1. Introduction
Awareness of the physical demands of exercise will often determine the strategies used to successfully complete it. Based on a combination of prior experience, ratings of perceived exertion (RPE), biomechanical and cardio-metabolic feedback while performing the exercise, participants will engage in a self-pacing strategy known as teleoanticipation [1]. By incorporating feedback and feed-forward mechanisms that encompass a variety of physiological systems, the brain will engage in selective neuromuscular regulation to efficiently and effectively complete the exercise [2]. However, predetermining an endpoint for the exercise is necessary to ensure optimal performance under the teleoanticipatory model [3]. In the absence of an endpoint, physiological resources will be conserved to account for extended or intensified exercise [4,5]. Conversely, knowledge of when an endpoint is approaching will allow this conservation to subside while subsequently increasing the work rate [6].
An individual’s RPE, which primarily drives teleoanticipation, is commonly measured using the Borg RPE scale [7]. The Borg RPE scale ranges from ‘6’ to ‘20’, with each score designed to correspond to a tenfold increase in heart rate (e.g., 8 equals 80 beats/min) [8]. In previous research, RPE scores have been used to drive accurate, individualized exercise prescriptions [9]. Although RPE scores are subjective, there is a general consensus that these are largely attributed to the intensity of the exercise being performed [10]. This trend has been demonstrated to be consistent with running and resistance training, as a higher RPE has been associated with higher-intensity exercise [11].
Utilizing deception to manipulate an exercise experience has become increasingly common. Whether through the incorporation of a digital competitor [12] or hypnotic manipulation [13], deception has demonstrated the potential to change how people perceive exercise. Recently, visual hill gradients observed through virtual reality while cycling have been shown to alter perceptions of effort and breathlessness [14]. In this particular study conducted by Finnegan et al., participants who observed a steeper hill gradient experienced a higher level of breathlessness and cycled with more power despite the resistance of the cycle ergometer remaining consistent throughout. However, a systematic review conducted by Jones et al. found that while a majority of research has investigated manipulations of the exercise endpoint, there is little research available examining the effects of exercise intensity deception [15]. Only two studies were identified within this review that evaluated the effects of pre-exercise intensity deception, both of which yielded no significant differences in selected outcome measures (RPE and heart rate, respectively) [16,17]. However, Jones et al. contend that the methodologies used in these investigations varied greatly, such as the way in which participants were deceived and differences in training experience of the participants. As a result, further study is required to ascertain if there is a relationship between the deception of exercise intensity and exercise variables.
Virtual reality (VR) has emerged as a viable tool for exercise studies involving deception. VR allows individuals to engage with simulated content in virtual environments outside of the real world [18]. More recently, as VR technology has improved, immersive virtual reality (IVR) has been developed to enhance the experience. IVR, unlike its predecessor, incorporates unique sensorimotor elements (i.e., visual, auditory, and/or tactile stimuli) that alter a user’s experience to make them feel as if what they are perceiving is their actual surroundings [19]. From feeling a touch on the shoulder induced by a small actuator to ostensibly depicted arteries and organs in a training simulation for surgeons, IVR can provide sensations that mirror reality. In a study conducted by North and North (2016), traditional VR and IVR apparatuses depicting the sights and sounds of an airplane interior and exterior were compared against one another [20]. Mean Likert Scale ratings of presence revealed a stronger sense of immersion within the IVR environment compared to the traditional VR environment. However, these authors noted that physiological measurements were not used within their study and could be used to further evaluate a participant’s sense of presence. Furthermore, IVR has the unique potential to produce “place illusion”, which refers to the sensation of “being there” in the surroundings [21]. Early study employing IVR during exercise has yielded encouraging implications, as it was found to yield lower RPE levels while improving satisfaction during biking [22,23]. These findings, while salient, highlight the need for more study employing deception tactics with IVR to determine if they extend to other training modalities, such as a treadmill.
To our knowledge, there are no studies that have investigated the efficacy of IVR in conjunction with deception strategies during treadmill exercise. This study investigated the performance of participants exposed to speed deception on heart rate and ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) during immersive virtual reality while walking on a treadmill. We hypothesized that participants in the IVR condition would report lower RPE scores and equally yield lower heart rates (HRs) in comparison to the non-IVR condition.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
Sixteen college-aged healthy and physically active volunteers (aged 19–23 years, eight females) were recruited from the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) campus. Exclusion criteria included the presence of any disorders that would preclude moderate- to high-intensity exercise participation and testing. All participants provided written informed consent at the beginning of the study. This research was approved by the UCLA Institutional Review Board and carried out in accordance with the ethical standards of the Helsinki Declaration.
2.2. Study Design
Using a single-blinded, crossover research design, participants were tested during two separate days, each consisting of three, 10 min treadmill walking trials separated by a 3 min rest period, with a 24 h washout period in between each day. All participants were randomly assigned to a participant number produced from a random number generator, which was operated by an investigator independent from recruitment and testing. Each participant number corresponded to a randomized sequence of testing for the two different days. The order for the speeds of the treadmill (3.3 mph, 3.5 mph, and 3.7 mph) were randomized, as well as the order for the IVR or non-IVR trials (i.e., IVR trials occurred on either the first day or second day of testing). The non-IVR trials used the same treadmill albeit with no head-mounted display (HMD). Therefore, while the non-IVR trials did not contain the same sensorimotor elements utilized with the HMD, the same speed order and testing sequence used in the IVR trials was applied for consistency. Participants were told (and deceived) that all trials occurred at 3.5 mph for blinding purposes. While one researcher was responsible for monitoring the participant during their rest periods in a nearby room without the treadmill, the other researcher was tasked with changing the speeds based on the assigned order. The first researcher, blinded to the speeds of the treadmill and their order, was solely responsible for assessing the RPE and HR data to ensure that a blinded approach was achieved. All testing was administered by trained research personnel under the supervision of the lab director in the UC Fit-Digital Health Exercise Physiology Research Laboratory at UCLA during the early spring of 2023 for a one-month period. Study participants were asked to refrain from completing moderate–vigorous physical activity at least 24 h prior to beginning the study until its conclusion. For baseline measures, body mass was measured on a calibrated medical scale (accuracy ± 0.1 kg), and height was determined using a precision stadiometer (Seca, Hanover, MD, USA; accuracy ± 0.01 m). Body fat percentage was measured using a validated octipolar, multi-frequency, multi-segmental bioelectrical impedance analyzer (InBody Co., Seoul, Republic of Korea) [24]. The instrument measured resistance and reactance using proprietary algorithms. To ensure accuracy, participants adhered to standard pre-measurement BIA guidelines recommended by the American Society of Exercise Physiologists [25].
2.3. IVR Intervention
The IVR intervention made use of an innovative 2.0 software application (Octonic VR, New York, NY, USA) developed to enable a variety of viewing experiences while walking on a treadmill. On an HMD (Meta Quest 2, Menlo Park, CA, USA), a standardized, pre-selected immersive environment portraying an island in the sky (i.e., “Sky Island”) was projected, as shown in Figure 1. As participants walked on the treadmill, in-app music played as their view dynamically changed in response to their pace and direction of gaze, as shown in Figure 2. The visual input while wearing the HMD entirely concealed the participants’ immediate surroundings, as is customary with IVR. However, a mixed reality feature unique to this program permitted a real-world visual perspective of the treadmill overlayed by the IVR environment to ensure safety while walking.
2.4. Equipment
Participants completed both IVR and non-IVR trials on a treadmill (Redliro, New York, NY, USA) while donning a forearm-worn optical heart rate monitor (Rhythm + TM 2.0, Scosche Industries, Oxnard, CA, USA). HR data were collected and graphically displayed using a fitness app (iCardio®, Cupertino, CA, USA) monitored by research administrators.
2.5. Experimental Procedures
Two independent research administrators assessed each participant individually during the 6 trials. A designated study administrator read a standardized set of instructions prior to the start of the first experiment. Participants were instructed that they would be performing three trials, separated by three-minute rest periods, while always walking at 3.5 mph on a treadmill on two different days (six trials total). One day of testing included three trials while wearing the IVR HMD, whereas the second day included three trials sans IVR. After each 10 min session, participants were asked to rate their perceived exertion (RPE) using the Borg RPE Scale, with a score ranging from 6 to 20. Furthermore, individuals’ heart rates were monitored throughout every trial. After completion of the first day of testing, a 24 h washout period was given prior to the subsequent trials.
Deception occurred in several ways, the first of which was the treadmill’s speed. Participants were instructed that all treadmill trials would be completed at 3.5 mph. However, each day of testing included two deceptive speed conditions (3.3 and 3.7 mph) in addition to the ‘true’ 3.5 mph speed condition. The ‘manual’ option in the app menu was chosen so that the HMD did not affect the treadmill speed. A separate research administrator modified the in-app display beforehand so that participants always saw a speed of 3.5 mph regardless of the deceptive pace they were actually reaching. The same research administrator altered the speeds for each participant, whereas the other research administrator was blinded to the speeds.
Participants were also given false information in regard to the IVR trials. In order to ‘deceive’ the participants from the true aim of the study, they were informed that there was a component of the IVR background that was changing in between trials. No further details were provided, and research administrators were not permitted to answer any questions pertaining to the background. After the completion of the second day of testing, participants were asked if they noticed any differences between the first and second days of testing. Responses were recorded and transcribed.
2.6. Statistical Analysis
Descriptive statistics are presented as mean and standard deviation (SD). A 3 × 2 repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was utilized: independent variables were treadmill speed (3 levels) and presence of IVF (2 levels), while dependent variables were RPE and HR. Effect sizes of main effects and interactions were measured by partial eta squared (η2). These calculations were followed up by paired t-tests using the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure to maintain the familywise error rate. Effect sizes were measured by Cohen’s d. Statistical significance was determined by α = 0.05 and all tests were two-tailed. All data were exported to IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 22 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) for analysis.
3. Results
3.1. Demographics
All sixteen participants (mean age = 20.5 ± 1.2 years old) successfully completed the study. Mean height, weight, and body fat collectively for all participants were 171 ± 14 cm, 69 ± 15 kg, and 19 ± 7.5%, respectively.
3.2. RPE Scores
Although there was no significant difference in RPE between the 3.3 and 3.5 mph trials within the non-IVR group, these scores were lower compared to those associated with the 3.7 mph trials (p = 0.002, 95% CI: (−1.3, −0.7), d = 0.71; p = 0.002, 95% CI: (−1.4, −0.5), d = 0.75, respectively). No significant differences in RPE were observed within the IVR group. During the 3.5 and 3.7 mph trials, the IVR group reported lower RPE scores in comparison to the non-IVR group (p = 0.002, 95% CI: (0.6, 1.8), d = 1.40; p = 0.002, 95% CI (1.2, 2.7), d = 1.88, respectively) (Table 1). Correlational analysis revealed a weak relationship between RPE and HR (r = 0.27, p = 0.008). When partitioned by sex, a moderate correlation was found between RPE and HR in females (r = 0.45, p = 0.001), whereas a weak relationship was identified in males (r = 0.14, p = 0.361).
3.3. Heart Rate
In the non-IVR group, there was a significantly higher HR in the 3.7 mph trials compared to the 3.5 mph trials (p = 0.027, 95% CI: (−6.8, −1.8), d = 0.27). No significant differences in HR were observed within the IVR group. Between groups, only the 3.7 mph trials yielded a lower HR in the IVR group compared to the non-IVR group (p = 0.049, 95% CI: (2.0, 12.7), d = 0.56) (Table 1).
3.4. Post-IVR Session Responses
Through an in-person post-study interview, all but one of the sixteen participants reported “feeling no differences in treadmill speed while wearing the HMD” between trials. However, 75% (12 participants) reported feeling “a different treadmill speed” between trials while not donning the HMD. Moreover, participants commonly reported that “time went by faster” during the IVR trials, which were also deemed to be “more stimulating” and “more enjoyable”.
4. Discussion
IVR exercise has been linked to a variety of benefits. IVR exercise has been associated with reductions in fatigue, stress, and depression, as well as increases in tranquility and quality of life [26]. In addition, the capacity of IVR exergaming to be physically demanding while enjoyable allows it to be an effective and engaging form of exercise [27]. These advantages, when combined with generally high satisfaction and usage scores, have revealed the potential for IVR to vastly improve health and well-being [28]. Despite these advantages, there is a scarcity of studies testing the efficacy of deception via IVR exercise across different modalities. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first of its kind to examine the effects of IVR in conjunction with treadmill speed deception.
The results of the present study support the first of our hypotheses, as the mean RPE scores corresponding to the IVR trials were significantly lower than those from the non-IVR trials at 3.5 and 3.7 mph (7.4 and 7.6 versus 8.6 and 9.5; p < 0.002, respectively). This finding is consistent with a deceptive study using a training bike conducted by Löchtefeld et al., which found that the manipulation of visual and haptic cues could allow for an increase in the user’s speed by 15.2% without the user’s awareness [29]. Similarly, in regard to resistance training, both a 5% and 10% deceptive increase in loading while leg pressing have been shown to elicit nearly identical strength measures without negatively affecting physical self-efficacy [30]. In contrast to our results, however, Leininger et al. found no difference in RPE between exergaming and matched-intensity treadmill walking [31]. An important distinction between both studies is the use of immersive VR versus two-dimensional, VR-only. Being engaged in an IVR environment may heighten dissociation and/or distract the user from the perceived effort of the exercise.
The second of our hypotheses was partially supported. Only the HR response during the trials at 3.7 mph was lower in the IVR group than the non-IVR group (107 bpm versus 115 bpm; p < 0.049, respectively). Little research has assessed heart rate trends in protocols that integrate deception and exergaming together, but there is a limited amount of each in isolation. In an IVR exergaming study with geriatric participants, mean HR measurements increased for three out of four individuals between two different training sessions [32]. Furthermore, a comparison between four exergames found that 75% of the exergames produced a lower perceived exertion than actual exertion based on their average heart rate as a percentage of their maximum heart rate (66%, 78%, and 55% of maximum heart rate, respectively) [33]. Conversely, in a deceptive treadmill and cycling study, lower heart rate measurements were observed during the condition in which the endpoint was not specified compared to when an endpoint was predetermined [34]. Further research is warranted to evaluate trends in HR during deceptive training in order to provide informed exercise recommendations, such as appropriate training HR zones, for IVR exergaming users.
The findings of the present study may be attributed to several different factors; first, the preservation of teleoanticipation. Previous research has shown that manipulating the endpoint can negatively affect exercise performance by disrupting the anticipatory setting of the pace [35]. Teleoanticipatory pacing strategies are crucial to delay fatigue and optimize performance [36]. The protocol used in this study did not modify the duration of the exercise nor deceive the participants of the endpoint. Hence, participants should have been able to fully complete the exercise without calculated or spontaneous adjustments affecting the use of physiological resources in both non-IVR and IVR trials. It is possible that establishing a workload or endpoint may factor into attenuated RPE and HR.
Furthermore, the differences in RPE and heart rate between the two conditions may have been a result of the IVR intervention. A commonly reported benefit of IVR is the ability to empower one to escape negative emotions, stresses, and affairs [37]. IVR has even been shown to reduce discomfort and minimize pain to a degree comparable to clinical analgesic treatments [38]. However, the environment in which the user is immersed also matters. Simulated urban environments have been reported to produce feelings of increased fatigue and decreased levels of self-esteem, whereas nature-based environments have yielded decreased levels of negative emotions (i.e., fatigue, tension, depression, etc.) [39]. In the present study, the selected environment incorporated several different natural elements (e.g., waterfalls, rocks, beaches, unique cloud formations, etc.) into view. It is possible that this environment allowed participants to divert their attention away from daily stressors and the exercise they were performing. Consequently, as we showed, a faster deceptive speed would not be noticed by the user or reflected by their HR response (i.e., autonomic nervous system). Rutkowski et al. recently demonstrated a similar phenomenon through heart rate variability analysis wherein lower heart rate responses during IVR were attributed to reductions in stress levels [40]. They noted that the “calming environment” altered autonomic nervous system activity, which was responsible for the observed heart rate behavior with virtual reality immersion.
The implications of these results should serve as a guide for IVR exergaming as it continues to evolve. The trend of lower HR and RPE scores corresponding to exercising with IVR may suggest that it has the potential to lower the perceived difficulty of exercise, which may be particularly beneficial for sedentary individuals. Furthermore, the lower HR and RPE scores produced in the 3.7 mph trials may suggest that IVR may have utility as a ‘deceptive’ exercise intervention. None of the participants reported a conscious awareness of the 5.7% increase in speed, nor was it reflected in their RPE. These results suggest that participants may perform exercise at a greater intensity while using IVR. Further research is warranted to determine if prolonged IVR training in different simulated environments can yield similar results. Moreover, a threshold for how much intensity (i.e., speed) can be increased before a participant notices should also be studied.
This study is not without its limitations. This study could have benefited from a larger sample size to add statistical power. To this end, the sample was almost entirely composed of fit, college-aged individuals. A more diverse sample would have been ideal to improve generalizability. Additionally, due to the design of the protocol, the findings of the present study may be limited to short-term effects of IVR. Future research should be conducted to examine if these effects would be consistent over a prolonged period of time and/or in response to repeated exercise. Notwithstanding, several other confounding variables may have affected the results of the present study and should be controlled in future studies. Given that walking styles and centers of gravity may vary across individuals, standardized walking patterns should be established in future studies to ensure that biomechanical loads are similar. Furthermore, since the Borg RPE score is inherently a subjective measurement, it is not without its limitations and may be subject to a nonnegligible degree of variability. Alternative measures of perceived exertion should also be considered in future research.
5. Conclusions
The findings of this study suggest that using deception via an immersive virtual reality exergaming experience may attenuate participant perceived effort and heart rate response without the user noticing the effect.
Conceptualization, T.Y., T.K.S., C.A.M. and B.A.D.; methodology, T.Y., T.K.S., C.A.M. and B.A.D.; formal analysis, T.Y., T.K.S., C.A.M., M.S.M., E.V.N. and B.A.D.; data curation, T.Y., T.K.S., C.A.M., T.L.N., A.E.B., R.J.L., T.H.N., D.M.B. and J.P.F.; writing—original draft preparation, T.Y., T.K.S., C.A.M., M.S.M., E.V.N. and B.A.D.; writing—review and editing, T.Y., T.K.S., C.A.M., A.E.B., R.J.L., T.H.N., D.M.B., J.P.F., M.S.M., E.V.N. and B.A.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of UCLA (protocol code: 11-003190, 22 April 2020).
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
We are appreciative of the willingness of devoted participants to give so generously their time and effort to support this work.
The authors declare no conflicts of interest. Octonic had no role in the study design, data collection, analysis, interpretation, or writing of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Figure 1. Snapshot view of Sky Island (Octonic VR app) environment from the head-mounted display (HMD) during the immersive virtual reality intervention while walking on a treadmill.
Figure 2. (a) Experimental setup during the IVR intervention mirroring participant’s HMD to a display for researchers; (b) perspective from the participant’s HMD showing the treadmill speed settings and overlays in the IVR environment.
Mean heart rate and Borg RPE scores corresponding to deceptive treadmill speed in the non-immersive virtual reality (IVR) and IVR trials.
Non-IVR | IVR | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Deceptive | 3.3 | 3.5 | 3.7 | 3.3 | 3.5 | 3.7 |
HR (±SD) (bpm) | 111 (13) | 110 (16) | 115 (16) | 107 (14) | 105 (10) | 107 (10) |
Borg RPE (±SD) | 8.5 (1.5) | 8.6 (1.1) | 9.0 (1.3) | 7.5 (0.5) | 7.4 (0.5) | 7.6 (0.6) |
References
1. Wittekind, A.L.; Micklewright, D.; Beneke, R. Teleoanticipation in all-out short-duration cycling. Br. J. Sports Med.; 2011; 45, pp. 114-119. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2009.061580] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19679576]
2. Edwards, A.M.; Bentley, M.B.; Mann, M.E.; Seaholme, T.S. Self-pacing in interval training: A teleoanticipatory approach. Psychophysiology; 2011; 48, pp. 136-141. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01034.x] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20536904]
3. Lambert, E.V.; St Clair Gibson, A.; Noakes, T.D. Complex systems model of fatigue: Integrative homoeostatic control of peripheral physiological systems during exercise in humans. Br. J. Sports Med.; 2005; 39, pp. 52-62. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2003.011247] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15618343]
4. Baden, D.A.; McLean, T.L.; Tucker, R.; Noakes, T.D.; St Clair Gibson, A. Effect of anticipation during unknown or unexpected exercise duration on rating of perceived exertion, affect, and physiological function. Br. J. Sports Med.; 2005; 39, pp. 742-746. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2004.016980] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16183771]
5. Coquart, J.B.; Garcin, M. Knowledge of the endpoint: Effect on perceptual values. Int. J. Sports Med.; 2008; 29, pp. 976-979. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-2008-1038741] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18600607]
6. Tucker, R. The anticipatory regulation of performance: The physiological basis for pacing strategies and the development of a perception-based model for exercise performance. Br. J. Sports Med.; 2009; 43, pp. 392-400. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2008.050799]
7. Day, M.L.; Mcguigan, M.R.; Brice, G.; Foster, C. Monitoring Exercise Intensity During Resistance Training Using the Session RPE Scale. J. Strength. Cond. Res.; 2004; 18, pp. 353-358. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1519/R-13113.1] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15142026]
8. Williams, N. The Borg Rating of Perceived Exertion (RPE) scale. Occup. Med.; 2017; 67, pp. 404-405. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1093/occmed/kqx063]
9. Glass, S.C.; Knowlton, R.G.; Becque, M.D. Accuracy of RPE from graded exercise to establish exercise training intensity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.; 1992; 24, pp. 1303-1307. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1249/00005768-199211000-00017] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1435183]
10. Lagally, K.M.; Robertson, R.J.; Gallagher, K.I.; Gearhart, R.; Goss, F.L. Ratings of perceived exertion during low- and high-intensity resistance exercise by young adults. Percept. Mot. Ski.; 2002; 94, pp. 723-731. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.2002.94.3.723]
11. Ceci, R.; Hassmen, P. Self-monitored exercise at three different RPE intensities in treadmill vs field running. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.; 1991; 23, pp. 732-738. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1249/00005768-199106000-00013] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1886482]
12. Corbett, J.; Barwood, M.J.; Ouzounoglou, A.; Thelwell, R.; Dicks, M. Influence of Competition on Performance and Pacing during Cycling Exercise. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.; 2012; 44, pp. 509-515. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e31823378b1]
13. Williamson, J.W.; McColl, R.; Mathews, D.; Mitchell, J.H.; Raven, P.B.; Morgan, W.P. Hypnotic manipulation of effort sense during dynamic exercise: Cardiovascular responses and brain activation. J. Appl. Physiol.; 2001; 90, pp. 1392-1399. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jappl.2001.90.4.1392] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11247939]
14. Finnegan, S.L.; Dearlove, D.J.; Morris, P.; Freeman, D.; Sergeant, M.; Taylor, S.; Pattinson, K.T.S. Breathlessness in a virtual world: An experimental paradigm testing how discrepancy between VR visual gradients and pedal resistance during stationary cycling affects breathlessness perception. PLoS ONE; 2023; 18, e0270721. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0270721] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37083693]
15. Jones, H.S.; Williams, E.L.; Bridge, C.A.; Marchant, D.; Midgley, A.W.; Micklewright, D.; McNaughton, L.R. Physiological and Psychological Effects of Deception on Pacing Strategy and Performance: A Review. Sports Med.; 2013; 43, pp. 1243-1257. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40279-013-0094-1] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24002790]
16. Pires, F.O.; Hammond, J. Manipulation effects of prior exercise intensity feedback by the Borg scale during open-loop cycling. Br. J. Sports Med.; 2012; 46, pp. 18-22. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2010.079053] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21266335]
17. Hampson, D.B.; St Clair Gibson, A.; Lambert, M.I.; Dugas, J.P.; Lambert, E.V.; Noakes, T.D. Deception and Perceived Exertion during High-Intensity Running Bouts. Percept. Mot. Ski.; 2004; 98, pp. 1027-1038. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.98.3.1027-1038] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15209320]
18. Han, D.I.D.; Bergs, Y.; Moorhouse, N. Virtual reality consumer experience escapes: Preparing for the metaverse. Virtual Real.; 2022; 26, pp. 1443-1458. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10055-022-00641-7]
19. Slater, M.; Sanchez-Vives, M.V. Enhancing Our Lives with Immersive Virtual Reality. Front. Robot. AI; 2016; 3, 74. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2016.00074]
20. North, M.M.; North, S.M. A Comparative Study of Sense of Presence of Traditional Virtual Reality and Immersive Environments. Australas. J. Inf. Syst.; 2016; 20, [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3127/ajis.v20i0.1168]
21. Slater, M.; Pérez Marcos, D.; Ehrsson, H.; Sanchez-Vives, M.V. Inducing illusory ownership of a virtual body. Front. Neurosci.; 2009; 3, pp. 214-220. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/neuro.01.029.2009] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20011144]
22. Zang, N.; Pope, Z.; Gao, Z. Acute Effect of Virtual Reality Exercise Bike Games on College Students’ Physiological and Psychological Outcomes. Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw.; 2017; 20, pp. 453-457. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2017.0042] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28715263]
23. Mestre, D.R.; Ewald, M.; Maiano, C. Virtual reality and exercise: Behavioral and psychological effects of visual feedback. Annu. Rev. Cybertherapy Telemed.; 2011; 2011, pp. 122-127.
24. Dolezal, B.A.; Lau, M.J.; Abrazado, M.; Storer, T.W.; Cooper, C.B. Validity of two commercial grade bioelectrical impedance analyzers for measurement of body fat percentage. J. Exerc. Physiol. Online; 2013; 16, 74.
25. Heyward, V. ASEP methods recommendation: Body composition assessment. J. Exerc. Physiol.; 2001; 4, pp. 1-12.
26. Qian, J.; McDonough, D.J.; Gao, Z. The Effectiveness of Virtual Reality Exercise on Individual’s Physiological, Psychological and Rehabilitative Outcomes: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health; 2020; 17, 4133. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17114133] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32531906]
27. Yoo, S.; Kay, J. VRun: Running-in-place virtual reality exergame. Proceedings of the 28th Australian Conference on Computer-Human Interaction; Launceston, Tasmania, Australia, 29 November–2 December 2016; Association for Computing Machinery: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 562-566. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3010915.3010987]
28. Xu, W.; Liang, H.N.; Baghaei, N.; Ma, X.; Yu, K.; Meng, X.; Wen, S. Effects of an Immersive Virtual Reality Exergame on University Students’ Anxiety, Depression, and Perceived Stress: Pilot Feasibility and Usability Study. JMIR Serious Games; 2021; 9, e29330. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.2196/29330] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34813487]
29. Löchtefeld, M.; Krüger, A.; Gellersen, H. DeceptiBike: Assessing the Perception of Speed Deception in a Virtual Reality Training Bike System. Proceedings of the 9th Nordic Conference on Human-Computer Interaction; Gothenburg, Sweden, 23–27 October 2016; Association for Computing Machinery: New York, NY, USA, 2016; 40. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1145/2971485.2971513]
30. Piper, T.; Radlo, S.; Gerhardt, K.; Brooks, D.; Schnaiter, J. The Effects of Deception on Maximal Strength, Goals, and Physical Self-Efficacy. Int. J. Strength Cond.; 2023; 3, [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.47206/ijsc.v3i1.124]
31. Leininger, L.; Coles, M.G.; Gilbert, J.N. Comparing enjoyment and perceived exertion between equivalent bouts of physically interactive video gaming and treadmill walking. Health Fit. J.; 2010; 3, pp. 12-18. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.14288/hfjc.v3i1.49]
32. Campo-Prieto, P.; Rodríguez-Fuentes, G.; Cancela-Carral, J.M. Immersive Virtual Reality Exergame Promotes the Practice of Physical Activity in Older People: An Opportunity during COVID-19. Multimodal Technol. Interact.; 2021; 5, 52. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/mti5090052]
33. Yoo, S.; Ackad, C.; Heywood, T.; Kay, J. Evaluating the Actual and Perceived Exertion Provided by Virtual Reality Games. Proceedings of the 2017 CHI Conference Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems; Denver, CO, USA, 6–11 May 2017; Association for Computing Machinery: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 3050-3057. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3027063.3053203]
34. Eston, R.; Stansfield, R.; Westoby, P.; Parfitt, G. Effect of deception and expected exercise duration on psychological and physiological variables during treadmill running and cycling. Psychophysiology; 2012; 49, pp. 462-469. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01330.x] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22220852]
35. Ansley, L.; Robson, P.J.; St Clair Gibson, A.; Noakes, T.D. Anticipatory pacing strategies during supramaximal exercise lasting longer than 30 s. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.; 2004; 36, pp. 309-314. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1249/01.MSS.0000113474.31529.C6] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14767256]
36. Stone, M.R.; Thomas, K.; Wilkinson, M.; Jones, A.M.; St Clair Gibson, A.; Thompson, K.G. Effects of deception on exercise performance: Implications for determinants of fatigue in humans. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.; 2012; 44, pp. 534-541. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e318232cf77] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21886012]
37. Melancon, J. Consumer profiles in reality vs fantasy-based virtual worlds: Implications for brand entry. J. Res. Interact. Mark.; 2011; 5, pp. 298-312. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1108/17505931111191500]
38. Gromala, D.; Tong, X.; Choo, A.; Karamnejad, M.; Shaw, C.D. The Virtual Meditative Walk: Virtual Reality Therapy for Chronic Pain Management. Proceedings of the 33rd Annual ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems; Seoul, Republic of Korea, 18–23 April 2015; Association for Computing Machinery: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 521-524.
39. Yu, C.-P.; Lee, H.-Y.; Luo, X.-Y. The effect of virtual reality forest and urban environments on physiological and psychological responses. Urban For. Urban Green; 2018; 35, pp. 106-114. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ufug.2018.08.013]
40. Rutkowski, S.; Szary, P.; Sacha, J.; Casaburi, R. Immersive Virtual Reality Influences Physiologic Responses to Submaximal Exercise: A Randomized, Crossover Trial. Front. Physiol.; 2021; 12, 702266. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2021.702266] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34658904]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Virtual reality and technology-driven ‘exergaming’ have grown in tandem to expand the possibilities of exercise. However, few studies have investigated these aspects together in the context of treadmill walking. This study examined the effects of immersive virtual reality (IVR) on heart rate (HR) and ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) while walking on a treadmill at three different speeds, only one of which was disclosed to participants. Using a single-blinded, randomized control trial with a crossover research design, sixteen college-aged adults (eight females, mean age of 20.5 ± 1.2 years) were randomly assigned a testing order for six trials of treadmill walking. Three 10 min trials were conducted with IVR and three were conducted without on two separate days; a 3 min break was given in between trials. Borg RPE scores were assessed after each trial, while HR was monitored throughout. The IVR trials produced lower mean RPE scores in comparison to the non-IVR group at 3.5 and 3.7 mph. The IVR trials also yielded a lower mean HR compared to the non-IVR trials at the highest speed of 3.7 mph. These findings suggest that IVR may be able to attenuate RPE scores and HR while walking on a treadmill.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details






1 Airway & UCFit-Digital Health-Exercise Physiology Research Laboratory, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA;
2 Airway & UCFit-Digital Health-Exercise Physiology Research Laboratory, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA;
3 Washington University School of Medicine, Washington University, Saint Louis, MO 63110, USA
4 Donald and Barbara Zucker School of Medicine at Hofstra/Northwell, Hofstra University, Hempstead, NY 11549, USA