Diabetic ketoacidosis (DKA), a common complication of type 1 diabetes (T1D),1 is associated with cognitive declines,2–6 even when no obvious neurological symptoms occur during DKA. Young children may be more vulnerable and at greater risk of developing these declines.7 Previous paediatric studies examining associations between DKA and cognition have focused on broad age ranges, extending beyond early childhood.2–6 In a small-scale study, DKA was associated with larger memory declines if it occurred before age 7 years.2 However, children were assessed between 7 and 16 years of age, making it impossible to determine whether declines manifested soon after DKA exposure or emerged later and worsened over time.7 In a recent study from our group, lower pH at diagnosis of DKA, which was experienced at diabetes onset, was associated with lower intellectual quotient (IQ)6 in a sample of newly diagnosed 6- to 18-year-old children, suggesting small effects soon after the onset of T1D. However, there were no significant group differences in IQ scores between children who did and did not experience DKA; group differences were restricted to long-term memory. In the current study, we asked whether group differences in IQ scores would be evident in young children tested 2–6 months after DKA at onset of T1D. Such a finding would suggest greater susceptibility to cognitive decline in young children.
METHODSWe recruited 3- to 5-year-old children who were newly diagnosed with T1D and exhibited DKA at onset from 12 centres participating in the Pediatric Emergency Care Applied Research Network (PECARN) Fluid Therapies Under Investigation in DKA (FLUID) trial.8 All of these children were randomized in the trial. We distinguished moderate/severe DKA (pH ≤ 7.19 or serum bicarbonate concentration ≤9 mmol/L) and mild DKA (pH between 7.20 and 7.25, or serum bicarbonate concentration between 10 and 15 mmol/L).6 Children with recent T1D diagnosis (<2 years), but no DKA episodes based on guardian history and medical review, were recruited from the paediatric diabetes clinics at the participating PECARN centres. Neurocognitive assessment sessions were delayed or rescheduled if children had either hypoglycaemia (glucose <70 mg/dL) or hyperglycaemia (glucose >350 mg/dL). All participants were English-speaking.
We administered the English version of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI-III),9 which is validated for children ranging in age from 2 years and 6 months to 7 years and 3 months of age (normed IQ M = 100; SD = 15). Research coordinators trained by a study investigator with a doctoral degree in psychology administered the WPPSI. This measure was administered once between 2- to 6-months after DKA for children with exposure to DKA or soon after recruitment for children with T1D without exposure to DKA. To limit the duration of the assessment and the child's fatigue, we only administered the core subtests of the WPPSI-III corresponding to the minimal number of subsets that yielded a reliable and valid IQ score based on the test manual.9 The ability of the WPPSI to yield reliable and valid IQ scores from a subset of core subtests has been demonstrated across multiple versions.9,10 For 3-year-olds, these core tests included four subtests, namely Block Design, Arithmetic, Vocabulary and Comprehension. For 4- and 5-year-olds, core tests included seven subtests, namely Block Design, Information, Matrix Reasoning, Vocabulary, Picture Concepts, Word Reasoning and Coding. The WPPSI is a highly regarded assessment tool to provide a comprehensive, reliable and valid measure of cognitive ability9 as demonstrated by its frequent used as an outcome measure in paediatric studies, including observational11,12 and randomized clinical trial8,13 investigations.
Statistical analysesWe tested for differences in frequency distribution of demographics as a function of DKA status (severe/moderate DKA vs mild DKA, vs. no DKA) using chi-squared tests. We tested for differences in IQ scores as a function of DKA status using analysis of variance tests. We subsequently included socioeconomic status (SES), and ethnicity as covariates and conducted an analysis of covariance. Age was not included in the models because IQ scores are age-normed. We performed the analyses using IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 27).
RESULTSWe assessed 73 children (Table 1): 22 who had experienced moderate/severe DKA, 24 mild DKA and 27 no DKA. Patient demographic variables did not statistically differ as a function of DKA status (Table 1). However, children who experienced DKA, regardless of its severity, exhibited significantly lower IQ scores than children who did not experience DKA, F(2, 70) = 6.26, p = .003, partial η2 = .15. Given nonsignificant trends for children who experienced DKA to be of lower SES or be of Hispanic ethnicity, we included SES and ethnicity as covariates in an additional analysis. The effect of DKA status persisted in this analysis, F(2, 63) = 3.88, p = .03, partial η2 = .11 (Figure 1).
TABLE 1 Participant characteristics as a function of DKA status.
