Keywords:
reproductive senescence, maternal effects, sexually selected infanticide, folivory
Across mammals, fertility and offspring survival are often lowest at the beginning and end of females' reproductive careers. However, extrinsic drivers of reproductive success - including infanticide by males-could stochastically obscure these expected age-related trends. Here, we modelled reproductive ageing trajectories in two cercopithecine primates that experience high rates of male infanticide: the chacma baboon (Papio ursinus) and the gelada (Theropithecus gelada). We found that middle-aged mothers generally achieved the shortest interbirth intervals in chacma baboons. By contrast, old gelada females often showed shorter interbirth intervals than their younger group-mates with one exception: the oldest females typically failed to produce additional offspring before their deaths. Infant survival peaked in middle-aged mothers in chacma baboons but in young mothers in geladas. While infant mortality linked with maternal death increased as mothers aged in both species, infanticide risk did not predictably shift with maternal age. Thus, infanticide patterns cannot explain the surprising young mother advantage observed in geladas. Instead, we argue that this could be a product of their graminivorous diets, Which might remove some energetic constraints on early reproduction. In sum, our data suggest that reproductive ageing is widespread but may be differentially shaped by ecological pressures.
1. Introduction
Reproductive output, which includes both fertility and offspring viability, typically changes across the lifespans of female animals [1,2]. Young individuals generally exhibit poor reproductive outcomes, perhaps because they must balance finishing growth with commencing reproduction (e.g. [3,4], reviewed in [5]), while old individuals undergo reproductive senescence as they experience somatic decline [6-8]. These shortfalls at both extremes of the reproductive lifespan have been theoretically supported by models of life history evolution [9,10], empirically demonstrated in model systems (e.g. Caenorhabditis elegans: [11], Drosophila melanogaster: [12]), and suggest that reproductive ageing generally follows a n-shaped pattern (offspring survival: [1], fecundity: [2]).
However, not all species and populations conform to this pattern. When females delay breeding until growth is nearly complete, reproductive performance often peaks shortly after maturation and declines as mothers age (e.g. arthropods: [13,14]). By contrast, in taxa where ecological knowledge (e.g. elephants: [15], grey seals: [16]), maternal experience (e.g. horses: [17]) or maternal energetic resources (e.g. turtles: [18], pinnipeds: [19]) improve with age, older mothers can maintain high reproductive success. Reproductive ageing can also be obscured if the recruitment of non-breeding helpers [20,21] or compensatory increases in maternal care (i.e. 'terminal investment': [22]) offset age-related declines in maternal condition, or exacerbated if ecological challenges disproportionately impact the youngest or oldest individuals (e.g. seabirds: [7,23]). Given this wide variation in patterns of reproductive ageing, the ecological and evolutionary factors shaping reproductive ageing remain poorly understood.
Life history datasets from long-term, individual-based studies have increasingly enabled research-ers to capture age-related changes in reproductive performance (reviewed in [24]). Many of these reproductive ageing studies have focused on non-human primates (e.g. [25,26]), given their 'slow' life histories [27] and a long history of individual-based research [28]. In a recent analysis of repro-ductive ageing patterns across seven non-human primate species, all but one exhibited n-shaped or negative ageing trajectories [29]. Capuchins showed no changes in reproductive rates or infant survival as females aged, which was attributed in part to extreme droughts [30] and alpha male takeovers [31]-stochastic, unpredictable events linked with increases in infant mortality risk (including male infanticide). In other words, these salient extrinsic challenges could obscure reproductive ageing patterns by adding age-independent variance to reproductive outcomes. Because variably harsh environments and high rates of male infanticide strongly impact female reproductive success across many primate taxa (e.g. Alouatta seniculus: [32,33], Propithecus edwardsi: [34,35]), similarly negligible ageing effects could be more widespread than previously reported. Nevertheless, reproductive ageing can persist in populations facing extreme weather events (e.g. Uria aalge: [7]) and high infanticide rates (e.g. Panthera leo: [36]). If so, then variation in ecological pressures may better explain the presence and intensity of ageing effects across species.
Here, we tested the relationship between age and reproductive performance in the chacma baboon (Papio ursinus) and the gelada (Theropithecus gelada), two cercopithecine primates that share infanticide as a primary source of offspring mortality and have been the subject of long-term study [37,38]. Chacma baboons form large, cohesive social groups [39], while geladas live in multi-level societies in which several small, one-male 'units' range together to form herd-like 'bands' [40]. Dominant male tenures are also shorter in the multi-male groups of chacma baboons [41] than in the one-male units of geladas [42]. Given less frequent and more predictable patterns of alpha male turnover, female geladas experience lower overall rates of offspring mortality [37,38] and implement many counterstrategies that can further pre-empt infanticide (e.g. fetal loss, or the 'Bruce Effect': [43]). Additionally, predation - another presumably age-independent challenge -is a critical source of mortality in chacma baboons [38] but not geladas [40]. Thus, while infanticide risk might obscure quantitative signals of reproductive ageing in both species, the weaker extrinsic mortality risks observed in geladas might allow ageing effects to weakly persist (the infanticide hypothesis, table 1).
Alternatively, ageing patterns might more closely reflect ecological and dietary pressures. In general, folivorous primates produce proportionally smaller infants [44,45] and experience weaker forms of feeding competition [46,47]. Given the variable costs diet might thus impose on reproduction, ageing patterns could instead be strong in chacma baboons, which are omnivorous and face year-round contest competition over limited food resources [39,48], but weak in geladas, which mostly eat grasses and engage in contests primarily during a short dry season [49]. However, while diet has been both theoretically and empirically linked with cross-species variation in life history traits [50-52], its influence on the pace of ageing remains unclear (but see [53]). Because chacma baboons face harsher environmental conditions and show greater variation in offspring survival than geladas [37,38], but see [54], we may expect them to show stronger age-related changes in reproductive performance (the ecological hypothesis, table 1).
To quantify ageing patterns in these species and evaluate these hypotheses, we combined demographic records from two long-term study populations of chacma baboons and geladas and modelled female reproductive ageing using complementary strategies. First, we constructed life tables to document differences in age-specific fertility and survivorship across the two species. Using these life tables, we also evaluated whether these species experience quantitatively discernible post-reproductive periods. Second, we implemented survival analyses to determine whether maternal age predicted (i) the duration and closure of interbirth intervals and (ii) the probability of infant survival. In combination, these datasets and approaches allowed us to evaluate whether reproductive ageing is (i) obscured by infanticide in part or in full (the infanticide hypothesis) or (ii) persistent across species but more salient in populations that are prone to nutritional shortfalls (the ecological hypothesis).
2. Methods
2.1. Study sites and populations
Data on chacma baboons were collected from a wild population at the Moremi Game Reserve, Botswana. Between 1992 and 2007, researchers collected behavioural and demographic data from a social group that in any given month included 4-15 adult males, 18-28 adult females, and their dependent offspring. For this study, we used demographic data from 58 females who gave birth at least once during the study period. Data on geladas were collected from a wild population in the Simien Mountains National Park, Ethiopia. Between 2006 and 2022, the Simien Mountains Gelada Research Project collected routine behavioural and demographic data from 46 social units (20 founding units and 26 units that formed via fission and fusion events), each containing one leader male, 0-4 follower males, 1-12 adult females, and their dependent offspring. For this study, we used demographic data from 176 females who gave birth at least once.
2.2. Demographic data
For chacma baboons, research from a previous study period (1977-1991) allowed dates of birth to be known within one month for all but three mothers whose ages were estimated as juveniles. For geladas, dates of birth prior to the study period were unknown. Thus, maternal ages were determined from a combination of known birth dates (п = 49) and estimates drawn from life history information (n = 127). Analyses incorporating uncertainty in these age estimates were congruent with those using fixed estimates for maternal age (see electronic supplementary material, supporting information). Thus, we report analyses using these fixed estimates here.
We assessed reproductive performance across the lifespan of mothers using data on infant births (Achacmas = 191; Ngeladas = 453) and deaths (Nchacmas = 71; Ngeladas = 79). Given near-daily observations at the study sites, births were unlikely to have been missed. For chacma baboons, all infant births were known within a two-week window. For geladas, most infant births were also known within this interval (п = 408). When gelada infant births were not known with this accuracy (п = 45), dates of birth were assigned using the infant's face coloration (which fades from pink to grey within the first month of life) and coat coloration (which changes from black to brown by six months of age: [55]). Interbirth intervals were then calculated as the time difference between successive births. Here, we only used intervals following surviving offspring, as infant deaths hasten subsequent births [56,57]. For chacma baboons, our sample included 83 closed intervals and 20 that were right-censored when mothers died or when observations ended. For geladas, our sample included 240 closed intervals and 93 right-censored intervals.
Since we removed cases where interbirth intervals were extrinsically shortened by male infanticide, the remaining dataset should be well-poised to capture intrinsic, age-related changes in maternal condition and the resulting pace of reproduction. However, this reduction in sample size might also reduce statistical power to capture ageing effects [29]. Moreover, even after removing birth intervals associated with infant deaths, male geladas may still play a role in shaping these intervals. As noted above, pregnant geladas experience fetal loss when new males enter their social units [43]. Thus, we conducted additional analyses that accounted for the effects of male takeovers on these interbirth intervals in geladas (electronic supplementary material, figure S1 and table S2, supporting information), which are concordant with the results provided in the main text.
For both study populations, we assessed infant survival up to 1 year of age. This allowed us to match the analyses provided in Campos et al. [29] and focus on the developmental window when infanticide risk is highest [38,42]. Although infant deaths were rarely observed directly, we were able to attribute many infant deaths to (i) maternal death, (ii) infanticide and (iii) injury or illness based on the following criteria. For both species, infant deaths were classified as resulting from maternal death if they occurred within the 90 days following their mother's disappearance. For chacma baboons, deaths were classified as infanticides when infants died following an observed male attack or when such attacks were indirectly inferred from injuries to the offspring or mother. For geladas, deaths were classified as infanticides if they occurred within 180 days following a male takeover (1 = 101 takeovers), following [37]. Deaths were classified as resulting from injury or illness if infants were seen with an injury or exhibiting clear signs of ill health in the month leading up to their deaths. Predation events were never reported for infants in either population, although some infant deaths that co-occurred with maternal disappearance may have directly or indirectly resulted from predation. All remaining infant deaths were attributed to (iv) unknown causes. While unknown deaths were associated with circumstantial evidence (e.g. harsh weather events), corroborating information was too scant to create more fine-grained categories.
2.3. Life table construction and analysis
To describe and contrast life history patterns in female chacma baboons and geladas, we constructed life tables using the demographic data described above. Life tables divided all births, deaths and censoring events into yearly intervals (0-1 yr, 1-2 yr, etc.). Females entered the life table upon their birth or the onset of their group's study and exited the life table upon their death or at the end of the study period. For this analysis, we focused on two metrics: I, which notes the probability that a female survived from birth to age x; and mx, which notes the probability that a female gave birth during the interval at age x. Unlike the survival analyses below, this approach did not exclude cases where fertility was hastened by offspring death. To identify ageing trends within our study species, we constructed two Bayesian generalized models with binomial outcomes using the "R' package "brms' [58]. In these models, we counted the number of (model 1) births and (model 2) deaths within each age class as the outcome while accounting for the total number of individuals included in that age class. Female age (linear and quadratic), population and their interaction were included as fixed effects.
Additionally, constructing life tables allowed us to calculate post-reproductive representation (PrR) -a metric that reflects the proportion of adult females that have ceased reproduction [59]. PrR is derived from the equation TM /TB, where TM indicates the expected number of years that a female will be post-reproductive and Tp indicates the expected number of years that a female will be an adult. In this approach, females become 'adults' at the age class when 5% of lifetime fertility has been completed and become "post-reproductive" at the age class when 95% of lifetime fertility has been completed. To assess whether PrR is non-zero, we compared our observed PrR measurements with those derived from 10000 simulations in which the probability of reproducing and the probability of surviving decline in parallel during the years following peak fertility [59]. P-values were then calculated as the proportion of simulated PrR values that were greater than the observed PrR.
2.4. Statistical analyses
To assess the influence of maternal age on interbirth intervals, we constructed two hierarchical Bayesian survival models using 'rstanarm' [60], which modelled reproductive ageing effects using flexible M-splines and included maternal age and maternal age? as fixed effects. This approach allowed for the inclusion of right-censored cases, where mothers died or observations ceased before they gave birth to a subsequent offspring. Mothers entered the interbirth interval dataset once they reached the minimum observed interbirth interval duration in the population (chacma baboons: 0.99 yr; geladas: 1.10 yr). Because the effect of maternal age on interbirth intervals violated the assumption of proportional hazards, we included time-varying effects for maternal age and maternal age?. We used default boundary and internal knots, allowing for smooth changes in the impact of maternal age as interbirth intervals progressed. Maternal identity, study year and (for geladas) social unit identity were included as random effects in all models.
To assess the relationship between maternal age and infant survival, we similarly constructed two Bayesian survival models including maternal age and maternal age? as fixed effects. Here, the proportional hazards assumption was not violated, so no time transformations were necessary. For both interbirth interval and infant survival models, we included maternal identity and study year as random effects, used default priors and fit models using four chains, each including 2000 iterations. For both the interbirth interval and infant survival analyses, we fit additional models that included maternal parity (i.e. primiparous vs. multiparous) as an additional predictor. However, these did not substantially improve model fit. Thus, we report results from the models using maternal age alone.
Furthermore, to assess whether these changes in infant survival across maternal age were driven by specific sources of infant mortality, we constructed additional Bayesian generalized linear mixed models for both species. Specifically, we constructed logistic models assessing whether maternal age and maternal age? predicted whether infants died due to (i) maternal death, (ii) infanticide, (iii) illness or injury or (iv) unknown causes, each coded as a 1/0 response. For these models, we restricted our datasets to include only infants whose fates were known, removing all infants who were right-censored before reaching 1 year of age. We included birth year as a random effect and used weakly informative priors (i.e. normal(0,1)).
3. Results
3.1. Life table patterns
For both chacma baboons and geladas, fertility increased at 4 years of age, mostly plateaued from approximately 6 to 20 years, and then declined in later life (posterior mean + standard error: βage = 2.41 + 0.64; βage2 = -7.04 + 0.61; figure 14). Birth rates were generally higher in chacma baboons than in geladas (βgelada = -0.76 + 0.13), although chacma baboons began reproducing at later ages than geladas (chacma baboons: 6.89 + 0.20 yr; geladas: 6.39 + 0.09 yr). The fertility patterns identified for chacma baboons closely matched those identified in other long-term savannah baboon populations, while geladas showed much lower birth rates across their reproductive course (figure la). A chacma baboon female that survived to reproductive age gave birth to approximately 4.38 offspring, while a gelada female that did the same produced only 3.23 offspring over her reproductive lifespan.
In both chacma baboons and geladas, mortality rates peaked during infancy and again during later adulthood (βage = 2.35 ± 0.65; βage2 = 3.30 ± 0.57). However, mortality rates were generally higher in chacma baboons than in geladas (βspecies = -0.30 ± 0.15). This difference was largely driven by 1st-year mortality (35% vs. 13%), which is much lower in geladas than in both the chacma baboons and other savannah baboon populations (figure 1b). Given these differences, only 45% of chacma baboon females reached 5 years of age, compared with 71% of geladas. Thus, despite their faster reproductive rates, at birth, a chacma baboon female is expected to produce 1.98 offspring over her lifetime, while the average gelada female is expected to produce 2.23 offspring. For both chacma baboons and geladas, mortality rates rose again during old age (βgeladas x age = 2.30 ± 0.71; βgeladas x age2 = 0.35 ± 0.64).
PrR was low in both chacma baboons (PrRchacma = 0.044) and geladas (PrRgelada = 0.051), and simulation tests indicate that these patterns did not deviate from null expectations (Pchacma = 0.15; Pgelada = 0.45). Thus, although fertility appreciably declines in old age, these changes parallel, and do not precede, increases in mortality risk.
3.2. Interbirth intervals
Closed interbirth intervals averaged 2.00 ± 0.36 yr in chacma baboons and 2.57 ± 0.66 yr in geladas. For both species, interbirth intervals were generally longer in younger mothers than in older mothers (chacma baboons: βage = 0.80 ± 0.57 yr; geladas: βage = 0.89 ± 0.31 yr; figure 2a,b). The effect of maternal age on interbirth intervals was quadratic in both chacma baboons and geladas (chacma baboons: βage2 = -1.72 ± 0.60; geladas: βage2 = -0.48 ± 0.23). However, while closed interbirth intervals were generally longer for both young and old chacma baboons, closed interbirth intervals were initially longer in young compared with old geladas. For chacma baboons, this quadratic relationship diminished as interbirth intervals grew longer, and credibility intervals for this effect eventually overlapped with zero (electronic supplementary material, figure S2a). For geladas, however, the effect of maternal age on the probability of closing an interbirth interval gradually reversed, as old mothers often failed to close long interbirth intervals (electronic supplementary material, figure S2b). In other words, although some old gelada mothers achieved short interbirth intervals, those that did not reproduce quickly often died before giving birth to another offspring. By contrast, almost all young mothers eventually gave birth again, even when interbirth intervals became very long.
3.3. Infant survival
In chacma baboons, the infants of both young and old mothers experienced reduced survival (βage = -0.12 ± 0.13; βage2 = 0.35 ± 0.12; figure 2c). By contrast, in geladas, infant survival peaked in young mothers and decreased with maternal age (βage = 0.32 ± 0.14; βage2 = 0.03 ± 0.09; figure 2d). The sources of infant mortality shifted as mothers aged in both chacma baboons and geladas: infant deaths linked with maternal death were more common in old mothers than in mid-aged and young mothers (chacma baboons: βage = 1.14 ± 0.41; geladas: βage = 0.79 ± 0.21: figure 3a,b). Although infant deaths linked with infanticide were generally more frequent in young and old chacma baboon mothers and in old gelada mothers, neither of these trends had strong empirical support (chacma baboons: βage = -0.06 ± 0.25, βage2 = 0.06 ± 0.23; geladas: βage = 0.26 ± 0.23, βage2 = -0.04 ± 0.16). Additionally, infant deaths of unknown cause declined with maternal age in chacma baboons but not in geladas (chacma baboons: βage = -0.68 ± 0.33, βage2 = 0.24 ± 0.32; geladas: βage = 0.05 ± 0.29, βage2 = -0.04 ± 0.23).
4. Discussion
Here, we identified salient reproductive ageing trajectories in both chacma baboons and geladas, indicating that high rates of infanticide were not sufficient to obscure these patterns. Instead, reproductive ageing patterns supported the predictions of the ecological hypothesis: chacma baboons showed strong quadratic ageing patterns for both interbirth interval duration and infant survival, and middleaged mothers showed the highest reproductive performance across the board. In geladas, however, ageing effects were less pronounced and diverged across our two metrics of reproductive success. While both young and old mothers experienced longer interbirth intervals on average, time-varying effects indicate that these two age classes were not equivalently encumbered. Surprisingly, gelada infant survival declined as mothers aged. Further tests revealed that infant deaths linked with maternal death became more common as mothers aged, while infanticide risk was not associated with maternal age. Thus, changes in infanticide risk across maternal age cannot explain these species differences in infant survival trends. Below, we situate these results within our broader understanding of reproductive ageing in primates and other long-lived, social mammals.
4.1. Young and old mothers experienced longer interbirth intervals
For both chacma baboons and geladas, young females experienced longer interbirth intervals, which matches previous research across primates and other mammals [29,36,64]. Young or primiparous females generally provide lower-quality maternal care [65,66], wean their offspring at later ages [67,68] and take longer to recoup energetic losses incurred during reproduction [69,70]. In combination, these deficits probably produced the longer interbirth intervals seen in young females of both species. The impact of old age on fertility, however, differed slightly between the two species. Old chacma baboon females experienced long interbirth intervals, presumably due to deteriorating physical condition (e.g. reduced body mass: [71,72]). Few old chacma baboon females failed to close interbirth intervals; however, this could be an artefact of our data inclusion criteria, which reduced the sample from old females (46.9% of interbirth intervals greater than 16 yr removed due to infant death). By contrast, old gelada females appeared to adopt one of two life history strategies: (i) reproduce quickly to produce more offspring within their dwindling lifespans or (ii) slow down reproduction to allocate more resources towards their terminal offspring [22,73]. Further investigations of life history covariation will be necessary to identify such divergent life history strategies and their potential drivers [74].
4.2. Old mothers routinely experienced low infant survival
In both species, old mothers experienced lower rates of offspring survival. This was in large part the result of maternal death (i.e. orphaning), which decreases offspring survival in both the short and long term [75,76]. Nevertheless, the maternal deficits associated with advanced age often long precede maternal death [77]. For example, dental senescence can compromise maternal condition and hinder offspring success [78,79]. However, in chacma baboons, senescence-related deficits in maternal condition and behaviour may not directly explain this uptick in infant deaths. Indeed, the vast majority of chacma baboon infant and adult deaths can be respectively attributable to infanticide and predation [38]. Thus, the low rate of infant survival in older chacma baboon mothers might more closely reflect the sum of these extrinsic mortality risks, although reduced physical condition could exacerbate these patterns. By contrast, physical senescence may have a more direct impact on reproductive success in ageing geladas, as predation rates are lower and intrinsic mortality risks may be more salient. Indeed, female geladas from a different study population exhibited signs of reduced chewing efficiency in old age, particularly during the dry season when geladas feed on harder, nutrient-dense foods [80]. Forthcoming measurements of female physical condition in the gelada study population may further capture ageing trends in body mass, dental wear and frailty.
4.3. Young mothers showed low infant survival in chacma baboons, but high survival in geladas
Young chacma mothers joined many other mammalian mothers in exhibiting low infant survival, probably due to maternal inexperience [4,81], suboptimal physical condition [3], or other factors that could result in increased infant deaths of unknown causes. By contrast, despite showing long interbirth intervals, young gelada mothers actually showed the highest infant survival rates. Thus, the subcomponents of reproductive output do not always follow parallel ageing relationships. But how might young gelada mothers bypass these infant survival costs? One pathway to doing so might be to delay reproduction until growth is nearly complete, minimizing trade-offs between overlapping growth and reproduction. However, while geladas complete most of their linear growth by the start of reproduction [82], this only marginally exceeds the growth completion observed in primiparous baboons (captive: [83], wild: [84]) which nevertheless experience reduced infant survival [36,85]. Instead, young females may gain this advantage by timing their maturations to the arrival of new males [86], reducing the risk of takeover-related infanticide during their firstborn's infancy, and thereby driving this age effect. However, infanticide rates were only marginally lower in young gelada mothers, and simulation models suggest that these benefits only emerge in a narrow range of demographic conditions [87]. Thus, this life history strategy cannot fully explain the pattern of increased infant survival.
Alternatively, this pattern could be a by-product of geladas' graminivorous diet [49]. Perhaps not coincidentally, other primates that exhibit strong, negative relationships between maternal age and infant survival (e.g. sifakas, muriquis: [29,78]) also feed mainly on leafy, non-defensible foods (electronic supplementary material, table S2). As noted above, folivorous primates generally produce smaller infants relative to their body size [44,45], and young mothers may more readily nourish these 'cheaper' offspring. Moreover, folivorous primates often show weaker competitive regimes [46,47], in which young females can outrank and outperform their older group-mates (e.g. [88,89]). High infant survival rates in early adulthood could be the result of these two phenomena identified in many folivores. However, these advantages may be limited in their scope. Young folivore mothers rarely achieve faster reproductive rates in addition to higher offspring survival rates [29,90]. Moreover, in other folivorous mammals, reproductive performance variably peaks in early (e.g. red deer: [91]), mid (e.g. sheep: [3,92]) or even later adulthood (e.g. elephants: [15], horses: [17]). Thus, more comparative data-particularly from atelids, colobines and other folivorous mammals (e.g. marsupials, sloths)-are needed to draw links between diet and reproductive ageing patterns, both within primates and across all mammals.
4.4. High infanticide risk did not preclude the observation of reproductive senescence
Despite high infanticide risk (chacma baboons: 54%; geladas: 34% of infant deaths), reproductive senescence was apparent in both species. Thus, frequent infanticides (55% of infant deaths: [31]) may not fully explain the lack of empirical ageing effects in wild capuchins [29,77]. Instead, increases in infant mortality during extreme drought periods could obscure these ageing trends [29,30]. However, while infant survival was relatively stable across study years in geladas [93], infant survival rates fluctuated widely in the chacma baboons (22-92% across birth cohorts). Thus, year-to-year environmental changes might not prevent ageing patterns from emerging either. Alternatively, the high levels of allomaternal care observed in capuchins [94,95] could buffer mothers against maternal ageing, mirroring the helper benefits documented in some cooperative breeding birds [20,21]. While allomaternal care has been broadly linked with improved reproductive outcomes [96,97], more research is needed to determine if and how it buffers mothers against the adverse effects of ageing.
More broadly, the data presented here also indicate that reproductive ageing does not follow a uniform pattern, even across closely related species (electronic supplementary material, table S5). While some relationships are common and seemingly conserved (i.e. reduced fertility at young and old ages), others appear to be more evolutionarily or ecologically malleable (i.e. infant survival shortfalls vs. advantages in younger mothers). Indeed, life history traits can be quite plastic and often vary across populations of the same species [98,99], including chacma baboons and geladas [54,100,101]. Thus, the patterns reported here presumably reflect both evolved, species-specific life history traits and local ecological conditions. To better capture the broader drivers of this variation, future studies should focus on both cross-species and cross-population differences, joining datasets representing multiple clades with detailed ecological information and phylogenetic comparative methods.
Ethics. Research on chacma baboons in Moremi, Botswana followed the guidelines of the Department of Wildlife and National Parks of the Republic of Botswana and was conducted with the approval of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at the University of Pennsylvania. Research on geladas in the Simien Mountains National Park, Ethiopia was conducted with the permission of the Ethiopian Wildlife Conservation Authority and was approved by the IACUCs of the Uneivrsity of Michigan, Arizona State Unievrsity, and Stony Brook Unievrsity.
Data accessibility. Data and relevant code for this research work are stored in GitHub: https://github.com/ GeladaResearchProject/Feder_et_al_2024_Reproductive_aging and have been archived within the Zenodo repository [102].
Supplementary material is available online [103].
Declaration of AI use. We have not used AI-assisted technologies in creating this article.
Authors' contributions. J.A.F.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, funding acquisition, methodology, project administration, writing-original draft, writing-review and editing; I.A.S.-C.: conceptualization, data curation, funding acquisition, methodology, project administration, writing-original draft, writing-review and editing; J.C.B.: data curation, funding acquisition, project administration, writing-review and editing; T.J.B.: data curation, funding acquisition, project administration, writing-review and editing; R.M.S.: data curation, funding acquisition, project administration, writing-review and editing; J.B.S.: data curation, funding acquisition, project administration, writing-review and editing; N.S.-M.: data curation, funding acquisition, project administration, writing-review and editing; A.L.: conceptualization, data curation, funding acquisition, methodology, project administration, writing-original draft, writing-review and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Conflict of interest declaration. We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding. Research in Moremi was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation (IOS9514001), the National Institutes of Health, the DFG and KFN, the NSERC of Canada, the National Geographic Society, the Leakey Foundation, and the University of Pennsylvania. Research in the SMNP was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation (BCS-0715179, BCS-0824592, BCS-1723228, BCS-2235564, IOS-1255974, IOS-1854359), the Leakey Foundation, the National Geographic Society, Fulbright, the University of Michigan, and Stony Brook University. JAF was supported by a Social Sciences Postdoctoral Research Fellowship (SPRF) from the National Science Foundation (BCS-2313739).
Acknowledgements. We would like to thank the Office of the President of the Republic of Botswana and the Botswana Department of Wildlife and National Parks for permission to conduct research on chacma baboons in the Moremi Reserve. Data on chacma baboons were collected by C. Crockford, A. Engh, J. Fischer, M. Heesen, S. Johnson, D. Kitchen, L. Moscovice, J. Nicolson, R. Palombit, D. Rendall, R. Wittig, K. Seyfarth, C. Shaw, M. Mokopi and A. Mokopi. We also would like to thank the Ethiopian Wildlife Conservation Authority for permission to conduct research on geladas in the Simien Mountains National Park (SMNP), Ethiopia. Data on geladas were collected by E. Jejaw, A. Fanta, S. Girmay, J. Jarvey, M. Gomery, L. Morris, T. Regan, P. DeLacey, P. Clark, E. Sloan, L. Babbitt and M. Melton. We would also like to thank Zarin Machanda and two anonymous reviewers for their valuable feedback on this manuscript.
Cite this article: Feder JA, Schneider-Crease IA, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Seyfarth RM, Silk JB, Snyder-Mackler N, Lu A. 2025 Female reproductive ageing persists despite high infanticide risk in chacma baboons and geladas. R. Soc. Open Sci. 12: 241210. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.241210
Received: 17 July 2024
Accepted: 1 December 2024
Subject Category:
Organismal and evolutionary biology
Subject Areas:
ecology, evolution
Author for correspondence:
Jacob A. Feder
e-mail: [email protected]
Electronic supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.6084/ m9 figshare.c.7598812.
References
1. Ivimey-Cook E, Moorad J. 2020 The diversity of maternal-age effects upon pre-adult survival across animal species. Proc. Biol. Sci. 287, 20200972. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2020.0972)
2. Lemaître JF, Ronget V, Gaillard JM. 2020 Female reproductive senescence across mammals: a high diversity of patterns modulated by life history and mating traits. Mech. Ageing Dev. 192, 111377. (doi:10.1016/j.mad.2020.111377)
3. Festa-Bianchet M, Jorgenson JT, Lucherini M, Wishart WD. 1995 Life history consequences of variation in age of primiparity in bighorn ewes. Ecology 76, 871-881. (doi:10.2307/1939352)
4. Broussard DR, Dobson FS, Murie JO. 2008 Previous experience and reproductive investment of female Columbian ground squirrels. J. Mammal. 89, 145-152. (doi:10.1644/06-mamm-a-357.1)
5. . Stearns SC. 1992 The evolution of life histories. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
6. Lemaître J, Gaillard J. 2017 Reproductive senescence: new perspectives in the wild. Biol. Rev. 92, 2182-2199. (doi:10.1111/brv.12328)
7. Reed TE, Kruuk LEB, Wanless S, Frederiksen M, Cunningham EJA, Harris MP. 2008 Reproductive senescence in a long-lived seabird: rates of decline in late-life performance are associated with varying costs of early reproduction. Am. Nat. 171, E89-E101. (doi:10.1086/524957)
8. Dugdale HL, Pope LC, Newman C, Macdonald DW, Burke T. 2011 Age-specific breeding success in a wild mammalian population: selection, constraint, restraint and senescence. Mol. Ecol. 20, 3261-3274. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2011.05167.x)
9. Moorad JA, Nussey DH. 2016 Evolution of maternal effect senescence. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 362-367. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1520494113)
10. Hamilton WD. 1966 The moulding of senescence by natural selection. J. Theor. Biol. 12, 12-45. (doi:10.1016/0022-5193(66)90184-6)
11. Hughes SE, Evason K, Xiong C, Kornfeld K. 2007 Genetic and pharmacological factors that influence reproductive aging in nematodes. PLoS Genet. 3, e25. (doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.0030025)
12. Priest NK, Mackowiak B, Promislow DEL. 2002 The role of parental age effects on the evolution of aging. Evolution 56, 927-935. (doi:10.1111/j. 0014-3820.2002.tb01405.x)
13. Kern S, Ackermann M, Stearns SC, Kawecki TJ. 2001 Decline in offspring viability as a manifestation of aging in Drosophila melaongaster. Evolution (N. Y.) 55, 1822-1831. (doi:10.1111/j.0014-3820.2001.tb00831.x)
14. Moya-Laraño J. 2002 Senescence and food limitation in a slowly ageing spider. Funct. Ecol. 16, 734-741. (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2435.2002. 00685.x)
15. Moss CJ. 2001 The demography of an African elephant (Loxodonta africana) population in Amboseli, Kenya. J. Zool. 255, 145-156. (doi:10. 1017/s0952836901001212)
16. Allen SJJ, Bowen WD, den Heyer CE. 2022 Birth-site habitat selection in gray seals (Halichoerus grypus): effects of maternal age and parity and association with offspring weaning mass. Mar. Mamm. Sci. 38, 349-363. (doi:10.1111/mms.12867)
17. Cameron EZ, Linklater WL, Stafford KJ, Minot EO. 2000 Aging and improving reproductive success in horses: declining residual reproductive value or just older and wiser? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 47, 243-249. (doi:10.1007/s002650050661)
18. Congdon JD, Nagle RD, Kinney OM, van Loben Sels RC, Quinter T, Tinkle DW. 2003 Testing hypotheses of aging in long-lived painted turtles (Chrysemys picta). Exp. Gerontol. 38, 765-772. (doi:10.1016/s0531-5565(03)00106-2)
19. Le Boeuf B, Condit R, Reiter J. 2019 Lifetime reproductive success of northern elephant seals (Mirounga angustirostris). Can. J. Zool. 97, 1203- 1217. (doi:10.1139/cjz-2019-0104)
20. Hammers M, Kingma SA, van Boheemen LA, Sparks AM, Burke T, Dugdale HL, Richardson DS, Komdeur J. 2021 Helpers compensate for agerelated declines in parental care and offspring survival in a cooperatively breeding bird. Evol. Lett. 5, 143-153. (doi:10.1002/evl3.213)
21. Cooper EB, Bonnet T, Osmond HL, Cockburn A, Kruuk LEB. 2021 Aging and senescence across reproductive traits and survival in superb fairywrens (Malurus cyaneus). Am. Nat. 197, 111-127. (doi:10.1086/711755)
22. Clutton-Brock TH. 1984 Reproductive effort and terminal investment in iteroparous animals. Am. Nat. 123, 212-229. (doi:10.1086/284198)
23. Tompkins EM, Anderson DJ. 2021 Breeding responses to environmental variation are age- and trait-dependent in female Nazca boobies. Ecology 102, 3457. (doi:10.1002/ecy.3457)
24. Nussey DH, Froy H, Lemaitre JF, Gaillard JM, Austad SN. 2013 Senescence in natural populations of animals: widespread evidence and its implications for bio-gerontology. Ageing Res. Rev. 12, 214-225. (doi:10.1016/j.arr.2012.07.004)
25. Alberts SC et al. 2013 Reproductive aging patterns in primates reveal that humans are distinct. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 13440-13445. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1311857110)
26. Kappeler PM, Pethig L, Prox L, Fichtel C. 2022 Reproductive senescence in two lemur lineages. Front. Ecol. Evol. 10, 894344. (doi:10.3389/fevo. 2022.894344)
27. Charnov EL, Berrigan D. 1993 Why do female primates have such long lifespans and so few babies? or Life in the slow lane. Evol. Anthropol. 1, 191-194. (doi:10.1002/evan.1360010604)
28. Clutton-Brock T, Sheldon BC. 2010 Individuals and populations: the role of long-term, individual-based studies of animals in ecology and evolutionary biology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 25, 562-573. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2010.08.002)
29. Campos FA et al. 2022 Female reproductive aging in seven primate species: patterns and consequences. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 119, e2117669119. (doi:10.1073/pnas.2117669119)
30. Campos FA et al. 2020 Differential impact of severe drought on infant mortality in two sympatric neotropical primates. R. Soc. Open Sci. 7, 200302. (doi:10.1098/rsos.200302)
31. Fedigan LM, Hogan JD, Campos FA, Kalbitzer U, Jack KM. 2021 Costs of male infanticide for female capuchins: when does an adaptive male reproductive strategy become costly for females and detrimental to population viability? Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 176, 349-360. (doi:10.1002/ ajpa.24354)
32. Crockett CM, Rudran R. 1987 Red howler monkey birth data II: interannual, habitat, and sex comparisons. Am. J. Primatol. 13, 369-384. (doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350130403)
33. Crockett CM, Janson CH. 2000 Infanticide in red howlers: female group size, male membership, and a possible link to folivory. In Infanticide by males and its implications (eds CP van Schaik, CH Janson), pp. 75-98. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. (doi:10.1017/ cbo9780511542312.006)
34. Morelli TL, King SJ, Pochron ST, Wright PC. 2009 The rules of disengagement: takeovers, infanticide, and dispersal in a rainforest lemur, Propithecus edwardsi. Behaviour 146, 499-523. (doi:10.1163/15683908x399554)
35. Dunham AE, Erhart EM, Wright PC. 2011 Global climate cycles and cyclones: consequences for rainfall patterns and lemur reproduction in southeastern Madagascar. Glob. Chang. Biol. 17, 219-227. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2486.2010.02205.x)
36. Packer C, Tatar M, Collins A. 1998 Reproductive cessation in female mammals. Nature 392, 807-811. (doi:10.1038/33910)
37. Tinsley Johnson E, Feder JA, Bergman TJ, Lu A, Snyder-Mackler N, Beehner JC. 2021 The Goldilocks effect: female geladas in mid-sized groups have higher fitness. Proc. R. Soc. B 288, 20210820. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2021.0820)
38. Cheney DL et al. 2004 Factors affecting reproduction and mortality among baboons in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. Int. J. Primatol. 25, 401- 428. (doi:10.1023/b:ijop.0000019159.75573.13)
39. Silk J, Seyfarth R, Cheney D. 1999 The structure of social relationships among female savanna baboons in Moremi Reserve, Botswana. Behaviour 136, 679-703. (doi:10.1163/156853999501522)
40. Snyder-Mackler N, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ. 2012 Defining higher levels in the multilevel societies of geladas (Theropithecus gelada). Int. J. Primatol. 33, 1054-1068. (doi:10.1007/s10764-012-9584-5)
41. Palombit RA, Cheney DL, Fischer J, Johnson S, Rendall D, Seyfarth RM, Silk JB. 2000 Male infanticide and defense of infants in chacma baboons. In Infanticide by males and its implications (eds CP van Schaik, CH Janson), pp. 123-152. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. (doi:10. 1017/CBO9780511542312.008)
42. Beehner JC, Bergman TJ. 2008 Infant mortality following male takeovers in wild geladas. Am. J. Primatol. 70, 1152-1159. (doi:10.1002/ajp. 20614)
43. Roberts EK, Lu A, Bergman TJ, Beehner JC. 2012 A Bruce Effect in wild geladas. Science 335, 1222-1225. (doi:10.1126/science.1213600)
44. Leigh SR. 1994 Ontoaenetic correlates of diet in anthropoid primates. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 94, 499-522. (doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330940406)
45. Harvey PH, Clutton-Brock TH. 1985 Life history variation in primates. Evolution 39, 559-581. (doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.1985.tb00395.x)
46. Sterck EHM, Steenbeek R. 1997 Female dominance relationships and food competition in the sympatric Thomas langur and long-tailed macaque. Behaviour 134, 749-774. (doi:10.1163/156853997x00052)
47. Snaith TV, Chapman CA. 2007 Primate group size and interpreting socioecological models: do folivores really play by different rules? Evol. Anthropol. 16, 94-106. (doi:10.1002/evan.20132)
48. Johnson SE. 2003 Life history and the competitive environment: trajectories of growth, maturation, and reproductive output among chacma baboons. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 120, 83-98. (doi:10.1002/ajpa.10139)
49. Jarvey JC, Low BS, Azanaw Haile A, Chiou KL, Snyder-Mackler N, Lu A, Bergman TJ, Beehner JC, Schneider-Crease IA. 2024 Aggression rates increase around seasonally exploited resources in a primarily grass-eating primate. Behav. Ecol. 35, d079. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arad079)
50. Janson CH, van CP. 1993 Ecological risk aversion in juvenile primates: slow and steady wins the race. In Juvenile primates (eds ME Pereira, LA Fairbanks), pp. 57-74. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
51. Borries C, Lu A, Ossi-Lupo K, Larney E, Koenig A. 2011 Primate life histories and dietary adaptations: a comparison of Asian colobines and macaques. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 144, 286-299. (doi:10.1002/ajpa.21403)
52. Godfrey LR, Samonds KE, Jungers WL, Sutherland MR, Irwin MT. 2004 Ontogenetic correlates of diet in Malagasy lemurs. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 123, 250-276. (doi:10.1002/ajpa.10315)
53. Lemaître JF, Gaillard JM, Lackey LB, Clauss M, Müller DWH. 2013 Comparing free-ranging and captive populations reveals intra-specific variation in aging rates in large herbivores. Exp. Gerontol. 48, 162-167. (doi:10.1016/j.exger.2012.12.004)
54. Lycett JE, Henzi SP, Barrett L. 1998 Maternal investment in mountain baboons and the hypothesis of reduced care. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42, 49-56. (doi:10.1007/s002650050410)
55. Beehner JC, Gesquiere L, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Alberts SC, Altmann J. 2016 Corrigendum to 'Testosterone related to age and life-history stages in male baboons and geladas' [Horm. Behav. 56/4 (2009) 472-480]. Horm. Behav. 80, 149. (doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.08.004)
56. Cords M, Chowdhury S. 2010 Life history of Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya. Int. J. Primatol. 31, 433-455. (doi:10. 1007/s10764-010-9405-7)
57. Borries C, Koenig A, Winkler P. 2001 Variation of life history traits and mating patterns in female langur monkeys (Semnopithecus entellus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 50, 391-402. (doi:10.1007/s002650100391)
58. Bürkner PC. 2017 brms: An R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. J. Stat. Softw. 80, 01. (doi:10.18637/jss.v080.i01)
59. Levitis DA, Lackey LB. 2011 A measure for describing and comparing postreproductive life span as a population trait. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2, 446- 453. (doi:10.1111/j.2041-210X.2011.00095.x)
60. Goodrich B, Gabry J, Ali I, Brilleman S. 2018 rstanarm: Bayesian applied regression modeling via Stan. R. pack. version 2, 1758. (doi:10.32614/ CRAN.package.rstanarm)
61. Bronikowski AM, Alberts SC, Altmann J, Packer C, Carey KD, Tatar M. 2002 The aging baboon: comparative demography in a non-human primate. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 9591-9595. (doi:10.1073/pnas.142675599)
62. Bronikowski AM et al. 2016 Female and male life tables for seven wild primate species. In Scientific data p. 160006. PubMed Central. ()
63. Palombit RA. 2003 Male infanticide in wild savanna baboons: adaptive significance and intraspecific variation. In Sexual selection and reproductive competition in primates: new perspectives and directions (ed. CB Jones), pp. 367-411, vol. 3. Norman, OK: American Society of Primatologists.
64. Strum SC, Western JD. 1982 Variations in fecundity with age and environment in olive baboons (Papio anubis). Am. J. Primatol. 3, 61-76. (doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350030106)
65. Lang SLC, Iverson SJ, Bowen WD. 2011 The influence of reproductive experience on milk energy output and lactation performance in the grey seal (Halichoerus grypus). PLoS One 6, e19487. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0019487)
66. Andersen R, Gaillard J, Linnell JDC, Duncan P. 2000 Factors affecting maternal care in an income breeder, the European roe deer. J. Anim. Ecol. 69, 672-682. (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2656.2000.00425.x)
67. Eckardt W, Fawcett K, Fletcher AW. 2016 Weaned age variation in the Virunga mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei): influential factors. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70, 493-507. (doi:10.1007/s00265-016-2066-6)
68. Xiccato G, Trocino A, Sartori A, Queaque PI. 2003 Effect of weaning diet and weaning age on growth, body composition and caecal fermentation of young rabbits. Anim. Sci. 77, 101-111. (doi:10.1017/s1357729800053704)
69. Martin JGA, Festa-Bianchet M. 2012 Determinants and consequences of age of primiparity in bighorn ewes. Oikos 121, 752-760. (doi:10.1111/ j.1600-0706.2011.19962.x)
70. Bonenfant C. 2003 Sex-ratio variation and reproductive costs in relation to density in a forest-dwelling population of red deer (Cervus elaphus). Behav. Ecol. 14, 862-869. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arg077)
71. Nussey DH, Coulson T, Delorme D, Clutton-Brock TH, Pemberton JM, Festa-Bianchet M, Gaillard JM. 2011 Patterns of body mass senescence and selective disappearance differ among three species of free-living ungulates. Ecology 92, 1936-1947. (doi:10.1890/11-0308.1)
72. Trillmich F, Geißler E, Guenther A. 2019 Senescence and costs of reproduction in the life history of a small precocial species. Ecol. Evol. 9, 7069- 7079. (doi:10.1002/ece3.5272)
73. Hoffman CL, Higham JP, Mas-Rivera A, Ayala JE, Maestripieri D. 2010 Terminal investment and senescence in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) on Cayo Santiago. Behav. Ecol. 21, 972-978. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arq098)
74. Hamel S, Yoccoz NG, Gaillard JM. 2017 Assessing variation in life-history tactics within a population using mixture regression models: a practical guide for evolutionary ecologists. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 92, 754-775. (doi:10.1111/brv.12254)
75. Andres D, Clutton-Brock TH, Kruuk LEB, Pemberton JM, Stopher KV, Ruckstuhl KE. 2013 Sex differences in the consequences of maternal loss in a long-lived mammal, the red deer (Cervus elaphus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 1249-1258. (doi:10.1007/s00265-013-1552-3)
76. Watts HE, Tanner JB, Lundrigan BL, Holekamp KE. 2009 Post-weaning maternal effects and the evolution of female dominance in the spotted hyena. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 2291-2298. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.0268)
77. Zipple MN et al. 2021 Maternal death and offspring fitness in multiple wild primates. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, e2015317118. (doi:10.1073/ pnas.2015317118)
78. King SJ, Arrigo-Nelson SJ, Pochron ST, Semprebon GM, Godfrey LR, Wright PC, Jernvall J. 2005 Dental senescence in a long-lived primate links infant survival to rainfall. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 16579-16583. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0508377102)
79. Nussey DH, Metherell B, Moyes K, Donald A, Guinness FE, Clutton-Brock TH. 2007 The relationship between tooth wear, habitat quality and late-life reproduction in a wild red deer population. J. Anim. Ecol. 76, 402-412. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2007.01212.x)
80. Venkataraman VV, Glowacka H, Fritz J, Clauss M, Seyoum C, Nguyen N, Fashing PJ. 2014 Effects of dietary fracture toughness and dental wear on chewing efficiency in geladas (Theropithecus gelada). Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 155, 17-32. (doi:10.1002/ajpa.22571)
81. Fairbanks LA. 1996 Individual differences in maternal style: causes and consequences for mothers and offspring. Adv. Study Behav. 25, 579- 611. (doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60343-5)
82. Lu A, Bergman TJ, McCann C, Stinespring-Harris A, Beehner JC. 2016 Growth trajectories in wild geladas (Theropithecus gelada). Am. J. Primatol. 78, 707-719. (doi:10.1002/ajp.22535)
83. Leigh SR, Bernstein RM. 2006 Ontogeny, life history, and maternal investment in baboons. In Reproduction and fitness in baboons: behavioral, ecological, and life history perspectives (eds L Swedell, SR Leigh), pp. 225-255. Boston, MA: Springer US. (doi:10.1007/0-387-33674-5_10)
84. Levy EJ et al. 2023 Early life drought predicts components of adult body size in wild female baboons. Am. J. Biol. Anthropol. 182, 357-371. (doi: 10.1002/ajpa.24849)
85. Smuts B, Nicolson N. 1989 Reproduction in wild female olive baboons. Am. J. Primatol. 19, 229-246. (doi:10.1002/ajp.1350190405)
86. Lu A, Feder JA, Snyder-Mackler N, Bergman TJ, Beehner JC. 2021 Male-mediated maturation in wild geladas. Curr. Biol. 31, 214-219.(doi:10. 1016/j.cub.2020.10.003)
87. Lerch BA, Zipple MN, Gesquiere LR, Sloan ET, Beehner JC, Alberts SC. 2024 Male-mediated early maturation unlikely to evolve via adaptive evolution. Anim. Behav. 214, 219-240. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.002)
88. Lu A, Borries C, Gustison ML, Larney E, Koenig A. 2016 Age and reproductive status influence dominance in wild female Phayre's leaf monkeys. Anim. Behav. 117, 145-153. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.04.020)
89. Jones CB. 1980 The functions of status in the mantled howler monkey, Alouatta palliata Gray: intraspecific competition for group membership in a folivorous neotropical primate. Primates 21, 389-405. (doi:10.1007/bf02390468)
90. De Vries D, Koenig A, Borries C. 2016 Female reproductive success in a species with an age-inversed hierarchy. Integr. Zool. 11, 433-446. (doi: 10.1111/1749-4877.12201)
91. Nussey DH, Kruuk LEB, Morris A, Clements MN, Pemberton JM, Clutton-Brock TH. 2009 Inter- and intrasexual variation in aging patterns across reproductive traits in a wild red deer population. Am. Nat. 174, 342-357. (doi:10.1086/603615)
92. Hayward AD, Wilson AJ, Pilkington JG, Clutton-Brock TH, Pemberton JM, Kruuk LEB. 2013 Reproductive senescence in female Soay sheep: variation across traits and contributions of individual ageing and selective disappearance. Funct. Ecol. 27, 184-195. (doi:10.1111/1365-2435. 12029)
93. Sloan ET, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Lu A, Snyder-Mackler N, Jacquemyn H. 2022 Effects of climate variability on the demography of wild geladas. Ecol. Evol. 12, e8759. (doi:10.1002/ece3.8759)
94. Sargeant EJ, Wikberg EC, Kawamura S, Fedigan LM. 2015 Allonursing in white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) provides evidence for cooperative care of infants. Behaviour 152, 1841-1869. (doi:10.1163/1568539x-00003308)
95. Perry S. 2012 The behavior of wild white-faced capuchins: demography, life history, social relationships, and communication. Adv. Stud. Anim. Behav. 44, 135-181. (doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-394288-3.00004-6)
96. Heslin Piper LA, Dietz JM, Raboy BE. 2017 Multi-male groups positively linked to infant survival and growth in a cooperatively breeding primate. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 71, 176. (doi:10.1007/s00265-017-2404-3)
97. Guindre-Parker S, Rubenstein DR. 2018 Multiple benefits of alloparental care in a fluctuating environment. R. Soc. Open Sci. 5, 172406. (doi:10. 1098/rsos.172406)
98. Robbins MM et al. 2023 Comparative life history patterns of female gorillas. Am. J. Biol. Anthropol. 181, 564-574. (doi:10.1002/ajpa.24792)
99. Landsem TL et al. 2023 Raising offspring increases ageing: differences in senescence among three populations of a long-lived seabird, the Atlantic puffin. J. Anim. Ecol. 92, 774-785. (doi:10.1111/1365-2656.13884)
100. Barrett L, Henzi SP, Lycett JE. 2006 Whose life is it anyway? Maternal investment, developmental trajectories, and life history strategies in baboons. In Reproduction and fitness in baboons: behavioral, ecological, and life history perspectives (eds L Swedell, SR Leigh), pp. 199-224. Boston, MA: Springer US. (doi:10.1007/0-387-33674-5_9)
101. Nguyen N et al. 2015 Fitness impacts of tapeworm parasitism on wild gelada monkeys at Guassa, Ethiopia. Am. J. Primatol. 77, 579-594. (doi: 10.1002/ajp.22379)
102. Feder JA. 2024 GeladaResearchProject/Feder_et_al_2024_Reproductive_aging: Reproductive aging in chacma baboons and geladas (v1.0.0). Zenodo. (doi:10.5281/zenodo.13904225)
103. Feder JA, Schneider-Crease IA, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Seyfarth R, Silk J et al. 2024 Supplementary material from: Female reproductive aging persists despite high infanticide risk in chacma baboons and geladas. FigShare (doi:10.6084/m9.figshare.c.7598812)
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2025. This work is published under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Across mammals, fertility and offspring survival are often lowest at the beginning and end of females' reproductive careers. However, extrinsic drivers of reproductive success - including infanticide by males-could stochastically obscure these expected age-related trends. Here, we modelled reproductive ageing trajectories in two cercopithecine primates that experience high rates of male infanticide: the chacma baboon (Papio ursinus) and the gelada (Theropithecus gelada). We found that middle-aged mothers generally achieved the shortest interbirth intervals in chacma baboons. By contrast, old gelada females often showed shorter interbirth intervals than their younger group-mates with one exception: the oldest females typically failed to produce additional offspring before their deaths. Infant survival peaked in middle-aged mothers in chacma baboons but in young mothers in geladas. While infant mortality linked with maternal death increased as mothers aged in both species, infanticide risk did not predictably shift with maternal age. Thus, infanticide patterns cannot explain the surprising young mother advantage observed in geladas. Instead, we argue that this could be a product of their graminivorous diets, Which might remove some energetic constraints on early reproduction. In sum, our data suggest that reproductive ageing is widespread but may be differentially shaped by ecological pressures.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
1 Institute of Human Origins
2 School of Human Evolution and Social Change
3 Department of Psychology
4 Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA