05.
Discussion
Vigilance measured as the frequency of head movements showed some short-term contextual consistency between three situations (familiar–changed (object at neutral location)–novel environment) but no long-term temporal consistency across one year. Moreover, head movements showed some weak links to other personality traits. Vigilance was plastic across situations, decreased with age and was affected by group composition.
Vigilance showed short-term contextual consistency. Specifically, head movements in the familiar environment correlated with head movements in the novel environment and the changed environment with a novel object at a neutral location. This corroborates findings in junglefowl (Gallus gallus), which showed contextual consistency in vigilance between a novel environment and a novel object placed at a neutral location in the arena [29]. In hyaenas (Crocuta crocuta) vigilance was consistent within contexts (resting, feeding, nursing) but not between contexts [19]. Interestingly, vigilance in the changed situation with the object above the feeder did not correlate with vigilance in any of the other situations. This is surprising and requires further investigation. This was the only situation, in which a change occurred around an important resource. The motivation to feed may have affected vigilance, particularly as birds were food deprived. Indeed, some individuals became very agitated, moved a lot and showed high vigilance up to the end, whereas others gave up on the food and settled down after a while with fewer head movements.
Vigilance showed no long-term consistency (over one year). This contrasts with other studies, which found temporal consistency in hyaenas [19] and orange-winged amazons (Amazona amazonica) over one year [28], in redshanks (Tringa tetanus) over a 3-month period [26] and in cliff swallows (Petrochelidon pyrrhonata) within a breeding season [27]. This indicates that Gouldian finches show short-term contextual consistency but no long-term temporal consistency. One reason for the lack of long-term consistency could be that birds were tested with different partners as group composition has been shown to affect vigilance [e.g., 4, 18, 22]. Future studies should test to which extent group composition affects consistency and whether vigilance is consistent over time.
Vigilance was a poor predictor of other personality traits with only the frequency of head movements in the changed situation with the object above the feeder (neophobia situation) correlating with the latency to land on the perch with the novel object (exploration situation). This may indicate that highly vigilant individuals are also more fearful and consequently hesitate longer to engage in exploration than less vigilant individuals. In orange-winged amazons, neurotic birds (more anxious and vulnerable to stress) were more vigilant than less anxious [28]. It also weakly links to the risk-reward trade-off hypothesis that risk-takers are less vigilant [26, 30, 47]. However, it is unclear why this link was only found across contexts rather than within the same context with the object above the feeder. Generally, vigilance was not directly linked to the latencies to explore and or to approach the food (neophobia). This is in agreement with Zidar et al. [29] who did not find a link between vigilance and the latency to approach a novel object in a slightly novel environment in junglefowl. Likewise, the number of different foraging areas used (i.e., exploration of novel habitats) did not correlate with vigilance in redshanks [26]. Most studies so far found a link between vigilance and the personality trait extraversion (sociability) with more extrovert and social individuals being less vigilant [28, 33, 35, 36]. Sociability has not been considered in this study but should be included in future studies. Taken together, vigilance is a trait than can be affected by personality rather than a personality trait on its own.
Vigilance showed considerable plasticity across situations, which indicates that individuals adapt their vigilance to the degree of threat/ danger perceived. Such plasticity is well documented in a range of taxa in birds [22, 24, 26, 48] and mammals [2, 7, 20, 30, 49, 50]. Birds were most vigilant when a change occurred around an important resource (novel object above feeder). This indicates that they perceived this change as highly threatening. Higher vigilance often occurs in more risky situations e.g., when at the periphery of a flock [51], in a novel situation [23, 52] or once a predator has been detected [24, 53, 54]. Interestingly, a novel object at a neutral location did not increase vigilance as compared to the familiar situation. This is surprising as an earlier study found reduced vigilance when an object was positioned outside the cage [22]. This was interpreted as a more intense assessment of the novel object with spending more time assessing the novel object and consequently fewer head movements. It is unclear why the object in the cage did not cause a similar change in vigilance. Potential explanations could be a) the objects outside the cage were considerably larger than the star in the cage conveying different levels of threat [55]. b) The outside object may have required more assessment as it could only be explored from one direction, whereas the star could be approached from all directions providing more information about threats [56]. Last but not least, vigilance was lowest in the novel environment. This corroborates findings from an earlier study suggesting a tracking strategy to collect information about the new situation [22]. Few head movements mean that the bird is looking in one direction for longer. This allows collecting detailed information about this particular part of the environment [14]. While this is opposite to most studies, which find higher vigilance in unfamiliar situations [23, 52], it can be explained with the secrecy of this species as they sit quietly in a tree after arriving at a waterhole (own obs.).
