1. Introduction
The Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD) theory states that an individual’s nutritional status during critical periods of life (in utero and infancy) may impact their health status later in life [1]. Environmental factors, particularly nutritional factors, are recognized as essential influences on neurodevelopment in early life [2,3,4]. A growing body of evidence suggests that maternal nutrition during pregnancy may affect offspring’s cognitive development and behavior [5,6,7].
Most previous researchers focusing on maternal nutrition and children’s neurodevelopment have focused on the effects of a single nutrient or food group in pregnant women. For instance, maternal deficiencies in nutrients such as iron or iodine not only affect fetal development but also cause damage to children’s intelligence and cognition [8]. Folic acid supplementation during pregnancy helps prevent neural tube defects and has been shown to improve cognitive development [9]. However, the daily diet of individuals in the modern world often consists of various foods or nutrients, and analytical methods based on a single food or nutrient need to consider the interactions between different types of foods or nutrients. Dietary patterns provide a more comprehensive picture of the nutritional status of women during pregnancy.
The potential influence of maternal dietary patterns during pregnancy on the child’s neurodevelopment has been studied less, and the results vary depending on the difference in how the dietary pattern is defined. In the ALSPAC cohort study by Freitas-Vilela et al., children born of pregnant women in the “vegetables and fruits” group had higher intelligence quotient (IQ) scores compared to the “meat and potatoes” and “white bread and coffee” groups [10]. This is the first study to describe the association between food clusters during pregnancy and childhood IQ. A study of a birth cohort in Jiangsu province, China, showed that the “fish, shrimp, vegetables and algae” dietary pattern of pregnant women during pregnancy may influence the level of neurodevelopment of the offspring through the modulation of downstream metabolites. Concerning women with high scores on this dietary pattern, their infants showed a reduced risk of cognitive and gross motor developmental delays [11]. Maternal adherence to the Mediterranean dietary pattern (MD) during pregnancy was reported to be associated with a decreased risk of neurodevelopmental deficits in their children [12]. Conversely, the higher a woman’s score on unhealthy dietary patterns during pregnancy, the higher the risk of adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes in her offspring. The Generation R study showed that high adherence to the traditional Dutch diet during early pregnancy was associated with an increased risk of externalizing problems in children [13]. In addition, the offspring of mothers following ‘low health’ or ‘high Western’ dietary patterns were predisposed to symptoms of hyperactivity–inattention [14].
Several studies have modeled longitudinal dietary trajectories across the antenatal and postnatal periods [15,16]. These investigations have assessed the stability of diet over time by converting the continuous dietary indicator into an ordered categorical variable at each assessment point [16] or applied latent class transition analysis [15]. However, even less is known about the association between dietary trajectories during pregnancy and child development.
So far, reports on the association between maternal diet quality during pregnancy and childhood development in low- and middle-income countries are lacking. In this study, based on a China birth cohort, we prospectively collected food frequency data using the Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ) to investigate the association between dietary patterns in the three trimesters of pregnancy and dietary trajectories during pregnancy and children’s neurodevelopment.
2. Methods and Materials
2.1. Study Population
This study was based on the China National Birth Cohort (CNBC). Couples in early pregnancy who came for their first antenatal checkup at Ma’anshan Maternal and Child Health Center in Anhui Province, China, from May 2017 to September 2018 were recruited. The inclusion criteria were as follows: ① maternal age ≥ 18 years; ② spontaneous pregnancy; ③ planning to have routine obstetric checkups and delivery at the Center; ④ willingness to be followed up. Notably, women were included in the study regardless of their parity. Initially, a total of 1508 families were enrolled in the cohort. After excluding adverse pregnancy outcomes and twin births, 1423 families remained for inclusion in the study. The flowchart of the participants is shown in Figure 1.
2.2. Dietary Assessment
The Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ) was used to collect data on the frequency of 26 foods in the first, second, and third trimesters, respectively [17]. In the questionnaire, the pregnant woman reported how often she currently consumed a certain type of food: (1) hardly at all, (2) 1–3 times per month, (3) 1–3 times per week, (4) 4–6 times per week (5) every day. A score of 0, 1, 2, 3, and 4 was assigned to each of the five options, respectively.
To fill in the missing data on the FFQ, we utilized the SPSS Version 26.0 to perform multiple interpolations (MIs) on the data in each trimester of pregnancy, which was a flexible method to impute missing values [18,19]. It compensated for the shortcomings of single interpolation by modeling the likelihood distribution of the missing data so as to reduce the impact of the missing data on the modeling results without distorting them. Finally, five interpolated datasets were generated, and we chose the dataset with the largest Cronbach’s alpha coefficient (Alpha) for analysis.
2.3. Food Pattern Determination
From the dietary data collected by the FFQ, we excluded three categories of food, namely spicy food, puffed food, and candy sweets, because they are not part of the typical diet in daily life, and the frequency of their consumption is low. The remaining 24 food items were retained for analysis.
We used principal component analysis to extract dietary patterns during three trimesters of pregnancy. The applicability of factor analysis was first determined based on the KMO statistic and the Bardett spherical test. The KMO values of the first, second, and third trimesters of pregnancy were all greater than 0.5 (0.774, 0.717, and 0.706, respectively), and the p-values of Bardett’s spherical test were all less than 0.05, which indicated that the data were able to be processed by factor analysis. Three dietary patterns were identified in each period based on the gravel plot characteristic root (λ > 1.4). The greater the absolute value of the factor loading of a single food entry, the greater the contribution of that food to the principal components. Food entries with an absolute value of the factor loading greater than 0.3 were retained in this study (Supplementary Table S1). Individual dietary patterns were named according to the food components with high contributions to the dietary pattern, thus simplifying the factor structure and increasing interpretability. For each food item in a certain dietary pattern, a score was obtained by multiplying the standardized values of an individual food item’s dietary frequency with the weight of the corresponding principal component (i.e., the absolute value of the factor loading). The overall score of each dietary pattern was the sum of all scores of the food items that were involved in this pattern. After completing the extraction of dietary patterns, each woman would receive a principal component score for each pattern in three trimesters of pregnancy. High scores indicated that the woman favored the dietary pattern.
Three dietary patterns extracted in the first trimester were protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary patterns, low-iron dietary patterns, and pasta as the staple food dietary pattern. Three patterns in the second trimester were the protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern, iron-rich dietary pattern, and low-iron dietary pattern, and the three patterns in the third trimester were the protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern, tubers, fruits, baked food dietary pattern, and low-iron dietary pattern. Protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary patterns and low-iron dietary patterns were involved in all three trimesters.