Characteristics | Non-DKA (N = 27) | Mild DKA (N = 24) | Moderate/severe DKA (N = 22) | p-Value* |
Male: N (%) | 16 (59.3) | 11 (45.8) | 9 (40.1) | .40 |
Age in years at testing: Mean (SD; Min–Max) | 4.6 (0.8; 3.2–5.9) | 4.6 (0.8; 3.1–5.8) | 4.6 (0.9; 3.2–5.9) | .97 |
Age in years at T1D onset: Mean (SD; Min–Max)a | 3.9 (0.9; 2–5) | 3.8 (0.7; 2.9–5.5) | 3.7 (1.1; 2.6–5.7) | .77 |
Glucose (mg/dL) at assessment: Mean (SD; Min–Max)b | 250.7 (98.5; 74–464) | 203.1 (79.1;73–350) | 238.5 (112.1;72–485) | .22 |
Hypoglycaemic episodes prior to testing: N (%) | 0 (0.0) | 1 (4.2) | 1 (4.5) | ** |
Socioeconomic Status: N (%)c | ||||
High school/GED or less | 2 (7.4) | 4 (16.7) | 3 (13.6) | .10 |
Some college/vocational school | 5 (18.5) | 7 (29.2) | 10 (45.5) | |
College degree or more | 18 (66.7) | 13 (54.1) | 6 (27.3) | |
Race: N (%) | ||||
White | 25 (92.6) | 19 (79.2) | 17 (77.3) | .48 |
Black | 1 (3.7) | 2 (8.3) | 4(18.2) | |
Other | 1 (3.7) | 3(12.5) | 1 (4.5) | |
Ethnicity: N (%) | ||||
Hispanic | 0 (0.0) | 4 (16.7) | 4 (18.2) | .07 |
aAge at onset also corresponds to the age at which a single DKA episode was experienced in the Mild DKA and in the moderate/severe DKA groups.
bGlucose levels correspond to the initial reading before testing. For the seven children who initially showed glucose ≥350, assessment was delayed until glucose was within the 70–350 range.
cMaternal education (available only for 69 participants).
*p-Values are from likelihood ratio chi-square tests for categorical characteristics (those with N (%)), and analysis of variance F-tests for continuous characteristics (those with mean (SD)).
**p-Values were not calculated due to design-imposed relationships with DKA Status.
FIGURE 1. Intellectual quotient (IQ) score as a function of DKA status. Mean levels and standard errors around the means are reported. Children who did not experience DKA exhibited significantly higher IQ scores than children who experienced either mild or moderate/severe DKA (ps [less than] .05). This analysis adjusts for SES and ethnicity.
Diabetic ketoacidosis has been associated with cognitive decline in children with T1D.2–5 Our multi-centre study provides new evidence of a strong and sizeable association between DKA status and IQ score in 3-to 5-year-old children, just a few months after the onset of T1D. Our recruitment and assessment procedures were analogous to those used in a large study with 6- to 18-year-old children,6 in which no significant group differences in IQ scores were found between children with or without DKA exposure at diabetes onset, whose neurocgnitive function was also assessed 2- to 6- months after T1D diagnosis. Thus, following DKA, IQ declines may occur more quickly and/or be more robust in young children.
We recognize several limitations in the current study. First, the sample size is relatively small, although it is, to our knowledge, the largest study of cognitive functioning in very young children with T1D. Therefore, we were unable to measure the relative contribution of additional clinically relevant factors associated with cognitive functioning above and beyond DKA, such as acute kidney injury, which is a frequent complication of DKA.14 Second, we cannot exclude the possibility that the group differences in IQ scores reported here may depend, at least in part, on additional factors affecting the probability that children become exposed to DKA in the first place. For example, lower parental education may limit the ability to recognize early symptoms. Moreover, access to healthcare services is affected by such factors as SES and ethnicity. However, we controlled statistically for such factors in our analysis and the effects of DKA remained. Furthermore, previous findings have suggested that these socioeconomic factors play a sizeable role in previously diagnosed patients due to associations with the management of T1D.6 Future studies involving large, prospective samples are needed to fully characterize the relative contributions of these variables or other clinically relevant variables that may exacerbate cognitive declines in children with T1D (e.g. gestational age at birth15 and exposure to various environmental hazards16). Third, the absence of a comparison group of healthy children prevents us from examining associations with other risk factors for cognition in T1D.17 However, to the extent that the main focus of the research is the association with DKA, a comparison group of children with T1D without DKA is generally more informative.
CONCLUSIONSThe present study revealed that a single episode of DKA was associated with lower cognitive functioning in young children with T1D. These results suggest that young children may be particularly vulnerable to the negative effects of DKA, underscoring the importance of early detection of diabetes and prevention of DKA in young children.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONSSimona Ghetti: Conceptualization (lead); data curation (lead); formal analysis (lead); funding acquisition (supporting); investigation (supporting); methodology (supporting); project administration (supporting) writing – original draft (lead). Nathan Kuppermann: Conceptualization (lead); data curation (lead); formal analysis (supporting); funding acquisition (lead); investigation (lead); methodology (lead); project administration (lead); writing – original draft (supporting). Arleta Rewers: Data curation (supporting); investigation (supporting); project administration (supporting); supervision (equal); writing – review and editing (supporting). Sage R. Myers: Data curation (supporting); methodology (supporting); project administration (supporting); supervision (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). Jeff E. Schunk: Data curation (supporting); methodology (supporting); project administration (supporting); supervision (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). Michael J. Stoner: Methodology (supporting); project administration (supporting); supervision (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). Kimberly S. Quayle: Investigation (supporting); methodology (supporting); supervision (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). Kathleen M. Brown: Methodology (supporting); project administration (supporting); supervision (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). Jennifer L. Trainor: Investigation (supporting); methodology (supporting); supervision (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). Leah Tzimenatos: Investigation (supporting); methodology (supporting); supervision (supporting); writing – review and editing (equal). Andrew D. DePiero: Investigation (supporting); methodology (supporting); supervision (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). Julie K. McManemy: Methodology (supporting); project administration (supporting); supervision (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). Lise E. Nigrovic: Methodology (supporting); project administration (supporting); supervision (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). Maria Y. Kwok: Methodology (supporting); project administration (supporting); supervision (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). Cody S. Olsen: Formal analysis (supporting); validation (supporting); visualization (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). T. Charles Casper: Formal analysis (supporting); visualization (supporting); writing – review and editing (supporting). Nicole S. Glaser: Conceptualization (lead); data curation (lead); formal analysis (supporting); funding acquisition (lead); investigation (lead); methodology (lead); project administration (lead); writing – original draft (supporting).
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTSWe thank Marci Fjelstad and Amy Watson from the PECARN Data Coordinating Center for their assistance; the research coordinators in PECARN, without whom this study would not have been possible; the clinicians in PECARN who enrolled children in this study; the members of the data and safety monitoring board (Roger Lewis, M.D., Ph.D., Jeffrey Blumer, M.D., Ph.D., Andrew Bremer, M.D., Ph.D., Thomas Cook, Ph.D., and Beth Slomine, Ph.D., A.B.P.P.); and the members of the study outcome adjudication committee (Kathleen Meert, M.D., Jerry Zimmerman, M.D., Ph.D., and Robert Hickey, M.D.).
FUNDING INFORMATIONThis study was supported by grant U01HD062417 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development. This project was also supported in part by the Health Resources and Services Administration (HRSA), Maternal and Child Health Bureau (MCHB), Emergency Medical Services for Children (EMSC) Network Development Demonstration Program under cooperative agreement number U03MC00008, U03MC00001, U03MC00003, U03MC00006, U03MC00007, U03MC22684 and U03MC22685. This information or content and conclusions are those of the authors and should not be construed as the official position or policy of, nor should any endorsements be inferred by HRSA, HHS or the US Government. The funding agencies had no role in the study design and conduct.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENTNone of the authors have any financial arrangements that represent conflicts of interest related to the study.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENTThe data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2023. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Introduction
Young children with type 1 diabetes (T1D) may be at particularly high risk of cognitive decline following diabetic ketoacidosis (DKA). However, studies of cognitive functioning in T1D typically examine school-age children. The goal of this study was to examine whether a single experience of DKA is associated with lower cognitive functioning in young children. We found that recently diagnosed 3- to 5-year-olds who experienced one DKA episode, regardless of its severity, exhibited lower IQ scores than those with no DKA exposure.
Methods
We prospectively enrolled 46 3- to 5-year-old children, who presented with DKA at the onset of T1D, in a randomized multi-site clinical trial evaluating intravenous fluid protocols for DKA treatment. DKA was moderate/severe in 22 children and mild in 24 children. Neurocognitive function was assessed once 2–6 months after the DKA episode. A comparison group of 27 children with T1D, but no DKA exposure, was also assessed. Patient groups were matched for age and T1D duration at the time of neurocognitive testing.
Results
Children who experienced DKA, regardless of its severity, exhibited significantly lower IQ scores than children who did not experience DKA,
Conclusions
A single DKA episode is associated with lower IQ scores soon after exposure to DKA in young children.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details

1 Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis, Davis, California, USA; Center for Mind and Brain, University of California, Davis, Davis, California, USA
2 Department of Emergency Medicine, University of California Davis Health, University of California Davis, School of Medicine, Davis, California, USA; Department of Pediatrics, University of California Davis Health, University of California Davis, School of Medicine, Davis, California, USA
3 Division of Emergency Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, The Colorado Children's Hospital, University of Colorado-Denver School of Medicine, Aurora, Colorado, USA
4 Division of Emergency Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, Perelman School of Medicine at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
5 Department of Pediatrics, University of Utah School of Medicine, Salt Lake City, Utah, USA
6 Division of Emergency Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, Nationwide Children's Hospital, The Ohio State University School of Medicine, Columbus, Ohio, USA
7 Departments of Emergency Medicine and Pediatrics, Rhode Island Hospital, The Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island, USA
8 Division of Emergency Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, St. Louis Children's Hospital, Washington University School of Medicine in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri, USA
9 Division of Emergency Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, Children's National Medical Center, The George Washington School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Washington, District of Columbia, USA
10 Division of Emergency Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, Ann and Robert H. Lurie Children's Hospital of Chicago, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA
11 Department of Emergency Medicine, University of California Davis Health, University of California Davis, School of Medicine, Davis, California, USA
12 Division of Emergency Medicine, Nemours/A.I. DuPont Hospital for Children, Sidney Kimmel Medical College at Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
13 Division of Emergency Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, Texas Children's Hospital, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA
14 Division of Emergency Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, Boston Children's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
15 Division of Emergency Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, New York Presbyterian Morgan Stanley Children's Hospital, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York City, New York, USA
16 Department of Pediatrics, University of California Davis Health, University of California Davis, School of Medicine, Davis, California, USA