Vigilance showed an age effect with a decrease in vigilance the older the bird, which was particularly visible in the oldest birds (over 4 years). Greater experience [4] in older birds may reduce head movements as they may be less aroused than younger individuals. Alternatively, the old birds may be less vigilant due to cognitive deterioration [21].
Males made fewer head movements than females. This can be interpreted as using a tracking strategy, whereby males track each other to avoid attack [14]. As they focus on an individual for longer, the frequency of head movements declines. Females, in contrast, may have generally scanned the environment moving the head frequently to cover all parts of the environment [14].
Finally, group composition affected vigilance. Individuals in mixed head colour morph groups were more vigilant than birds in pure red-headed or black-headed morph groups. Higher vigilance in mixed head colour morph groups was also found in an earlier study of this species [22]. Red-headed birds are more aggressive than black-headed Gouldian finches [40, 57, 58]. Black-headed birds may increase their vigilance to avoid aggression in the presence of red-headed birds [22]. An increase in social vigilance has been observed across taxa in more competitive situations [3, 5–7, 25]. Red-headed birds, in contrast, may watch black-headed birds to extract information about potential environmental threats as black-heads take greater risk [40] and investigate changes in the familiar environment faster [40, 45]. In both head colour morphs, greater social vigilance alongside environmental vigilance resulted in overall higher vigilance in mixed morph groups.
In conclusion, vigilance showed short-term contextual consistency but no long-term temporal consistency and some weak relationships to other personality traits supporting the risk-reward trade-off hypothesis. Vigilance was highly plastic across situations with the highest vigilance when changes occurred around important resources. Finally, vigilance was higher in mixed morph groups pointing to increased social vigilance in these settings.
Supporting information
S1 Table. Data about vigilance.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0279066.s001
(XLSX)
Acknowledgments
I would like to thank Anna Temple for collecting some of the data for this study, Georgina Eccles for helping with the data collection, Blattner Heimtierfutter for sponsoring all bird food, Peter McGough and other breeders for donating some of the Gouldian Finches, and the animal care technicians for looking after the birds.
Citation: Mettke-Hofmann C (2022) Is vigilance a personality trait? Plasticity is key alongside some contextual consistency. PLoS ONE 17(12): e0279066. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0279066
About the Authors:
Claudia Mettke-Hofmann
Roles: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: School of Biological and Environmental Sciences, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, United Kingdom
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3874-5035
1. Ye Y, Jiang Y, Hu C, Liu Y, Qing B, Wang C, et al. What makes a tactile forager join mixed-species flocks? A case study with the endangered Crested Ibis (Nipponia nippon). Auk. 2017; 134: 421–431.
2. Fattorini N, Ferretti F. To scan or not to scan? Occurrence of the group‐size effect in a seasonally nongregarious forager. Ethology. 2019; 125: 263–275.
3. Pecorella I, Fattorini N, Macchi E, Ferretti F. Sex/age differences in foraging, vigilance and alertness in a social herbivore. Acta Ethol. 2019; 22: 1–8 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-018-0300-0.
4. Han L, Blank D, Wang M, Yanget W. Vigilance behaviour in Siberian ibex (Capra sibirica): Effect of group size, group type, sex and age. Behav Proc. 2020; 170: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2019.104021.