2.4. Evaluation of Children’s Neurodevelopment
The Ages and Stages Questionnaire, Third Edition, (ASQ-3) was adopted to evaluate children’s neurodevelopment at the age of 36 months. It is a widely used and validated parent-completed questionnaire for screening children’s communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and personal–social delays [20]. For each item, there were three responses to choose from: “yes”, “sometimes”, and “not yet”, which were scored as 10, 5, and 0. An overall domain of score was then obtained from the sum of all items in each domain. The maximum total scores were then obtained from the sum of all items, with a maximum score of 60. The higher the score, the better the skills and abilities in the given domain. According to the ASQ system of Shanghai Zhangyuan Information Technology Company, the criteria for children who are well-developed in different dimensions were scores of “>50” in the communication and gross motor dimension. “>35” in the fine motor dimension, and scores of “>45” in the problem-solving dimension and personal–social dimension. Otherwise, the children were considered to have developmental delays. The scale had high reliability and validity, with test–retest reliability of 0.82–0.94, internal reliability of 0.75–0.92, and calibrated validity of 0.76–0.94 [21].
2.5. Covariates
Potential confounders were identified by the relevant literature and directed acyclic graphs (DAG) [22], including maternal age, pre-pregnancy BMI, maternal occupation before pregnancy, family income, education level, parity, maternal smoking, alcohol consumption, anxiety, depression, and pregnancy complications (Supplementary Figure S1).
At the first antenatal visit, information on maternal age, occupation before pregnancy, family income, education level, maternal smoking, and maternal alcohol consumption were collected through a questionnaire. Furthermore, the obstetricians calculated pre-pregnancy BMI by women’s weight and height. Information on maternal pregnancy complications (gestational hypertension/diabetes) and parity were extracted from the medical notes.
Data on the psychosocial status of pregnant women were assessed using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) and Self-Anxiety Rating Scale (SAS). The CES-D consisted of 20 questions to screen for participants with symptoms of depression [23]. It was rated on a scale of 0–3, with a total score ranging from 0 to 60. The higher the score, the higher the level of depression. It has been widely used in pregnant women with a standard cut-off value of ≥16. The SAS consisted of 20 items reflecting subjective anxiety, each scored on a 4-point scale based on the frequency of symptoms [24]. Women with an SAS score of at least 40 were considered to have anxiety symptoms.
Information regarding infants’ gestational week, birth weight, exclusive breastfeeding and complementary food within six months were collected for sensitivity analysis. Information on the gestational week and birth weight were extracted from medical records. Exclusive breastfeeding within six months was collected from questionnaires completed by primary caregivers.
2.6. Statistical Analysis
Linear regression models were developed to analyze the associations between the five dietary pattern scores and children’s neurodevelopment during the three trimesters of pregnancy.
Group-based trajectory modeling (GBTM) was used to fit maternal dietary pattern score trajectories according to the individual principal component scores for each dietary pattern across the three trimesters of pregnancy using Stata 15.0 software [25,26]. As the ‘protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern’ and the ‘low-iron dietary pattern’ were the two dietary patterns consistently presented across all trimesters of pregnancy, the trajectories of these two patterns were fitted. The maximum likelihood method was used for parameter estimation and model fitting. Firstly, we explored two to five categories of potential trajectory models, considering existing relevant studies on dietary pattern score trajectories during pregnancy [27,28]. Based on the recommendations [26,29], the detailed selection criteria for the best-fitting model for trajectories were as follows: (1) Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC). The closer the BIC value was to 0, the better the model fit; (2) a higher value of entropy indicated how well the classification distinguishes from one group to another; (3) the average posterior probability was required to be >0.7, and (4) each trajectory contained at least 5% of the participants. Finally, we fitted two categories of protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern scores (moderate trajectories and high trajectories) throughout pregnancy (Figure 2). Additionally, we also fitted three categories of low-iron dietary pattern scores (low trajectories, moderate trajectories, and high trajectories) throughout pregnancy (Figure 3).
Linear regression models analyzed the association between protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary trajectories and low-iron dietary trajectories during pregnancy and children’s neurodevelopment.
Two sensitivity analyses were conducted in this study; Firstly, studies have shown that maternal Mediterranean dietary patterns might have a positive impact on perinatal outcomes, such as being premature and having a low birth weight [30], which in turn were associated with children’s neurodevelopment [31]. Birth weight and gestational age might be the potential mediators in the association between maternal dietary patterns during pregnancy and children’s neurodevelopment. Therefore, we further adjusted for birth weight z-scores in addition to the main analysis.
Second, we recognized that breastfeeding and complementary feeding were not mediating variables but rather were significant postnatal factors influencing children’s neurodevelopment [32,33,34]. As these variables were not regarded as relating to maternal dietary patterns, they served as precision variables and were further adjusted.
The software SPSS Version 26.0 and Stata Version 15.0 were used and a p-value of <0.05 was considered statistically significant.
3. Results
3.1. Baseline Characteristics of Participants
The overall characteristics of the 1423 participants are shown in Table 1. The mean age of the pregnant women included in the study was 28.7 years. The majority of mothers had a body mass index within the normal range. Most of the pregnant women did not smoke or drink alcohol. The median infant’s birth weight was 3353 g, and the average gestational week was 38.9 weeks.
3.2. Association between Dietary Pattern Scores during Pregnancy and Children’s Neurodevelopment
The linear association β (95% CI) between the first, second, and third trimesters’ dietary pattern scores and neurodevelopment scores of 36-month-olds is shown in Table 2 and Figure 4.
In any of the three trimesters of pregnancy, women with high protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern scores had children with better gross motor abilities (β = 0.059, 95% CI: 0.010, 0.108); (β = 0.068, 95% CI: 0.017, 0.119); (β = 0.063, 95% CI: 0.012, 0.114). During the first and second trimesters of pregnancy, women who followed a protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern had children with higher total developmental scores (β = 0.053, 95% CI: 0.006, 0.100); (β = 0.049, 95% CI: 0.001, 0.098), particularly in problem-solving abilities (β = 0.051, 95% CI: 0.004, 0.098); (β = 0.059, 95% CI: 0.010, 0.109). In the first trimester, there was a positive association between the protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern and communication scores (β = 0.051, 95% CI: 0.003, 0.098).
For iron dietary patterns, it was observed that women with low-iron dietary patterns had children with poorer neurodevelopment (first trimester: lower problem-solving scores (β = −0.069, 95% CI: −0.133, −0.006); second and third trimester: lower gross motor scores (β = −0.095, 95% CI: −0.167, −0.023); (β = −0.072, 95% CI: −0.140, −0.004); third trimester: lower communication (β = −0.068, 95% CI: −0.134, −0.003)).
In the second trimester, women with iron-rich dietary pattern scores had children with high total ASQ scores and communication and problem-solving scores (β = 0.078, 95% CI: 0.018, 0.138); (β = 0.088, 95% CI: 0.027, 0.148); and (β = 0.071, 95% CI: 0.011, 0.131).
3.3. Characteristics of the Distinct Protein- and Micronutrient-Rich Dietary Pattern Trajectory Groups
For the protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern trajectory, the majority (65.3%) of women followed a “moderate protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern trajectory.” This means this group of women had a moderate protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern score at each time point. Another 34.7% of women followed a “high protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern trajectory”, meaning they had a high protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern score at each period (Figure 2).
3.4. Characteristics of the Distinct Low-Iron Dietary Pattern Trajectory Groups
For the low-iron dietary pattern trajectory, the majority (57.4%) of women followed a “moderate” low-iron dietary pattern trajectory and 37.4% of women followed a “high in early pregnancy” low-iron dietary pattern trajectory. Furthermore, only 5.2% of women followed a “persistently increased” low-iron dietary pattern trajectory (Figure 3).
3.5. Associations between Trajectories of Women’s Protein- and Micronutrient-Rich Dietary Pattern Scores in the Three Periods of Pregnancy and Children’s Neurodevelopment
As shown in Table 3, after adjusting for potential confounders, children of mothers who had a protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern high-trajectory mothers had a high score of total ASQ (β = 5.107, 95% CI: 0.464, 9.750). Their children had relatively higher gross motor (β = 1.279, 95% CI: 0.289, 2.269) and problem-solving (β = 1.478, 95% CI: 0.221, 2.735) abilities.
3.6. Associations between Trajectories of Women’s Low-Iron Dietary Pattern Scores in the Three Periods of Pregnancy and Children’s Neurodevelopment
After adjusting for potential confounders, no statistically significant associations existed between the trajectories of women’s low-iron dietary pattern scores in the three periods of pregnancy and children’s neurodevelopment (Table 3).
3.7. Sensitivity Analyses
Sensitivity analyses did not fundamentally change the main results (Supplementary Tables S2 and S3).
4. Discussion
Our findings support that mothers’ adherence to a protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern during pregnancy was a protective factor for children’s neurodevelopment, and adherence to a low-iron dietary pattern was a risk factor for children’s neurodevelopment. Further analyses showed that children of mothers who had a high trajectory of protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary patterns had a lower risk of abnormal neurobehavioral development than those with a moderate trajectory.
Gestation is a critical window for development, and mothers should pay more attention to a proper diet during pregnancy to ensure appropriate and adequate nutrient intake. Nutrient deficiencies early in life, despite adequate supplementation later in life, still pose a risk of dysfunction and altered developmental trajectories in adults [35]. Unlike most previous studies that have explored the effects of intake of single or multiple nutrients on neurodevelopment, our results are studies of dietary patterns based on good food sources of a particular nutrient or nutrients. Dietary pattern studies can translate nutrient intake into food services and provide applicable dietary recommendations to the public.
We found that children of mothers who had protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary patterns during pregnancy had a lower risk of abnormal neurodevelopment. Protein is essential for maintaining human growth and development. Maternal protein malnutrition may lead to disrupted brain development in the offspring, with lasting effects on motor and cognitive function [36]. The Japanese Environment and Children Study also found that low protein intake in early pregnancy was associated with a higher risk of developmental delay in 3-year-old children [37]. In rodents, protein malnutrition in early life stages will alter neurogenesis, cell migration, differentiation, and plasticity [38,39,40,41]. The mechanisms involved may be maternal protein restriction, affecting neurotransmitters and hormone release in the hippocampus, cortex, and hypothalamus and neural stem cells’ (NSCs) ability to proliferate and differentiate [41,42,43,44]. In addition to protein, micronutrients such as iodine and iron and deficiencies of vitamin C, folic acid, and other B vitamins at all stages of pregnancy are detrimental to fetal neurodevelopment. Inadequate iodine intake during pregnancy affects thyroid hormone synthesis, which, in turn, affects neuronal proliferation and migration in the cortex and hippocampus of the offspring and impairs fetal brain development [45]. Severe iodine deficiency may lead to intellectual disability in children and is the most common cause of preventable brain damage worldwide [46]. Additionally, vitamin C is vital in fetal brain development during pregnancy. Prenatal vitamin C deficiency has been reported to severely impair hippocampal development and induce oxidative stress in the developing guinea pig brain [47]. The intake status of B vitamins, folic acid, vitamin B12, vitamin B6, and riboflavin may all affect the overall status of choline, which can have a direct impact on fetal neurodevelopment, both prenatally and postnatally, and may also have an effect on methylmercury toxicity [48]. Studies have shown that intake of folic acid and vitamin B12 during pregnancy can help prevent neural tube defects [49]. Our study highlighted that eating food groups containing both proteins and micronutrients throughout the pregnancy benefits the offspring’s neurodevelopmental behaviors. We acknowledge that while a balanced diet containing protein and micronutrients is beneficial for promoting fetal neurodevelopment, it is important to note that studies have suggested a potential risk associated with maternal high-protein diets during pregnancy, i.e., an increased risk of obesity and metabolic syndrome in offspring [50,51]. Therefore, while we encourage pregnant women to maintain a diverse diet that includes protein- and nutrient-rich foods, we also emphasize the importance of moderating protein intake to prevent overconsumption.
We also found that women’s adherence to a low-iron dietary pattern was negatively associated with children’s neurodevelopment. Iron deficiency and iron deficiency anemia are major nutritional disorders worldwide. Early iron deficiency (ID) was associated with altered brain function that supports active and passive cognitive control in childhood [52]. Good prenatal iron levels are essential for proper neurological development in children [53]. Iron is required for enzymes involved in specific brain functions, including myelin formation and synthesis of the neurotransmitters serotonin (tryptophan hydroxylase) and dopamine (tyrosine hydroxylase) and precursors of epinephrine and noradrenaline [54]. Animal experiments have found that the offspring of rats that followed an iron-deficient dietary pattern during pregnancy had less myelin formation in the subcortical white matter and hippocampal hairs in later life than those of rats who prenatally had an iron-sufficient dietary pattern, even though the pups were consumed postnatally on a diet containing sufficient iron [55]. Recent cohort studies have shown that maternal serum ferritin levels (ranging from 12 mg/L to 60 mg/L) and iron intake (ranging from 14.5 mg/day to 30.0 mg/day) are associated with higher scores regarding working memory and executive functioning in offspring, respectively [56]. Our discovery recommended that dietary iron intake be appropriately enhanced during pregnancy. Further studies are warranted to support the findings.
Our results showed that children of mothers who had a high trajectory of protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary patterns had a lower risk of abnormal neurobehavioral development than those with the moderate trajectory. GBTM has emerged as a focal point in recent research for modeling dietary trajectories during pregnancy. By leveraging longitudinal dietary intake data, researchers can employ GBTM to identify distinct dietary patterns and trends among different groups of pregnant women. The method’s advantage lies in its ability to uncover the diversity of dietary habits during pregnancy and identify potentially health-relevant food patterns. Recent studies have highlighted the significance of GBTM in understanding the impact of maternal dietary intake on the health of both pregnant women and their fetuses. However, studies on the association between diet trajectories during pregnancy and offspring neurodevelopment are scarce. To further examine changes in mothers’ dietary patterns during different periods of pregnancy and their associations with children’s neurobehavioral development, we constructed trajectories of the protein- and micronutrient-rich and the low-iron dietary patterns using repeated measures data. Women with moderate protein- and micronutrient-rich trajectories scored relatively high in the first stage, and women with high protein- and micronutrient-rich trajectories scored relatively low in the first stage. Our identified dietary patterns differed from other studies. However, the stability of the dietary pattern trajectories identified in our study is similar to studies reporting dietary changes over time in Chinese adults [57,58]. This approach deserves further replication to strengthen the evidence of dietary changes during pregnancy and their effects.
Our study has several strengths. One of the strengths of dietary pattern studies is the ability to translate nutrient intake into food supply and thus provide applicable dietary recommendations to the public. The results of this study support many of the previous results of many single foods or nutrients and children’s intellectual development. We used a fully validated FFQ to repeatedly collect data in three trimesters of pregnancy, allowing us to capture changes in dietary intake throughout pregnancy. In addition, many studies have begun to fit trajectories of dietary patterns during pregnancy based on data from repeated measures [27,28]. In contrast, almost no studies explore the effects of the trajectories of mothers’ dietary patterns during pregnancy on offspring neurodevelopment, which is the novelty of this paper. Finally, this study was based on a prospective birth cohort, and the data used in the analyses were obtained through a follow-up questionnaire that included exposure, outcome variables, and confounders, effectively reducing recall bias. Accuracy variables affecting neurodevelopment were also considered in sensitivity analyses to improve the accuracy of the findings.
There were, of course, some limitations in this study. We were unable to collect and control for all potential confounders; furthermore, we also did not collect some of the factors that may affect children’s neurodevelopment such as maternal sleeping status in pregnancy and infants’ feeding patterns [34,59]. Particularly, 36.8% infants had received complementary food before 6 months of age. Although further adjustment of this variable did not change the main findings, we still lacked detailed information on the complementary food that children had. This did not allow us to observe which kind of complementary food would or would not impact children’s neurodevelopment. In addition, the dietary patterns identified in this study were based on nutrient food sources and do not accurately represent the extent to which precise levels of intake of nutrients such as protein are associated with outcomes. Moreover, multiple methods should be used to comprehensively assess neurodevelopmental outcomes, such as the Bayley Scale of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition. Finally, this study is based on a cohort study in China, with limitations in extrapolating the results to people other than Chinese populations and slight limitations in inferring causality from observational data.
5. Conclusions
The study supports that following a protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern during pregnancy was positively associated with neurodevelopment in offspring. The risk of neurodevelopment abnormalities in the offspring increased when women followed a low-iron dietary pattern during pregnancy.
J.O.: writing original draft, methodology, formal analysis, review and editing. W.C.: investigation, methodology, formal analysis, review and editing. P.W.: investigation, methodology, formal analysis, review and editing. J.T.: resources, investigation, data curation. G.G.: resources, data curation, validation. S.Y.: resources, investigation, data curation, supervision, project administration. F.T.: conceptualization, resources, supervision, project administration, funding acquisition. K.H.: conceptualization, resources, supervision, project administration, funding acquisition. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Anhui Medical University (Number: 20160270, 30 March 2016).
Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.
The data are not publicly available due to privacy restrictions. If required, the data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author.
The authors are highly appreciative of Nanjing Medical University for their organization, administration, and support of the CNBC cohort platform. We thank all the women and children who participated in the birth cohort. We are also grateful for all researchers and medical workers involved in the study at the Ma’anshan Maternal and Child Health Care Center for their contributions.
The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.
Footnotes
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Figure 2. Trajectories of protein and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern score in pregnant women. The dashed lines indicated 95% confidence intervals of trajectories. The circles indicated protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern scores at corresponding observation point in each trajectory group.
Figure 3. Trajectories of low-iron dietary pattern score in pregnant women. The dashed lines indicated 95% confidence intervals of trajectories. The circles indicated low-iron dietary pattern scores at corresponding observation point in each trajectory group.
Figure 4. Linear regression between maternal dietary pattern scores during pregnancy and 36-month-old infants’ neurodevelopment [β (95% CI)].
Baseline characteristics of participants.
