BACKGROUND
Considering the anticipated substantial increase in numbers of people with dementia, prevention and treatment of dementia have become global health priorities. An increasing number of modifiable risk and protective factors for dementia risk reduction has attracted much attention. According to The Lancet Commission on Dementia Prevention, Intervention, and Care, ≈ 40% of all dementia cases worldwide could theoretically be prevented or delayed by tackling 12 common modifiable risk and protective factors. The potential for prevention might be even higher (up to 56%) in low- and middle-income countries and in minority groups within countries.
Several dementia risk scores have been developed to improve the identification of those at higher risk. The most used and validated risk scores include CAIDE (Cardiovascular Risk Factors, Aging, and Incidence of Dementia score), ANU-ADRI (Australian National University Alzheimer's Disease Risk Index), and LIBRA (LIfestyle for BRAin health index). CAIDE originates from 2006 and is based on a 20-year follow-up of the CAIDE study. This score aims to predict late-life risk of dementia based on a limited set of modifiable and non-modifiable risk factors (e.g., blood pressure, age, sex, etc.) and has been validated for both cognitive decline and dementia in multiple cohorts as well as a prevention trial outcome. ANU-ADRI was developed in 2013 as a risk score incorporating both non-modifiable and modifiable risk factors for Alzheimer's disease (AD) that could be readily assessed via self-report. It has been used as a predictor for dementia and cognitive decline in multiple cohorts as well as a surrogate outcome in clinical trials to assess intervention effectiveness.
The LIBRA index was also developed in 2013 based on the results of a systematic literature review and Delphi consensus study including eight dementia experts. The score is composed exclusively of modifiable risk (coronary heart disease, physical inactivity, chronic kidney disease, diabetes, cholesterol, smoking, midlife obesity, midlife hypertension, and depression) and protective factors (low-to-moderate alcohol consumption, healthy diet, and high cognitive activity) for cognitive decline and dementia that can be targeted by lifestyle interventions in primary care, thereby capturing lifestyle-based risk reduction potential on an individual level. LIBRA has been well validated for dementia risk, cognitive decline, and structural markers of brain damage in numerous population-based cohorts. In addition, it has also been used as an outcome measure in multidomain intervention studies.
Recently, the World Health Organization (WHO) guidelines on risk reduction of cognitive decline and dementia advocated the use of risk scores for the implementation of risk reduction guidelines. In the decade after establishment of CAIDE, ANU-ADRI, and LIBRA, research has identified modifiable risk factors that might be additional targets for dementia risk reduction and hence should be included in contemporary risk scores. Identifying and prioritizing the modifiable risk factors for use in updated risk scores could improve dementia risk prediction and inform the development of more effective risk reduction strategies. Therefore, the aims of the current study are (1) to summarize the evidence on modifiable risk and protective factors for dementia and (2) to identify and prioritize candidate factors for updating dementia risk scores.
METHODS
Phase 1: umbrella review
Data sources and search strategy
The search strategy was adapted from our previous study for the identification of modifiable dementia risk and protective factors and was composed of multiple text and Medical Subject Heading (MeSH) terms for exposure (e.g., “risk factor [ALL]”, “epidemiologic factor [MESH]”) and cognitive outcome (e.g., “dementia [ALL]”, “cognition disorders [MESH:noexp]”). The strategy was restricted to studies in humans written in Dutch or English. To ensure adequate coverage of the existing literature, we searched PubMed, Embase, Web of Science, and PsycINFO for the period from January 1, 2015 to May 23, 2021 to identify all systematic reviews (SR) and/or meta-analyses (MA) that reported modifiable dementia risk and protective factors. See Appendix in supporting information for the complete search strategies used in the different databases. The umbrella review was registered in PROSPERO (CRD42021266486) and conducted in line with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines.
RESEARCH IN CONTEXT
- Systematic review: The emergence of more recently described modifiable risk and protective factors for dementia highlights the need for continuous re-assessment of existing dementia risk scores. The authors systematically reviewed the literature using PubMed, Embase, Web of Science, and PsycINFO, combined with dementia experts’ opinions.
- Interpretation: Our findings identified new candidate risk factors, with the strongest support for hearing impairment, social contact, and sleep.
- Future directions: Updating dementia risk scores may improve their predictive validity and allow for more extensive and tailored dementia risk reduction strategies.
Study selection
We included SR/MA reporting studies that: (1) examined modifiable risk and protective factors as the exposures; (2) contained information on dementia, AD dementia, other dementia subtypes, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), cognitive impairment/decline, or change in cognitive performance as outcome; (3) used data from population-based samples; (4) had prospective cohort study designs with ≥ 2 years follow-up; (5) included ≥ 200 participants, who were (6) aged ≥ 18 years, and (7) reported either risk estimates for incident dementia, one of its subtypes, or MCI (hazard ratios, risk ratios, odds ratios) or regression coefficients (for continuous cognitive outcomes) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) in text, tables, or forest plots. Regarding the latter criterion, evidence from risk estimates was considered primary outcomes in studies that reported both risk estimates and regression coefficients.
Data extraction
After de-duplication of the search hits, two investigators (C.R., L.S.) screened titles and abstracts for broad suitability. Any disagreements were resolved by discussing with other investigators (K.D., S.K., M.v.B.). Subsequently, the following information was extracted from full texts using a predefined data-extraction spreadsheet (see Appendix in supporting information): (1) study title, (2) risk or protective factor(s) discussed, (3) exposed group, (4) reference group, (5) cohort, (6) cognitive outcome(s), (7) type of outcome measure(s), (8) risk estimator or regression coefficient with 95% CIs, (9) sample size, (10) follow-up time, and (11) average baseline age of participants (or approximation). As much information as possible was extracted from the SR and/or MA. In cases that did not report certain information, this was retrieved directly from the included primary studies.
Data synthesis
Based on the direction of the risk estimators or regression coefficients and the width of their 95% CIs, we decided whether an association was labeled a “risk association”, a “protective association”, or a “neutral association”. Consistency of the associations for every factor was calculated as the highest number of either “risk associations” or “protective associations”, divided by the total number of associations encountered for that factor. For feasibility, the number of associations assessed for every primary study was limited to one, unless multiple SR/MA discussed different risk factors that were investigated in the study. In cases in which different exposure levels of a particular factor were compared to the same reference, we selected the association with the most extreme exposure level compared to the reference as the basis of our decision, unless there was substantial evidence for a non-linear relationship between exposure and outcome. Additionally, when multiple cognitive outcomes were assessed, we used the association with the most generalizable outcome in the order of (1) all-cause dementia, (2) AD dementia, (3) other dementia subtypes, (4) MCI, (5) cognitive impairment/decline, or (6) change in cognitive performance based on neuropsychological testing. Whenever two studies reported the same study cohort, the association for the most generalizable cognitive outcome was included. If two papers were of the same study cohort and of the same outcome–exposure association, we chose the paper with the largest sample size or longest follow-up period. Appendix in supporting information gives a more detailed description of these data harmonization procedures.
Quality assessment
Study quality of the included SR/MA was assessed using the 7/8-item National Institutes of Health (NIH) Quality Assessment Tool of Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses. The quality assessment for all included studies can be found in Appendix in supporting information.
Phase 2: Delphi consensus study—first round
As in our previous Delphi study, this study was conducted among dementia prevention experts. A total of 42 experts were invited by e-mail (with one or two reminders in case of non-response), all of whom were assistant, associate, or full professors and were considered experts in the field based on their track records. We actively invited experts from different regions across the world with different research and/or clinical backgrounds. The experts gave informed consent and completed an online questionnaire (May 2022–June 2022) through a unique hyperlink using Qualtrics XM (see Appendix in supporting information). The participants were asked to indicate for every current LIBRA factor whether it should or should not be included in an updated version of this dementia risk score, with a free text field to support their choice in case of non-inclusion. Subsequently, they were asked to name additional modifiable dementia risk and protective factors and rank them in subjective order of importance. The experts gave each of these newly named risk and protective factors a rank score, which allowed us to aggregate responses across experts to calculate a factor's total rank score (see Appendix in supporting information). The researchers processed the responses using aggregated results to ensure blinding. This Delphi study was approved by the ethical committee of Maastricht University (FHML-REC/2022/086).
Phase 3: synthesis of information
Next, a risk factor inventory was constructed, based on the highest-ranking factors of both the umbrella review and the first Delphi round that were not previously included in the LIBRA index. The risk and protective factors encountered in the umbrella review were ranked based on their frequency in the literature and, subsequently, the consistency of their associations with cognitive outcomes, while the factors encountered in the first Delphi round were ranked according to the risk factor's total rank score (see Appendix ).
Phase 4: Delphi consensus study—second round
The second Delphi round (online, December 2022–January 2023, using Qualtrics XM, Appendix in supporting information) focused on the risk and protective factors not yet included in LIBRA. The same panel of experts was presented with the aggregated results of the umbrella review and the first Delphi round. The list of factors was composed of the factors mentioned in the first Delphi round (suitable for individual-level risk reduction), supplemented with the factors identified with our umbrella review that had a “consistency of association” of at least 50%. The experts were asked to weigh the candidate risk and protective factors for dementia risk reduction by freely distributing 100 points (more points = more important) across the inventory. The summation of these points across experts resulted in final scores for all of the different factors, which allowed us to create a definite curated list of the most important modifiable risk and protective factors. In addition, we asked experts’ opinions with regard to the operationalization of some specific factors.
RESULTS
Umbrella review
With the initial search, a total of 6540 abstracts were retrieved, yielding 4349 unique abstracts after de-duplication. Of these abstracts, 463 were included for full-text assessment. From these, 316 were excluded for various reasons (see Figure ), resulting in a total inclusion of 148 SR/MA in our umbrella review. After scrutinizing these SR/MA, a total of 608 unique primary studies included in those SR/MA were considered eligible for our analysis. For all the different risk and protective factors encountered in these studies, we determined the frequency and consistency of association with cognitive outcomes. For 9 of the 12 factors currently included in the LIBRA index, we found that more than half of the studies included in the SR/MA reported a significant association with cognitive outcomes. For the three other factors (low/moderate alcohol consumption, midlife obesity, and smoking), less than half of the studies included in the SR/MA reported a significant association between exposure and cognitive outcome (Table ). Besides the existing LIBRA factors, there were many other candidate factors. The most well studied are reported in Table and the overview of the encountered factors together with the primary literature in which they were investigated can be found in Appendix in supporting information.
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
TABLE 1 Frequency and consistency of the encountered factors currently included in the LIBRA index based on the umbrella review of 148 systematic literature reviews and meta-analyses.
LIBRA factor | Number of studies | Higher risk/decline | No association | Lower risk/decline | Consistency of association |
Diabetes | 61 | 31 | 30 | 0 | 51% |
Depression | 48 | 36 | 12 | 0 | 75% |
Midlife hypertension | 28 | 17 | 8 | 2 | 63% |
High leisure-time physical activity | 22 | 0 | 7 | 15 | 68% |
High alcohol consumption | 22 | 3 | 15 | 4 | 18% |
Chronic kidney disease | 20 | 14 | 6 | 0 | 70% |
High cognitive activity | 20 | 0 | 7 | 13 | 68% |
Healthy diet/Mediterranean diet | 18 | 0 | 8 | 10 | 56% |
Coronary heart disease | 15 | 10 | 5 | 0 | 67% |
Smoking | 14 | 6 | 8 | 0 | 43% |
Midlife obesity | 9 | 2 | 7 | 0 | 22% |
High midlife cholesterol | 8 | 5 | 3 | 0 | 63% |
TABLE 2 Overview of best-documented (newly identified) modifiable risk and protective factors (not previously included in the LIBRA index) based on the umbrella review of 148 systematic literature reviews and meta-analyses.
Non-LIBRA factor | Number of studies | Higher risk/decline | No association | Lower risk/decline | Consistency of association |
Low social engagement | 28 | 15 | 13 | 0 | 54% |
T2PD or impaired FBG | 19 | 4 | 15 | 0 | 21% |
Hearing impairment | 18 | 14 | 4 | 0 | 78% |
Long sleep duration | 16 | 7 | 9 | 0 | 44% |
Short sleep duration | 16 | 5 | 11 | 0 | 31% |
Vision impairment | 15 | 10 | 5 | 0 | 67% |
Atrial fibrillation | 14 | 11 | 3 | 0 | 79% |
Anxiety | 14 | 10 | 4 | 0 | 71% |
Low social network size | 11 | 2 | 9 | 0 | 18% |
Loneliness | 10 | 7 | 3 | 0 | 70% |
Tooth loss | 10 | 7 | 3 | 0 | 70% |
Poor sleep quality | 10 | 5 | 5 | 0 | 50% |
Orthostatic hypotension | 10 | 4 | 6 | 0 | 40% |
Living alone | 10 | 3 | 6 | 1 | 30% |
Olfactory impairment | 9 | 9 | 0 | 0 | 100% |
Insomnia | 9 | 4 | 4 | 1 | 44% |
Metabolic syndrome | 8 | 3 | 5 | 0 | 38% |
Daytime sleepiness | 5 | 5 | 0 | 0 | 100% |
High religious involvement | 5 | 0 | 2 | 3 | 60% |
Heart failure | 5 | 2 | 3 | 0 | 40% |
Sleep-disordered breathing | 5 | 2 | 3 | 0 | 40% |
Low emotional support | 5 | 0 | 5 | 0 | 0% |
Any heart disease | 4 | 1 | 3 | 0 | 25% |
Periodontal disease | 4 | 1 | 3 | 0 | 25% |
High psychological stress | 3 | 3 | 0 | 0 | 100% |
Social difficulties | 3 | 3 | 0 | 0 | 100% |
Low serum folate | 3 | 1 | 2 | 0 | 33% |
Pesticide exposure | 3 | 1 | 2 | 0 | 33% |
Delphi consensus study—first round
Of the invited 42 experts, 18 agreed to participate. The majority of experts agreed all 12 factors included in the current LIBRA index should continue to be included. Table shows the different factors with the percentage of experts that supported the inclusion of that factor in an updated LIBRA index. The lowest consensus was for the factors chronic kidney disease, coronary heart disease, and high cholesterol, for which consensus varied between 11 (61%) and 13 (72%) of the 18 experts. For the remaining 9 factors, 16 or more experts (≥ 89%) agreed on their inclusion. The main reasons for non-inclusion according to the experts were life course differences in effects, inconsistent evidence, and limited evidence for modifiability. In addition, experts named 27 new modifiable risk and protective factors for dementia risk reduction. Table shows the answers with the highest rank scores. A complete overview of the experts’ answers can be found in Appendix in supporting information. Some of the answers provided were omitted from the second Delphi round as they did not align with the rationale behind the LIBRA index, that is, dementia risk reduction on an individual level through self-management or primary care.
TABLE 3 Evaluation of the current LIBRA factors by the Delphi experts (N = 18) for inclusion in an updated index.
LIBRA factor | Supports inclusion | Does not support inclusion | Percentage supporting inclusion |
Diabetes | 18 | 0 | 100% |
Physical inactivity | 18 | 0 | 100% |
Smoking | 18 | 0 | 100% |
Depression | 17 | 1 | 94% |
Midlife hypertension | 17 | 1 | 94% |
Midlife obesity | 17 | 1 | 94% |
High cognitive activity | 16 | 2 | 89% |
Healthy diet/Mediterranean diet | 16 | 2 | 89% |
Low/moderate alcohol intake | 16 | 2 | 89% |
Cholesterol | 13 | 5 | 72% |
Coronary heart disease | 13 | 5 | 72% |
Chronic kidney disease | 11 | 7 | 61% |
TABLE 4 Ranking of (newly identified) modifiable risk and protective factors (not previously included in the LIBRA index) according to the experts (N = 18) in the first Delphi round.
Modifiable risk/protective factor | Frequency | Ranks | Rank score | Final rank |
Hearing impairment | 11 | 1,1,1,2,2,3,3,3,4,4,8 | 761 | 1 |
Social isolation/loneliness | 13 | 1,2,2,2,3,3,3,3,3,4,4,5,7 | 723 | 2 |
Traumatic brain injury | 11 | 1,1,2,2,3,3,3,4,4,5,7 | 704 | 3 |
Education | 8 | 1,1,2,2,2,3,4,5 | 592 | 4 |
Sleep | 8 | 1,1,1,2,2,4,5,6 | 572 | 5 |
Stroke/cerebrovascular disease | 5 | 1,1,1,5,8 | 345 | 6 |
Air pollution | 7 | 1,3,4,5,5,6,7 | 326 | 7 |
Psychological stress | 3 | 2,3,8 | 154 | 8 |
Visual impairment | 4 | 3,4,5,12 | 149 | 9 |
Life course inequalities | 1 | 1 | 100 | 10 |
Atrial fibrillation | 3 | 4,6,8 | 83 | 11 |
Delphi consensus study—second round
In the second round, 17 of the 18 experts of the first round participated. They individually assigned a total of 100 points to the most important modifiable risk and protective factors identified in the umbrella review and first Delphi round. Table shows the average number of points, the range, and the frequency of point allocation to each factor. Most points were allocated to (1) hearing impairment, (2) social contact, (3) sleep, (4) atrial fibrillation, (5) life course inequalities, and (6) psychological stress. The other factors identified in the umbrella review and first Delphi round received considerably fewer points and were therefore not considered for further inclusion in LIBRA. For the factors social contact and sleep, the experts were additionally asked to indicate which operationalization encountered in the literature would be the most suitable for modification. Social engagement (i.e., participation in activities with a social component such as volunteer work or organized group activities) and sleep-disordered breathing were indicated as the most suitable operationalizations. However, the difference among the most suitable operationalizations for sleep were minimal (see Appendix in supporting information).
TABLE 5 Ranking of emerging modifiable risk and protective factors (not previously included in the LIBRA index) according to the experts (N = 17) in the second Delphi round.
Modifiable risk/protective factor | Allocated points (mean) | Standard deviation | Frequency of point allocation | Allocation range |
Hearing impairment | 17.94 | 13.29 | 17 | 5–65 |
Life course inequalities | 15.00 | 20.91 | 14 | 3–90 |
Social contact | 13.82 | 6.54 | 17 | 5–30 |
Atrial fibrillation | 12.94 | 8.75 | 16 | 5–40 |
Sleep | 12.24 | 8.88 | 15 | 3–30 |
Psychological stress | 12.15 | 7.15 | 16 | 3–30 |
Visual impairment | 6.47 | 6.13 | 11 | 5–20 |
Anxiety | 5.50 | 5.61 | 12 | 2.5–20 |
Tooth loss | 2.94 | 3.46 | 8 | 5–10 |
Olfactory impairment | 1.00 | 1.91 | 4 | 2–3 |
Summary of the most important candidate risk and protective factors
Hearing impairment
In the umbrella review, one SR and four MA focused on hearing impairment and included a total of 18 unique prospective cohort studies. Fourteen (78%) of these studies found a significantly increased risk of dementia or cognitive decline. The two meta-analyses that used dementia as the outcome reported combined effect sizes of 1.49 (95% CI 1.30–1.67) and 1.28 (95% CI 1.02–1.59). Even higher combined effect sizes of 2.82 (95% CI 1.47–5.42) and 3.21 (95% CI 1.49–8.69) were calculated in two MA using cognitive impairment or AD as outcomes. The latter study reported a higher effect estimate for severely versus moderately impaired central auditory processing, suggesting a potential dose–response effect.
Recent cohort studies found that hearing aid use was associated with reduced risk of (progression to) dementia among hearing-impaired adults. The first randomized controlled trial (RCT) on auditory rehabilitation, the Aging and Cognitive Health Evaluation in Elders (ACHIEVE) trial, showed a decrease of 48% in cognitive decline over 3 years in individuals with several risk factors for cognitive decline. However, this did not occur in a cohort of healthier individuals. Nevertheless, the experts considered hearing impairment the most important factor in both the first and second Delphi round (conducted before results of the ACHIEVE trial were published).
There are some mechanisms that could explain the association between hearing loss and dementia. One of the most well-described theories is the information-degradation hypothesis, suggesting that hearing impairment could affect cognitive performance by shifting cognitive resources from higher-level cognitive processes toward sensory perception as a compensation mechanism. Alternatively, the relationship between hearing impairment and cognitive decline or dementia could be mediated by depression or social isolation. Finally, it has not been excluded that hearing impairment and dementia could have a common cause such as overall neural degeneration or systemic vascular disease, referred to as the common cause hypothesis.
Social contact
Different operationalizations of social contact were encountered in this umbrella review. Some SR/MA focused on specific operationalizations of social contact such as loneliness or living alone, while others investigated multiple operationalizations of social contact. Functional operationalizations (“loneliness” and “social engagement”) of social contact were more consistently significantly associated with risk of dementia or cognitive decline compared to structural operationalizations (“social network size” and “living alone”) according to this umbrella review. One meta-analysis including data from eight different prospective cohort studies found a relative risk of 1.26 (95% CI 1.14–1.40) for dementia or AD in lonely individuals. The meta-analysis by Kuiper et al., found that the operationalizations “social participation”, “frequency of social contact”, and “loneliness” were associated with the risk of incident dementia, while this was not the case for “social network size”. A recent UK Biobank study reported that social isolation was significantly associated with a 26% increased risk of dementia, independent of loneliness and depression. Additionally, 75% of the dementia risk related to loneliness was mediated by depression.
Despite the potentially overlapping nature of social and cognitively stimulating activities, the study by Duffner et al. reported independent effects for engagement in cognitive and social leisure activities. These results support the notion that social contact could be a suitable independent factor for dementia risk reduction, although more conclusive evidence is needed to determine which operationalization of social contact would be best targeted. As it appears difficult to address the effect of social contact in a controlled and systematic fashion, it is not possible to draw strong conclusions as to whether intervening on social contact will result in decreased dementia risk. The experts allocated the second and third highest number of points to social contact in the first and second Delphi study round, respectively. When we asked the experts to choose the most suitable operationalizations for intervention from a predefined list of operationalizations that we encountered in the literature, they also considered the earlier mentioned functional operationalizations to be more important than other operationalizations (see Appendix ). The experts noted the overlapping nature of the different operationalizations and mentioned the need for high-quality studies with a long follow-up time to minimize the risk of reverse causation.
Social contact, a form of environmental enrichment, might influence cognitive function through multiple mechanisms. This includes epigenetic regulation of brain-derived neurotropic factor, which influences synaptic plasticity, neural repair, and neurogenesis. Alternatively, social contact could also increase brain connectivity and cognitive reserve, thereby improving brain resilience. Finally, social isolation and loneliness have been postulated to increase dementia risk through systemic inflammation.
Sleep
As with social contact, many different operationalizations of sleep were used in the literature. The majority of the SR/MA focused on sleep duration, while others focused on sleep-disordered breathing, insomnia, or investigated multiple operationalizations of sleep. The only operationalization of sleep that was found to be consistently associated with an increased risk of dementia or cognitive decline in the umbrella review was “excessive daytime sleepiness”. For all the other operationalizations of sleep, like “sleep-disordered breathing”, “long sleep duration”, “short sleep duration”, “insomnia”, or “poor sleep quality”, only half or less of the primary studies found a significant association with cognitive outcomes in later life (Table , Appendix ). The most recent meta-analysis encountered in our umbrella review which analyzed multiple operationalizations of sleep reported effect sizes of 1.17 (95% CI 0.95–1.43), 1.18 (1.02–1.36), and 1.19 (1.11–1.29) for insomnia, sleep-disordered breathing, and any sleep disturbance, respectively. However, there was considerable heterogeneity in how these operationalizations were made and measured in the different primary studies.
In both the first and second Delphi study rounds, sleep was allocated the fifth highest number of points. In contrast to social contact, for which the experts had a clear preference for certain operationalizations over others, there were no operationalizations of sleep that clearly stood out among the rest. From all the operationalizations encountered in our umbrella review, “sleep-disordered breathing”, “short sleep duration”, “insomnia”, and “general poor sleep quality” were allocated the highest number of points, with minimal differences between them (see Appendix ). Experts expressed their concerns with regard to the overlapping nature of the operationalizations and the risk of reverse causality. Considering the mixed results from both our umbrella review and our Delphi consensus study, multiple operationalizations of sleep could potentially be used as entry points for dementia risk reduction. Whether disturbed sleep is a cause or an effect/symptom of dementia remains to be further elucidated. As such, well-designed prospective cohort studies with clear definitions for the different operationalizations of sleep and long follow-up times are warranted to better understand the association between (the different aspects/operationalizations of) sleep and dementia.
Atrial fibrillation
In 11 out of the 14 (79%) unique primary studies included in four different MA, atrial fibrillation was significantly associated with worse cognitive outcome in later life. The four MA included in our umbrella review reported effect sizes of 1.39 (95% CI 1.25–1.53), 1.30 (95% CI 1.01–1.59), 1.34 (95% CI 1.24–1.44), and 1.36 (95% CI 1.23–1.51) on risk for dementia, AD, or cognitive decline. Only three experts mentioned atrial fibrillation in the first Delphi round and all gave it rather low priority. Hence, it was not included in the top 10 factors in the first Delphi round. In the second Delphi round, however, atrial fibrillation was allocated the fourth highest number of points. The association between atrial fibrillation and dementia risk is most likely multifactorial and includes mechanisms such as cerebral hypoperfusion, resulting in unfavorable “downstream” mechanisms, and a higher thrombotic state that could lead to silent micro-infarctions. A recent large study including 18,813 primary care patients with atrial fibrillation found that use of oral coagulants was associated with a hazard ratio of 0.59 (95% CI 0.28–0.74) for dementia. Additionally, another recent study found that a healthier lifestyle in patients with new-onset atrial fibrillation reduced risk for dementia, thereby advocating for the promotion of a healthy lifestyle in atrial fibrillation patient care.
Life course inequalities
Life course inequalities are not easily modifiable at the individual level and, therefore, do not align with the purpose of the LIBRA index. Accordingly, this factor was a priori excluded from our umbrella review. In the first Delphi round, one participant mentioned life course inequalities as a new modifiable factor, while 15 experts allocated points to this factor in the second Delphi round. As such, it ended as the second most important factor in our Delphi consensus study. A recent meta-analysis of 39 prospective studies by Wang et al. found that individuals with low socioeconomic position have a 40% (95% CI 1.12–1.74) higher risk for all-cause dementia compared to individuals with high socioeconomic position. In addition, a study by Klee et al. found that area-level socioeconomic deprivation (air pollution, green spaces, etc.) was predictive of dementia, even after adjustment for individual-level socioeconomic deprivation (e.g., household income and housing type). Mediation analysis within the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing also showed that the dementia risk (based on an algorithm combining physician diagnosis, self-report, and informant report) difference between the highest and lowest wealth tertile was 52% mediated by differences in LIBRA scores, highlighting the importance of socioeconomic position in dementia risk reduction. As such, reducing social inequalities by targeting factors on both the individual and area level could play an important part in public health strategies addressing brain health.
Psychological stress
We only identified one suitable SR on psychological stress, which included three prospective cohort studies that all observed a significant association between psychological stress and increased risk of dementia or cognitive decline. Psychological stress was mentioned by three experts, with relatively high priority. As such, psychological stress did reach the factor top 10 in the first Delphi round. In the second Delphi round, the experts allocated the sixth highest number of points to this factor. It has been argued that high psychological stress might influence dementia risk by dysregulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis, thereby accelerating AD pathogenesis.
DISCUSSION
Research on prevention of dementia has made major strides in the past 10 years. The present study adds knowledge by identifying and cross-validating modifiable risk factors by combining quantitative information from an extensive umbrella review with quantitative and qualitative information from a Delphi consensus study including 18 internationally renowned dementia experts. After triangulation, strongest support was found for hearing impairment, social contact, and sleep within the scope of dementia risk reduction on an individual level. These factors could be added to current dementia risk scores (like LIBRA) to inform and advise people on their potential for improvement.
Re-evaluation of established risk and protective factors
Most of the experts supported the inclusion of the original factors within an updated version of LIBRA. However, for 3 of the 12 factors (low/moderate alcohol intake, smoking, and midlife obesity), we found that less than half of the studies reported a significant association between the factor and cognitive outcome. For low/moderate alcohol intake, this could be due to considerable differences in cut-off values for defining exposure categories (times/week, drinks/day, g/day, etc.) among the included studies, therefore increasing methodological heterogeneity and hindering direct comparison. In addition, because we only assessed exposure levels with the largest contrast (abstinence vs. highest drinking category reported), we might have missed differences between the exposed and reference group due to the poor general health of abstainers or misclassification bias. Despite the seemingly limited consistency between alcohol consumption and cognitive outcome in our umbrella review, the most recent encountered MA reports a significantly increased risk for dementia among excessive drinkers. Additionally, high alcohol consumption could lead to other comorbidities such as hypertension, thereby increasing dementia risk even further. Considering the methodological challenges that might have affected our results, together with the strong evidence from both the MA as well as experts’ opinions, tackling high alcohol consumption remains a relevant factor for dementia risk reduction strategies. This is particularly important in light of increasing alcohol consumption.
For the factors smoking and midlife obesity, the number of included prospective cohort studies was relatively low (N = 14 and N = 9), with six (43%) and two (22%) unique primary studies reporting a significantly increased risk for dementia or cognitive decline upon exposure, respectively. While these simple consistency percentages do not take effect size and sample size of the primary studies into account, the most recent MA encountered in our review reported effect sizes of 1.52 (95% CI 1.19–1.93) and 1.31 (95% CI 1.02–1.68) for smoking and midlife obesity, respectively. As such, the evidence from both the MA and the experts indicate that smoking and midlife obesity remain relevant targets for reducing dementia risk.
Identification and operationalization of candidate risk factors
Modifiable factors suitable for dementia risk reduction on an individual level
Hearing impairment is rather unidimensional, can be objectively assessed with clearly defined cut-off values, and hence can be conveniently used to identify individuals at elevated risk for dementia. Additionally, both primary and secondary prevention of hearing impairment can be readily achieved by reducing exposure to excessive noise and, where unavoidable, promoting the use of hearing protection and hearing aids. The latter has been found to be associated with less cognitive decline compared to individuals with hearing impairment who do not use hearing aids. In contrast to hearing impairment, sleep and social contact are multidimensional.
Based on the current umbrella review and Delphi study, different aspects of social contact and sleep appear to be suitable targets for dementia risk reduction. It appears that functional aspects of social contact (i.e., loneliness, social engagement) are more consistently associated with cognitive outcome than structural aspects (i.e., living alone, social network size), and they might, therefore, be more suitable targets for dementia risk reduction. This factor could also be readily and inexpensively implemented in daily practice by stimulating individuals to participate in activities with a social component, such as attending cultural events, volunteering, or becoming member of an association or club. Additionally, this could have positive impact on other interrelated dementia risk factors, such as depression.
In contrast to social contact, results for sleep were ambiguous, as no operationalization was clearly preferred over others by experts. Within the context of identifying and informing individuals at risk for this factor, it might, therefore, be more suitable to approach sleep from a more holistic perspective instead of focusing on one specific operationalization. This can, for example, be done with broad instruments, such as the Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index, which captures several sleep-related operationalizations that we encountered in our umbrella review. Although promoting sleep hygiene could be an inexpensive strategy that can be readily incorporated in daily practice, it remains to be investigated which operationalization of sleep is most suitable for effective intervention.
Modifiable factors less suitable for dementia risk reduction on an individual level
Life course inequalities, atrial fibrillation, and psychological stress are considered less suitable for individual-level dementia risk reduction. Life course inequalities were considered outside of the scope of LIBRA, considering that they are not easily modifiable at the individual level and more likely an outcome of exposure to both up- and downstream determinants of health. Thus, they could be a suitable factor for population-wide risk reduction strategies.
In contrast, atrial fibrillation could be considered a suitable risk factor for individual-level dementia risk reduction. However, based on a relative risk of 1.39 and an estimated prevalence of 1.77% among western Europeans between 60 and 65 years old, the population attributable fraction of atrial fibrillation (approximately 0.7%) is substantially lower than the other factors previously included in dementia risk scores. As such, the added value in updated risk scores would be rather limited.
Given the current evidence, the risk factor psychological stress might also be less suitable for inclusion in updated risk scores for multiple reasons. First, the most recent meta-analysis on high psychological stress and risk for dementia (identified with a separate search after our umbrella review) based itself on only two studies for either high perceived stress or stressful life events. Thus, the evidence for stress as a risk factor is considered too low compared to other established factors. Second, psychological stress is an ill-defined concept, and therefore difficult to measure, with potential distinctions among chronic psychological stress, acute psychological stress, stressful life events, and gray areas between. Third, psychological stress is highly correlated with depression, which itself is considered a well-validated risk factor for dementia. As such, more long-term cohort studies that investigate associations between clearly defined operationalizations of psychological stress and cognitive outcomes are highly encouraged to make more definite conclusions possible.
Finally, there were four factors (visual impairment, anxiety, tooth loss, and olfactory impairment) that were identified as potentially important factors in the umbrella review (frequently reported with consistent results), but were scored rather low during the second Delphi round. This discrepancy might be caused by “confirmation bias” toward factors that are more commonly described in recent reports on modifiable dementia risk factors (e.g., the 2020 report of the Lancet Commission on Dementia Prevention, Intervention, and Care or the 2019 WHO Guidelines on Risk Reduction of Cognitive Decline and Dementia). As a result, the importance of the aforementioned four factors might have been underestimated.
Modifiable factors suitable for population-level dementia risk reduction
Some of the factors that were encountered in the first Delphi round and the umbrella review were excluded from further evaluation because they were not considered suitable for dementia risk reduction on an individual level through self-management or primary care. However, those factors still may be great entry points for risk reduction on a population level. For instance, while the risk of traumatic brain injury (TBI) can be modified (e.g., wearing a helmet when cycling), it is not possible to reverse the effect after its first occurrence. This factor would, therefore, have a static negative impact on someone's dementia risk score, without being amenable to change and providing room for improvement (except for preventing recurring TBI). For similar reasons, cerebrovascular disorders were not included as a risk factor in the original LIBRA index. Formal education was also excluded from further evaluation, as it is less amenable to change after adolescence. Instead, the LIBRA index focuses on encouraging cognitively stimulating activities at midlife. Although there is reasonable evidence for associations between various air pollutants and dementia, this field of research mainly focuses on ambient outdoor exposure, over which there is little personal control and which tends to be correlated with socioeconomic position. Despite its limited modifiability on an individual level, outdoor air pollution remains an urgent public health priority that should be addressed by policy makers, especially considering that the WHO estimates that 99% of the global population breathes air that exceeds the WHO guideline limits. In conclusion, the degree of modifiability has prompted us to exclude certain factors from consideration for an updated LIBRA index. However, such factors should be considered alongside individualized indices as part of a multi-level approach to dementia prevention.
Strengths and weaknesses
The main strength of our research design is that quantitative data from an extensive umbrella review including prospective cohort studies was combined with quantitative and qualitative data from 18 world-renowned dementia experts. As such, we acquired the best possible evidence with regard to emerging etiological factors for dementia and cognitive decline, enabling us to prioritize factors for incorporation in dementia risk scores and risk reduction strategies. Second, the extensive search strategy of our umbrella review ensured wide coverage of the literature by comparing primary studies included in all encountered SR and MA. Third, expert opinions on different operationalizations of multidimensional factors such as sleep and social contact provided insight into how these factors could be best implemented in daily practice. Finally, the relatively high number of experts included compared to the previous Delphi study, both with and without clinical background, ensured that different perspectives and research areas were well represented.
Despite the scrupulous design of our study, limitations remain. First, the umbrella review limited itself to SR/MA including observational studies, which are more vulnerable to methodological biases, such as publication bias, information bias, reverse causation (as some studies have short follow-up times), selection bias, and confounding, compared to RCTs. However, because we applied strict inclusion and exclusion criteria to the included primary studies of each SR/MA, we selected only the most suitable studies for causal inference. We realize that by excluding RCTs, some information was lost. Considering the nature of our methods, we decided to limit the review to observational studies for improved comparability among the identified factors, as RCTs are subject to short follow-up times, small sample sizes, limited generalizability, and not all factors can be subjected to RCTs. Second, as we conducted an umbrella review that included other SR/MA, our coverage of the primary literature was dependent on the inclusion and exclusion criteria applied in those SR/MA. As a result, it remains possible that some primary literature has not been included in our umbrella review. However, considering our wide search strategy we expect the amount of missed literature to be limited. Third, we only took the direction of effect sizes into account, and not their magnitude, level of adjustment (as individuals frequently have multiple risks), nor the size of the populations from which they were computed. As such, the consistency that we used to order the encountered risk factors might have been low, despite large, well-designed studies or MA reporting significant effect sizes. By cross-validating the findings from the umbrella review in the Delphi study, we limited the methodological bias that might have arisen in our review. Fourth, because multiple ethnic minorities are underrepresented across literature addressing dementia risk factors, the generalizability of our results is limited to a selected group of Western individuals living in middle- and high-income economies. Fifth, despite our efforts to include Delphi experts from diverse backgrounds, most participants originated from high-income countries. Additionally, it is possible that seminal reports on modifiable factors (like the 2020 report of the Lancet Commission on Dementia Prevention, Intervention, and Care or the 2019 WHO dementia risk reduction guidelines) might have biased the experts’ opinions toward particular factors. As such, not well-known factors and factors that might be particularly helpful for dementia risk reduction in low- and middle-income countries might have been underrepresented in this study. Sixth, interactions between modifiable factors and non-modifiable factors (e.g., biological sex, age, genetics) were not considered in this study. Therefore, it remains to be determined whether the (magnitudes of the) associations are the same within different populations. This makes a strong case for unravelling severity of risk factors, gene–environment interactions, and epigenetic mechanisms, which might explain the potentially differential effects of specific risk factors and indices on the risk for dementia or cognitive decline in distinct genetic strata.
Knowledge gaps and future directions
This Delphi study and umbrella review highlights that there is considerable high-quality observational evidence for the factors previously included in the LIBRA index and new candidate risk and protective factors for dementia risk reduction. However, for some multidimensional factors, it's not entirely clear which operationalization should be targeted. Therefore, we would benefit from more long-term cohort studies investigating the association between clearly defined operationalizations of the multidimensional factors social contact and sleep with dementia or cognitive decline, to identify the most impactful components and make more tailored recommendations with regard to brain health promotion. Additionally, the vast majority of dementia prevention literature focuses on mid- and late-life exposure, even though exposure to many dementia risk factors starts or even peaks throughout early adulthood and prolongs into very late life. This highlights the importance of future initiatives addressing brain health from a life course perspective to better tailor preventive strategies to specific age groups.
Despite the rather consistent observational evidence for the factors highlighted in the current study, the evidence from RCTs intervening on these factors is limited. Some clinical trials that address single modifiable risk factors in selected groups have found success (e.g., pharmacological treatment of hypertension), while others didn't (e.g., pharmacological treatment of type 2 diabetes or hypercholesterolemia). Whenever possible, more well-designed RCTs are needed to establish that there are indeed causal, reversible associations between some of the modifiable factors and dementia. It should be noted, however, that some of these factors could never be the subject of RCTs because of practical or ethical restraints (e.g., smoking, TBI, social contact, etc.) and some of the most successful public health interventions, like smoking cessation, have taken place without RCTs. As for the most relevant factors identified in this study, these will be tested for incorporation in an updated version of the LIBRA index. We plan to externally validate the updated LIBRA index in prospective (population-based) cohort studies to compare its predictive validity to the original LIBRA index.
To date, multiple studies have been undertaken to assess the effect of multifactorial lifestyle interventions on cognitive decline and dementia. One of the first was the Prevention of Dementia by Intensive Vascular Care (preDIVA) trial, which found no reduction of all-cause dementia risk after a 6-year multidomain cardiovascular intervention in a non-selected population. Similarly, the Multidomain Alzheimer Preventive Trial (MAPT) investigating the effect of a multidomain intervention, either alone or combined with omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation, did not show a reduction in cognitive decline over a 3-year period in people recruited from memory clinics. In contrast, the Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability (FINGER) trial is currently being conducted in multiple countries around the world (World-Wide FINGER network) after a successful trial in Finland that showed that a multidomain lifestyle intervention reduced the rate of cognitive decline compared to a control group that received regular lifestyle advice. Alongside validating the factors identified in the current study in RCTs and prioritizing them for public health, the challenge remains to translate this knowledge into societal action at appropriate levels, whether at population, community, household, or individual levels. This highlights the need for close collaboration between diverse players in all our societies across research disciplines but also policy makers, third and fourth sectors to health care and public health, to create an environment supporting sustainable lifestyle changes.
CONCLUSION
Based on the triangulation of all the current evidence, hearing impairment, social contact, and sleep have strong evidence for integration in risk scores and preventive interventions addressing brain health on the individual level. Simultaneously, more research on the different components of social contact and sleep is needed to identify the most suitable targets, considering their multidimensionality. Future studies will use these factors in an updated version of the LIBRA index for external validation. While life course inequalities, TBI, education, and air pollution were considered less suitable as targets for individualized dementia risk reduction, they appear to be relevant for population-level risk reduction.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors have nothing to report.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT
The authors declare no conflicts of interest. Author disclosures are available in the .
World Health Organization. Global action plan on the public health response to dementia 2017‐2025. 2017.
Alzheimer's Disease International. World Alzheimer Report 2021. 2021.
Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396:413‐446.
Mukadam N, Sommerlad A, Huntley J, Livingston G. Population attributable fractions for risk factors for dementia in low‐income and middle‐income countries: an analysis using cross‐sectional survey data. Lancet Glob Health. 2019;7:e596‐e603.
Ma'u E, Cullum S, Cheung G, Livingston G, Mukadam N. Differences in the potential for dementia prevention between major ethnic groups within one country: a cross sectional analysis of population attributable fraction of potentially modifiable risk factors in New Zealand. Lancet Reg Health West Pac. 2021;13: [eLocator: 100191].
Anstey KJ, Zheng L, Peters R, et al. Dementia risk scores and their role in the implementation of risk reduction guidelines. Front Neurol. 2021;12: [eLocator: 765454].
Kivipelto M, Ngandu T, Laatikainen T, Winblad B, Soininen H, Tuomilehto J. Risk score for the prediction of dementia risk in 20 years among middle aged people: a longitudinal, population‐based study. Lancet Neurol. 2006;5:735‐741.
Chosy EJ, Edland SD, Gross N, et al. The CAIDE Dementia Risk Score and the Honolulu‐Asia Aging Study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2019;48:164‐171.
Kaffashian S, Dugravot A, Elbaz A, et al. Predicting cognitive decline: a dementia risk score vs. the Framingham vascular risk scores. Neurology. 2013;80:1300‐1306.
Solomon A, Handels R, Wimo A, et al. Effect of a multidomain lifestyle intervention on estimated dementia risk. J Alzheimers Dis. 2021;82:1461‐1466.
Anstey KJ, Cherbuin N, Herath PM. Development of a new method for assessing global risk of Alzheimer's disease for use in population health approaches to prevention. Prev Sci. 2013;14:411‐421.
Anstey KJ, Cherbuin N, Herath PM, et al. A self‐report risk index to predict occurrence of dementia in three independent cohorts of older adults: the ANU‐ADRI. PLoS One. 2014;9: [eLocator: e86141].
Exalto LG, Quesenberry CP, Barnes D, Kivipelto M, Biessels GJ, Whitmer RA. Midlife risk score for the prediction of dementia four decades later. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10:562‐570.
Andrews SJ, Eramudugolla R, Velez JI, Cherbuin N, Easteal S, Anstey KJ. Validating the role of the Australian National University Alzheimer's Disease Risk Index (ANU‐ADRI) and a genetic risk score in progression to cognitive impairment in a population‐based cohort of older adults followed for 12 years. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2017;9:16.
Anstey KJ, Bahar‐Fuchs A, Herath P, et al. Body brain life: a randomized controlled trial of an online dementia risk reduction intervention in middle‐aged adults at risk of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2015;1:72‐80.
Hall A, Barbera M, Stephen R, et al. The association of the ANU‐ADRI Alzheimer risk index score with cognition in the FINGER study. Alzheimer's Dement. 2021;17: [eLocator: e053857].
Deckers K, van Boxtel MP, Schiepers OJ, et al. Target risk factors for dementia prevention: a systematic review and Delphi consensus study on the evidence from observational studies. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2015;30:234‐246.
Schiepers OJG, Kohler S, Deckers K, et al. Lifestyle for Brain Health (LIBRA): a new model for dementia prevention. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2018;33:167‐175.
Deckers K, Barbera M, Kohler S, et al. Long‐term dementia risk prediction by the LIBRA score: a 30‐year follow‐up of the CAIDE study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2020;35:195‐203.
Deckers K, Cadar D, van Boxtel MPJ, Verhey FRJ, Steptoe A, Kohler S. Modifiable risk factors explain socioeconomic inequalities in dementia risk: evidence from a population‐based prospective cohort study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;71:549‐557.
Vos SJB, van Boxtel MPJ, Schiepers OJG, et al. Modifiable risk factors for prevention of dementia in midlife, late life and the oldest‐old: validation of the LIBRA index. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;58:537‐547.
Deckers K, Nooyens A, van Boxtel M, Verhey F, Verschuren M, Kohler S. Gender and educational differences in the association between lifestyle and cognitive decline over 10 years: the Doetinchem Cohort study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;70:S31‐S41.
Heger IS, Deckers K, Schram MT, et al. Associations of the lifestyle for brain health index with structural brain changes and cognition: results from the Maastricht study. Neurology. 2021;97:e1300‐e1312.
Deckers K, Kohler S, Ngandu T, et al. Quantifying dementia prevention potential in the FINGER randomized controlled trial using the LIBRA prevention index. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17:1205‐1212.
van Middelaar T, Hoevenaar‐Blom MP, van Gool WA, et al. Modifiable dementia risk score to study heterogeneity in treatment effect of a dementia prevention trial: a post hoc analysis in the preDIVA trial using the LIBRA index. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2018;10:62.
Bramer WM, Rethlefsen ML, Kleijnen J, Franco OH. Optimal database combinations for literature searches in systematic reviews: a prospective exploratory study. Syst Rev. 2017;6:245.
Bramer WM, Giustini D, de Jonge GB, Holland L, Bekhuis T. De‐duplication of database search results for systematic reviews in EndNote. J Med Libr Assoc. 2016;104:240‐243.
Bramer WM, Milic J, Mast F. Reviewing retrieved references for inclusion in systematic reviews using EndNote. J Med Libr Assoc. 2017;105:84‐87.
Thomson RS, Auduong P, Miller AT, Gurgel RK. Hearing loss as a risk factor for dementia: a systematic review. Laryngoscope Investig Otolaryngol. 2017;2:69‐79.
Ford AH, Hankey GJ, Yeap BB, Golledge J, Flicker L, Almeida OP. Hearing loss and the risk of dementia in later life. Maturitas. 2018;112:1‐11.
Loughrey DG, Kelly ME, Kelley GA, Brennan S, Lawlor BA. Association of age‐related hearing loss with cognitive function, cognitive impairment, and dementia: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. JAMA Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2018;144:115‐126.
Yuan J, Sun Y, Sang S, Pham JH, Kong WJ. The risk of cognitive impairment associated with hearing function in older adults: a pooled analysis of data from eleven studies. Sci Rep. 2018;8:2137.
Zheng Y, Fan S, Liao W, Fang W, Xiao S, Liu J. Hearing impairment and risk of Alzheimer's disease: a meta‐analysis of prospective cohort studies. Neurol Sci. 2017;38:233‐239.
Amieva H, Ouvrard C, Giulioli C, Meillon C, Rullier L, Dartigues JF. Self‐reported hearing loss, hearing aids, and cognitive decline in elderly adults: a 25‐year study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63:2099‐2104.
Anstey KJ, Luszcz MA, Sanchez L. Two‐year decline in vision but not hearing is associated with memory decline in very old adults in a population‐based sample. Gerontology. 2001;47:289‐293.
Davies HR, Cadar D, Herbert A, Orrell M, Steptoe A. Hearing impairment and incident dementia: findings from the English Longitudinal study of ageing. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2017;65:2074‐2081.
Deal JA, Betz J, Yaffe K, et al. Hearing impairment and incident dementia and cognitive decline in older adults: the Health ABC Study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2017;72:703‐709.
Deal JA, Sharrett AR, Albert MS, et al. Hearing impairment and cognitive decline: a pilot study conducted within the atherosclerosis risk in communities neurocognitive study. Am J Epidemiol. 2015;181:680‐690.
Fischer ME, Cruickshanks KJ, Schubert CR, et al. Age‐related sensory impairments and risk of cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2016;64:1981‐1987.
Gallacher J, Ilubaera V, Ben‐Shlomo Y, et al. Auditory threshold, phonologic demand, and incident dementia. Neurology. 2012;79:1583‐1590.
Gates GA, Anderson ML, McCurry SM, Feeney MP, Larson EB. Central auditory dysfunction as a harbinger of Alzheimer dementia. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2011;137:390‐395.
Gates GA, Cobb JL, Linn RT, Rees T, Wolf PA, D'Agostino RB. Central auditory dysfunction, cognitive dysfunction, and dementia in older people. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 1996;122:161‐167.
Golub JS, Luchsinger JA, Manly JJ, Stern Y, Mayeux R, Schupf N. Observed hearing loss and incident dementia in a multiethnic cohort. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2017;65:1691‐1697.
Gurgel RK, Ward PD, Schwartz S, Norton MC, Foster NL, Tschanz JT. Relationship of hearing loss and dementia: a prospective, population‐based study. Otol Neurotol. 2014;35:775‐781.
Heywood R, Gao Q, Nyunt MSZ, et al. Hearing Loss and risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia: findings from the Singapore Longitudinal Ageing Study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2017;43:259‐268.
Hong T, Mitchell P, Burlutsky G, Liew G, Wang JJ. Visual impairment, hearing loss and cognitive function in an older population: longitudinal findings from the Blue Mountains Eye Study. PLoS One. 2016;11: [eLocator: e0147646].
Lin FR, Metter EJ, O'Brien RJ, Resnick SM, Zonderman AB, Ferrucci L. Hearing loss and incident dementia. Arch Neurol. 2011;68:214‐220.
Lin FR, Yaffe K, Xia J, et al. Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Intern Med. 2013;173:293‐299.
Lin MY, Gutierrez PR, Stone KL, et al. Vision impairment and combined vision and hearing impairment predict cognitive and functional decline in older women. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52:1996‐2002.
Lyu J, Kim HY. Gender‐specific incidence and predictors of cognitive impairment among older Koreans: findings from a 6‐year Prospective Cohort study. Psychiatry Investig. 2016;13:473‐479.
Tomioka K, Okamoto N, Morikawa M, Kurumatani N. Self‐reported hearing loss predicts 5‐year decline in higher‐level functional capacity in high‐functioning elderly adults: the Fujiwara‐Kyo Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63:2260‐2268.
Bucholc M, McClean PL, Bauermeister S, et al. Association of the use of hearing aids with the conversion from mild cognitive impairment to dementia and progression of dementia: a longitudinal retrospective study. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2021;7: [eLocator: e12122].
Byun H, Chung JH, Lee SH, Kim EM, Kim I. Dementia in a hearing‐impaired population according to hearing aid use: a nationwide population‐based study in Korea. Ear Hear. 2022;43:1661‐1668.
Jiang F, Mishra SR, Shrestha N, et al. Association between hearing aid use and all‐cause and cause‐specific dementia: an analysis of the UK Biobank cohort. Lancet Public Health. 2023;8:e329‐e338.
Lin FR, Pike JR, Albert MS, et al. Hearing intervention versus health education control to reduce cognitive decline in older adults with hearing loss in the USA (ACHIEVE): a multicentre, randomised controlled trial. The Lancet. 2023; 402(10404):786‐797.
Wayne RV, Johnsrude IS. A review of causal mechanisms underlying the link between age‐related hearing loss and cognitive decline. Ageing Res Rev. 2015;23:154‐166.
Powell DS, Oh ES, Reed NS, Lin FR, Deal JA. Hearing loss and cognition: what we know and where we need to go. Front Aging Neurosci. 2021;13: [eLocator: 769405].
Boss L, Kang DH, Branson S. Loneliness and cognitive function in the older adult: a systematic review. Int Psychogeriatr. 2015;27:541‐553.
Lara E, Martin‐Maria N, De la Torre‐Luque A, et al. Does loneliness contribute to mild cognitive impairment and dementia? A systematic review and meta‐analysis of longitudinal studies. Ageing Res Rev. 2019;52:7‐16.
Desai R, John A, Stott J, Charlesworth G. Living alone and risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Ageing Res Rev. 2020;62: [eLocator: 101122].
Kuiper JS, Zuidersma M, Oude Voshaar RC, et al. Social relationships and risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta‐analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Res Rev. 2015;22:39‐57.
Kuiper JS, Zuidersma M, Zuidema SU, et al. Social relationships and cognitive decline: a systematic review and meta‐analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Int J Epidemiol. 2016;45:1169‐1206.
Shen C, Rolls E, Cheng W, et al. Associations of social isolation and loneliness with later dementia. Neurology. 2022; 99(2):e164‐e175.
Duffner LA, Deckers K, Cadar D, Steptoe A, de Vugt M, Köhler S. The role of cognitive and social leisure activities in dementia risk: assessing longitudinal associations of modifiable and non‐modifiable risk factors. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2022;31: [eLocator: e5].
Kelly ME, Duff H, Kelly S, et al. The impact of social activities, social networks, social support and social relationships on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review. Syst Rev. 2017;6:259.
Hsiao YH, Chang CH, Gean PW. Impact of social relationships on Alzheimer's memory impairment: mechanistic studies. J Biomed Sci. 2018;25:3.
Sachdev PS. Social health, social reserve and dementia. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2022;35:111‐117.
Smith KJ, Gavey S, NE RI, Kontari P, Victor C. The association between loneliness, social isolation and inflammation: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020;112:519‐541.
Devore EE, Grodstein F, Schernhammer ES. Sleep duration in relation to cognitive function among older adults: a systematic review of observational studies. Neuroepidemiology. 2016;46:57‐78.
Fan L, Xu W, Cai Y, Hu Y, Wu C. Sleep duration and the risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta‐analysis of prospective cohort studies. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2019;20:1480‐1487. e5.
Kim HB, Myung SK, Lee SM, Park YC. Korean Meta‐Analysis Study G. Longer Duration of Sleep and Risk of Cognitive Decline: a Meta‐Analysis of Observational Studies. Neuroepidemiology. 2016;47:171‐180.
Liang Y, Qu LB, Liu H. Non‐linear associations between sleep duration and the risks of mild cognitive impairment/dementia and cognitive decline: a dose‐response meta‐analysis of observational studies. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2019;31:309‐320.
Lo JC, Groeger JA, Cheng GH, Dijk DJ, Chee MW. Self‐reported sleep duration and cognitive performance in older adults: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Sleep Med. 2016;17:87‐98.
Wu L, Sun D, Tan Y. A systematic review and dose‐response meta‐analysis of sleep duration and the occurrence of cognitive disorders. Sleep Breath. 2018;22:805‐814.
Lam EWK, Chung F, Wong J. Sleep‐disordered breathing, postoperative delirium, and cognitive impairment. Anesth Analg. 2017;124:1626‐1635.
Leng Y, McEvoy CT, Allen IE, Yaffe K. Association of sleep‐disordered breathing with cognitive function and risk of cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. JAMA Neurol. 2017;74:1237‐1245.
Zhu X, Zhao Y. Sleep‐disordered breathing and the risk of cognitive decline: a meta‐analysis of 19,940 participants. Sleep Breath. 2018;22:165‐173.
de Almondes KM, Costa MV, Malloy‐Diniz LF, Diniz BS. Insomnia and risk of dementia in older adults: systematic review and meta‐analysis. J Psychiatr Res. 2016;77:109‐115.
Kitamura T, Miyazaki S, Sulaiman HB, Akaike R, Ito Y, Suzuki H. Insomnia and obstructive sleep apnea as potential triggers of dementia: is personalized prediction and prevention of the pathological cascade applicable? EPMA J. 2020;11:355‐365.
Bubu OM, Brannick M, Mortimer J, et al. Sleep cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Sleep. 2017:40.
Shi L, Chen SJ, Ma MY, et al. Sleep disturbances increase the risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2018;40:4‐16.
Bailey MJ, Soliman EZ, McClure LA, et al. Relation of atrial fibrillation to cognitive decline (from the REasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke [REGARDS] Study). Am J Cardiol. 2021;148:60‐68.
Chen LY, Norby FL, Gottesman RF, et al. Association of atrial fibrillation with cognitive decline and dementia over 20 years: the ARIC‐NCS (Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Neurocognitive Study). J Am Heart Assoc. 2018;7.
de Bruijn RF, Heeringa J, Wolters FJ, et al. Association between atrial fibrillation and dementia in the general population. JAMA Neurol. 2015;72:1288‐1294.
Ding M, Fratiglioni L, Johnell K, et al. Atrial fibrillation, antithrombotic treatment, and cognitive aging: a population‐based study. Neurology. 2018;91:e1732‐e1740.
Dublin S, Anderson ML, Haneuse SJ, Heckbert SR, Crane PK, Breitner JC, et al. Atrial fibrillation and risk of dementia: a prospective cohort study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59:1369‐1375.
Elias MF, Sullivan LM, Elias PK, et al. Atrial fibrillation is associated with lower cognitive performance in the Framingham offspring men. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis. 2006;15:214‐222.
Marengoni A, Qiu C, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Atrial fibrillation, stroke and dementia in the very old: a population‐based study. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32:1336‐1337.
Rusanen M, Kivipelto M, Levälahti E, et al. Heart diseases and long‐term risk of dementia and Alzheimer's disease: a population‐based CAIDE study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;42:183‐191.
Rydén L, Zettergren A, Seidu NM, et al. Atrial fibrillation increases the risk of dementia amongst older adults even in the absence of stroke. J Intern Med. 2019;286:101‐110.
Singh‐Manoux A, Fayosse A, Sabia S, et al. Atrial fibrillation as a risk factor for cognitive decline and dementia. Eur Heart J. 2017;38:2612‐2618.
Tilvis RS, Kähönen‐Väre MH, Jolkkonen J, Valvanne J, Pitkala KH, Strandberg TE. Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged people over a 10‐year period. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59:268‐274.
Forti P, Maioli F, Pisacane N, Rietti E, Montesi F, Ravaglia G. Atrial fibrillation and risk of dementia in non‐demented elderly subjects with and without mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2007;44(1):155‐165. Suppl.
Haring B, Leng X, Robinson J, et al. Cardiovascular disease and cognitive decline in postmenopausal women: results from the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study. J Am Heart Assoc. 2013;2: [eLocator: e000369].
Rastas S, Verkkoniemi A, Polvikoski T, et al. Atrial fibrillation, stroke, and cognition: a longitudinal population‐based study of people aged 85 and older. Stroke. 2007;38:1454‐1460.
Islam MM, Poly TN, Walther BA, et al. Association between atrial fibrillation and dementia: a meta‐analysis. Front Aging Neurosci. 2019;11:305.
Koh YH, Lew LZW, Franke KB, et al. Predictive role of atrial fibrillation in cognitive decline: a systematic review and meta‐analysis of 2.8 million individuals. Europace. 2022;24:1229‐1239.
Liu DS, Chen J, Jian WM, Zhang GR, Liu ZR. The association of atrial fibrillation and dementia incidence: a meta‐analysis of prospective cohort studies. J Geriatr Cardiol. 2019;16:298‐306.
Proietti R, AlTurki A, Vio R, et al. The association between atrial fibrillation and Alzheimer's disease: fact or fallacy? A systematic review and meta‐analysis. J Cardiovasc Med (Hagerstown). 2020;21:106‐112.
Rivard L, Friberg L, Conen D, et al. Atrial fibrillation and dementia: a report from the AF‐SCREEN International Collaboration. Circulation. 2022;145:392‐409.
Bezabhe WM, Bereznicki LR, Radford J, et al. Oral anticoagulant treatment and the risk of dementia in patients with atrial fibrillation: a population‐based cohort study. J Am Heart Assoc. 2022;11: [eLocator: e023098].
Park SH, Lee SR, Choi EK, et al. low risk of dementia in patients with newly diagnosed atrial fibrillation and a clustering of healthy lifestyle behaviors: a Nationwide Population‐Based Cohort Study. J Am Heart Assoc. 2022;11: [eLocator: e023739].
Wang AY, Hu HY, Ou YN, et al. Socioeconomic status and risks of cognitive impairment and dementia: a systematic review and meta‐analysis of 39 prospective studies. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2023;10:83‐94.
Klee M, Leist AK, Veldsman M, Ranson JM, Llewellyn DJ. Socioeconomic deprivation, genetic risk, and incident dementia. Am J Prev Med. 2023; 64(5):621‐630.
Stuart KE, Padgett C. A systematic review of the association between psychological stress and dementia risk in humans. J Alzheimers Dis. 2020;78:335‐352.
Islamoska S, Ishtiak‐Ahmed K, Hansen ÅM, et al. Vital exhaustion and incidence of dementia: results from the Copenhagen City Heart Study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;67:369‐379.
Johansson L, Guo X, Waern M, et al. Midlife psychological stress and risk of dementia: a 35‐year longitudinal population study. Brain. 2010;133:2217‐2224.
Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Bennett DA, et al. Proneness to psychological distress and risk of Alzheimer disease in a biracial community. Neurology. 2005;64:380‐382.
Justice NJ. The relationship between stress and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Stress. 2018;8:127‐133.
Fillmore KM, Stockwell T, Chikritzhs T, Bostrom A, Kerr W. Moderate alcohol use and reduced mortality risk: systematic error in prospective studies and new hypotheses. Ann Epidemiol. 2007;17:S16‐23.
Zeisser C, Stockwell TR, Chikritzhs T. Methodological biases in estimating the relationship between alcohol consumption and breast cancer: the role of drinker misclassification errors in meta‐analytic results. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014;38:2297‐2306.
Xu W, Wang H, Wan Y, et al. Alcohol consumption and dementia risk: a dose‐response meta‐analysis of prospective studies. Eur J Epidemiol. 2017;32:31‐42.
Roerecke M, Kaczorowski J, Tobe SW, Gmel G, Hasan OSM, Rehm J. The effect of a reduction in alcohol consumption on blood pressure: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Lancet Public Health. 2017;2:e108‐e120.
Grucza RA, Sher KJ, Kerr WC, et al. Trends in adult alcohol use and binge drinking in the early 21st‐century United States: a meta‐analysis of 6 national survey series. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2018;42:1939‐1950.
Niu H, Qu Y, Li Z, et al. Smoking and risk for Alzheimer disease: a meta‐analysis based on both case‐control and cohort study. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2018;206:680‐685.
Qu Y, Hu HY, Ou YN, et al. Association of body mass index with risk of cognitive impairment and dementia: a systematic review and meta‐analysis of prospective studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020;115:189‐198.
Domènech‐Abella J, Lara E, Rubio‐Valera M, et al. Loneliness and depression in the elderly: the role of social network. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2017;52:381‐390.
Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF 3rd, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28:193‐213.
Chugh SS, Havmoeller R, Narayanan K, et al. Worldwide epidemiology of atrial fibrillation: a Global Burden of Disease 2010. Study Circulation. 2014;129:837‐847.
Franks KH, Bransby L, Saling MM, Pase MP. Association of stress with risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. J Alzheimers Dis. 2021;82:1573‐1590.
Crosswell AD, Lockwood KG. Best practices for stress measurement: how to measure psychological stress in health research. Health Psychol Open. 2020;7: [eLocator: 2055102920933072].
Hammen C. Stress and depression. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1:293‐319.
Quaglio G, Gallucci M, Brand H, Dawood A, Cobello F. Traumatic brain injury: a priority for public health policy. Lancet Neurol. 2017;16:951‐952.
Peters R, Ee N, Peters J, Booth A, Mudway I, Anstey KJ. Air pollution and dementia: a systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;70:S145‐S163.
World Health Organization. Ambient (outdoor) air pollution. 2022.
Hammer GP, du Prel JB, Blettner M. Avoiding bias in observational studies: part 8 in a series of articles on evaluation of scientific publications. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2009;106:664‐668.
Anstey KJ, Peters R, Zheng L, et al. Future directions for dementia risk reduction and prevention research: an International Research Network on Dementia Prevention Consensus. J Alzheimers Dis. 2020;78:3‐12.
Farina FR, Booi L, Occhipinti JA, et al. Young adult brain capital: a new opportunity for dementia prevention. J Alzheimers Dis. 2023;94:415‐423.
Peters R, Breitner J, James S, et al. Dementia risk reduction: why haven't the pharmacological risk reduction trials worked? An in‐depth exploration of seven established risk factors. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). 2021;7: [eLocator: e12202].
Moll van Charante EP, Richard E, Eurelings LS, et al. Effectiveness of a 6‐year multidomain vascular care intervention to prevent dementia (preDIVA): a cluster‐randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2016;388:797‐805.
Andrieu S, Guyonnet S, Coley N, et al. Effect of long‐term omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation with or without multidomain intervention on cognitive function in elderly adults with memory complaints (MAPT): a randomised, placebo‐controlled trial. Lancet Neurol. 2017;16:377‐389.
Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at‐risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2015;385:2255‐2263.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2024. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the "License"). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
A 2013 systematic review and Delphi consensus study identified 12 modifiable risk and protective factors for dementia, which were subsequently merged into the “LIfestyle for BRAin health” (LIBRA) score. We systematically evaluated whether LIBRA requires revision based on new evidence. To identify modifiable risk and protective factors suitable for dementia risk reduction, we combined an umbrella review of systematic reviews and meta‐analyses with a two‐round Delphi consensus study. The review of 608 unique primary studies and opinions of 18 experts prioritized six modifiable factors: hearing impairment, social contact, sleep, life course inequalities, atrial fibrillation, and psychological stress. Based on expert ranking, hearing impairment, social contact, and sleep were considered the most suitable candidates for inclusion in updated dementia risk scores. As such, the current study shows that dementia risk scores need systematic updates based on emerging evidence. Future studies will validate the updated LIBRA score in different cohorts.
Highlights
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
1 Alzheimer Centrum Limburg, Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MHeNs), Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands
2 UNSW Ageing Futures Institute, Kensington, New South Wales, Australia
3 Cambridge Public Health, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
4 Centre for Healthy Brain Ageing (CHeBA), Discipline of Psychiatry and Mental Health, School of Clinical Medicine, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
5 Norwegian National Centre for Ageing and Health, Vestfold Hospital Trust, Tønsberg, Norway
6 Feinberg School of Medicine, Department of Medical Social Sciences, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois, USA
7 Departments of Psychiatry, Neurology, and Epidemiology, School of Medicine and School of Public Health, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA
8 Division of Psychiatry, University College London, London, UK
9 Richman Family Precision Medicine Center of Excellence in Alzheimer's Disease, Johns Hopkins Bayview, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland, USA
10 Theme Inflammation and Aging, Medical Unit Aging, Karolinska University Hospital, Stockholm, Sweden
11 Schlegel‐UW Research Institute for Aging, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
12 Radboudumc Alzheimer Center, Donders Center of Medical Neurosciences, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
13 School of Biomedical Sciences, University of New South Wales, Kensington, New South Wales, Australia
14 Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, New York, USA
15 Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway