1. Introduction
Childhood obesity is a critical global health issue, described by the World Bank as a “ticking time bomb”. Rising prevalence rates, driven by poor dietary habits and insufficient physical activity, have made it a pressing concern worldwide [1]. According to joint estimates from UNICEF and the World Health Organization, 6% of children under 5 years old and 18% of children aged 5–19 years old are overweight, accounting for over 380 million children globally [2]. In Türkiye, the situation is similarly concerning, with 8% of under-five children classified as overweight or obese [3]. Although specific data for Samsun are unavailable, the region’s socio-economic diversity provides a unique context for exploring childhood obesity, influenced by both Eastern and Western cultural norms. In Türkiye, the Ministry of Health’s programs are present to promote balanced nutrition and physical activity in early childhood [4]. However, these efforts rely heavily on parental awareness, particularly the maternal recognition of children’s weight status, to initiate preventive measures at home.
Maternal perception—specifically, the ability to recognize a child’s overweight status [5,6]—is pivotal in early obesity prevention. However, maternal misperception—failure to accurately assess a child’s weight [7,8]—frequently delays action, perpetuating unhealthy behaviors. Childhood obesity increases the risk of chronic diseases such as cardiovascular disease, diabetes, and cancer later in life [1,4].
While substantial research has investigated parental feeding practices, such as restrictive or pressuring behaviors [9,10,11,12], the relation between maternal perceptions and children’s eating behaviors remains understudied. Emotional eating, for instance, may be shaped by familial stressors, while food responsiveness reflects parental modeling [13]. Additionally, satiety responsiveness has been linked to consistent feeding practices that encourage self-regulation [14]. Mothers’ understanding of their children’s eating styles and weight status is crucial, as it directly affects their ability to implement appropriate nutritional decisions. Misjudging a child’s eating behaviors or body size can lead to overfeeding, underfeeding, or inappropriate dietary restrictions, potentially contributing to unhealthy weight trajectories or nutritional imbalances [9,10,12,15]. In addition, the Children’s Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ) provides valuable insights into children’s eating behaviors, including food responsiveness, emotional eating, and satiety responsiveness, all of which influence body weight outcomes [16].
Parenting styles—defined as the emotional climate in which child-rearing behaviors are expressed—may play a crucial role in shaping children’s eating behaviors and weight outcomes. Authoritative parenting, which balances warmth and control, is consistently associated with healthier body weight and eating behaviors, fostering self-regulation and autonomy in dietary choices. This parenting style promotes open communication about food preferences, encouraging healthier eating behaviors and body image perceptions in children [17,18,19]. Authoritarian parenting, defined by high control and low warmth, often leads to power struggles over food, prompting children to eat in response to external pressures rather than their internal cues. This approach has been linked to a higher risk of childhood obesity and problematic eating behaviors [18,19,20]. Permissive parenting, characterized by high warmth but low control, poses unique challenges due to its lack of structure in promoting healthy eating habits. Children raised by permissive parents often have greater freedom to consume unhealthy foods and may struggle with self-regulation [21,22]. Overprotective parenting, characterized by excessive involvement in children’s choices—including dietary decisions—can undermine autonomy and self-regulation, often resulting in unhealthy eating habits and distorted body image perceptions [20,21,23]. The Nurturing Care Framework emphasizes the importance of responsive feeding, encouraging parents to recognize and appropriately respond to their children’s hunger and satiety cues [24,25]. This approach not only fosters a deeper understanding of children’s eating behaviors but also supports healthy growth and enables timely nutritional interventions. While authoritative parenting naturally aligns with responsive feeding by promoting autonomy and self-regulation, other parenting styles—such as the authoritarian, permissive, and overprotective styles—may require tailored strategies to effectively integrate these principles [19,21,22,25].
This study is guided by Bronfenbrenner’s Ecological Systems Theory, the Bidirectional Parent–Child Interaction Model, and Social Comparison Theory to explore the dynamics between maternal perception and childhood obesity. Bronfenbrenner’s Ecological Systems Theory provides a broader framework to understand how environmental factors across multiple levels influence maternal perception and child health outcomes [26,27,28]. In Samsun, a region blending Eastern and Western cultural influences, this framework highlights how socio-economic diversity impacts the maternal recognition of weight status, with higher socio-economic status hypothesized to improve accuracy. The Bidirectional Parent–Child Interaction Model underscores the mutual influence between parents and children, where maternal feeding practices shape children’s eating behaviors, and these behaviors, in turn, affect maternal strategies. For instance, a child’s food responsiveness might prompt specific parental approaches, reinforcing patterns that influence the maternal perception of body size [11,29,30]. Finally, Social Comparison Theory offers insight into how maternal perceptions are shaped by comparisons with peers and societal norms. In Samsun, cultural ideals associating larger body sizes with health and prosperity may contribute to the underestimation of children’s weight status, delaying necessary interventions. By integrating these frameworks, this study explores how socio-cultural, familial, and individual factors interact to influence maternal perception, providing valuable insights for addressing childhood obesity in diverse contexts [31,32,33].
We hypothesize that mothers with higher socio-economic status, better education, less information taken from relatives, authoritative parenting styles, and no significant health conditions are more likely to accurately perceive their children’s body size. Conversely, mothers with authoritarian parenting styles are more prone to misperception. Additionally, children’s eating behaviors—as reported by mothers—can further influence mothers’ recognition of their children’s body size. This theoretical framework aligns with research showing that parenting styles and child feeding practices interact to shape children’s health outcomes [34,35]. We aim to address the gap by examining the alignment between child eating behaviors and mothers’ accurate estimation of their children’s body size among kindergarten-aged children in Samsun, Turkey, while also considering parenting styles, socio-economic factors, and maternal health status. The findings of this study will contribute to the achievement of Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs), particularly Goal 2 (Zero Hunger) and Goal 3 (Good Health and Well-being), by emphasizing early maternal awareness and culturally relevant interventions that promote healthy child growth and responsive feeding practices, thereby informing sustainable strategies for addressing childhood obesity [36].
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Study Design
This cross-sectional survey was conducted between November and December 2023 in preschools in Samsun, Türkiye, and data analysis was performed in January–April 2024.
2.2. The Determination of the Study Sample
According to Turkish Statistical Institute data, 92,792 children aged 5–9 reside in Samsun, with approximately 20,000 estimated to be 5 years old [37]. To detect a condition prevalence of 50% with a margin of error of d = 0.05, the required sample size was calculated as 377. To account for potential non-participation and data entry errors, an additional 20% was added, leading to a target sample size of 452.
2.3. Participant Selection and Sampling
This study’s sample was stratified across five regions within Samsun, classified by the Socio-Economic Development Ranking Index (SEGE). Sample sizes for each region were calculated and weighted proportionally to the number of children in each region to ensure representativeness [38]. Stratified sampling was employed based on the SEGE, ensuring that each region’s socio-economic diversity was proportionally represented. Within each SEGE-defined region, public preschools were randomly selected for participant recruitment, further enhancing the representativeness of the sample.
Ethical approval was granted by the Samsun University Clinical Research Ethics Committee, and permissions, along with a list of preschools and enrolment numbers, were obtained from the Samsun Provincial Directorate of National Education. Children aged 60–72 months old attending these institutions were then grouped according to SEGE-defined regions. For regions comprising multiple districts, a random sampling approach using a random numbers table was applied to select districts for this study. Within each selected district, data collection began at schools with the highest child enrollment. If the target sample size was unmet, additional schools were randomly selected until the target was achieved.
The inclusion criteria were as follows: mother–child pairs, with the child aged 5–6 years old, enrolled in a preschool under the Ministry of National Education in Samsun, accompanied by the mother, residing in Samsun, willing to participate, and providing informed consent.
Exclusion criteria were non-compliance with these criteria, refusal to participate, single-parent families, mothers who do not speak Turkish, families without internet access, children with a Body Mass Index Z-score (BAZ) < −1 (indicating risk of underweight), and children with chronic illnesses affecting growth.
2.4. Participant Flow and Data Cleaning Process
A total of 512 mothers were invited to participate in this study, of which 431 agreed to participate. However, certain cases were excluded during the data cleaning process. Specifically, 12 mother–child dyads were excluded because the mothers were from single-parent or broken families, which could influence the mother’s perception of the child’s body size and eating behaviors. In these cases, the primary caregiver might not have been the participating mother, or the child might have been under the care of multiple caregivers. Additionally, 42 children had missing anthropometric data, 38 children had a BAZ below −1, and 21 children had chronic illnesses that could potentially affect growth, including asthma, epilepsy, PFAPA (Periodic Fever, Aphthous Stomatitis, Pharyngitis, and Adenitis), Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus (DM), and cerebral palsy. After these exclusions, the final analysis included 318 mother–child pairs (see Figure 1).
2.5. Measures
Data collection was initially conducted through Google Forms surveys for the mothers’ questionnaires. Following maternal consent, children’s height and weight were measured at their respective schools.
2.5.1. Surveys
The surveys for this study were designed and distributed to participating mothers via Google Forms, enabling secure, efficient, and accessible online data collection. Links to the surveys were shared with mothers through their children’s teachers, ensuring accessibility and efficient data collection. This digital approach minimized transcription errors by automating data entry, thus streamlining data preparation for analysis. To maintain participant anonymity, no personally identifiable information (PII) was collected. Each mother and child were assigned a unique code, allowing for the accurate pairing of data from mother–child pairs without compromising confidentiality. These codes were essential for data alignment during analysis, preserving the integrity and validity of responses.
The survey consisted of four primary sections that assess socio-demographic characteristics, child eating behaviors, parenting attitudes, and maternal perceptions of child body size using a modified visual assessment tool.
The Socio-Demographic Characteristics section gathered key information, including parents’ and child’s age, parents’ education attainment and employment status, family structure (e.g., nuclear or extended), financial support status, resources consulted by the mother for educational and health-related decisions, primary caregivers supporting the mother, birth-related details (e.g., birth weight, prematurity, birth order), child’s history of hospital admissions, and duration of preschool education.
The Children’s Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ), developed by Wardle et al. [16], was used to evaluate various eating behaviors. This validated tool consists of 35 Likert-scale items (ranging from 1 = never to 5 = always) across eight subscales: food responsiveness, emotional overeating, emotional undereating, enjoyment of food, desire to drink, satiety responsiveness, slowness in eating, and food fussiness. These subscales assess a wide range of eating patterns and behavioral traits linked to risks such as obesity and eating disorders. The Turkish version of the CEBQ, adapted by Yılmaz et al. [39], demonstrated reliability, with Cronbach’s alpha values ranging from 0.61 to 0.86 across subscales. In our study, this tool was administered via Google Forms, enabling efficient and standardized data collection. Table 1 summarizes the percentile distribution and reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) of the CEBQ scores as calculated in our study. Among the CEBQ subscales, ’Enjoyment of Food’ displayed the highest reliability (α = 0.86), while ’Emotional Overeating’ and ’Emotional Undereating’ exhibited relatively lower reliability values (α = 0.67 and α = 0.69, respectively). These results confirm acceptable internal consistency across all subscales in the context of our sample.
Parenting attitudes were assessed using the Parental Attitude Scale (PAS), a validated tool designed for Turkish parents of young children [40]. The PAS comprises 46 Likert-scale items (ranging from 1 = never to 5 = always) and categorizes parenting styles into four groups: authoritative, authoritarian, overprotective, and permissive. As shown in Table 1, Authoritative Parenting emerged as the most reliable subscale (α = 0.91) and had the highest mean score (75.8), highlighting its predominance among the participating mothers. Conversely, ’Permissive Parenting’ showed the lowest reliability (α = 0.68), with a mean score of 20.0, indicating its less frequent preference among this population.
Maternal perceptions of child body size were evaluated using a modified visual assessment tool adapted from previous studies. Warschburger and Kröller [41] used silhouette scales to evaluate maternal perceptions of weight and associated health risks, reporting a 64.5% accuracy rate in identifying overweight silhouettes and highlighting the influence of maternal education on perception accuracy. Hager et al. [42] developed the Toddler Silhouette Scale, demonstrating robust validity and reliability measures, including content validity (κ = 0.710, p < 0.001) and concurrent validity (r = 0.633, p < 0.001). Similarly, Yalçın et al. [5] used a validated visual tool to assess maternal perception, reporting significant correlation between verbal and visual assessments (r = −0.620, p < 0.001). The tool consisted of seven gender-neutral images representing a range of weight percentiles, ensuring broad applicability across different contexts [5,41,42]. To confirm its clarity and suitability for the study population, the tool was pre-tested with a sample of 30 participants prior to the main study. The visual tool was integrated into the Google Forms survey, where mothers were asked to respond to the following question: “Which image best represents your child’s current size?” For analytical purposes, maternal responses were grouped into three categories: scores 1–3 were categorized as underweight, score 4 as normal weight, and scores 5–7 as overweight. This categorization enabled a structured analysis of maternal perceptions, offering valuable insights into how mothers perceive their child’s body size relative to their actual weight status.
2.5.2. Anthropometric Measures
Children’s anthropometric measurements, including height and weight, were directly measured by the researcher to ensure data accuracy and consistency. Height was measured to the nearest 0.1 cm using a wall-mounted paper stadiometer, and weight was recorded to the nearest 0.1 kg using Xiaomi Mi Body Composition Scale 2 (Xiaomi Inc., Beijing, China). All measurements were conducted under standardized conditions, with children in light clothing and without shoes. The Weight-for-Age Z-score (WAZ), Height-for-Age Z-score (HAZ), and BMI-for-age Z-score (BAZ) according to age and sex were subsequently calculated using the World Health Organization’s AnthroPlus software version 1.0.4. BAZ values were categorized in accordance with the World Health Organization’s standards for children aged 5–19 years old [43]. Scores below −2 SD were classified as thinness, while those between −2 SD and −1 SD indicated a risk of underweight. Scores ranging from −1 SD to +1 SD were considered normal weight. Values between >+1 SD and ≤+2 SD represented overweight status, and scores above +2 SD were categorized as obesity.
2.6. Variables
The primary independent variables included in this study were family characteristics (parents’ age, education level, and employment status; family structure [nuclear or extended]; and financial support status), parenting attitudes (authoritative, authoritarian, overprotective, and permissive styles), sources of information and caregiving support, birth and health history of the child, years spent in preschool education, and the subscales of the CEBQ.
The dependent variables were maternal estimation accuracy (derived from BMI Z-scores) and the maternal perception of the child’s body size. The maternal perceptions of child weight were assessed using a visual tool, and the actual weight category was matched with the maternally perceived child size. These were coded into three categories:
Accurate Estimation: Maternal perception aligned with the child’s actual weight category.
Underestimation: Maternal perception was smaller than the child’s actual weight category.
Overestimation: Maternal perception was larger than the child’s actual weight category.
For the final analysis, three cases of underestimation were excluded to ensure statistical robustness. The remaining data were categorized into two groups for comparison: accurate estimation and overestimation.
2.7. Bias
Several measures were implemented to minimize potential sources of bias and ensure the validity of the study findings. Data collection was conducted via Google Forms, which reduced social desirability bias by allowing participants to respond more candidly online. Anonymity was maintained by assigning unique codes to each mother and child pair, promoting honest responses. All anthropometric measurements were conducted by a single trained researcher to ensure consistency and minimize inter-observer variability. Additionally, standardized protocols were followed for measuring children’s height and weight, which helped reduce potential measurement bias. Although stratified sampling was employed to ensure representativeness across socio-economic strata, the exclusion of certain groups, such as single-parent families or children with chronic illnesses, may have introduced selection bias. These exclusions were made because chronic illnesses can influence growth patterns, potentially altering maternal perceptions of weight. Similarly, single-parent families or those with shared custody arrangements might experience differences in maternal perceptions due to unique family dynamics and caregiving structures. By excluding these groups, this study aimed to focus on maternal perceptions under more typical family conditions and reduce confounding effects.
Validated tools, such as the CEBQ and PAS, were confirmed with high Cronbach’s alpha values, supporting the robustness of the assessments. Finally, a pre-test of the visual tool helped ensure clarity and prevent interpretation bias among participants. By implementing these measures, this study aimed to mitigate potential biases and strengthen the reliability of its conclusions.
2.8. Statistical Analysis
Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 23.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Descriptive statistics were presented as frequencies and percentages for categorical variables and as the mean ± standard deviation or median with interquartile ranges (IQRs) for continuous variables, depending on the data distribution. The distribution characteristics of continuous variables were assessed using histograms, kurtosis, skewness, and the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test.
The primary variable of interest was the accuracy of the maternal assessment of the child’s body size (accurate vs. underestimate). Differences in child characteristics and maternal–child variables between the “accurate” and “underestimate” groups were evaluated using the independent samples t-test or Mann–Whitney U test for continuous variables, based on distribution. Differences in categorical variables were assessed using the chi-square test or Fisher’s exact test, as appropriate.
Variables with a p-value < 0.2 in univariate analysis were included in a multiple logistic regression model to examine their associations with accurate maternal assessments of normal weight, overweight, and combined (overall) child groups. Additionally, interquartile ranges (IQRs) were calculated for each subscale of the Children’s Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ), and the association between each unit change in subscale scores and maternal assessment accuracy was analyzed. Adjusted odds ratios (AORs) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were reported.
A p-value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
3. Results
3.1. General Characteristics of Participants
This study involved 318 mother–child pairs. Mothers had a mean age of 34.7 ± 5.0 years, while fathers were slightly older at 37.9 ± 5.8 years. Most mothers (81.1%) and fathers (81.8%) had completed at least 12 years of formal education. Approximately one-third of the mothers (30.8%) were employed. Regarding health, 8.2% of mothers reported having a chronic illness, while 11.9% were taking medication during the study period. Most families were nuclear (93.1%), and a minority (6.6%) relied on financial support either from the government or private institutions.
Children had a mean age of 5.5 ± 0.3 years old, with a slight majority being male (51.3%). Birth order data indicated that 51.9% were firstborns, and 20.8% were only children. The majority (83.6%) were born at a normal weight (2.5–3.9 kg), with 8.5% born at a low birth weight (<2.5 kg). Prematurity was reported in 12.3% of the children. A substantial proportion (34.9%) had a history of hospital admissions, and approximately one-third (33.0%) were in their first year of preschool (Table 2).
Primary caregivers assisting mothers included fathers (79.9%), grandparents (33.0%), and elder siblings (10.1%). Few families relied on nannies or other caregivers (1.6% each). The most frequently consulted sources for educational information were teachers (76.7%) and books (47.8%), while healthcare advice was primarily obtained from pediatricians (86.5%) and family physicians (70.8%).
The scores of maternal parenting styles are given in Table 2.
Children’s eating behaviors included a median food responsiveness score of 9 [6–11], a mean enjoyment of food score of 15.4 ± 4.8, and an average satiety responsiveness score of 22.6 ± 5.5, highlighting differences in response to food cues. Additional behaviors such as emotional overeating (median: 6 [4–8]), desire to drink (mean: 8.1 ± 3.5), slowness in eating (mean: 10.6 ± 4.0), emotional undereating (mean: 11.5 ± 3.8), and food fussiness (median: 6.5 [5–9]) (Table 2).
Among the mothers surveyed, 59.7% underestimated their child’s weight, with only 39.3% accurately assessing their child’s weight status. Overestimation was rare, occurring in just 0.9% of cases (Table 3). Due to the small size of the overestimation subgroup, meaningful statistical analysis could not be performed, and these cases were not included in the subgroup comparisons.
Maternal perceptions of children’s weight status, assessed through a visual tool, were compared with these objective measures to evaluate accuracy and trends in underestimation or overestimation (Figure 2). Maternal perceptions of child size were categorized into three groups, underweight (V1–V3), normal weight (V4), and overweight (V5–V7), based on the distribution pattern and the slope of the green line representing obesity rates (BAZ > +1). For the underweight category (V1–V3), obesity rates ranged from 16% to 21.7%. In the normal weight category (V4), the obesity rate was 37.3%. For the overweight category (V5–V7), obesity rates ranged from 77.8% to 87.5%.
3.2. Comparative Analysis Between Maternal Accurate and Underestimation Groups
An analysis of familial characteristics revealed that maternal or paternal age, education level, maternal employment status, and maternal health conditions showed no statistically significant association with accurate maternal estimation. However, family structure was associated with accurate estimation, with mothers in nuclear families more likely to underestimate their child’s weight (62.1%) compared to those in extended families (36.4%), with a statistically significant difference (p = 0.017, Table 4).
No significant differences in parenting style scores were observed between accurate estimators and underestimators across all styles, including authoritative, authoritarian, overprotective, and permissive parenting (p > 0.05, Table 4).
While no significant differences were observed in the educational information sources consulted by mothers, health information sources revealed a notable difference in accurate estimation (p > 0.05). Mothers who consulted relatives for health-related advice were significantly more likely to underestimate their child’s weight (74.6%) compared to those who did not (56.5%, p = 0.007, Table 4). This suggests that reliance on non-professional health advice may contribute to the maternal underestimation of weight status.
The analysis of child characteristics revealed a significant association between birth order and maternal estimation accuracy. The mothers of firstborn children were more likely to estimate accurately (44.2%) compared to those with later-born children, where underestimation was higher (65.3%), with a significant difference (p = 0.024). However, no significant associations were found for child’s age, sex, gestational duration, birth weight, or hospital admission history and years of preschool education (p > 0.05, Table 5).
Maternal estimation accuracy also showed a strong relation with BMI-for-age Z-scores (BAZs). Among children with a BAZ ≤ 1 (normal weight), 52.2% were accurately estimated, whereas underestimation was prevalent (84.9%) among children with a BAZ > 1 (overweight) (p < 0.001). This highlights a critical trend where maternal perception becomes less accurate as the child’s weight increases, potentially delaying the recognition of weight-related issues (Table 5).
Mothers with accurate perceptions reported higher scores in food responsiveness (median: 9 [7–12] vs. 5 [4–8], p = 0.028) and the enjoyment of food (mean: 16.3 ± 4.8 vs. 14.6 ± 4.7, p = 0.003). Conversely, underestimators reported higher satiety responsiveness (mean: 23.2 ± 5.7 vs. 21.9 ± 5.2, p = 0.042) and slowness in eating (mean: 11.2 ± 4.2 vs. 9.9 ± 3.6, p = 0.004). These findings suggest that children’s responsiveness to food cues and eating patterns may influence maternal perception, with underestimation potentially linked to less observable cues such as satiety and eating speed. No significant associations were identified for other child characteristics, including age, sex, gestational duration, birth weight, breastfeeding duration, or hospital admission history. Similarly, years of preschool education showed no significant association (p > 0.05, Table 5).
3.3. Multiple Logistic Regression Analysis of Factors Influencing Maternal Perception in Normal Weight, Overweight, and Overall Child Groups
A logistic regression analysis examined factors influencing maternal perception in normal weight, overweight, and combined (overall) child groups (Table 6).
In the normal weight group, emotional overeating was associated with a 2.38-fold increase in accurate perception likelihood (OR = 0.42, 95% CI = 0.20–0.88, p = 0.021). Additionally, being an only child increased the likelihood of accurate maternal perception compared to children with siblings (OR = 0.37, 95% CI = 0.14–0.99, p = 0.047). Consulting relatives for educational advice also improved perception accuracy (OR = 11.16, 95% CI = 1.05–118.77, p = 0.046).
In the overweight group, employed mothers were less likely to underestimate their child’s weight compared to non-employed mothers (OR = 0.05, 95% CI = 0.00–0.56, p = 0.015). Higher scores in the enjoyment of food (OR = 0.03, 95% CI = 0.00–0.35, p = 0.005) and satiety responsiveness (OR = 0.09, 95% CI = 0.01–0.67, p = 0.019) reduced underestimation likelihood. First-year school attendance was similarly linked to improved perception accuracy (OR = 0.50, 95% CI = 0.25–0.98, p = 0.045).
In the overall group, slow eating was associated with a higher likelihood of underestimation (OR = 1.62, 95% CI = 1.04–2.54, p = 0.033). Conversely, first-year school attendance (OR = 0.54, 95% CI = 0.30–0.97, p = 0.041) and higher BAZs (OR = 12.16, 95% CI = 5.80–25.51, p < 0.001) were associated with improved accuracy in weight perception.
4. Discussion
4.1. Maternal Perceptions, Ecological Influences, and Implications for Childhood Obesity Prevention
This study provides a comprehensive analysis of the maternal perceptions of child weight status, identifying critical trends and associations with socio-cultural, familial, and child-specific factors. Grounded in Bronfenbrenner’s Ecological Systems Theory, the findings highlight the multi-layered determinants of maternal underestimation, a significant global barrier to addressing childhood obesity [26,27,28].
4.2. The Underestimation of Child Weight: A Global Challenge
Our findings revealed that 59.7% of mothers underestimated their child’s weight, with underestimation reaching 84.9% among overweight children. This trend mirrors findings from diverse cultural settings, emphasizing its pervasive nature. This finding is consistent with studies from diverse cultural settings. For instance, traditional preferences for heavier child body types in Latin America and lower socio-economic groups contribute to frequent misperceptions [8,44]. Similarly, cultural norms associating larger body sizes with health and well-being are evident in Türkiye, where socio-economic disparities and marginalized populations further complicate maternal awareness [6]. The persistence of underestimation in high-resource settings, such as the Netherlands, suggests that addressing maternal misperception requires more than public awareness campaigns; it necessitates tackling implicit biases and societal norms [45,46]. Aligning with frameworks like the SDGs and the UNICEF Nurturing Care Framework, equitable interventions can bridge these gaps and empower families [2,24,25,47].
4.3. Microsystem: The Family Context
At the microsystem level, nuclear family structures were associated with higher underestimation rates (62.1%) compared to extended families (36.4%, p = 0.017). This discrepancy suggests that the absence of diverse familial perspectives in nuclear families may limit opportunities for maternal discussions or comparisons regarding a child’s weight status. Extended family members, particularly grandparents, might provide additional input that helps mothers evaluate their child’s body size more accurately. Birth order also played a significant role: firstborn children were more accurately perceived than their later-born counterparts, supporting Social Comparison Theory. First-time mothers, likely relying more on professional guidance and direct observations, exhibited enhanced perception accuracy. This finding aligns with prior research suggesting that maternal perceptions are shaped by early parenting experiences and external influences [31,32].
Child-specific factors also influenced perception. Emotional overeating in the normal weight group increased the likelihood of accurate perception by 2.38-fold (OR = 0.42, 95% CI = 0.20–0.88, p = 0.021). Conversely, low satiety responsiveness and slow eating were associated with underestimation, reflecting maternal misinterpretations of appetite cues. These findings align with the Bidirectional Parent–Child Interaction Model, which posits that children’s eating behaviors directly influence parental perceptions and feeding practices [29]. Contrary to expectations, no significant associations were found between parenting styles and maternal perception accuracy. This may suggest that socio-cultural norms, educational attainment, and exposure to health messaging exert a stronger influence than parenting style alone. These findings emphasize the complexity of maternal perception, highlighting the need for future longitudinal studies to explore how these factors interact over time to shape maternal awareness.
4.4. Mesosystem: Interactions with Schools and Healthcare Providers
The mesosystem level highlights the role of schools and healthcare providers in shaping maternal perceptions. Paradoxically, first-year school attendance was associated with higher underestimation (OR = 0.54, 95% CI = 0.30–0.97, p = 0.041), potentially due to the maternal normalization of higher weights when comparing their child with peers. This phenomenon, explained by Social Comparison Theory, highlights the potential for school environments to unintentionally perpetuate misperceptions. Consulting relatives for educational advice was positively associated with accurate perception in the normal weight group (OR = 11.16, 95% CI = 1.05–118.77, p = 0.046). This underscores the nuanced influence of non-professional sources. However, reliance on relatives for healthcare advice warrants further exploration, as it may perpetuate cultural biases and misperceptions.
4.5. Macrosystem: Societal Norms and Global Frameworks
At the macrosystem level, societal norms associating higher child body weights with resilience or prosperity remain entrenched, complicating public health efforts. Systemic interventions are required to challenge these narratives and promote accurate maternal perceptions. Global frameworks such as the UNICEF Nurturing Care Framework and the WHO’s Obesity Prevention Strategies advocate for multifaceted approaches to tackle childhood obesity. These frameworks align with the SDGs, particularly Goal 3, which emphasizes health equity and well-being. Targeting socio-economically disadvantaged families through culturally sensitive education and leveraging community resources can bridge the gap between maternal perceptions and clinical realities, ensuring that no child is left behind [2,6,36,48].
4.6. Responsive Feeding and Nurturing Care: A Path Forward
This study underscores the relevance of responsive feeding as a key component of the Nurturing Care Framework. Mothers who accurately perceived their child’s weight reported higher levels of food responsiveness and enjoyment of food in their children, consistent with research linking child-centered feeding practices to better maternal awareness [25]. Conversely, mothers who underestimated their child’s weight reported lower satiety responsiveness and higher emotional overeating in their children. These findings emphasize the importance of moving from restrictive parent-centered feeding to responsive child-centered feeding approaches, that respect children’s hunger and satiety cues. Low satiety responsiveness was associated with the maternal underestimation of child weight, potentially reflecting the maternal misinterpretation of appetite cues. According to the Bidirectional Parent–Child Interaction Model, children’s eating behaviors can shape maternal perceptions and feeding practices, reinforcing these misperceptions. While our study did not directly assess responsive feeding practices, the CEBQ results highlight their potential in improving parental awareness of children’s nutritional status and reducing underestimation [2,16,39]. By respecting children’s hunger and satiety cues, responsive feeding practices can improve parental awareness of children’s nutritional status and reduce underestimation. This aligns with recommendations by UNICEF and WHO for promoting healthy growth and development in early childhood [2,16,25,49].
4.7. Strengths and Limitations
This study provides valuable insights into the maternal perceptions of child weight status and their associations with familial and child-specific factors within the socio-cultural context of Samsun, Türkiye. By including participants from all socio-economic development levels (SEGE) in Samsun, this study ensures representation across diverse socio-economic contexts, enhancing its generalizability within the region. Moreover, its focus on preschool-aged children provides critical data on maternal awareness during a pivotal stage for shaping long-term health behaviors. The inclusion of maternal reports on children’s eating behaviors offers a novel, child-centered perspective, shifting the focus from parental practices to children’s dietary patterns. This approach provides fresh insights into the interplay between maternal perceptions and body size recognition, an area previously underexplored in research.
Despite these strengths, several limitations should be acknowledged. The cross-sectional design precludes causal inferences, and future longitudinal studies are necessary to explore how maternal misperceptions influence child health outcomes over time. Additionally, reliance on maternal self-reported data introduces potential biases, such as social desirability bias, which may affect responses about sensitive topics like parenting styles. Although confidentiality measures were implemented, these biases cannot be fully excluded.
The assessment of parenting styles was based on maternal self-reports at a single time point, limiting the ability to capture the dynamic and evolving nature of parenting behaviors. Longitudinal research could provide a more comprehensive understanding of how these styles influence maternal perception accuracy and child outcomes over time. Furthermore, maternal BMI, a known factor influencing weight perceptions, was not collected, representing a gap in the dataset. Including maternal BMI in future research could provide insights into how mothers’ own weight status affects their perceptions of their children’s weight.
This study excluded fathers. However, paternal parenting styles and involvement likely impact child weight perceptions. The decision to focus solely on mothers aimed to create a homogeneous sample; however, future studies including fathers would provide a more holistic perspective. Similarly, the exclusion of children from private preschools or those not enrolled in any early education program introduces potential selection bias, limiting the generalizability of the findings to children in alternative educational settings. Certain groups, such as single-parent families and children with chronic illnesses, were excluded to reduce confounding factors. While this decision was made with the aim to focus on maternal perceptions under typical family conditions, these exclusions may overlook unique dynamics that influence perceptions. Chronic illnesses, for instance, affect growth patterns and maternal views on weight, while single-parent families bring distinctive caregiving challenges.
The small sample size of mothers who overestimated their children’s weight restricted subgroup analyses, underscoring the need for larger and more diverse sample sizes in future research. Additionally, this study’s findings are specific to the socio-cultural and economic context of Samsun, and maternal perceptions may differ across regions with varying cultural norms, socio-economic profiles, and healthcare access. Expanding research to broader national or global scales is necessary to capture these variations and inform culturally tailored public health interventions.
Anthropometric measurements, such as skinfold thickness, were not included, limiting the ability to assess detailed body fat distribution. Including such measurements in future studies could enhance the accuracy of obesity classification and deepen the understanding of maternal perception in relation to objective weight measures.
By addressing these limitations through methodological enhancements and expanded participant diversity, future research can build on the current study’s findings and contribute to a more nuanced understanding of maternal perceptions and their role in preventing childhood obesity. Despite these limitations, this study makes a significant contribution by highlighting the multifaceted factors influencing the maternal perceptions of child weight status. Its novel focus on children’s eating behaviors, as perceived by mothers, provides a foundation for future research and the development of targeted strategies to prevent childhood obesity.
5. Conclusions
This study demonstrates the pervasive nature of the maternal underestimation of child weight status, particularly among overweight children, with rates as high as 84.9%. These findings highlight critical barriers to early intervention for childhood obesity. While no significant associations were observed between parenting styles and maternal perception accuracy, child-specific factors, such as food responsiveness and the enjoyment of food, emerged as influential determinants. This emphasizes the need for targeted early education programs for mothers that promote the awareness of healthy weight standards and encourage the adoption of responsive feeding practices as part of nurturing care.
To advance this field, future research should focus on longitudinal studies examining the long-term impact of maternal perceptions and feeding practices on child weight outcomes. Additionally, exploring how diverse cultural and socio-economic contexts shape these dynamics will be crucial. Public health strategies should align with global frameworks, such as the Nurturing Care Framework and the Sustainable Development Goals, to ensure culturally sensitive and inclusive interventions. These efforts can empower parents to address childhood obesity effectively, fostering healthier growth and development outcomes for children worldwide.
Conceptualization, S.S.Y. and R.G.S.; methodology, S.S.Y. and R.G.S.; validation, S.S.Y. and R.G.S.; formal analysis, S.S.Y. and R.G.S.; investigation, R.G.S.; data curation, S.S.Y. and R.G.S.; writing—original draft preparation, R.G.S.; writing—review and editing, S.S.Y.; supervision, S.S.Y. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Ethical approval for this study was granted on 15 February 2023 under protocol code SÜ-KAEK-2023 3/11. Subsequently, permission for research implementation was obtained from the Samsun Provincial Directorate of National Education, Republic of Turkey, on 8 May 2023, with the official letter reference E-67180656-604.01.01.-75766931. This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from all participating mothers prior to data collection.
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study.
The data presented in this study are available upon request from the corresponding author due to ethical considerations.
This manuscript was prepared in adherence to the STROBE (Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology) guidelines to ensure the comprehensive and transparent reporting of this study’s methodology, findings, and limitations. We sincerely thank the participating schools, their staff, and the mothers of the students for their valuable cooperation in facilitating this study.
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Footnotes
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Figure 1. Participant flow diagram: illustrating selection and exclusion process.
The percentile distribution of the Children’s Eating Behavior Questionnaire scores for preschoolers and the Parental Attitude Scale scores for their mothers.
Mean | Percentile Values | Number of Items | Cronbach’s | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
5 | 15 | 25 | 50 | 75 | 85 | 95 | ||||
Children’s Eating Behavior Questionnaire | ||||||||||
Food responsiveness | 9.3 | 5 | 5 | 6 | 9 | 11 | 13 | 17 | 5 | 0.75 |
Emotional overeating | 6.3 | 4 | 4 | 4 | 6 | 8 | 9 | 11 | 4 | 0.67 |
Enjoyment of food | 15.4 | 7 | 10 | 12 | 16 | 19 | 20 | 24 | 5 | 0.86 |
Desire to drink | 8.1 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 8 | 11 | 12 | 15 | 3 | 0.79 |
Satiety responsiveness | 22.6 | 13 | 17 | 19 | 22 | 27 | 29 | 32 | 7 | 0.74 |
Slowness in eating | 10.6 | 5 | 6 | 8 | 10 | 13 | 15 | 19 | 4 | 0.77 |
Emotional undereating | 11.5 | 5 | 8 | 9 | 11 | 14 | 16 | 18 | 4 | 0.69 |
Food fussiness | 7.1 | 3 | 3 | 5 | 7 | 9 | 11 | 13 | 3 | 0.78 |
Parental Attitude Scale | ||||||||||
Authoritative parenting | 75.8 | 57 | 69 | 73 | 78 | 82 | 84 | 85 | 17 | 0.91 |
Authoritarian parenting | 19.2 | 11 | 13 | 15 | 18 | 22 | 25 | 30 | 11 | 0.78 |
Overprotective parenting | 33.3 | 21 | 26 | 29 | 34 | 38 | 41 | 44 | 9 | 0.79 |
Permissive parenting | 20.0 | 12 | 14 | 16 | 20 | 23 | 25 | 29 | 9 | 0.68 |
General characteristics, n = 318.
Familial Characteristics | N (%) | Child Characteristics | N (%) |
---|---|---|---|
Mother’s age | 34.7± 5.0 | Birth order, first child | 165 (51.9) |
Father’s age | 37.9± 5.8 | Only child, yes | 66 (20.8) |
Maternal education, ≥12 years | 258 (81.1) | Child age | 5.5 ± 0.3 |
Paternal education, ≥12 years | 260 (81.8) | Sex, male | 163 (51.3) |
Working mother, yes | 98 (30.8) | Gestation duration < 37 weeks | 39 (12.3) |
Mother’s chronic disease status, yes | 26 (8.2) | Birth weight, kg | |
Mother’s medication use, yes | 38 (11.9) | <2.5 | 27 (8.5) |
Family type, nuclear | 296 (93.1) | 2.5–3.9 | 266 (83.6) |
Financial support, yes | 21 (6.6) | ≥4.0 | 25 (7.9) |
Residence (SEGE Index) | Breastfeeding duration | ||
Level 1 | 93 (29.2) | <6 months | 66 (20.8) |
Level 2 | 76 (23.9) | 6–11 months | 52 (16.4) |
Level 3 | 65 (20.4) | 12–23 months | 98 (30.8) |
Level 4 | 46 (14.5) | ≥24 months | 102 (32.1) |
Level 5 | 38 (11.9) | Child’s hospital admissions, yes | 111 (34.9) |
Years of preschool education | |||
Sources Consulted by the Mother | First year | 105 (33.0) | |
Educational Information Sources | Second year | 164 (51.6) | |
Teachers | 244 (76.7) | ≥Third year | 49 (15.4) |
Books | 152 (47.8) | Children’s Eating Behavior | |
Online sources | 147 (46.2) | Food responsiveness | 9 (6–11) |
Relatives | 92 (28.9) | Emotional overeating | 6 (4–8) |
Friends | 174 (54.7) | Enjoyment of food | 15.4 ± 4.8 |
Health Information Sources | Desire to drink | 8.1 ± 3.5 | |
Pediatricians | 275 (86.5) | Satiety responsiveness | 22.6 ± 5.5 |
Family physicians | 225 (70.8) | Slowness in eating | 10.6 ± 4.0 |
Family health center nurses | 53 (16.7) | Emotional undereating | 11.5 ± 3.8 |
Healthcare professionals | 305 (95.9) | Food fussiness | 6.5 (5–9) |
Books | 21 (6.6) | Maternal Parenting Style | |
Online resources | 71 (22.3) | Authoritative parenting | 78 (73–82) |
Relatives | 68 (21.4) | Authoritarian parenting | 18 (15–22) |
Friends | 50 (15.7) | Overprotective parenting | 34 (29–38) |
Permissive parenting | 20 ± 5.2 |
Anthropometric measurements of children and perceptions of their mothers, n = 318.
Anthropometric Measurements | N (%) | Perception/Accurate Estimation | N (%) |
---|---|---|---|
Weight-for-Age Z-score | Maternal perception of child’s size | ||
Median [25–75 percentile] | 0.46 [(−0.23)–(1.27)] | Underweighted, 1–3 | 188 (59.1) |
>(2) z-score | 32 (10.1) | Normal weighted, 4 | 75 (23.6) |
Height-for-Age Z-score | Overweighted, 5–7 | 55 (17.3) | |
Median [25–75 percentile] | 0.18 [(0.52)–(0.89)] | ||
<(−1)–≥(−2) z-score (at risk of stunted) | 35 (11.0) | ||
<(−2) z-score (stunted) | 2 (0.6) | Accurate estimation compared to BAZ | |
BMI-for-Age Z-score | Overestimation | 3 (0.9) | |
Median [25–75 percentile] | 0.47 [(0.17)–(1.31)] | Accurate estimation | 125 (39.3) |
>1–≤2 z-score (overweight) | 68 (21.4) | Underestimation | 190 (59.7) |
>2 z-score (obese) | 38 (12.0) |
Differences in familial characteristics, information sources, and maternal parenting styles between accurate and underestimation groups (n = 315).
Accurate Estimation * | Underestimation * | p | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
n | 125 | 190 | ||
Familial characteristics | ||||
Mother’s age | 34.5 ± 4.5 | 34.9 ± 5.3 | 0.485 | |
Father’s age | 34.5 ± 5.7 | 34.9 ± 5.3 | 0.312 | |
Maternal education | ≥12 years | 104 (40.6) | 152 (59.4) | 0.476 |
<12 years | 21 (35.6) | 38 (64.4) | ||
Paternal education | ≥12 years | 102 (39.5) | 156 (60.5) | 0.909 |
<12 years | 23 (40.4) | 34 (59.6) | ||
Family type | Nuclear | 111 (37.9) | 182 (62.1) | 0.017 |
Extended | 14 (63.6) | 8 (36.4) | ||
Mother’s chronic disease status | Yes | 9 (34.6) | 17 (65.4) | 0.581 |
No | 116 (40.1) | 173 (59.9) | ||
Mother employed | Yes | 46 (47.4) | 51 (52.6) | 0.061 |
No | 79 (36.2) | 139 (63.8) | ||
Financial/social support | Yes | 5 (23.8) | 16 (76.2) | 0.124 |
No | 120 (40.8) | 174 (59.2) | ||
Educational Information Sources of the Mother | ||||
Teachers | Yes | 101 (41.7) | 141 (58.3) | 0.175 |
No | 24 (32.9) | 49 (67.1) | ||
Books | Yes | 62 (41.6) | 87 (58.4) | 0.508 |
No | 63 (38.0) | 103 (62.0) | ||
Online sources | Yes | 59 (40.1) | 88 (59.9) | 0.878 |
No | 66 (39.3) | 102 (60.7) | ||
Relatives | Yes | 30 (32.6) | 62 (67.4) | 0.099 |
No | 95 (42.6) | 128 (57.4) | ||
Friends | Yes | 68 (39.3) | 105 (60.7) | 0.880 |
No | 57 (40.1) | 85 (59.9) | ||
Health Information Sources of the Mother | ||||
Healthcare professionals | Yes | 122 (40.4) | 180 (59.6) | 0.211 |
No | 3 (23.1) | 10 (76.9) | ||
Books | Yes | 6 (28.6) | 15 (71.4) | 0.281 |
No | 119 (40.5) | 175 (59.5) | ||
Online resources | Yes | 25 (35.2) | 46 (64.8) | 0.382 |
No | 100 (41.0) | 144 (59.0) | ||
Relatives | Yes | 17 (25.4) | 50 (74.6) | 0.007 |
No | 108 (43.5) | 140 (56.5) | ||
Friends | Yes | 16 (32.0) | 34 (68.0) | 0.226 |
No | 109 (41.1) | 156 (58.9) | ||
Maternal parenting style | ||||
Authoritative parenting | 79 [73.5–83] | 78 [73–82] | 0.587 | |
Authoritarian parenting | 18 [15–22] | 19 [15–22.3] | 0246 | |
Overprotective parenting | 34 [30–38] | 33 [28–38] | 0.531 | |
Permissive parenting | 20.5 ± 5.6 | 19.8 ± 4.8 | 0.226 |
* Mean ± SD or median [25th–75th percentile] or N (row percentage).
Differences in child characteristics and children’s eating behavior styles between accurate and underestimation groups (n = 315).
Accurate Estimation * | Underestimation * | p | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Birth order | First child | 73 (44.2) | 92 (55.8) | 0.024 |
≥2 | 52 (34.7) | 98 (65.3) | ||
Only child | Yes | 31 (47.0) | 35 (53.0) | 0.083 |
No | 94 (37.8) | 155 (62.2) | ||
Child’s age | 5.4 ± 0.3 | 5.5 ± 0.3 | 0.130 | |
Sex | Female | 65 (42.5) | 88 (57.5) | 0.323 |
Male | 60 (37.0) | 102 (63.0) | ||
Gestational duration | <37 weeks | 12 (30.8) | 27 (69.2) | 0.224 |
≥37 weeks | 113 (40.9) | 163 (59.1) | ||
Birth weight, kg | ≥2500 | 114 (39.6) | 174 (60.4) | 0.906 |
<2500 | 11 (40.7) | 16 (59.3) | ||
Breastfeeding duration | <6 months | 26 (39.4) | 40 (60.6) | 0.412 |
6–11 months | 15 (29.4) | 36 (70.6) | ||
12–23 months | 41 (42.3) | 56 (57.7) | ||
≥24 months | 43 (42.6) | 58 (57.4) | ||
Child’s hospital admissions | Yes | 44 (40.7) | 64 (59.3) | 0.782 |
No | 81 (39.1) | 126 (60.9) | ||
Years of preschool education | First year | 50 (47.6) | 55 (52.4) | 0.126 |
≥Second year | 75 (35.7) | 135 (64.3) | ||
BMI-for-Age Z-score | ≤1 | 109 (52.2) | 100 (47.8) | <0.001 |
>1 | 16 (15.1) | 90 (84.9) | ||
Children’s Eating Behavior | ||||
Food responsiveness | 9 [7–12] | 5 [4–8] | 0.028 | |
Emotional overeating | 6 [4–8] | 5 [4–8] | 0.056 | |
Enjoyment of food | 16.3 ± 4.8 | 14.6 ± 4.7 | 0.003 | |
Desire to drink | 7.8 ± 3.3 | 8.3 ± 3.6 | 0.247 | |
Satiety responsiveness | 21.9 ± 5.2 | 23.2 ± 5.7 | 0.042 | |
Slowness in eating | 9.9 ± 3,6 | 11.2 ± 4.2 | 0.004 | |
Emotional undereating | 11.4 ± 3.8 | 11.7 ± 3.8 | 0.513 | |
Food fussiness | 7 [5–9] | 6 [5–9] | 0.586 |
* Mean ± SD or median [25th–75th percentile] or N (row percentage).
Multiple logistic regression analysis of factors influencing maternal underestimation in normal weight, overweight, and overall child groups.
Normal | Overweight | Overall | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AOR | 95% CI | p | AOR | 95% CI | p | AOR | 95% CI | p | |
Birth Order (Firstborn vs. Others) | 0.55 | 0.27–1.12 | 0.098 | 0.09 | 0.01–1.22 | 0.070 | 0.55 | 0.30–1.03 | 0.062 |
Only Child (Only vs. Siblings) | 0.37 | 0.14–0.99 | 0.047 | 7.03 | 0.64–76.88 | 0.110 | 0.63 | 0.29–1.36 | 0.237 |
First Year in School vs. Second Year or More | 0.50 | 0.25–0.98 | 0.045 | 0.12 | 0.02–0.93 | 0.042 | 0.54 | 0.30–0.97 | 0.041 |
Family Structure (Extended vs. Nuclear) | 0.72 | 0.22–2.37 | 0.589 | 0.46 | 0.02–8.77 | 0.602 | 0.48 | 0.17–1.35 | 0.163 |
Working Mother (Yes vs. No) | 0.95 | 0.49–1.85 | 0.873 | 0.05 | 0.00–0.56 | 0.015 | 0.66 | 0.37–1.20 | 0.175 |
Financial/Social Support (Yes vs. No) | 0.68 | 0.16–2.96 | 0.605 | 0.49 | 0.01–18.75 | 0.701 | 0.78 | 0.24–2.57 | 0.684 |
Consulting Relatives for Education (Yes vs. No) | 11.16 | 1.05–118.77 | 0.046 | 0.61 | 0.02–22.71 | 0.787 | 4.68 | 0.88–24.85 | 0.070 |
Consulting Teachers for Education (Yes vs. No) | 0.57 | 0.28–1.19 | 0.136 | 2.79 | 0.42–18.53 | 0.287 | 0.88 | 0.46–1.68 | 0.699 |
BMI-for-Age Z-score (BAZ 1 vs. 0) | 12.16 | 5.80–25.51 | 0.000 | ||||||
Food Responsiveness, for each IQR increase | 1.29 | 0.65–2.58 | 0.469 | 0.62 | 0.18–2.12 | 0.444 | 0.72 | 0.43–1.20 | 0.204 |
Emotional Overeating, for each IQR increase | 0.42 | 0.20–0.88 | 0.021 | 2.13 | 0.57–8.00 | 0.263 | 0.81 | 0.47–1.40 | 0.452 |
Enjoyment of Food, for each IQR increase | 1.01 | 0.50–2.03 | 0.984 | 0.03 | 0.00–0.35 | 0.005 | 0.67 | 0.37–1.23 | 0.200 |
Satiety Responsiveness, for each IQR increase | 1.32 | 0.74–2.37 | 0.347 | 0.09 | 0.01–0.67 | 0.019 | 0.95 | 0.57–1.58 | 0.839 |
Slow Eating, for each IQR increase | 1.54 | 0.92–2.60 | 0.102 | 1.79 | 0.45–7.05 | 0.406 | 1.62 | 1.04–2.54 | 0.033 |
IQR: interquartile range.
References
1. Shekar, M.; Popkin, B. Obesity: Health and Economic Consequences of an Impending Global Challenge; World Bank Publications: Washington, DC, USA, 2020.
2. UNICEF. UNICEF Advocacy Strategy Guidance for the Prevention of Overweight and Obesity in Children and Adolescents; UNICEF: New York, NY, USA, 2020.
3. Hacettepe University Institute of Population. 2018 Turkey Demographic and Health Survey (TDHS); Hacettepe University Institute of Population Studies: Ankara, Türkiye, 2019.
4. Barışkın, E.; Ersoy, G.; Görpelioğlu, S.; Karaoğlu, L.; Kılıç, B.G.; Köksal, G.; Pekcan, G.; Yalçın, S.S.; Yetkin, İ.; Zergeroğlu, A.M. Birinci Basamak Hekimler İçin Obesite ile Mücadele El Kitabı; T.C. Sağlık Bakanlığı Türkiye Halk Sağlığı Kurumu Obezite Diyabet ve Metabolik Hastalıklar Daire Başkanlığı: Ankara, Türkiye, 2013.
5. Yalçın, S.S.; Serdaroğlu, E.; İnce, O.T. Parental perception and child’s nutritional status. Turk. J. Pediatr.; 2016; 58, pp. 63-68. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.24953/turkjped.2016.01.009] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27922238]
6. Yalcin, S.S.; Savcı, R.G.; Yalcin, S.; Eryurt, M.A. Accuracy of Syrain Refugee Mothers’ perceptions of Newborn’s birth size: Insights from a National Survey in Turkey. J. Immigr. Minor. Health; 2023; 25, pp. 1363-1373. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10903-023-01519-9]
7. Lundahl, A.; Kidwell, K.M.; Nelson, T.D. Parental underestimates of child weight: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics; 2014; 133, pp. e689-e703. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-2690] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24488736]
8. Pasch, L.A.; Penilla, C.; Tschann, J.M.; Martinez, S.M.; Deardorff, J.; de Groat, C.L.; Gregorich, S.E.; Flores, E.; Butte, N.F.; Greenspan, L.C. Preferred Child Body Size and Parental Underestimation of Child Weight in Mexican-American Families. Matern. Child Health J.; 2016; 20, pp. 1842-1848. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10995-016-1987-z] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27016351]
9. Vollmer, R.L.; Mobley, A.R. Parenting styles, feeding styles, and their influence on child obesogenic behaviors and body weight. A review. Appetite; 2013; 71, pp. 232-241. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.08.015]
10. Webber, L.; Cooke, L.; Hill, C.; Wardle, J. Child adiposity and maternal feeding practices: A longitudinal analysis. Am. J. Clin. Nutr.; 2010; 92, pp. 1423-1428. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.2010.30112]
11. Yalcin, S.; Oflu, A.; Akturfan, M.; Yalcin, S.S. Characteristics of picky eater children in Turkey: A cross-sectional study. BMC Pediatr.; 2022; 22, 431. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12887-022-03458-0]
12. Shloim, N.; Edelson, L.R.; Martin, N.; Hetherington, M.M. Parenting Styles, Feeding Styles, Feeding Practices, and Weight Status in 4-12 Year-Old Children: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Front. Psychol.; 2015; 6, 1849. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01849]
13. Michels, N.; Sioen, I.; Braet, C.; Eiben, G.; Hebestreit, A.; Huybrechts, I.; Vanaelst, B.; Vyncke, K.; De Henauw, S. Stress, emotional eating behaviour and dietary patterns in children. Appetite; 2012; 59, pp. 762-769. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.08.010]
14. Boutelle, K.N.; Manzano, M.A.; Eichen, D.M. Appetitive traits as targets for weight loss: The role of food cue responsiveness and satiety responsiveness. Physiol. Behav.; 2020; 224, 113018. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113018]
15. Özen, G.; Güneş, B.; Yalçın, S.; Yalçın, S.S. Mother-child pairs’ eating and feeding behaviours in two different nutritional status from two distinct provinces. BMC Pediatr.; 2024; 24, 25. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12887-023-04481-5] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38191315]
16. Wardle, J.; Guthrie, C.A.; Sanderson, S.; Rapoport, L. Development of the Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry; 2001; 42, pp. 963-970. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00792] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11693591]
17. Baumrind, D. Effects of authoritative parental control on child behavior. Child Dev.; 1966; 37, pp. 887-907. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1126611]
18. Maccoby, E. Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction. Handbook of Child Psychology; Mussen, P.H.; Hetherington, E.M. Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1983; Volume 4, pp. 1-100.
19. Blissett, J. Relationships between parenting style, feeding style and feeding practices and fruit and vegetable consumption in early childhood. Appetite; 2011; 57, pp. 826-831. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.05.318] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21651932]
20. Ventura, A.K.; Birch, L.L. Does parenting affect children’s eating and weight status?. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act.; 2008; 5, 15. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-5-15]
21. Savage, J.S.; Fisher, J.O.; Birch, L.L. Parental influence on eating behavior: Conception to adolescence. J. Law Med. Ethics; 2007; 35, pp. 22-34. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-720X.2007.00111.x]
22. Patrick, H.; Nicklas, T.A.; Hughes, S.O.; Morales, M. The benefits of authoritative feeding style: Caregiver feeding styles and children’s food consumption patterns. Appetite; 2005; 44, pp. 243-249. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2002.07.001]
23. Birch, L.; Savage, J.S.; Ventura, A. Influences on the Development of Children’s Eating Behaviours: From Infancy to Adolescence. Can. J. Diet Pract. Res.; 2007; 68, pp. s1-s56.
24. Black, M.M.; Aboud, F.E. Responsive feeding is embedded in a theoretical framework of responsive parenting. J. Nutr.; 2011; 141, pp. 490-494. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3945/jn.110.129973]
25. World Health OrganizationUnited Nations Children’s FundWorld Bank Group. Nurturing Care for Early Childhood Development: A Framework for Helping Children Survive and Thrive to Transform Health and Human Potential; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2018.
26. Bronfenbrenner, U. The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1979; Volume 2, pp. 139-163.
27. Bronfenbrenner, U.; Morris, P.A. The bioecological model of human development. Handbook of Child Psychology; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2007; Volume 1.
28. Tudge, J.R.; Mokrova, I.; Hatfield, B.E.; Karnik, R.B. Uses and misuses of Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological theory of human development. J. Fam. Theory Rev.; 2009; 1, pp. 198-210. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-2589.2009.00026.x]
29. Bell, R.Q. A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychol. Rev.; 1968; 75, pp. 81-95. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0025583] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/4870552]
30. Lollis, S.; Kuczynski, L. Beyond One Hand Clapping: Seeing Bidirectionality in Parent-Child Relations. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh.; 1997; 14, pp. 441-461. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407597144002]
31. Festinger, L. A theory of social comparison processes. Hum. Relat.; 1954; 7, pp. 117-140. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001872675400700202]
32. Suls, J.; Wheeler, L. A Selective History of Classic and Neo-Social Comparison Theory. Handbook of Social Comparison: Theory and Research; Suls, J.; Wheeler, L. Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 3-19.
33. Wood, J.V. Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal attributes. Psychol. Bull.; 1989; 106, pp. 231-248. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.231]
34. Boles, R.E.; Reiter-Purtill, J.; Zeller, M.H. Persistently obese youth: Interactions between parenting styles and feeding practices with child temperament. Clin. Pediatr.; 2013; 52, pp. 1098-1106. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0009922813497091]
35. Mason, T.B.; Smith, K.E.; Dunton, G.F. Maternal parenting styles and ecological momentary assessment of maternal feeding practices and child food intake across middle childhood to early adolescence. Pediatr. Obes.; 2020; 15, e12683. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ijpo.12683]
36. United Nations Children’s Fund. Department of Economic and Social Affairs. Sustainable Development. Available online: https://sdgs.un.org/goals (accessed on 19 October 2024).
37. Turkish Statistical Institute (TÜİK). İstatistiklerle Çocuk, 2021. Available online: https://data.tuik.gov.tr/Bulten/Index?p=Istatistiklerle-Cocuk-2021-45633 (accessed on 29 November 2024).
38. T.C. Sanayi ve Kalkınma Bakanlığı (Republic of Turkey Ministry of Industry and Technology). İlçelerin Sosyo-Ekonomik Gelişmişlik Sıralaması Araştırması (Socio-Economic Development Ranking Survey of Districts). 2022; Available online: https://www.sanayi.gov.tr/merkez-birimi/b94224510b7b/sege (accessed on 29 November 2024).
39. Yilmaz, R.; Haluk, E.; Erkorkmaz, Ü. Adaptation study of the Turkish Children’s Eating Behavior Questionnaire. Anadolu Psikiyatr. Derg.; 2011; 12, pp. 287-294.
40. Karabulut Demir, E. Ebeveyn Tutum Ölçeği (ETÖ). MS Thesis; Sosyal Bilimler Enstitüsü, İstanbul Üniversitesi: İstanbul, Türkiye, 2007.
41. Warschburger, P.; Kroller, K. Maternal perception of weight status and health risks associated with obesity in children. Pediatrics; 2009; 124, pp. e60-e68. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-1845]
42. Hager, E.R.; McGill, A.E.; Black, M.M. Development and validation of a toddler silhouette scale. Obesity; 2010; 18, pp. 397-401. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2009.293]
43. Rolland-Cachera, M.F.; Group, E.C.O. Childhood obesity: Current definitions and recommendations for their use. Int. J. Pediatr. Obes.; 2011; 6, pp. 325-331. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3109/17477166.2011.607458]
44. Duarte, L.S.; Fujimori, E.; Minagawa Toriyama, A.T.; Palombo, C.N.; Borges, A.L.; Kurihayashi, A.Y. Brazilian Maternal Weight Perception and Satisfaction With Toddler Body Size: A Study in Primary Health Care. J. Pediatr. Nurs.; 2016; 31, pp. 490-497. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2016.03.022] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27132799]
45. Ruiter, E.L.; Saat, J.J.; Molleman, G.R.; Fransen, G.A.; van Der Velden, K.; van Jaarsveld, C.H.; Engels, R.C.; Assendelft, W.J. Parents’ underestimation of their child’s weight status. Moderating factors and change over time: A cross-sectional study. PLoS ONE; 2020; 15, e0227761. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227761] [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31945129]
46. Vrijkotte, T.G.M.; Varkevisser, T.; van Schalkwijk, D.B.; Hartman, M.A. Maternal Underestimation of Child’s Weight at Pre-School Age and Weight Development between Age 5 and 12 Years: The ABCD-Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health; 2020; 17, 5197. [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17145197]
47. World Health Organization. Guideline: Assessing and Managing Children at Primary Health-Care Facilities to Prevent Overweight and Obesity in the Context of the Double Burden of Malnutrition; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2017.
48. United Nations Children’s Fund. Sustainable Development Group. Leave No One Behind. Available online: https://unsdg.un.org/2030-agenda/universal-values/leave-no-one-behind (accessed on 11 January 2024).
49. Richter, L. The Importance of Caregiver-Child Interactions for the Survival and Healthy Development of Young Children: A Review; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2004.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Background: Accurate maternal perceptions of children’s weight status are crucial for early childhood obesity prevention, with evidence suggesting that maternal misperception may delay timely interventions. This study investigated the accuracy of maternal perceptions of child weight and examined associations with parenting styles and children’s eating behaviors and demographic factors among preschool-aged children in Samsun, Türkiye. Methods: This cross-sectional study included 318 mother–child pairs recruited from preschools in socio-economically diverse areas of Samsun. Maternal perceptions of child weight status were assessed using a visual scale, while children’s anthropometric measurements (height, weight) were recorded to calculate BMI-for-age Z-scores. Parenting styles and children’s eating behaviors were evaluated using the Parental Attitude Scale (PAS) and the Children’s Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ), respectively. Data analysis involved chi-square tests and multiple logistic regression to explore associations between maternal perception accuracy, parenting styles, and children’s eating behaviors. Results: Among 318 mother–child pairs, 59.7% underestimated their child’s weight status, with this figure being 84.9% among overweight children. Authoritative parenting was the most common style (78%); no significant association was found between parenting style and maternal perception accuracy. Accurate estimators showed significantly higher scores for food responsiveness (median: 9 (7–12), p = 0.028) and the enjoyment of food (mean: 16.3 ± 4.8, p = 0.003), whereas underestimators scored higher for satiety responsiveness (mean: 23.2 ± 5.7, p = 0.042) and slowness in eating (mean: 11.2 ± 4.2, p = 0.004). Conclusions: Maternal underestimations of child weight are prevalent, particularly for overweight children, are associated with children’s specific eating behaviors. Targeted educational interventions focusing on improving maternal awareness and promoting responsive feeding practices are essential to combat childhood obesity.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details


1 Alaçam State Hospital, 55800 Samsun, Türkiye; Department of Social Pediatrics, Institute of Health Sciences and Institute of Child Health, Hacettepe University, 06230 Ankara, Türkiye
2 Department of Social Pediatrics, Institute of Health Sciences and Institute of Child Health, Hacettepe University, 06230 Ankara, Türkiye