It appears you don't have support to open PDFs in this web browser. To view this file, Open with your PDF reader
Abstract
Theoretical and empirical accounts suggest that adolescence is associated with heightened reward learning and impulsivity. Experimental tasks and computational models that can dissociate reward learning from the tendency to initiate actions impulsively (action initiation bias) are thus critical to characterise the mechanisms that drive developmental differences. However, existing work has rarely quantified both learning ability and action initiation, or it has relied on small samples. Here, using computational modelling of a learning task collected from a large sample (N = 742, 9-18 years, 11 countries), we test differences in reward and punishment learning and action initiation from childhood to adolescence. Computational modelling reveals that whilst punishment learning rates increase with age, reward learning remains stable. In parallel, action initiation biases decrease with age. Results are similar when considering pubertal stage instead of chronological age. We conclude that heightened reward responsivity in adolescence can reflect differences in action initiation rather than enhanced reward learning.
Adolescence is often associated with heightened reward learning and impulsivity. Here the authors show in 742 people aged 9-18 that reward learning in fact remains stable with age, whilst punishment learning increases and action initiation decreases.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details




1 University of Birmingham, Centre for Human Brain Health, School of Psychology, Birmingham, UK (GRID:grid.6572.6) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7486)
2 Radboud University, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (GRID:grid.5590.9) (ISNI:0000000122931605)
3 RWTH Aachen University, Child Neuropsychology Section, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Aachen, Germany (GRID:grid.1957.a) (ISNI:0000 0001 0728 696X); Faculty of Medicine, TU, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Dresden, Germany (GRID:grid.4488.0) (ISNI:0000 0001 2111 7257)
4 University of Oxford, Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford, UK (GRID:grid.4991.5) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8948); University of Oxford, Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, Oxford, UK (GRID:grid.4991.5) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8948)
5 University of Birmingham, Centre for Human Brain Health, School of Psychology, Birmingham, UK (GRID:grid.6572.6) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7486); University of Birmingham, Institute for Mental Health, School of Psychology, Birmingham, UK (GRID:grid.6572.6) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7486)
6 National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Department of Psychiatry, Medical School, Athens, Greece (GRID:grid.5216.0) (ISNI:0000 0001 2155 0800)
7 Szeged University, Faculty of Medicine, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of the Child Health Center, Szeged, Hungary (GRID:grid.9008.1) (ISNI:0000 0001 1016 9625)
8 University of Bath, Department of Psychology, Bath, UK (GRID:grid.7340.0) (ISNI:0000 0001 2162 1699)
9 Basurto University Hospital, Bilbao, Spain (GRID:grid.414269.c) (ISNI:0000 0001 0667 6181)
10 RWTH Aachen University, Child Neuropsychology Section, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Aachen, Germany (GRID:grid.1957.a) (ISNI:0000 0001 0728 696X)
11 University Hospital Mutua Terrassa, Barcelona, Spain (GRID:grid.414875.b) (ISNI:0000 0004 1794 4956)
12 RWTH Aachen University, Child Neuropsychology Section, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Aachen, Germany (GRID:grid.1957.a) (ISNI:0000 0001 0728 696X); JARA-Brain Institute II, Molecular Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, RWTH Aachen and Research Centre Jülich, Jülich, Germany (GRID:grid.8385.6) (ISNI:0000 0001 2297 375X)
13 VU University Medical Center, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Amsterdam, Netherlands (GRID:grid.16872.3a) (ISNI:0000 0004 0435 165X)
14 Psychiatric University Hospital, University of Basel, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Basel, Switzerland (GRID:grid.6612.3) (ISNI:0000 0004 1937 0642)
15 University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Frankfurt am Main, Germany (GRID:grid.411088.4) (ISNI:0000 0004 0578 8220)
16 University of Birmingham, Centre for Human Brain Health, School of Psychology, Birmingham, UK (GRID:grid.6572.6) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7486); University of Oxford, Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford, UK (GRID:grid.4991.5) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8948); University of Oxford, Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, Oxford, UK (GRID:grid.4991.5) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8948); University of Birmingham, Institute for Mental Health, School of Psychology, Birmingham, UK (GRID:grid.6572.6) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7486)