Characteristics | Distributions |
---|---|
Demographic characteristics | |
Maternal educational level [n (%)] | |
Junior high school or below | 8 (0.6) |
Senior middle school | 507 (35.6) |
Junior college or above | 908 (63.8) |
Annual household income (CNY one million) * [n (%)] | |
<5 | 192 (13.5) |
5~10 | 698 (49.1) |
10~20 | 412 (29.0) |
20~30 | 95 (6.7) |
>30 | 24 (1.7) |
Maternal characteristics | |
Maternal age, years (Mean ± SD) | 28.7 ± 4.0 |
Body mass index before pregnancy * [n (%)] | |
Underweight (BMI < 18.5) | 227 (16.0) |
Normal (18.5 ≤ BMI < 24.9) | 1028 (72.2) |
Overweight obesity (BMI ≥ 24.9) | 164 (11.5) |
Maternal occupation [n (%)] | |
Brain work | 637 (44.8) |
Manual work | 509 (35.8) |
Else | 88 (6.2) |
No work | 189 (13.3) |
Smoking during pregnancy * [n (%)] | |
Yes | 30 (2.1) |
No | 1389 (97.6) |
Alcohol during pregnancy * [n (%)] | |
Yes | 352 (24.7) |
No | 1067 (75.0) |
Hypertension during pregnancy * [n (%)] | |
Yes | 13 (0.9) |
No | 1302 (91.5) |
Diabetes mellitus during pregnancy * [n (%)] | |
Yes | 317 (22.3) |
No | 998 (70.1) |
Pregnancy anxiety * (Mean ± SD) | 39.5 ± 8.0 |
Pregnancy depression * (Mean ± SD) | 13.7 ± 7.9 |
Parity * (Mean ± SD) | |
0 | 11 (0.8) |
1 | 574 (40.3) |
2 | 306 (21.5) |
3 | 2 (0.1) |
Children’s characteristics | |
Gestational weeks * (Mean ± SD) | 38.9 ± 1.4 |
Neonatal weight (g) * (Mean ± SD) | 3353 ± 463 |
Exclusive breastfeeding for 6 months * [n (%)] | |
Yes | 633 (44.5) |
No | 603 (49.4) |
Complementary food within 6 months * [n (%)] | |
Yes | 524 (36.8) |
No | 845 (59.4) |
* Missing data: 2 in annual household income; 4 in body mass index before pregnancy, smoking, alcohol during pregnancy; 108 in hypertension, diabetes mellitus during pregnancy; 187 in exclusive breastfeeding for 6 months; 54 in complementary food within 6 months; 5 in pregnancy anxiety, depression; 111 in gestational weeks and neonatal weight; 530 in parity. Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; SD, standard deviation.
Linear regression analysis between maternal dietary pattern scores during pregnancy and 36-month-old infants’ neurodevelopment [β (95% CI)].
Maternal Dietary Pattern | Model | Total ASQ Score | Communication | Gross Motor | Fine Motor | Problem-Solving | Personal-Social |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
First trimester | |||||||
Protein- and micronutrient-rich | 1 | 0.035 | 0.041 | 0.021 | 0.015 | 0.033 | 0.021 |
2 | 0.053 | 0.051 | 0.059 | 0.009 | 0.051 | 0.039 | |
Low-iron | 1 | −0.042 | −0.056 | −0.033 | −0.011 | −0.062 | −0.003 |
2 | −0.038 | −0.056 | −0.035 | 0.016 | −0.069 | −0.022 | |
Pasta as the staple food | 1 | −0.028 | −0.033 | 0.006 | −0.021 | −0.009 | −0.035 |
2 | −0.041 | −0.076 | 0.055 | −0.029 | −0.025 | −0.052 | |
Second trimester | |||||||
Protein- and micronutrient-rich | 1 | 0.021 | 0.017 | 0.025 | 0.006 | 0.034 | 0.001 |
2 | 0.049 | 0.043 | 0.068 | 0.011 | 0.059 | 0.016 | |
Iron-rich | 1 | 0.056 | 0.076 | 0.036 | 0.010 | 0.057 | 0.039 |
2 | 0.078 | 0.088 | 0.061 | 0.035 | 0.071 | 0.039 | |
Low-iron | 1 | −0.032 | −0.048 | −0.049 | −0.014 | −0.001 | −0.017 |
2 | −0.069 | −0.050 | −0.095 | −0.047 | −0.023 | −0.039 | |
Third trimester | |||||||
Protein- and micronutrient-rich | 1 | 0.017 | 0.013 | 0.028 | 0.003 | 0.020 | 0.006 |
2 | 0.040 | 0.048 | 0.063 | 0.004 | 0.044 | 0.013 | |
Tubers; fruits; baked food | 1 | 0.004 | −0.020 | −0.022 | 0.004 | 0.028 | 0.011 |
2 | 0.014 | 0.016 | −0.019 | 0.006 | 0.047 | −0.002 | |
Low-iron | 1 | −0.037 | −0.055 | −0.039 | −0.028 | −0.014 | −0.004 |
2 | −0.062 | −0.068 | −0.072 | −0.049 | −0.031 | −0.009 |
Model 1: Unadjusted models; Model 2: Adjusting for the confounding factors such as maternal age, pre-pregnancy BMI, maternal occupation before pregnancy, family income, education level, maternal smoking, alcohol consumption, parity, anxiety, depression, and pregnancy complications. Bolding indicated p-values < 0.05.
Association between different trajectory groups of dietary pattern during pregnancy and 36-month-old children’s neurodevelopment [β (95% CI)].
Maternal Dietary Pattern Trajectories | Total ASQ Score | Communication | Gross Motor | Fine Motor | Problem-Solving | Personal-Social |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Protein- and micronutrient-rich | ||||||
Model 1 | 3.007 | 0.500 | 0.725 | 0.766 | 0.897 | 0.119 |
Model 2 | 5.107 | 0.939 | 1.279 | 0.994 | 1.478 | 0.417 |
Low-iron | ||||||
Model 1 | −2.063 | −0.040 | −0.334 | −0.882 | −0.478 | −0.329 |
Model 2 | −1.032 | −0.410 | −0.112 | −0.854 | 0.098 | 0.246 |
Model 1: Unadjusted models; Model 2: Adjusting for the confounding factors such as maternal age, pre-pregnancy BMI, maternal occupation before pregnancy, family income, education level, maternal smoking, alcohol consumption, parity, anxiety, depression, and pregnancy complications. Bolding indicated p-values < 0.05.
Supplementary Materials
The following supporting information can be downloaded at:
References
1. Barker, D.J.P. The origins of the developmental origins theory. J. Intern. Med.; 2007; 261, pp. 412-417. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2796.2007.01809.x] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17444880]
2. Prado, E.L.; Larson, L.M.; Cox, K.; Bettencourt, K.; Kubes, J.N.; Shankar, A.H. Do effects of early life interventions on linear growth correspond to effects on neurobehavioural development? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Glob. Health; 2019; 7, pp. e1398-e1413. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S2214-109X(19)30361-4] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31537370]
3. Fleming, T.P.; Watkins, A.J.; Velazquez, M.A.; Mathers, J.C.; Prentice, A.M.; Stephenson, J.; Barker, M.; Saffery, R.; Yajnik, C.S.; Eckert, J.J. et al. Origins of lifetime health around the time of conception: Causes and consequences. Lancet; 2018; 391, pp. 1842-1852. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30312-X] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29673874]
4. Cunha-Rodrigues, M.; Rosário, R.; Duarte, A.; Silva, M.J.; Augusto, C.; Rodrigues, M.; Padrão, P.; Moreira, P. Neurodevelopment and Dietary Intake in Toddlers—A Cross-Sectional Study from the Healthy Children 2021 Project. Nutrients; 2023; 15, 5105. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/nu15245105] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38140364]
5. Koletzko, B.; Brands, B.; Demmelmair, H. The Early Nutrition Programming Project (EARNEST): 5 y of successful multidisciplinary collaborative research. Am. J. Clin. Nutr.; 2011; 94, pp. S1749-S1753. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.110.000471] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21974891]
6. Cortés-Albornoz, M.C.; García-Guáqueta, D.P.; Velez-van-Meerbeke, A.; Talero-Gutiérrez, C. Maternal Nutrition and Neurodevelopment: A Scoping Review. Nutrients; 2021; 13, 3530. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/nu13103530] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34684531]
7. Basak, S.; Mallick, R.; Duttaroy, A.K. Maternal Docosahexaenoic Acid Status during Pregnancy and Its Impact on Infant Neurodevelopment. Nutrients; 2020; 12, 3615. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/nu12123615]
8. Gernand, A.D.; Schulze, K.J.; Stewart, C.P.; West, K.P.; Christian, P. Micronutrient deficiencies in pregnancy worldwide: Health effects and prevention. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol.; 2016; 12, pp. 274-289. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrendo.2016.37]
9. Pitkin, R.M. Folate and neural tube defects. Am. J. Clin. Nutr.; 2007; 85, pp. 285S-288S. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/85.1.285S]
10. Freitas-Vilela, A.A.; Pearson, R.M.; Emmett, P.; Heron, J.; Smith, A.D.A.C.; Emond, A.; Hibbeln, J.R.; Castro, M.B.T.; Kac, G. Maternal dietary patterns during pregnancy and intelligence quotients in the offspring at 8 years of age: Findings from the ALSPAC cohort. Matern. Child Nutr.; 2017; 14, e12431. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/mcn.12431]
11. Lv, S.; Qin, R.; Jiang, Y.; Lv, H.; Lu, Q.; Tao, S.; Huang, L.; Liu, C.; Xu, X.; Wang, Q. et al. Association of Maternal Dietary Patterns during Gestation and Offspring Neurodevelopment. Nutrients; 2022; 14, 730. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/nu14040730] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35215380]
12. Dai, F.; Wang, P.; Li, Q.; Zhang, L.; Yu, L.; Wu, L.; Tao, R.; Zhu, P. Mediterranean diet during pregnancy and infant neurodevelopment: A prospective birth cohort study. Front. Nutr.; 2023; 9, 1078481. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnut.2022.1078481] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36726814]
13. Steenweg-de Graaff, J.; Tiemeier, H.; Steegers-Theunissen, R.P.M.; Hofman, A.; Jaddoe, V.W.V.; Verhulst, F.C.; Roza, S.J. Maternal dietary patterns during pregnancy and child internalising and externalising problems. The Generation R Study. Clin. Nutr.; 2014; 33, pp. 115-121. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clnu.2013.03.002] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23541912]
14. Galera, C.; Heude, B.; Forhan, A.; Bernard, J.Y.; Peyre, H.; Van Der Waerden, J.; Pryor, L.; Bouvard, M.P.; Melchior, M.; Lioret, S. et al. Prenatal diet and children’s trajectories of hyperactivity–inattention and conduct problems from 3 to 8 years: The EDEN mother–child cohort. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry; 2018; 59, pp. 1003-1011. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12898]
15. Sotres-Alvarez, D.; Herring, A.H.; Siega-Riz, A.-M. Latent Transition Models to Study Women’s Changing of Dietary Patterns from Pregnancy to 1 Year Postpartum. Am. J. Epidemiol.; 2013; 177, pp. 852-861. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1093/aje/kws303]
16. Lee, Y.Q.; Colega, M.; Sugianto, R.; Lai, J.S.; Godfrey, K.M.; Tan, K.H.; Shek, L.P.C.; Loy, S.L.; Müller-Riemenschneider, F.; Padmapriya, N. et al. Tracking of dietary patterns between pregnancy and 6 years post-pregnancy in a multiethnic Asian cohort: The Growing Up in Singapore Towards healthy Outcomes (GUSTO) study. Eur. J. Nutr.; 2022; 61, pp. 985-1001. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00394-021-02703-z]
17. Hu, F.B.; Rimm, E.; Smith-Warner, S.A.; Feskanich, D.; Stampfer, M.J.; Ascherio, A.; Laura, S.; Walter C, W. Reproducibility and validity of dietary patterns assessed with a food-frequency questionnaire. Am. J. Clin. Nutr.; 1999; 69, pp. 243-249. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/69.2.243] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9989687]
18. Hayati Rezvan, P.; Lee, K.J.; Simpson, J.A. The rise of multiple imputation: A review of the reporting and implementation of the method in medical research. BMC Med. Res. Methodol.; 2015; 15, 30. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12874-015-0022-1]
19. Sterne, J.A.C.; White, I.R.; Carlin, J.B.; Spratt, M.; Royston, P.; Kenward, M.G.; Wood, A.M.; Carpenter, J.R. Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: Potential and pitfalls. BMJ; 2009; 338, b2393. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b2393]
20. Kerstjens, J.M.; Bos, A.F. Support for the global feasibility of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire as developmental screener. Early Hum. Dev.; 2009; 85, pp. 443-447. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2009.03.001]
21. Manti, F.; Giovannone, F.; Ciancaleoni, M.; De Vita, G.; Fioriello, F.; Gigliotti, F.; Sogos, C. Psychometric Properties and Validation of the Italian Version of Ages & Stages Questionnaires Third Edition. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health; 2023; 20, 5014. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20065014] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36981921]
22. Diemer, E.W.; Hudson, J.I.; Javaras, K.N. More (Adjustment) Is Not Always Better: How Directed Acyclic Graphs Can Help Researchers Decide Which Covariates to Include in Models for the Causal Relationship between an Exposure and an Outcome in Observational Research. Psychother. Psychosom.; 2021; 90, pp. 289-298. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000517104] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34252900]