5. Fattorini N, Lovari S, Brunetti C, Baruzzi C, Cotza A, Macchi E. et al. Age, seasonality, and correlates of aggression in female Apennine chamois. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2018; 72: 171 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-018-2584-5.
6. Beauchamp G. On how risk and group size interact to influence vigilance. Biol Rev. 2019; 94: 1918–1934. pmid:31270943
7. Stears K, Schmitt MH, Wilmer CC, Shrader AM. Mixed-species herding levels the landscape of fear. Proc R Soc B. 2020; 287: 20192555. pmid:32126952
8. Rieucau G, Blanchard P, Martin JGA, Favreau F-R, Goldizen AW, Pays O. Investigating differences in vigilance tactic use within and between the sexes in Eastern Grey Kangaroos. PLoS ONE. 2012; 7(9): e44801. pmid:22984563
9. Arenz CL, Leger DW. Antipredator vigilance of juvenile and adult thirteen-lined ground squirrels and the role of nutritional need. Anim Behav. 2000; 59; 535–541. pmid:10715175
10. Childress MJ, Lung MA. Predation risk, gender and the group size effect: does elk vigilance depend upon the behaviour of conspecifics? Anim Behav. 2003; 66: 389–398.
11. Baker DJ, Stillman RA, Smart SL, Bullock JM, Norris KJ. Are the costs of routine vigilance avoided by granivorous foragers? Func Ecol. 2011; 25: 617–627.
12. Barnier F, Duncan P, Fritz H, Blanchard P, Rubenstein DI, Pays O. Between‑gender differences in vigilance do not necessarily lead to differences in foraging‑vigilance tradeoffs. Oecologia. 2016; 181: 757–768. pmid:27017605
13. Fernandez-Juricic E, Gall MD, Dolan T, O’Rourke C, Thomas S, Lynch J. Visual systems and vigilance behaviour of two ground-foraging avian prey species: white-crowned sparrows and California towhees. Anim Behav. 2011; 81: 705–713.
14. Fernandez-Juricic E. Sensory basis of vigilance behavior in birds: Synthesis and future prospects. Behav Proc. 2012; 89: 143–152. pmid:22101130
15. Krebs H, Weyers P, Macht M, Weijers H-G, Janke W. Scanning behavior of rats during eating under stressful noise. Physiol Behav. 1997; 62: 151–154. pmid:9226355
16. Griesser M. Nepotistic vigilance behavior in Siberian jay parents. Behav Ecol. 2003; 14: 245–250.
17. Fernandez-Juricic E, Beauchamp G, Treminio R, Hoover M. Making heads turn: association between head movements during vigilance and perceived predation risk in brown-headed cowbird flocks. Anim Behav. 2011; 82: 573–577.
18. Di Blanco y Hirsch BT. Determinants of vigilance behavior in the ring-tailed coati (Nasua nasua): the importance of within-group spatial position. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006; 61: 173–182.
19. Pangle WM, Holekamp KE. Functions of vigilance behaviour in a social carnivore, the spotted hyaena, Crocuta crocuta. Anim Behav. 2010; 80: 257–267.
20. Lashley MA, Chitwood C, Biggerstaff MT, Morina DL, Moorman CE, DePerno CS. White-tailed deer vigilance: The influence of social and environmental factors. PLOS ONE. 2014; 9(3): e90652. pmid:24599090
21. Moore H, Dudchenko P, Bruno JP, Sarter M. Toward modelling age-related changes of attentional abilities in rats: Simple and choice reaction time tasks and vigilance. Neurobiol Aging. 1992; 13: 759–772.
22. Mettke-Hofmann 2021. Morph composition matters in the Gouldian Finch (Chloebia gouldiae): Involvement of red-headed birds increases vigilance. Birds. 2021; 2: 404–414. https://doi.org/10.3390/birds2040030.
23. Barros M, Alencar C, de Souza Silva MA, Tomaz C. Changes in experimental conditions alter anti-predator vigilance and sequence predictability in captive marmosets. Behav Proc. 2008; 77: 351–356. pmid:18006248
24. Mathot KJ, van den Hout PJ, Piersma T, Kempenaers B, Reale D, Dingemanse NJ. Disentangling the roles of frequency-vs. state-dependence in generating individual differences in behavioural plasticity. Ecol Lett. 2011; 14: 1254–1262. pmid:21978266
25. Aviles JM, Bednekoff PA. How do vigilance and feeding by common cranes Grus grus depend on age, habitat, and flock size? J Avian Biol. 2007; 38: 690–697.
26. Couchoux C, Cresswell W. Personality constraints versus flexible antipredation behaviors: how important is boldness in risk management of redshanks (Tringa totanus) foraging in a natural system? Behav Ecol. 2011; 23: 290–301.
27. Roche EA, Brown CR. Among-individual variation in vigilance at the nest in colonial cliff swallows. Wils J Orn. 2013; 125: 685–695.
28. Cussen VA, Mench JA. Personality predicts cognitive bias in captive psittacines, Amazona amazonica. Anim Behav. 2014; 89: 123–130.
29. Zidar J, Sorato E, Malmqvist A-M, Jansson E, Rosher C, Jensen P, et al. Early experience affects adult personality in the red junglefowl: A role for cognitive stimulation? Behav Proc. 2016; 134: 78–86. pmid:27329431
30. Mazza V, Jacob J, Dammhahn M, Zaccaroni M, Eccard JA. Individual variation in cognitive style reflects foraging and antipredator strategies in a small mammal. Sci R. 2019; 9: 10157 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-46582-1.
31. Szopa-Comley AW, Donald WG, Ioannou CC. Predator personality and prey detection: inter-individual variation in responses to cryptic and conspicuous prey. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2020; 74: 70 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-020-02854-9.
32. Sih A, Del Giudice M. Linking behavioural syndromes and cognition: a behavioural ecology perspective. Phil Trans R Soc Biol B. 2012; 367: 2762–2772. pmid:22927575
33. Hadziahmetovic M, Koso-Drljevic N. Predicting vigilance by HEXACO model of personality. Pers Ind Diff. 2022; 186: 111297.
34. Pascal J, Senar JC. Antipredator behavioural compensation of proactive personality trait in male Eurasian siskins. Anim Behav. 2014; 90: 297–303.
35. Koelega HS. Extraversion and Vigilance Performance: 30 years of Inconsistencies. Psych Bull. 1992; 112: 239–258. pmid:1454894
36. Shaw TH, Matthews G, Warm JS, Finomore VS, Silverman L, Costa PT Jr.. Individual differences in vigilance: Personality, ability and states of stress. J Res Pers. 2010; 44: 297–308.
37. Risko EF, Anderson NC, Lanthier S, Kingstone A. Curious eyes: Individual differences in personality predict eye movement behavior in scene-viewing. Cogn. 2012; 122: 86–90. pmid:21983424
38. Brush AH, Seifried H. Pigmentation and feather structure in genetic variants of the Gouldian finch, Poephila gouldiae. Auk. 1968; 85: 416–430.
39. Dostine PL, Johnson GC, Franklin DC, Zhang Y, Hempel C. Seasonal use of savanna landscapes by the Gouldian finch, Erythrura gouldiae, in the Yinberrie Hills area, Northern Territory. Wildl Res. 2001; 28: 445–458.
40. Williams LJ, King AJ, Mettke-Hofmann C. Colourful characters: Head-colour reflects personality in a social bird, the Gouldian finch (Erythrura gouldiae). Anim Behav. 2012; 84: 159–165.
41. Greenberg R, Mettke-Hofmann C. Ecological aspects of neophobia and neophilia in birds. Curr Orn. 2001; 16: 119–178.
42. Mettke-Hofmann C, Rowe KC, Hayden TJ, Canoine V. Effects of experience and object complexity on exploration in garden warblers (Sylvia borin). J Zool. 2006; 268: 405–413.
43. Mettke-Hofmann C, Winkler H, Leisler B. The significance of ecological factors for exploration and neophobia in parrots. Ethology. 2002; 108: 249–272.
44. Greggor AL, Thornton Al, Clayton NS. Neophobia is not only avoidance: improving neophobia tests by combining cognition and ecology. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015; 6: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.10.007.
45. Mettke-Hofmann C. The effect of head colour and age on personality traits in a social setting. Ethology. 2012; 118: 906–916.
46. ASAB. Guidelines for the treatment of animals in behavioural research and teaching. Anim Behav. 2020; 159: i–xi.
47. Bergvall UA, Schäpers A, Kjellander P, Weiss A. Personality and foraging decisions in fallow deer, Dama dama. Anim Behav. 2011; 81: 101–112.
48. Beauchamp G, Ruxton GD. Vigilance decreases with time at loafing sites in gulls (Larus spp.). Ethology. 2012; 118: 733–739.
49. Van der Meer E, Fritz H, Pays O. Anti-predator behaviour of kudu and response to mimicked African wild dog do age and sex. Behav. 2015; 152: 1209–1228.
50. Uchida K, Suzuki KK, Shimamoto T, Yanagawa H, Koizumi I. Decreased vigilance or habituation to humans? Mechanisms on increased boldness in urban animals. Behav Ecol. 2019; 30: 1583–1590.
51. Robinson B, Merrill EH. Foraging vigilance trade-offs in a partially migratory population: comparing migrants and residents on a sympatric range. Anim Behav. 2013; 85: 849–856.
52. Shonfield J. The effect of familiarity on vigilance behaviour in grey squirrels. McGill Sci Undergrad Res J. 2011; 6: 45–49.
53. Periquet S, Valeix M, Loveridge AJ, Madzikanda H, Macdonald DW, Fritz H. Individual vigilance of African herbivores while drinking: the role of immediate predation risk and context. Anim Behav. 2010; 79: 665–671.
54. Severcan C, Yamac E. The effects of flock size and human presence on vigilance and feeding behavior in the Eurasian Coot (Fulica atra L.) during breeding season. Acta Ethol. 2011; 14: 51–56.
55. Greenberg R. The role of neophobia in determining the degree of foraging-specialisation in some migrant warblers. Am Nat. 1983; 122: 444–453.
56. Nicolai J. Steinbacher J. Prachtfinken: Australien, Ozeanien, Suedostasien. Stuttgart: Ulmer publisher; 2001.
57. Pryke SR, Griffith SC. Red dominates black: agonistic signalling among head morphs in the colour polymorphic Gouldian finch. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2006; 273: 949–957. pmid:16627280
58. Pryke SR. Fiery red heads: female dominance among head color morphs in the Gouldian finch. Behav Ecol. 2007; 18: 621–627.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2022 Claudia Mettke-Hofmann. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Animals regularly scan their environment for predators and to monitor conspecifics. However, individuals in a group seem to differ in their vigilance linked to age, sex or state with recent links made to personality. The aims of the study were to investigate whether a) individuals differ consistently in their vigilance, b) vigilance is linked to other personality traits and c) other factors affect vigilance in the colour polymorphic Gouldian finch. Birds were tested in same (red-headed or black-headed) or mixed head colour morph same sex pairs in four contexts (novel environment, familiar environment, two changed environments). Vigilance was measured as horizontal head movements. Vigilance showed contextual consistency but no long-term temporal consistency over a year. Head movements were only weakly linked to other personality traits indicative of a risk-reward trade-off with more explorative individuals being less vigilant. Vigilance was highly plastic across situations and affected by group composition. Mixed head colour morph pairs made more head movements, potentially linked to higher social vigilance. Results indicate that vigilance is a highly plastic trait affected by personality rather than a personality trait on its own, which allows adapting vigilance to different situations.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer