Introduction
Bats must decide when to emerge from their roost daily. They seek to optimise foraging gain (‘profit’) by accounting for the trade-off between their ability to feed, their energetic costs, peak prey availability and predation risk in order to survive (Russo, Cistrone, and Jones 2007). Emerge too late and they may miss the peak prey availability, or too early and risk predation. Jones and Rydell (1994) suggest that foraging strategy and predation risk were most important at determining broader patterns of emergence timing for many species, but others have suggested that abiotic factors such as temperature and canopy structure are most likely to influence emergence times because of their effect on bat prey availability (invertebrates) (O'Donnell 2000; Taylor 1963) and protection from predators (Russo, Cistrone, and Jones 2007), respectively.
Emergence timing is also driven by energetic costs largely set by phenological and physiological constraints, the effects of the cost of flight and the need to maintain a high metabolic rate when pregnant, lactating or during spermatogenesis (Racey and Speakman 1987; Racey and Tam 1974). As an in utero foetus grows, for some species, the increasing load increases the energetic costs of flight for their mothers and decreases their manoeuvrability and ability to evade predators, so emergence becomes later (Duvergé et al. 2000; Russo, Cistrone, and Jones 2007). High energetic costs of lactation, and risks of dehydration, are reflected in lactating females emerging earlier as their dependent young grew (Anthony and Kunz 1977; Duvergé et al. 2000; Kurta, Johnson, and Kunz 1987; McLean and Speakman 1999). Young greater horseshoe bats (Rhinolophus ferrumequinum), for whom flight is new and costly, at first emerged later than their parents/other adults, but then began emerging earlier as their flying improved (Duvergé et al. 2000).
Light levels and predation risk at the roost site also likely impact emergence timing decisions. Canopy structure and distance from the forest edge have been found to affect when bats of different species emerge from their roosts (Arndt et al. 2018; Funakoshi and Uchida 1978; Russo, Cistrone, and Jones 2007; Rydell, Entwistle, and Racey 1996). Some species of bats may assess predation risk of vision-dependent predators to be lower in relatively cluttered, high canopy cover areas, so they emerge earlier, allowing for prolonged feeding durations (Russo, Cistrone, and Jones 2007; Rydell, Entwistle, and Racey 1996).
Temperature may influence emergence indirectly because of its effect on the prey of insectivorous bats (Anthony, Stack, and Kunz 1981; Rydell 1989), but this has not be found for all species (Viele, Kurta, and Kath 2002). Cloud coverage and precipitation level effects on emergence timing also vary (Arndt et al. 2018; Funakoshi and Uchida 1978; Swift 1980; Thomas and Jacobs 2013). Cloud coverage likely offers similar benefits to canopy cover as it decreases a predator's visibility by blocking moonlight (Arndt et al. 2018; Welbergen 2006). Rain can impede flight and be energetically costly (Voigt et al. 2011), so bats may alter their emergence time to avoid heavy rain or showers (Doty, Gonsalves, and Law 2019; Funakoshi and Uchida 1978; Shiel and Fairley 1999); however, this is not a pattern found in all bat species (Swift 1980). Some bat species may also emerge earlier after periods of low prey availability caused by inclement weather (Frick et al. 2012).
When the number of bats within a roost is greater, bats often begin to emerge earlier (Arndt et al. 2018; Warren and Witter 2002), but because of the large number of individuals queueing to leave (Hristov et al. 2010) median time of emergence may be later (Avery 1986; McAney and Fairley 1988). Again, this is not a pattern found for all species (Jones 1995; Russo, Cistrone, and Jones 2007). The lack of a universal pattern for all these influences means that the effects of factors on a species emergence pattern must be examined for each species separately for them to be meaningful.
Long-tailed bats (Chalinolobus tuberculatus, pekapeka) are one of two extant endemic New Zealand bat species. They are a nocturnal insectivorous temperate species belonging to the family Vespertilionidae and are classified as Nationally Critical under the New Zealand Threat Classification System (O'Donnell et al. 2023) and Critically Endangered by the International Union for Conservation of Nature (O'Donnell 2021). Long-tailed bats typically roost within tree cavities and under peeling bark (Sedgeley and O'Donnell 1999a) but some also roost within caves (O'Donnell 2002a). As bats are New Zealand's only native terrestrial mammal, their only suspected natural predators are birds. These include the nocturnal morepork (Ninox novaeseelandiae) (Dwyer 1962), extinct laughing owl (Ninox albifacies) (Worthy and Holdaway 2002), diurnal Australasian harrier (Circus approximans) and New Zealand falcon (Falco novaeseelandiae) (Sedgeley and O'Donnell 1999b). Research into long-tailed bats' emergence timing patterns has been limited to assessments of the effect of weather and moonlight intensity to date. Minimum overnight temperature was a strong determinant of whether or not long-tailed bats emerged at all (O'Donnell 2000). Griffiths (1996) found that temperature at sunset was the only significant predictor of emergence timing of long-tailed bats in South Canterbury (44°11 S, 170°01 E). Nocturnal activity of long-tailed bats in Hawke's Bay (39°27 S, 176°50 E) began earlier under low mean moonlight intensity, likely due to prey insects avoiding emergence under high moonlight intensities (Gillingham 1996).
Visual emergence surveys (emergence or roost watches) are used to understand roost use (O'Donnell and Sedgeley 1999) and population size (Kunz et al. 2009), identify maternity roosts (Mitchell-Jones 2004), and ensure bats are not present in roosts when trees or buildings are modified (Collins et al. 2016; Scheunert, Zahn, and Kiefer 2010). The reliability of these measures is dependent on understanding emergence patterns and their drivers. Bats (Chiroptera) are members of a highly diverse order where a one-size-fits-all conservation method does not exist; each species is unique and possesses different behaviours, warranting species-specific management plans (Fenton 1997). Detailed understanding of long-tailed bat emergence therefore represents a gap in the knowledge that is crucial for informing conservation practises such as tree-felling protocols. These protocols aim to identify and protect potential roost trees by performing emergence watches; understanding the correct time to carry these out is essential.
Based on global literature, we predicted long-tailed bats would emerge earlier:
- as the summer progresses and bats shift from pre-parturition towards post-lactation period (Lee and McCracken 2001; Russo, Cistrone, and Jones 2007) and more specifically, as lactation advances (Duvergé et al. 2000; Russo, Cistrone, and Jones 2007),
- with increasing sunset temperature (Anthony, Stack, and Kunz 1981; Griffiths 1996; Rydell 1989),
- under higher tree density and/or cloud cover (Arndt et al. 2018; Funakoshi and Uchida 1978; Russo, Cistrone, and Jones 2007; Rydell, Entwistle, and Racey 1996),
- with decreased level of precipitation (Funakoshi and Uchida 1978; Shiel and Fairley 1999) and
- with decreasing roost population size (Arndt et al. 2018; Warren and Witter 2002).
Moreover, bats were predicted to continuously emerge later as gestation progresses (Duvergé et al. 2000; Russo, Cistrone, and Jones 2007).
Materials and Methods
Study Sites
Long-tailed bat emergence data were collected from the Kinleith Forest, a large exotic plantation forest primarily composed of plots with uniformly planted pine trees (Pinus radiata) of similar size, in the Waikato Region of the North Island (38°17′ S, 175°53′ E). The site had a mean elevation of 632 m above sea level (a.s.l.) and was 96,652 ha at the time of data collection (Figure 1) over three consecutive austral summers (November to March of 2006–2009).
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
Emergence timings of long-tailed bats were also collected from the Eglinton Valley, a native forest (elevation of 300 m (a.s.l.)) mainly comprising of native red beech (Fuscospora fusca), mountain beech (F. cliffortioides) and silver beech (Lophozonia menziesii) in Fiordland National Park (44°58′ S, 168°01′ E) (Figure 1). The forest was old and complex with multiple strata and spreading crowns. Data were collected over three consecutive austral summers (October to March of 1993–1996) and supplemented with observations from January and February 2022.
Data Collection
Roost watches were undertaken in relatively fine weather (not in heavy rain) and where roosts could be safely accessed by observers in the dark. These limitations are unlikely to be a large bias as bats do not typically emerge under heavy precipitation (Funakoshi and Uchida 1978; Shiel and Fairley 1999) and those that roost far from the flats of Eglinton Valley are likely to be post-lactation and therefore solitary (roosts that contain only one bat) (O'Donnell and Sedgeley 1999). In conjunction with visual counts of emerging bats, we used a handheld bat detector to corroborate observations.
Kinleith Forest
Long-tailed bats were captured and radio-tagged. Roost trees were located using radio telemetry and emergence watches took place as described by Borkin, O'Donnell, and Parsons (2011). Each individual bat's emergence time stamp was subtracted from the sunset time (from the NZ Nautical Almanac; LINZ (2006, 2007, 2008)) to produce their emergence time relative to sunset, the dependent variable (negative values denote emergences before sunset, positive values denote after). A Vertex III and Transponder T3 (Haglöf Sweden AB) were used to measure distance from the roost tree to determine the number of pine trees planted within a 10 m radius as a measure for tree density. Weather variables (temperature at sunset and level of rainfall in the hour before sunset) were obtained retrospectively from the Athol base weather station situated at the study site through the Rural Fire Protection Authority.
Eglinton Valley
Long-tailed bats were captured and radio-tagged. Roost trees were located and confirmed using radio telemetry and emergence watches took place as described in O'Donnell and Sedgeley (1999). Emergence times were recorded and subtracted from sunset times (from the NZ Nautical Almanac; Lamont (1993, 1994, 1995)) to produce their emergence time relative to sunset (dependent variable). Cloud cover was measured by eye at sunset in Oktas (8 representing 100% coverage and 0 as 0%). Rain presence at sunset was recorded as yes (1) or no (0). Temperature at sunset was extracted retrospectively via NIWA (National Institute of Water and Atmospheric Research 2022) recorded by Knob's Flat weather station situated at the study site.
In the 2022 study season, free standing harp traps were set up in known high bat traffic areas to catch suitable lactating female bats for transmitter attachment (Model BD2, Holohil, Canada). To maximise bat attraction, acoustic bat lures (Sussex Autobat 1 and 2, Sussex University, United Kingdom) were attached to some traps. These devices played a synthesised social bat-call known as ‘Bechsteins2’ (Hill and Greenaway 2005) and were connected to a two-way ultrasonic speaker (Polaroid 600 Series, Environmental Grade electrostatic transducer). Traps were checked 1 h before sunrise, whereby each bat was processed following methods outlined by O'Donnell and Sedgeley (1999). Roost watches were carried out from 1 h before sunset until 20 min after the last observed bat emergence. Time was noted for each emergence using a phone application ‘TimeStamp’ (version 0.4.1; developed by m_c8bit). Sunset for each roost on each roost watch night was calculated via Global Monitoring Laboratory (2009) using roost latitude–longitude coordinates (geographic coordinate system). Minutes from sunset were calculated as above.
Data Analysis
Solitary bats from the Eglinton Valley frequently enter torpor due to low temperatures even in summer, so we excluded these from our analyses of emergence data (McNab and O'Donnell 2018). Roosting communally increases the overall temperature of the roost, so bats are less likely to enter torpor and emerge from their roosts (Webb 1999). Solitary bats were not excluded from the Kinleith Forest dataset as bats are more likely to be active there each night as temperatures are on average higher, and the types of roosts most commonly used—under loose bark—means that they cannot house large numbers (Sedgeley and O'Donnell 1999b).
To understand factors influencing emergence time, linear mixed models were fitted with restricted maximum likelihood using the package ‘lmerTest’ (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, and Christensen 2017). As bats in the same roost are not independent of each other, true observations were at the roost level for both study sites. As some roosts were visited more than once (36%) across both sites over the study period, a visitation number was assigned to each roost watch and a nested random effect was added (visit number nested within each unique roost) in all analyses. This accounts for the non-independence of individual bats in the same roost, allowing us to use each individual bat's emergence as the dependent variable. As different roosts watched on the same day did not occur often, the observation date was not added as a random effect. We used the Satterthwaite method (see Giesbrecht and Burns (1985)) to determine degrees of freedom and p values as this method gives sufficiently accurate Type I error rates at the 0.05 level, even with smaller sample sizes (Luke 2017). As long-tailed bat maternity roosting aggregations are outnumbered by reproductive females (O'Donnell and Sedgeley 1999) and may not respond to the variables the same way at different points in the summer, the datasets were split into pre-parturition (October to December) and post-parturition (January to March) (O'Donnell 2002b) and analysed separately as well as together. Reproductive seasonality was measured in months whereby the number one was assigned to the first month data collection took place each season, two to the second month and so on, and was treated as a continuous variable. Before model fitting, diagnostic checks were implemented to ensure normality in the emergence time of each individual bat relative to sunset (minutes) dependent variable for each subset. For the Kinleith datasets (full, pre-parturition and post-parturition), we included month, temperature at sunset, rain, pine tree density and number of bats in roost as the predictor variables. For the Eglinton datasets, we included month, temperature at sunset, rain, cloud cover and number of bats in roost. We found each observation's influence using package ‘influence.ME’ (Nieuwenhuis, Te Grotenhuis, and Pelzer 2012) and then calculated the Cook's Distance of these values. No datapoints surpassed the 4/N threshold (where N represents the true number of replicates), and thus none were deemed influential.
All datasets followed a normal distribution during diagnostic checks, and all models visually satisfied model checking requirements. Multicollinearity was not detected as variance inflation factors of all predictors from all models were between 1 and 2. No outliers were detected during model checking.
Regression coefficient plots were constructed on the standardised scale for each data subset to visualise variable estimates and their standardised confidence intervals (CIs) using the ‘jtools’ package (Long 2022). Coefficients and their CIs were standardised for appropriate comparison of relative importance as the predictors are on the same scale. The binary variable rain presence was coded as a 0/1 dummy variable for appropriate standardisation; care was taken to ensure all variable point estimates and CIs were interpreted on the natural scale rather than the visualised standardised scale, with all other effects held fixed. Effects from pre-parturition and post-parturition subsets were interpreted only if the effect over the full season was non-significant at the type I error rate of 0.05. Due to their arbitrary definition, we do not report p-values; we instead report t-values and their associated degrees of freedom being. Reported 95% coefficient CIs were calculated following the Wald method using Satterthwaite-derived degrees of freedom via package ‘parameters’ (Lüdecke et al. 2020). All statistical analyses were carried out in R version 4.0.2 (RStudio Team 2021).
Results
In total, we observed 324 bats emerging from 30 unique roosts over three breeding seasons in the Kinleith Forest, and 4948 bats emerging from 103 unique roosts over four breeding seasons in the Eglinton Valley. Most observed roosts in the Kinleith forest were under peeling bark, whereas in the Eglinton Valley, the observed roosts were mostly tree cavities. The median number of bats occupying the same roost in the Kinleith Forest was 1 (range = 1 to 20), whereas the median number of bats seen emerging from the same roost in the Eglinton Valley was 32 (range = 2 to 122). Of the 134 independent roost watches carried out in the Kinleith Forest, 94 were solitary roosts (70%) whereas 45% were solitary (115 out of 259) in the Eglinton Valley. Solitary observations from the Eglinton Valley were omitted from analyses reported below due to the probability this resulted from temperature induced torpor (see Methods). As solitary roosts were found proportionally more in October and March, these most likely represent the males that have not yet become active in the breeding season and females that have completed the breeding season, respectively (O'Donnell and Sedgeley 1999).
During the study period, temperatures at sunset ranged from 10°C to 21.3°C (mean = 16.2°C) in Kinleith Forest and 7.1°C–29.5°C (mean = 14.8°C) in Eglinton Valley on nights that bats emerged. In Kinleith Forest, the number of planted P. radiata surrounding the roost tree within a 10 m radius (P. radiata density) ranged between 5 and 23 (median = 11) and the precipitation in the hour before sunset ranged between 0 and 1.02 mm (median = 0 mm). As this is classified as light rain (Meteorological Service of Canada 2013), the data were converted into binary code (1 was assigned to nights that rained in the hour before sunset and 0 otherwise). Thus, it rained in the hour before sunset for seven independent roost watches (5.5%) in Kinleith Forest. In Eglinton Valley, it rained at sunset for 17 independent roost watches (11.8%) and the mean cloud cover was 2.5 oktas (32%).
Variation of Emergence Timing
In total, we included data from 134 independent roost watches for analysis in the Kinleith dataset and 144 in the Eglinton dataset. Bats were observed emerging in Kinleith Forest from 14 min before sunset to 68 min after sunset and in Eglinton Valley from 56 min before sunset to 66 min after sunset. Most first emergences occurred before sunset in the Eglinton Valley and after sunset in Kinleith Forest (Figure 2a,b). Most emergence times from Kinleith Forest were centred on 30 min after sunset (Figure 2c) and on sunset time (0 min) in Eglinton Valley (Figure 2d).
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
Timing of Bat Emergence in the Kinleith Forest
Although there was no overall effect of month across the full study period and during post-parturition, bats emerged later as pre-parturition progressed (coefficient = 10.5, i.e., 10.5 min later per month, CI = 3.0–17.9, t35.4 = 2.826) (Figures 3a and 4a). Temperature at sunset had a significant negative relationship with emergence timing only during pre-parturition such that the warmer it was at sunset, the earlier bats emerged (coef. = −1.8, i.e., 1.8 min earlier per 1°C increase in temperature, CI = −2.9 to −0.6, t46.3 = −2.997). The higher the tree density in the Kinleith Forest, the earlier bats emerged across the full study period (coef. = −1.2, CI = −2.0 to −0.5, t26.2 = −3.285) (Figures 3a and 5). Rain presence in the hour before sunset was not a significant predictor for emergence during any period (Figure 3a). During pre-parturition, the more bats in the roost, the earlier bats emerged (coef. = −2.2, CI = −4.3 to −0.1, t19.6 = −2.188). On the standardised scale, the most important drivers of emergence timing during pre-parturition appeared to be the change in months, followed by tree density, roost population size and temperature at sunset (Figure 3a).
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
Timing of Bat Emergences in the Eglinton Valley
Bats emerged earlier during pre-parturition (coef. = −7.9, CI = −13.0 to −2.8, t24 = −3.187) and later during post-parturition (coef. = 8.5, CI = 4.2 to 12.8, t66 = 3.963) (Figures 3b and 4b) contrary to our predictions. As temperature at sunset increased, bats emerged earlier across the full study period (coef. = −0.7, CI = −1.3 to −0.1, t158.0 = −2.167) (Figures 3b and 6). Cloud cover at sunset and presence of rain at sunset was not a significant predictor for emergence timing across the full study period nor during pre-parturition and post-parturition (Figure 3b). Although the relationship between the total number of bats seen emerging from the roost and emergence timing was not significant over the full study period nor during post-parturition (Figure 3b), it was significantly positive during pre-parturition whereby the more bats there were occupying the roost, the later bats emerged (coef. = 0.2, CI = 0.1–0.3, t39 = 2.459). On the standardised scale, reproductive seasonality (month) and roost population size were the most important predictors followed by temperature at sunset (Figure 3b).
[IMAGE OMITTED. SEE PDF]
Discussion
Long-tailed bats in the Eglinton Valley emerged consistently earlier than those in the Kinleith Forest, and the factors driving emergence timing differed between the two sites. Reproductive seasonality was an important predictor at both sites during pre-parturition but affected emergence timing inversely. This opposing effect at the two sites was also seen in the number of bats occupying the roost during pre-parturition. Weather variables were not the key drivers of emergence in this study; temperature had a small effect and rain had no effect at both study sites. Tree density and cloud cover acted as proxies for drivers of anti-predator behaviour in the Kinleith Forest and Eglinton Valley, respectively; however, only tree density was detected to influence emergence timing. As this relationship is not fully understood, more research is required.
Our study of two long-tailed bat populations enabled us to examine influences on nightly emergence timing at maternity roosts over the summer period when bats were heavily pregnant (pre-parturition) or lactating and newly volant (post-parturition). During pre-parturition, emergence timing was more sensitive to environmental factors in the Kinleith Forest than during times when bats were lactating and newly volant, likely driven by female reproductive status. Several species of bats are active for shorter periods when heavily pregnant than when lactating (Anthony, Stack, and Kunz 1981; Swift 1980) including long-tailed bats (O'Donnell 2002b). Flight likely has high energetic costs for pregnant bats, whilst the requirement to maintain relatively high metabolic levels for foetal development means they are less likely to enter torpor. Consequently, we expect that pregnant bats are under more pressure to emerge at an optimal time to balance these needs. We note that the physiological demands within pregnancy in long-tailed bats are quite variable in comparison to post-parturition (O'Donnell 2002b). Thus, we expect that female bats in earlier pregnancy may respond differently to environmental cues compared to those at late pregnancy, explaining the large CI seen for effects during pre-parturition.
We expected that long-tailed bats would emerge earlier as bats progressed from pre-parturition towards post-lactation. We only observed this pattern of earlier emergence as the summer progressed during pre-parturition in the Kinleith Forest. In the Eglinton Valley, we observed long-tailed bats emerging earlier as pregnancy advanced and then later as lactation advanced towards weaning. We suspect that during post-parturition, emergence timing may be influenced by the late emergence of juvenile bats (Petrželková and Zukal 2001), resulting in a non-significant effect when looking at the summer as a whole. The result may have been different if we were able to exclude juvenile bats from our analyses. Juvenile bats are relatively inexperienced poor fliers and tend to emerge much later likely to avoid predation (Petrželková and Zukal 2001). We suspect that juvenile long-tailed bats were weaned relatively quickly, with 65% of captured females classified as post-lactating in the first week of February (O'Donnell 2002c), allowing later emergence by mothers as the energetic cost of lactation subsides as seen in Mexican free-tailed bats (Tadarida brasiliensis) (Lee and McCracken 2001; Lima and O'Keefe 2013). It is noted that in some species, juveniles emerge earlier than adults to keep up with nutritional needs once weaned (Lee and McCracken 2001; Reichard et al. 2009), or as their flying abilities improve (Duvergé et al. 2000). The counteracting patterns of emergence timing between age classes may also explain the non-significant effect of changing months in our study across the whole breeding season.
The vast majority of current literature shows bats emerge later as pregnancy progresses and gradually emerge earlier as they progress from lactation towards weaning (Duvergé et al. 2000; Jones 1995; Russo, Cistrone, and Jones 2007). However, Arndt et al. (2018) found that first emerging Indiana bats (Myotis sodalis) departed earlier as pregnancy progressed, associating this to the variation of prey availability, predator behaviour and abiotic factors. During one summer, Petrželková and Zukal (2001) found that serotine bats (Eptesicus serotinus) emerged earlier during the post-parturition period than pre-parturition period; however, the opposite was true in the previous summer, with the authors citing annual differences in prey availability. Invertebrate availability increased and peaked during post-parturition in the Eglinton Valley (O'Donnell 2000); thus, lactating long-tailed bats may not need to emerge progressively earlier during post-parturition to feed their growing young (Duvergé et al. 2000).
We found that the warmer the temperature at sunset, the earlier long-tailed bats emerged, consistent with results for other temperate bat species (Anthony, Stack, and Kunz 1981; Griffiths 1996; Rydell 1989). Previous research found that long-tailed bats emerged earlier on nights when the sunset temperature was higher (Griffiths 1996). Temperature can guide bats in matching nightly foraging activity with peak invertebrate activity, and they may take longer to arouse from torpor on colder days and thus leave the roost later (O'Shea and Vaughan 1977). The presence of light rain did not influence emergence timing at both study sites. Although emergence timing in many bat species is not affected by light rain, some species emerge after or between bouts of heavy rain (Entwistle, Racey, and Speakman 1996; Erkert 1982; Shiel and Fairley 1999; Weinbeer, Meyer, and Kalko 2006). As we did not monitor emergences under heavy rain, we could not test this hypothesis, highlighting an opportunity for further research.
We observed that bats in Kinleith Forest rarely emerged before sunset, whereas the majority (80%) of first bats emerged from roosts before sunset in the Eglinton Valley. These two populations occupy habitats which vastly differ in vegetation structure, roost types used and latitude. These may all have implications for emergence.
We suspect that the higher latitude (44°58′ S) of the Eglinton Valley compared to the Kinleith Forest (38°17′ S) could help explain this difference. Rydell, Entwistle, and Racey (1996) suggested that bats living in higher latitudes benefit more by emerging early compared to those at lower latitudes. At high latitudes, the nights are generally shorter and cooler, so invertebrates are less active and their abundance drops more quickly on shorter nights (O'Donnell 2000; Rautenbach, Kemp, and Scholtz 1988; Taylor 1963). Over the summer, nights become shorter towards the summer solstice and longer afterwards. This rate of change is greater at higher latitudes compared to areas of lower latitude. For example, night length of the 2021 summer solstice (22 December) was 8 h and 26 min in the Eglinton Valley, whereas it was 9 h and 15 min in the Kinleith Forest (Global Monitoring Laboratory 2009). Rate of change in night duration is thus greater in the Eglinton Valley moving towards the autumn equinox, perhaps driving the earlier emergence towards the summer solstice and later emergence afterwards at pre-parturition and post-parturition, respectively.
Roosts under peeling bark within plantation forests do not support the relatively high temperatures found in roost cavities used by bats in the Eglinton Valley (Borkin and Parsons 2011; Sedgeley 2001), which may contribute to later emergence times in the Kinleith Forest. The effect of roost population was only significant during pre-parturition at both sites but in opposite directions; an increase of bats in a roost was associated with earlier emergence in the Kinleith Forest, whilst it resulted in later emergence in the Eglinton Valley. Roost population sizes were far smaller within the Kinleith Forest compared to the Eglinton Valley. Bats in smaller group sizes take longer to warm up after daily torpor (Willis and Brigham 2007) and need not grapple to avoid getting stuck in the queue to leave (Hristov et al. 2010). Similar to Avery (1986) and McAney and Fairley (1988), we found that bats in the Eglinton Valley left later as the roost population grew. In the Eglinton Valley, long-tailed bats may start emergence earlier to compensate for the need to wait for their turn to leave (Feng, unpublished data). Increased roost population size is also associated with higher ambient temperature in roosts (Willis and Brigham 2007), when particularly high, bats increase their rates of evaporative cooling through respiration and through their skin (Reeder and Cowles 1951). This can lead to dehydration (Korine, Zinder, and Arad 1999), forcing earlier emergence. There was not a threshold roost size whereby long-tailed bats started to emerge earlier (Feng, unpublished data). We noted no relationship between roost population size and emergence time during post-parturition at both study sites, indicating other factors are more important when juveniles are present.
The Kinleith Forest and the Eglinton Valley are both occupied by morepork. Plantation forests are thought to have high densities of falcon (Seaton 2007) due to increased bird prey abundance, and are known for their importance in supporting native avian predators (Pawson et al. 2010). Therefore, long-tailed bats at Kinleith Forest may be exhibiting predator defence mechanisms including emerging after sunset and emerging earlier under higher tree densities. Tree density and canopy cover were lower at roost trees compared to available trees in the Eglinton Valley as emerging in more open forest structures may allow better manoeuvring (Sedgeley and O'Donnell 1999a). This can potentially be useful for both predator evasion and feeding efficiency. More research is required around these interpretations.
Conservation Implications
Long-tailed bats most often roost in the oldest trees available in the landscape (Borkin 2010; Sedgeley and O'Donnell 1999b), and these trees are often targeted during logging operations or for firewood in plantation forests (Borkin, O'Donnell, and Parsons 2011; Sedgeley 2001; Sedgeley and O'Donnell 2004) or removed due to risks to humans. Where trees are felled in bat habitats, current best practice protocols aim to reduce the likelihood of killing or injuring bats in their roost, following strict guidelines to ensure no bats are inside during felling (New Zealand Department of Conservation's Bat Recovery Group 2021). Checking for bat presence involves a step-by-step approach at times requiring emergence watches. The guidance recommends emergence watches begin at sunset and continue until it is too dark to see. If no bats are observed during the roost emergence watches, it is concluded that the roost tree of interest is not occupied, and the tree can be felled. As first emergences in the Eglinton Valley occurred consistently before sunset, we strongly recommend revising these protocols so that they start earlier to observe all bats leaving roosts there. There may be other colonies where these protocols do not capture all emergence times because these vary according to location (Figure 1) and time of year. We recommend using high quality of infrared or thermal cameras particularly at lower latitudes where bats likely emerge after dark, although this may vary by habitat types that trigger other pressures on emergence timings. Our findings can also be used to improve monitoring for conservation purposes as they aid researchers in timing surveys for maximum detectability.
Author Contributions
Bonnie Feng: conceptualization (supporting), data curation (supporting), formal analysis (lead), methodology (supporting), project administration (lead), writing – original draft (lead), writing – review and editing (equal). Kerry M. Borkin: conceptualization (lead), data curation (lead), funding acquisition (lead), methodology (lead), supervision (supporting), writing – original draft (supporting), writing – review and editing (equal). Colin F. J. O'Donnell: conceptualization (supporting), data curation (lead), funding acquisition (lead), methodology (lead), resources (supporting), supervision (supporting), writing – review and editing (supporting). Joanne M. Monks: conceptualization (supporting), formal analysis (supporting), methodology (supporting), project administration (lead), resources (supporting), supervision (lead), writing – original draft (supporting), writing – review and editing (equal).
Acknowledgements
We thank all staff and volunteers across the Eglinton Valley and Kinleith Forest long-tailed bat monitoring seasons and their ongoing devotion to long-tailed bat conservation. We thank Dr. Peter Dillingham for excellent guidance in understanding data structure and data wrangling. KB was generously supported during fieldwork for her PhD by an Enterprise Scholarship funded mainly by the Tertiary Education Commission and Hancock Forest Management, with contributions from Carter Holt Harvey Forests, Blakely Pacific Limited, Pan Pacific Forest Products Limited, P.F. Olsen Ltd. and Kaingaroa Timberlands. KB was also supported by funding from Waikato Regional Council, Bat Conservation International, J.S. Watson Trust, E.B. Firth Charitable Trust, O.T.P.P. New Zealand Forest Investments Ltd. and the Valder Award. C.H.H. Kinleith Pulp and Paper, A.D.T. Security and Forest Protech provided logistical support. Department of Conservation Research and Collection Permit BP-18899-RES (under Section 53, Wildlife Act 1953) and authorisation from the University of Auckland Animal Ethics Committee (AEC 08/2004/R282) were obtained prior to bat handling in the Kinleith Forest. Eglinton Valley research was undertaken with funding from the Department of Conservation Science Investigations 1578 and 3665, World Wide Fund for Nature (NZ) through the Telecom Conservation Fund, New Zealand Lottery Grants Board and the University of Otago. The 2022 data collection in the Eglinton Valley was supported by the University of Otago's Ngāi Tahu Research Consultation Committee. Monitoring of Eglinton Valley long-tailed bat colonies takes place annually as a part of Department of Conservation Science Investigation 4230. The handling, radio-transmitter attachment and radio tracking of bats adhered to Department of Conservation Animal Ethics Best Practice (Sedgeley et al. 2012). Open access publishing facilitated by University of Otago, as part of the Wiley - University of Otago agreement via the Council of Australian University Librarians.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Data Availability Statement
Data files, software file, Supporting Information file and README file available on online repository Dryad using reviewer link .
Anthony, E., M. Stack, and T. Kunz. 1981. “Night Roosting and the Nocturnal Time Budget of the Little Brown Bat, Myotis lucifugus: Effects of Reproductive Status, Prey Density, and Environmental Conditions.” Oecologia 51: 151.
Anthony, E. L., and T. H. Kunz. 1977. “Feeding Strategies of the Little Brown Bat, Myotis lucifugus, in Southern New Hampshire.” Ecology 58: 775.
Arndt, R. J., J. M. O'Keefe, W. A. Mitchell, J. B. Holmes, and S. L. Lima. 2018. “Do Predators Influence the Behaviour of Temperate‐Zone Bats? An Analysis of Competing Models of Roost Emergence Times.” Animal Behaviour 145: 161.
Avery, M. 1986. “Factors Affecting the Emergence Times of Pipistrelle Bats.” Journal of Zoology 209: 293.
Borkin, K. M. 2010. Ecology of New Zealand's Long‐Tailed Bat (Chalinolobus tuberculatus) in Exotic Plantation Forest. PhD thesis, Auckland: University of Auckland.
Borkin, K. M., C. O'Donnell, and S. Parsons. 2011. “Bat Colony Size Reduction Coincides With Clear‐Fell Harvest Operations and High Rates of Roost Loss in Plantation Forest.” Biodiversity and Conservation 20: 3537.
Borkin, K. M., and S. Parsons. 2011. “Sex‐Specific Roost Selection by Bats in Clearfell Harvested Plantation Forest: Improved Knowledge Advises Management.” Acta Chiropterologica 13: 373.
Collins, J., P. Charleston, I. Davidson‐Watts, S. Markham, and L. Kerslake. 2016. Bat Surveys for Professional Ecologists: Good Practice Guidelines. 3rd ed. London, UK: Bat Conservation Trust.
Doty, A. C., L. Gonsalves, and B. S. Law. 2019. “Activity Patterns of Insectivorous Bats During a Seasonal Transition Period From Hibernation to Reproduction.” Australian Mammalogy 42: 1.
Duvergé, P. L., G. Jones, J. Rydell, and R. D. Ransome. 2000. “Functional Significance of Emergence Timing in Bats.” Ecography 23: 32.
Dwyer, P. D. 1962. “Studies on the Two New Zealand Bats.” Zoology Publications from Victoria University of Wellington 28: 1.
Entwistle, A., P. Racey, and J. Speakman. 1996. “Habitat Exploitation by a Gleaning Bat, Plecotus auritus.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 351: 921.
Erkert, H. G. 1982. “Ecological Aspects of Bat Activity Rhythms.” In Ecology of Bats, edited by T. H. Kunz, 201–242. Boston, MA: Springer.
Fenton, M. B. 1997. “Science and the Conservation of Bats.” Journal of Mammalogy 78: 1.
Frick, W. F., P. M. Stepanian, J. F. Kelly, et al. 2012. “Climate and Weather Impact Timing of Emergence of Bats.” PLoS One 7: e42737.
Funakoshi, K., and T. Uchida. 1978. “Studies on the Physiological and Ecological Adaptation of Temperate Insectivorous Bats: III. Annual Activity of the Japanese House‐Dwelling Bat, Pipistrellus abramus.” Journal of The Faculty of Agriculture Kyushu University 23: 95.
Giesbrecht, F. G., and J. C. Burns. 1985. “Two‐Stage Analysis Based on a Mixed Model: Large‐Sample Asymptotic Theory and Small‐Sample Simulation Results.” Biometrics 41: 477.
Gillingham, N. J. 1996. The Behaviour and Ecology of Long‐Tailed Bats (Chalinolobus tuberculatus Gray) in the Central North Island. Master's thesis, Turitea, New Zealand: Massey University.
Global Monitoring Laboratory. 2009. NOAA Solar Calculator. 2022. National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration. https://gml.noaa.gov/grad/solcalc/.
Griffiths, R. 1996. Aspects of the Ecology of a Long‐Tailed Bat, Chalinolobus tuberculatus (Gray, 1843), Population in a Highly Fragmented Habitat. Master's thesis, Canterbury: Lincoln University.
Hill, D. A., and F. Greenaway. 2005. “Effectiveness of an Acoustic Lure for Surveying Bats in British Woodlands.” Mammal Review 35: 116.
Hristov, N. I., M. Betke, D. E. Theriault, A. Bagchi, and T. H. Kunz. 2010. “Seasonal Variation in Colony Size of Brazilian Free‐Tailed Bats at Carlsbad Cavern Based on Thermal Imaging.” Journal of Mammalogy 91: 183.
Jones, G. 1995. “Flight Performance, Echolocation and Foraging Behaviour in Noctule Bats Nyctalus noctula.” Journal of Zoology 237: 303.
Jones, G., and J. Rydell. 1994. “Foraging Strategy and Predation Risk as Factors Influencing Emergence Time in Echolocating Bats.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences 346: 445.
Korine, C., O. Zinder, and Z. Arad. 1999. “Diurnal and Seasonal Changes in Blood Composition of the Free‐Living Egyptian Fruit Bat (Rousettus aegyptiacus).” Journal of Comparative Physiology. B 169: 280.
Kunz, T. H., M. Betke, N. I. Hristov, and M. J. Vonhof. 2009. “Methods for Assessing Colony Size, Population Size, and Relative Abundance of Bats.” In Ecological and Behavioral Methods for the Study of Bats, edited by T. H. Kunz and S. Parsons. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Kurta, A., K. A. Johnson, and T. H. Kunz. 1987. “Oxygen Consumption and Body Temperature of Female Little Brown Bats (Myotis lucifugus) Under Simulated Roost Conditions.” Physiological Zoology 60: 386.
Kuznetsova, A., P. B. Brockhoff, and R. H. B. Christensen. 2017. “lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models.” Journal of Statistical Software 82: 1.
Lamont, I. C. 1993. New Zealand Nautical Almanac 1993/1994 (NZ204). Auckland & Wellington, New Zealand: Hydrographic Office, Royal New Zealand Navy & GP Publications Ltd.
Lamont, I. C. 1994. New Zealand Nautical Almanac 1995 (NZ204). Auckland & Wellington, New Zealand: Hydrographic Office, Royal New Zealand Navy & GP Publications Ltd.
Lamont, I. C. 1995. New Zealand Nautical Almanac 1996 (NZ204). Auckland & Wellingtom, New Zealand: Hydrographic Office, Royal New Zealand Navy & GP Publications Ltd.
Lee, Y.‐F., and G. F. McCracken. 2001. “Timing and Variation in the Emergence and Return of Mexican Free‐Tailed Bats Tadarida brasiliensis Mexicana.” Zoological Studies 40: 309.
Lima, S. L., and J. M. O'Keefe. 2013. “Do Predators Influence the Behaviour of Bats?” Biological Reviews 88: 626.
LINZ. 2006. New Zealand Nautical Almanac 2006/07 Edition (NZ204). New Zealand: Land Information New Zealand.
LINZ. 2007. New Zealand Nautical Almanac 2007/08 Edition (NZ204). New Zealand: Land Information New Zealand.
LINZ. 2008. New Zealand Nautical Almanac 2008/09 Edition (NZ204). New Zealand: Land Information New Zealand.
Long, J. A. 2022. “jtools: Analysis and Presentation of Social Scientific Data.” Journal of Open Source Software 9: 6610.
Lüdecke, D., M. S. Ben‐Shachar, I. Patil, and D. Makowski. 2020. “Extracting, Computing and Exploring the Parameters of Statistical Models Using {R}.” Journal of Open Source Software 5: 2445.
Luke, S. G. 2017. “Evaluating Significance in Linear Mixed‐Effects Models in R.” Behavior Research Methods 49: 1494.
McAney, C., and J. Fairley. 1988. “Activity Patterns of the Lesser Horseshoe Bat Rhinolophus hipposideros at Summer Roosts.” Journal of Zoology 216: 325.
McLean, J., and J. Speakman. 1999. “Energy Budgets of Lactating and Non‐Reproductive Brown Long‐Eared Bats (Plecotus auritus) Suggest Females Use Compensation in Lactation.” Functional Ecology 13: 360.
McNab, B. K., and C. O'Donnell. 2018. “The Behavioral Energetics of New Zealand's Bats: Daily Torpor and Hibernation, a Continuum.” Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology 223: 18.
Meteorological Service of Canada. 2013. MANOBS‐Manual of Surface Weather Observations. 7th ed. Ottawa, Canada: Environment Canada.
Mitchell‐Jones, A. J. 2004. Bat Mitigation Guidelines. Peterborough, Cambridgeshire, UK: External Relations Team, English Nature.
National Institute of Water and Atmospheric Research. 2022. NIWA Neon System. 1999 & 2022. New Zealand: National Institute of Water and Atmosphere.
New Zealand Department of Conservation's Bat Recovery Group. 2021. Protocols for Minimising the Risk of Felling Bat Roosts (Bat Roost Protocols (BRP), Version 2.2022). Wellington, New Zealand: Department of Conservation.
Nieuwenhuis, R., H. Te Grotenhuis, and B. Pelzer. 2012. “Influence. ME: Tools for Detecting Influential Data in Mixed Effects Models.” R Journal 4: 38.
O'Donnell, C. F. 2000. “Influence of Season, Habitat, Temperature, and Invertebrate Availability on Nocturnal Activity of the New Zealand Long‐Tailed Bat (Chalinolobus tuberculatus).” New Zealand Journal of Zoology 27: 207.
O'Donnell, C. F. 2002a. “Variability in Numbers of Long‐Tailed Bats (Chalinolobus tuberculatus) Roosting in Grand Canyon Cave, New Zealand: Implications for Monitoring Population Trends.” New Zealand Journal of Zoology 29: 273.
O'Donnell, C. F. 2002b. “Influence of Sex and Reproductive Status on Nocturnal Activity of Long‐Tailed Bats (Chalinolobus tuberculatus).” Journal of Mammalogy 83: 794.
O'Donnell, C. F. 2002c. “Timing of Breeding, Productivity and Survival of Long‐Tailed Bats Chalinolobus tuberculatus (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae) in Cold‐Temperate Rainforest in New Zealand.” Journal of Zoology 257: 311.
O'Donnell, C. F. 2021. “Chalinolobus tuberculatus.” IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2021: e.T4425A21985132.2023.
O'Donnell, C. F., and K. M. Borkin. 2021. “Families Vespertilionidae and Mystacinidae.” In The Handbook of New Zealand Mammals, edited by C. King and D. Forsyth, 95–130. Melbourne, Australia: CSIRO Publishing.
O'Donnell, C. F., K. M. Borkin, J. Christie, et al. 2023. Conservation Status of Bats in Aotearoa New Zealand, 2022. Wellngton, New Zealand: Department of Conservation.
O'Donnell, C. F., and J. A. Sedgeley. 1999. “Use of Roosts by the Long‐Tailed Bat, Chalinolobus tuberculatus, in Temperate Rainforest in New Zealand.” Journal of Mammalogy 80: 913.
O'Shea, T. J., and T. A. Vaughan. 1977. “Nocturnal and Seasonal Activities of the Pallid Bat Antrozous Pallidus.” Journal of Mammalogy 58: 269.
Pawson, S. M., C. E. Ecroyd, R. Seaton, W. B. Shaw, and E. G. Brockerhoff. 2010. “New Zealand's Exotic Plantation Forests as Habitats for Threatened Indigenous Species.” New Zealand Journal of Ecology 34: 342.
Petrželková, K., and J. Zukal. 2001. “Emergence Behaviour of the Serotine Bat (Eptesicus serotinus) Under Predation Risk.” Netherlands Journal of Zoology 51: 395.
Racey, P., and J. Speakman. 1987. “The Energy Costs of Pregnancy and Lactation in Heterothermic Bats.” Symposia of the Zoological Society 57: 107.
Racey, P., and W. Tam. 1974. “Reproduction in Male Pipistrellus pipistrellus (Mammalia: Chiroptera).” Journal of Zoology 172: 101.
Rautenbach, I., A. Kemp, and C. Scholtz. 1988. “Fluctuations in Availability of Arthropods Correlated With Microchiropteran and Avian Predator Activities.” Koedoe 31: 77.
Reeder, W. G., and R. B. Cowles. 1951. “Aspects of Thermoregulation in Bats.” Journal of Mammalogy 32: 389.
Reichard, J. D., L. E. Gonzalez, C. M. Casey, L. C. Allen, N. I. Hristov, and T. H. Kunz. 2009. “Evening Emergence Behavior and Seasonal Dynamics in Large Colonies of Brazilian Free‐Tailed Bats.” Journal of Mammalogy 90: 1478.
RStudio Team. 2021. RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R. Boston, MA: RStudio, PBC.
Russo, D., L. Cistrone, and G. Jones. 2007. “Emergence Time in Forest Bats: The Influence of Canopy Closure.” Acta Oecologica 31: 119.
Rydell, J. 1989. “Feeding Activity of the Northern Bat Eptesicus nilssoni During Pregnancy and Lactation.” Oecologia 80: 562.
Rydell, J., A. Entwistle, and P. A. Racey. 1996. “Timing of Foraging Flights of Three Species of Bats in Relation to Insect Activity and Predation Risk.” Oikos 76: 243.
Scheunert, A., A. Zahn, and A. Kiefer. 2010. “Phenology and Roosting Habits of the Central European Grey Long‐Eared Bat Plecotus austriacus (Fischer 1829).” European Journal of Wildlife Research 56: 435.
Seaton, R. 2007. The Ecological Requirements of the New Zealand Falcon (Falco novaseelandiae) in Plantation Forestry. PhD thesis, Turitea, New Zealand: Massey University.
Sedgeley, J., C. F. O'Donnell, J. Lyall, et al. 2012. “DOC Best Practice Manual of Conservation Techniques for Bats.” In Biodiversity Inventory and Monitoring Toolbox, edited by T. Greene and K. McNutt, 1–172. Wellington, New Zealand: Department of Conservation.
Sedgeley, J. A. 2001. “Quality of Cavity Microclimate as a Factor Influencing Selection of Maternity Roosts by a Tree‐Dwelling Bat, Chalinolobus tuberculatus, in New Zealand.” Journal of Applied Ecology 38: 425.
Sedgeley, J. A., and C. F. O'Donnell. 1999a. “Roost Selection by the Long‐Tailed Bat, Chalinolobus tuberculatus, in Temperate New Zealand Rainforest and Its Implications for the Conservation of Bats in Managed Forests.” Biodiversity and Conservation 88: 261.
Sedgeley, J. A., and C. F. O'Donnell. 1999b. “Factors Influencing the Selection of Roost Cavities by a Temperate Rainforest Bat (Vespertilionidae: Chalinolobus tuberculatus) in New Zealand.” Journal of Zoology 249: 437.
Sedgeley, J. A., and C. F. O'Donnell. 2004. “Roost Use by Long‐Tailed Bats in South Canterbury: Examining Predictions of Roost‐Site Selection in a Highly Fragmented Landscape.” New Zealand Journal of Ecology 28: 1.
Shiel, C., and J. Fairley. 1999. “Evening Emergence of Two Nursery Colonies of Leisler's Bat (Nyctalus leisleri) in Ireland.” Journal of Zoology 247: 439.
Swift, S. M. 1980. “Activity Patterns of Pipistrelle Bats (Pipistrellus pipistrellus) in North‐East Scotland.” Journal of Zoology 190: 285.
Taylor, L. 1963. “Analysis of the Effect of Temperature on Insects in Flight.” Journal of Animal Ecology 32: 99.
Thomas, A. J., and D. S. Jacobs. 2013. “Factors Influencing the Emergence Times of Sympatric Insectivorous Bat Species.” Acta Chiropterologica 15: 121.
Viele, D. P., A. Kurta, and J. A. Kath. 2002. “Timing of Nightly Emergence.” In The Indiana Bat: Biology Management an Endangered Species, edited by A. Kurta and J. Kennedy, 199–207. Austin, TX: Bat Conservation International.
Voigt, C. C., K. Schneeberger, S. L. Voigt‐Heucke, and D. Lewanzik. 2011. “Rain Increases the Energy Cost of Bat Flight.” Biology Letters 7: 793.
Warren, R. D., and M. S. Witter. 2002. “Monitoring Trends in Bat Populations Through Roost Surveys: Methods and Data From Rhinolophus hipposideros.” Biodiversity and Conservation 105: 255.
Webb, P. 1999. A Provisional Report on a Study on the Use of Summer Torpor by Long‐Tailed and Short‐Tailed Bats in the Eglinton Valley, 1998–1999. Unpublished report. Dunedin, New Zealand: Zoology Department, University of Otago.
Weinbeer, M., C. F. Meyer, and E. K. Kalko. 2006. “Activity Pattern of the Trawling Phyllostomid Bat, Macrophyllum macrophyllum, in Panamá.” Biotropica 38: 69.
Welbergen, J. A. 2006. “Timing of the Evening Emergence From Day Roosts of the Grey‐Headed Flying Fox, Pteropus poliocephalus: The Effects of Predation Risk, Foraging Needs, and Social Context.” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 60: 311.
Willis, C. K., and R. M. Brigham. 2007. “Social Thermoregulation Exerts More Influence Than Microclimate on Forest Roost Preferences by a Cavity‐Dwelling Bat.” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62: 97.
Worthy, T., and R. Holdaway. 2002. The Lost World of the Moa. Christchurch, New Zealand: Canterbury University Press.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2025. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the "License"). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
ABSTRACT
Understanding temporal behavioural patterns in animals can be crucial to their conservation management. Emergence timing in bats, that is, the decision on when to depart day‐roosts for foraging, is one such example and is well studied in Northern Hemisphere bats. The emergence timing of New Zealand long‐tailed bats (
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details



1 Department of Zoology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
2 Department of Conservation, Fauna Science, Terrestrial Biodiversity Unit, Taupō, New Zealand
3 Department of Conservation, Fauna Science, Terrestrial Biodiversity Unit, Christchurch, New Zealand