23. Cosco, T.D.; Lachance, C.C.; Blodgett, J.M.; Stubbs, B.; Co, M.; Veronese, N.; Wu, Y.T.; Prina, A.M. Latent structure of the Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) in older adult populations: A systematic review. Aging Ment. Health; 2020; 24, pp. 700-704. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2019.1566434] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30661386]
24. Zung, W.W.K. A Rating Instrument for Anxiety Disorders. Psychosomatics; 1971; 12, pp. 371-379. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0033-3182(71)71479-0] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/5172928]
25. Jones, B.L.; Nagin, D.S. A Note on a Stata Plugin for Estimating Group-based Trajectory Models. Sociol. Methods Res.; 2013; 42, pp. 608-613. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0049124113503141]
26. Nagin, D.S. Group-Based Trajectory Modeling: An Overview. Ann. Nutr. Metab.; 2014; 65, pp. 205-210. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000360229] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25413659]
27. Dalrymple, K.V.; Vogel, C.; Flynn, A.C.; Seed, P.T.; Godfrey, K.M.; Poston, L.; Inskip, H.M.; Crozier, S.R. Longitudinal dietary trajectories from pregnancy to 3 years post delivery in women with obesity: Relationships with adiposity. Obesity; 2023; 31, pp. 1159-1169. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1002/oby.23706] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36876599]
28. Pervin, S.; Emmett, P.; Northstone, K.; Townsend, N.; Fatima, Y.; Huda, M.M.; McIntyre, H.D.; Mamun, A.A. Trajectories of dietary patterns from pregnancy to 12 years post-pregnancy and associated maternal characteristics: Evidence from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Eur. J. Nutr.; 2023; 62, pp. 2763-2777. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00394-023-03185-x] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37294362]
29. Kim, S.-Y. Determining the Number of Latent Classes in Single- and Multiphase Growth Mixture Models. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J.; 2014; 21, pp. 263-279. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2014.882690]
30. Xu, J.; Wang, H.; Bian, J.; Xu, M.; Jiang, N.; Luo, W.; Zu, P.; Yin, W.; Zhu, P. Association between the Maternal Mediterranean Diet and Perinatal Outcomes: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Adv. Nutr.; 2024; 15, 100159. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.advnut.2023.100159]
31. Perumal, N.; Manji, K.P.; Darling, A.M.; Kisenge, R.R.; Kvestad, I.; Hysing, M.; Belinger, D.C.; Urassa, W.; Strand, T.A.; Duggan, C.P. et al. Gestational Age, Birth Weight, and Neurocognitive Development in Adolescents in Tanzania. J. Pediatr.; 2021; 236, pp. 194-203.e6. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2021.04.036] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33901518]
32. Koh, K. Maternal breastfeeding and children’s cognitive development. Soc. Sci. Med.; 2017; 187, pp. 101-108. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.06.012] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28672220]
33. Amaro, A.; Baptista, F.I.; Matafome, P. Programming of future generations during breastfeeding: The intricate relation between metabolic and neurodevelopment disorders. Life Sci.; 2022; 298, 120526. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lfs.2022.120526] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35367466]
34. Xiang, X.; Yang, T.; Chen, J.; Chen, L.; Dai, Y.; Zhang, J.; Li, L.; Jia, F.; Wu, L.; Hao, Y. et al. Association of feeding patterns in infancy with later autism symptoms and neurodevelopment: A national multicentre survey. BMC Psychiatry; 2023; 23, 174. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-04667-2] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36927467]
35. Georgieff, M.K.; Ramel, S.E.; Cusick, S.E. Nutritional influences on brain development. Acta Paediatr.; 2018; 107, pp. 1310-1321. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/apa.14287] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29468731]
36. Belluscio, L.M.; Berardino, B.G.; Ferroni, N.M.; Ceruti, J.M.; Cánepa, E.T. Early protein malnutrition negatively impacts physical growth and neurological reflexes and evokes anxiety and depressive-like behaviors. Physiol. Behav.; 2014; 129, pp. 237-254. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.02.051]
37. Miyake, K.; Mochizuki, K.; Kushima, M.; Shinohara, R.; Horiuchi, S.; Otawa, S.; Akiyama, Y.; Ooka, T.; Kojima, R.; Yokomichi, H. et al. Maternal protein intake in early pregnancy and child development at age 3 years. Pediatr. Res.; 2023; 94, pp. 392-399. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41390-022-02435-8] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36624288]
38. Godoy, M.A.D.; Souza, A.S.D.; Lobo, M.A.; Sampaio, O.V.K.; Moraes, L.; Baldanza, M.R.; Magri, T.P.R.; de Castro, J.P.S.W.; do Carmo, M.D.G.T.; Soares-Mota, M. et al. Effects of protein restriction during gestation and lactation on cell proliferation in the hippocampus and subventricular zone: Functional implications. Protein restriction alters hippocampal/SVZ cell proliferation. Brain Res.; 2013; 1496, pp. 10-27. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.10.047] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23123702]
39. Rotta, L.N.; Schmidt, A.P.; Souza, K.B.; Izquierdo, I.A.; Perry, M.L.S.; Souza, D.O. Effects of Undernutrition on Glutamatergic Parameters in Rat Brain. Neurochem. Res.; 2003; 28, pp. 1181-1186. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1024272227219] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12834257]
40. Gressens, P.; Muaku, S.M.; Besse, L.; Nsegbe, E.; Gallego, J.; Delpech, B.; Gaultier, C.; Evrard, P.; Ketelslegers, J.M.; Maiter, D. Maternal protein restriction early in rat pregnancy alters brain development in the progeny. Dev. Brain Res.; 1997; 103, pp. 21-35. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-3806(97)00109-0]
41. Mokler, D.J.; Torres, O.I.; Galler, J.R.; Morgane, P.J. Stress-induced changes in extracellular dopamine and serotonin in the medial prefrontal cortex and dorsal hippocampus of prenatally malnourished rats. Brain Res.; 2007; 1148, pp. 226-233. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.031] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17368432]
42. Kehoe, P.; Mallinson, K.; Bronzino, J.; McCormick, C.M. Effects of prenatal protein malnutrition and neonatal stress on CNS responsiveness. Dev. Brain Res.; 2001; 132, pp. 23-31. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-3806(01)00292-9] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11744104]
43. Resnick, O.; Morgane, P.J. Ontogeny of the levels of serotonin in various parts of the brain in severely protein malnourished rats. Brain Res.; 1984; 303, pp. 163-170. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(84)90224-5] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6203619]
44. Amarger, V.; Lecouillard, A.; Ancellet, L.; Grit, I.; Castellano, B.; Hulin, P.; Parnet, P. Protein Content and Methyl Donors in Maternal Diet Interact to Influence the Proliferation Rate and Cell Fate of Neural Stem Cells in Rat Hippocampus. Nutrients; 2014; 6, pp. 4200-4217. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/nu6104200] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25317634]
45. Glinoer, D. Iodine Nutrition Requirements During Pregnancy. Thyroid; 2006; 16, pp. 947-948. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1089/thy.2006.16.947] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17042676]
46. Becker, D.V.; Braverman, L.E.; Delange, F.; Dunn, J.T.; Franklyn, J.A.; Hollowell, J.G.; Lamm, S.H.; Mitchell, M.L.; Pearce, E.; Robbins, J. et al. Iodine Supplementation for Pregnancy and Lactation—United States and Canada: Recommendations of the American Thyroid Association. Thyroid; 2006; 16, pp. 949-951. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1089/thy.2006.16.949] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17042677]
47. Coker, S.J.; Smith-Díaz, C.C.; Dyson, R.M.; Vissers, M.C.M.; Berry, M.J. The Epigenetic Role of Vitamin C in Neurodevelopment. Int. J. Mol. Sci.; 2022; 23, 1208. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijms23031208] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35163133]
48. Strain, J.J.; Bonham, M.P.; Duffy, E.M.; Wallace, J.M.W.; Robson, P.J.; Clarkson, T.W.; Shamlaye, C. Nutrition and neurodevelopment: The search for candidate nutrients in the Seychelles Child Development Nutrition Study. Neurotoxicology; 2020; 81, pp. 300-306. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro.2020.09.021] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33741113]
49. Thilakaratne, R.; Lin, P.-I.D.; Rifas-Shiman, S.L.; Wright, R.O.; Hubbard, A.; Hivert, M.-F.; Bellinger, D.; Oken, E.; Cardenas, A. Mixtures of Metals and Micronutrients in Early Pregnancy and Cognition in Early and Mid-Childhood: Findings from the Project Viva Cohort. Environ. Health Perspect.; 2023; 131, 087008. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1289/EHP12016] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37585348]
50. Jahan-Mihan, A.; Rodriguez, J.; Christie, C.; Sadeghi, M.; Zerbe, T. The Role of Maternal Dietary Proteins in Development of Metabolic Syndrome in Offspring. Nutrients; 2015; 7, pp. 9185-9217. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/nu7115460]
51. Hallam, M.C.; Reimer, R.A. A maternal high-protein diet predisposes female offspring to increased fat mass in adulthood whereas a prebiotic fibre diet decreases fat mass in rats. Br. J. Nutr.; 2013; 110, pp. 1732-1741. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0007114513000998] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23561448]
52. Hua, M.; Shi, D.; Xu, W.; Zhu, L.; Hao, X.; Zhu, B.; Shu, Q.; Lozoff, B.; Geng, F.; Shao, J. Differentiation between fetal and postnatal iron deficiency in altering brain substrates of cognitive control in pre-adolescence. BMC Med.; 2023; 21, 167. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12916-023-02850-6] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37143078]
53. Iglesias, L.; Canals, J.; Arija, V. Effects of prenatal iron status on child neurodevelopment and behavior: A systematic review. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr.; 2018; 58, pp. 1604-1614. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10408398.2016.1274285] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28084782]
54. McCann, J.C.; Ames, B.N. An overview of evidence for a causal relation between iron deficiency during development and deficits in cognitive or behavioral function. Am. J. Clin. Nutr.; 2007; 85, pp. 931-945. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/85.4.931] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17413089]
55. Wu, L. Effect of perinatal iron deficiency on myelination and associated behaviors in rat pups. Behav. Brain Res.; 2008; 188, pp. 263-270. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.11.003] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18093669]
56. Arija, V.; Hernández-Martínez, C.; Tous, M.; Canals, J.; Guxens, M.; Fernández-Barrés, S.; Ibarluzea, J.; Babarro, I.; Soler-Blasco, R.; Llop, S. et al. Association of Iron Status and Intake During Pregnancy with Neuropsychological Outcomes in Children Aged 7 Years: The Prospective Birth Cohort Infancia y Medio Ambiente (INMA) Study. Nutrients; 2019; 11, 2999. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/nu11122999] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31817835]
57. Cao, Y.; Xu, X.; Shi, Z. Trajectories of Dietary Patterns, Sleep Duration, and Body Mass Index in China: A Population-Based Longitudinal Study from China Nutrition and Health Survey, 1991–2009. Nutrients; 2020; 12, 2245. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/nu12082245]
58. Wu, F.; Pahkala, K.; Juonala, M.; Rovio, S.P.; Sabin, M.A.; Rönnemaa, T.; Buscot, M.J.; Smith, K.J.; Männistö, S.; Jula, A. et al. Dietary Pattern Trajectories from Youth to Adulthood and Adult Risk of Impaired Fasting Glucose: A 31-year Cohort Study. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab.; 2021; 106, pp. e2078-e2086. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1210/clinem/dgab044]
59. Zhao, Q.; Peng, C.; Wu, X.; Chen, Y.; Wang, C.; You, Z. Maternal sleep deprivation inhibits hippocampal neurogenesis associated with inflammatory response in young offspring rats. Neurobiol. Dis.; 2014; 68, pp. 57-65. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nbd.2014.04.008]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Background: Research studies have showed that maternal diet may influence fetal neurodevelopment, but most studies have only assessed single nutrients or food groups. Objective: To investigate the impact of maternal prenatal dietary patterns during pregnancy on child neurodevelopment. Methods: Study participants were obtained from the China National Birth Cohort. The Ages and Stages Questionnaire, Third Edition, was used to assess children’s neurodevelopment at 36 months old. Maternal antenatal dietary data were collected over three trimesters using food frequency questionnaires. Five distinct maternal dietary patterns throughout pregnancy were identified by principal component analysis, namely protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary patterns, low-iron dietary patterns, pasta as the staple food dietary patterns, iron-rich dietary patterns, tubers, fruits, and baked food dietary patterns. Group-based trajectory modeling was performed for dietary patterns present in all three periods. Multiple linear regression models were used for statistical analysis. Results: Children of mothers who followed a high protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern trajectory during pregnancy presented better neurodevelopment, including higher gross motor and problem-solving scores. Furthermore, it was observed that children born of women with low-iron dietary patterns had poorer neurodevelopment. In detail, children born to mothers with a low-iron dietary pattern during the first trimester had lower problem-solving scores, while to those who were exposed to a low-iron dietary pattern in the second and third trimesters had lower gross motor scores. Additionally, children with mothers who had a low-iron dietary pattern in the third trimester had lower communication scores. Conclusions: A nutrition-balanced protein- and micronutrient-rich dietary pattern and adequate iron dietary pattern for mothers throughout pregnancy may be beneficial to children’s neurodevelopment.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details

1 School of Public Health, Anhui Medical University, No 81 Meishan Road, Hefei 230032, China;
2 School of Public Health, Anhui Medical University, No 81 Meishan Road, Hefei 230032, China;