Introduction
Humans are inherently social beings. Many of our everyday decisions, including what we eat, drink and buy, are shaped by the actions, beliefs, and emotions of other individuals1, 2–3. This phenomenon—social influence—has been widely studied across many fields and under different operationalizations. One major pathway to influence involves interpersonal communication4,5. Online and offline conversations about health topics relate to a wide range of behaviors such as alcohol use6, 7, 8–9 dietary habits2,10,11 exercise12 and smoking cessation13particularly among young adults. However, young adults vary widely in how likely they are to change their behavior in response to health-related conversations14.
What kinds of psychological processes help explain associations between conversations and future behaviors? A window into the nature of these processes is offered by functional neuroimaging (fMRI), which can identify neural systems associated with affective and social cognitive processes, and their engagement across individuals15. To examine between-person differences in the links between conversations and future behavior, we drew on recent paradigms that combine fMRI with ecological momentary assessment (EMA)16. fMRI can capture real time insights into how individuals process social cues17 and integrate inputs into health behavior18 while EMA can track health behaviors in real-world settings, for example through repeated text message surveys19. In this study, we combined fMRI and EMA to examine how individual differences in neural responses to social cues—specifically, the faces of peers—relate to the likelihood of drinking following alcohol conversations, using lagged analyses9. We focused on alcohol use as a prevalent behavior that influences health and well-being, particularly among a key population of young adults20 and in college settings21.
Neuroimaging studies have suggested the key role of several brain systems in processing social cues and subsequent behavior, including the reward system22,23 and the mentalizing system24, 25–26. The reward system, including the bilateral ventral striatum, dorsal striatum (i.e., caudate and putamen), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, is implicated in tracking rewards, for example, social rewards (e.g., approval from friends), food, or monetary rewards27. Individual differences in reward activity to social cues consistently correlate with conformity; with stronger reward activity and connectivity associated with higher likelihood to conform across substance use28, risky driving29, food choices30 and mobile app rating contexts24,31. A theoretical perspective underlying these findings suggests that humans value alignment with others, which fosters social connections and shared experiencess32,33. According to this view, individuals who are more likely to expect, or experience drinking-related rewards (e.g., social approval) as reflected by stronger activity in the reward system34 may also be more likely to conform to local drinking norms, i.e., by showing a more positive association between alcohol conversations and future drinking.
Consistent with this perspective, college drinking is largely social21,35 and shaped by social learning, where young adults often conform to drinking behaviors that are normative within their peer group36. Many students experiment with alcohol in the presence of peers and report drinking due to a desire to conform37. In parallel, in the brain, peer influence is often modulated by reward processing15with evidence that stronger reward activity to anticipated social rewards moderates links between peer norms and risk-taking susceptibility among adolescents28. Consistently, we anticipated that individuals with stronger reward responses to drinking peers versus non-drinking peers—defined here as peers with whom one drinks frequently vs. rarely2,35—would show a more positive association between alcohol conversations and drinking, compared to those without these reward-related brain patterns.
In addition to the brain’s reward system, the mentalizing system plays a central role in inferring and predicting mental states, or what peers think and expect38. Key regions involved in the mentalizing system, include the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC), temporoparietal junction (TPJ), posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)26,39. Numerous studies have linked stronger activity in mentalizing regions with increased conformity among both young adults and adolescents24,25,31,40, 41–42. Individuals with stronger activity in mentalizing regions may be more likely to consider peer viewpoints as more influential inputs to health decision-making34,44. Building on this work, we reasoned that, within college drinking contexts, individuals who show stronger mentalizing activity to drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers, possibly indicative of sensitivity to peer drinking-related expectations, may also show a more positive association between alcohol conversations and future drinking. Together, previous work points to the value of considering brain activity in both the reward and mentalizing systems to better understand individual differences in how conversations relate to future behavior.
Peers play a key source of influence, and may influence alcohol use directly (i.e., through feedback and conversations) and indirectly (i.e., through one’s mental representations of what peers might think and expect)43,44. For example, mental representations of drinking vs. non-drinking peers (with whom one drinks often vs. rarely) may evoke different values or social rewards attached to alcohol use. Further, mental representations of drinking peers may indirectly make local drinking norms salient, even without explicit peer pressure45,46, thereby potentially moderating to alcohol conversations and drinking47,48.
In parallel, the brain spontaneously tracks information about peers, such as their beliefs, traits, and behaviors during passive face viewing. Existing knowledge and perceptions of peers can be spontaneously retrieved during passive face processing49 and social attributes, such as status, can be tracked by activity in neural regions associated with reward and mentalizing17,50,51. Such mental schemas allow people to store and activate information about others rapidly via cognitive shortcuts or heuristics49. It is possible that different mental schemas of peers, evoked during passive face-viewing, may also become more (vs. less) activated during conversations. Bridging these disparate areas of research, individual differences in brain activity to one type of social signal—a peer’s face—may evoke different alcohol-related norms, expectations, or anticipated rewards, depending on prior drinking interactions with a given peer2,35. Individuals with stronger mentalizing and reward-related brain activity to the faces of drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers may more readily evoke pro-drinking schemas and anticipated social rewards, thereby showing a more positive association between alcohol conversations and drinking.
With the above considerations in mind, we examined how individual differences in reward and mentalizing activity to faces of drinking vs. non-drinking peers relate to lagged temporal associations between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking. We combined fMRI, with real-world social group information, and twice-a-day EMA over 28-days, to capture conversations and drinking within students’ daily lives9,19. We focused on existing, on-campus social groups where drinking is common52 and exacts a significant toll on the health, intellectual, and social lives of students53. Drawing on prior work suggesting that individuals with stronger neural sensitivity to social cues are more susceptible to social influence24,29,31,34 we hypothesized that individuals with stronger reward and mentalizing activity to the faces of drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers, will show a higher likelihood of next-day drinking following alcohol conversations.
Results
Descriptives
Throughout the 28-day EMA period, 99 out of 104 participants (~ 95%) reported drinking at least once and 97 (~ 93%) and reported at least one alcohol conversation. The average number of drinking occasions was 7.69 over 28 days (median = 6; SD = 6.07; range = 0–28) and the average number of alcohol conversations was 9.46 (median = 8; SD = 7.59; range = 0–34). No participants reported having both zero drinking occasions and zero alcohol conversations. See Fig. S4 for alcohol and conversation sample distributions. Participants were highly compliant with the study protocol, responding to a median of 95% (53/56) of alcohol conversation prompts (M = 50.17; SD = 7.8; range = 13–56) and 96% (53.5/56) of EMA alcohol use prompts (M = 51.03; SD = 7.2; range = 18–56). In total, we collected 4760 EMA data points. We observed large individual differences in neural responses to peer faces, with grand mean centered raw units of neural responses in the reward system ranging from − 0.56 to 0.54 (median =−0.01, SD = 0.24) and mentalizing activity ranging from − 0.84 to 0.80 (median =−0.03; SD = 0.31). Correlations between key study variables can be found in Supplement B Table S10. See Fig. S4. for alcohol conversations and drinking occasion sample distributions.
Neural responses to peer faces, alcohol conversations and alcohol use
We found that talking about alcohol was associated with an increased likelihood of next-day drinking (OR = 1.59, 95% CI [1.30–1.94], p <.001). We next tested whether including a neural index of activity to the faces of a drinking vs. non-drinking peers, as a moderator, would provide additional information about who is more (vs. less) likely to drink following alcohol conversations (and would improve the model fit as measured via Akaike information criterion [AIC]54 and chi-square tests). Specifically, we tested whether including an interaction term between alcohol conversation and brain activity to drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers would improve model fit relative to (i) a main effects model (alcohol conversation and brain activity to peers as separate predictors) and (ii) a null model (no information about alcohol conversations and brain activity). We repeated this comparison twice to consider brain activity in the reward and the mentalizing regions separately, as presented below.
Reward and mentalizing ROIs
The two-way interaction model (reward activity*alcohol conversation) significantly improved model fit (AIC = 3697.4), compared to a main effects model for alcohol conversation and activity in reward-related regions (AIC = 3699.6) and to a null model (AIC = 3746.9). Chi-squared tests showed that the interaction model significantly improved fit over the main effects model (2(1) = 4.16, p =.041) as well as the null model (2(1) = 57.45, p = < 0.001). We observed similar patterns when considering mentalizing activity. The two-way interaction model (mentalizing activity*alcohol conversation) significantly improved model fit (AIC = 3695.7) compared to a main efforts model for alcohol conversation and mentalizing activity (AIC = 3699.5) and to a null model (AIC = 3746.9). The interaction model significantly improved fit over the main effects model (2(1) = 6.05, p =.014) and the null model (2(1) = 59.16, p = < 0.001). Together, considering individual differences in brain activity to drinking (vs. non-drinking) peer faces—in reward and mentalizing systems—explained additional variance in associations between alcohol conversation and next-day drinking.
Neural responses to peer faces moderate the association between conversations and drinking
We tested two hypotheses about the interaction between brain activity to peers and alcohol conversations on next-day drinking. We predicted that stronger brain activity in reward and mentalizing systems to drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers would moderate the temporal association between talking about alcohol and next-day drinking, with individuals with stronger brain activity showing a more positive association between alcohol conversation and next-day drinking.
Consistent with our first hypothesis, we observed a significant interaction between reward activity to drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers and alcohol conversations on next-day drinking (OR = 2.89, 95% CI [1.15–7.26], p =.024), such that stronger activity in the reward system was associated with higher likelihood of next-day drinking following alcohol conversations. Follow-up analyses showed that talking about alcohol was associated with higher likelihood of next-day drinking, and this association was stronger for individuals whose brains showed stronger (+ 1 SD) or near average reward-related activity to the faces of drinking peers (+ 1 SD reward activity: OR = 1.86, 95% CI [1.38–2.50], p <.001; near average reward activity: OR = 1.44, 95% CI [1.17–1.77], p =.006, Fig. 1). In other words, participants who discussed alcohol also showed a stronger likelihood of next-day drinking. Specifically, their likelihood of drinking the next day increased by about 4% (95% CI: 2–6%), from 11% (95% CI: 8–14%) to 15% (95% CI: 10–20%, Fig. 1). Additionally, stronger reward-related brain activity in response to drinking peers correlated with an increased likelihood of next-day drinking by approximately 7% (95% CI [4–8%]), from 10% (95% CI [7–14%]) to 17% (95% CI [11–22%], Fig. 1). By contrast, for participants whose brains showed stronger activity to non-drinking peers (−1 SD), talking about alcohol was not significantly associated with next-day drinking (−1 SD reward activity: OR = 1.12, 95% CI [0.82–1.52], p =.42; Fig. 1).
We observed similar patterns when considering brain activity in the mentalizing system. Consistent with our second hypothesis, we observed a significant interaction between mentalizing related-activity and alcohol conversations on next-day drinking, with stronger activity in the mentalizing system associated with increased likelihood of drinking (OR = 2.53, 95% CI [1.25–5.11], p =.010). Talking about alcohol was associated with increased drinking likelihood among individuals whose brains showed stronger (+ 1 SD) or near average mentalizing activity to the faces of drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers (+ 1 SD mentalizing activity: OR = 1.93, 95% CI [1.43–2.60], p <.001; near average mentalizing activity: OR = 1.45, 95% CI [1.18–1.78], p =.005, Fig. 1). Stronger mentalizing activity to drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers associated with an increased likelihood of drinking following alcohol conversations by approximately 7% (95% CI [4–10%]), from 9% (95% CI [7–12%]) to 16% (95% CI [11–22%], Fig. 1). By contrast, for people whose brains showed stronger activity to non-drinking (vs. drinking) peers, talking about alcohol was not significantly associated with next-day drinking (−1 SD mentalizing activity: OR = 1.09, 95% CI [0.80–1.47], p =.5912, Fig. 1). Together, these results suggest that individuals with stronger activity within the two hypothesized brain systems—reward and mentalizing—showed more positive associations between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking. See Tables 1 and 2 for full models with covariates.
[See PDF for image]
Fig. 1
Neural responses to drinking vs. non-drinking peers moderate associations between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking.
Among participants who showed stronger or near average activity in (A) reward and (B) mentalizing systems to drinking peers, talking about alcohol was associated with higher likelihood of next-day drinking (+ 1 SD; pink line; mean; yellow line). Among participants with stronger activity to non-drinking peers (−1SD; blue line), talking about alcohol was not significantly associated with next-day drinking.
Additional analyses
Sensitivity analyses and robustness checks are included in the Supplement B. These analyses include tests of whether neural responses to drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers moderate associations between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking when including outliers (Tables S1 & S2); removing covariates (Tables S3 and S4); controlling for self-reported peer ratings of peer closeness and peer liking (Tables S6 and S7); and applying zero-inflated negative binomial models, which separately model the count and likelihood of alcohol use occasions (Tables S8 and S9). We also explored the specificity of our results by replacing conversations about alcohol with conversations about non-alcohol topics, and neural responses tracking general perceived peer closeness (Tables S11 and S12) and conversational valence. Across all approaches, we observed parallel results such that stronger neural responses to drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers in the reward and mentalizing systems was associated with increased likelihood of drinking following alcohol conversations, and these relationships were specific to alcohol (vs. non-alcohol related) conversations.
Discussion
Conversations play a key part in shaping health behaviors, and individuals vary widely in how they process and respond to such influences. To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine how individual differences in neural responses to real peers relate to associations between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking behavior. We sampled students at two Northeastern US college campuses who were members of existing social groups (e.g. sports teams and fraternities). Using fMRI, we observed brain activity while participants viewed photographs of the faces of peers within their social groups. Critically, faces showed peers with whom participants engaged in social drinking at varying frequencies. Next, using EMA, we tracked alcohol conversations and drinking behavior twice daily for 28 days. Controlling for individual differences in baseline drinking, we found that participants with stronger reward and mentalizing brain activity to the faces of drinking peers—with whom they drank more frequently—showed a stronger, positive association between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking. Conversely, participants with stronger brain activity to non-drinking peers—with whom they drank less frequently—showed no significant association between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking.
Our results are consistent with a growing body of work highlighting the role of mentalizing and reward processes in the brain in tracking relational status of others17,50,51,55 and social influence processes11,12,30. Our results extend existing literature in several important ways.
Prior neuroimaging studies have largely studied how individuals respond to social influence in controlled experimental conditions, for example by manipulating participant beliefs about perceived peer preferences for appetitive stimuli (e.g., to food or art) or by presenting normative ratings ostensibly made by anonymous others25,30,56. However, real-world health behavior processes are often more complex and nuanced, and knowledge about how young adults respond to naturalistic peer cues remains limited in substance use contexts57. Our study revealed that the associations between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking varied based on individuals’ mental representations of peers within their social groups. While we conceptually replicated the positive correlation between discussing alcohol and subsequent drinking in a naturalistic, longitudinal setting9we found that the direction and strength of this association varied significantly based on neural responses to peers in brain systems involved in motivational, affective, and social cognitive processing.
Consistently, social learning theory suggests that the nature of peer drinking relationships can shape drinking through learning mechanisms. For example, when peers frequently engage in drinking together, these interactions can reinforce similar behaviors and provide models for future alcohol consumption through observational learning. This often happens as young adults seek peer approval and try to avoid rejection35. In line with this view, it is possible that participants who responded more strongly to peers with whom they drank more frequently—in brain regions associated with social rewards and mentalizing—may be more likely to activate schemas related to expectations from peers or anticipated social rewards for drinking, thereby motivating a more positive association between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking, and a stronger vulnerability to conform to future drinking behaviors. When drinking with their peers, individuals may experience more positive social rewards, such as increased approval, acceptance, and feelings of belonging. Additionally, as individuals drink more with peers, it may lead to more alcohol-related conversations, creating a mutually reinforcing cycle.
With respect to specificity, sensitivity analyses revealed that the main interaction effect was specific to conversations about alcohol, versus other topics. Additionally, the main interaction between brain activity in response to drinking (versus non-drinking) peers remained robust after accounting for self-reported measures of peer closeness and liking. This finding suggests that the association between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking may be modified by how individuals evoke mental representations about peers in relation to alcohol, rather than general representations of peer closeness or liking. However, future research is needed to isolate this distinction more specifically. In our data, drinking frequency and peer closeness were correlated (r =.64); as well as drinking frequency and peer liking (r =.33; Supplement B, Table S5). Participants in our sample did not always report drinking with their closest or most liked peers. Thus, our data are consistent with the idea that links between alcohol conversations and drinking behavior may not be driven by general friendship alone. However, when examining neural responses to close vs. distant peers, we also observed a non-significant interaction on alcohol conversations and next-day drinking trending in the same direction, suggesting some potential overlap between peer-drinking frequency and peer-closeness at the neural level. Future research may fruitfully isolate how distinct aspects of peer relationships contribute to both alcohol conversations and drinking behavior.
A key complementary finding was that individuals with stronger reward and mentalizing activity to the faces of non-drinking peers (vs. drinking peers) showed no significant associations between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking, regardless of their baseline drinking levels; potentially suggesting a protective mechanism. It is possible that stronger reward and mentalizing activity to non-drinking (vs. drinking) peers may be more likely to activate schemas unrelated to drinking during alcohol conversations, thereby helping steer participants away from alcohol consumption. This finding aligns with previous research among adolescents, which suggests that perceptions of high peer support can modulate neural sensitivity to risk-taking and buffer risk taking behaviors58. This finding further motivates how to harness positive peer cues to possibly discourage unhealthy drinking habits among college students.
While our analyses are correlational, our findings may help generate future hypotheses for experimental strategies aimed at reshaping alcohol-related conversations and potentially shifting social norms around drinking. Prior experimental work has shown that changing the tone of alcohol-related conversations can reduce binge drinking intentions and influence perceived social norms, on average59. Additionally, text-based reminders that highlight the perspectives of non-drinking peers have been found to causally reduce alcohol use frequency among college students, on average60. Our study extends these findings by suggesting that individual differences in neural responses to peers—particularly in mentalizing and reward-related brain regions—correlate with conversation-drinking associations, and may perhaps help identify who is more vs. less likely to benefit from potential strategies that aim to decouple conversational influences on drinking.
Building on these associations, future studies could examine whether individuals who show stronger neural responsiveness to non-drinking peers (vs. drinking peers) are more receptive to interventions that redirect alcohol-related conversations. Just-in-time adaptive interventions, which deliver support during moments of heightened vulnerability—specific time windows when students are most susceptible to alcohol-related peer influence—and deliver targeted prompts (e.g., cues from non-drinking peers) during those periods represent a promising framework for testing these possibilities61,62. Thus, our findings generate specific hypotheses for future experimental work aimed at testing whether individual differences in neural responses to peers and temporal patterns in alcohol-related conversations can predict differential responsiveness to tailored interventions that seek to decouple peer influence from drinking behavior.
The current results should be interpreted considering the strengths and limitations of the study. Here, we combined fMRI and information about peers from existing social groups within an EMA design, embedded in individuals’ daily lives in a correlational, observational context. This multimodal approach allowed us to capture time-sensitive links between alcohol conversations and drinking behavior in everyday environments, thereby avoiding common biases that may arise when participants are asked to recall information about longer periods of time (e.g., alcohol consumed in the previous 30 days63). Also notable is the fact we employed the same study protocol across two college campuses, thereby enhancing the robustness of our findings. Although intensive assessment can raise data compliance concerns, we found minimal non-compliance among our sample. The intensive sampling approach produced high response rates, with ~ 95% median response rate over 28 days. This research also directly aligns with recent calls to connect laboratory findings on neural responses to real-world social contexts57 and to consider individual differences64 to better understand health behaviors like drinking.
With respect to limitations, our data cannot be used to generalize about samples beyond specific types of college students who are social drinkers (i.e., without alcohol dependence) who are part of social groups. Importantly, our sample includes students from two Ivy League institutions in the Northeastern United States, with a large portion of students coming from highly educated families, with high socioeconomic status. This limited demographic scope may restrict the findings’ generalizability to students from other educational backgrounds, regions, or socioeconomic statuses. The unique experiences and perspectives of students attending more diverse institutions and with more diverse backgrounds may not align with those of our sample, and consequently, the results may not accurately reflect the behaviors and attitudes of the broader college student population. We recruited students from pre-existing social groups on two campuses (and included groups in which 80% or more expressed interest). Although we controlled for group membership the non-independence may have confounded effects in unmeasurable ways. Additionally, 32% of participants were members of sports teams; however, we did not gather information on whether these teams were in season during the study. This temporal context could have influenced the students’ drinking patterns during the study.
A key limitation of this study is that we did not measure with whom participants talked about alcohol, who initiated these conversations, their involvement level, or conversation content, all of which could have an unmeasured influence on the association between alchol conversations and next-day drinking. Although we did measure conversational valence, i.e., how positive or negative each alcohol conversation was, our study did not indicate that valence interacted with brain activity and drinking behavior. In our sample, most alcohol conversations were positive, and the drinking norms on the studied college campuses favored drinking as normative. While the study was framed broadly around wellness, participants’ possible awareness of the focus on alcohol may have also possibly influenced their alcohol conversations, thereby reducing ecological validity. Future studies may more precisely capture the nature of alcohol conversations, potentially through the use of natural language processing and passive mobile sensing.
It is important to note that the main interaction finding—specifically the relationship between neural responses to peers, alcohol conversations, and next-day drinking—should not be interpreted as causal. Our data show that neural responses to peers are correlated with different levels of drinking after alcohol conversations. This interaction represents a correlational association rather than a direct cause-and-effect relationship, and does not imply that conversations about alcohol or differences in brain activity directly cause increased drinking. It is plausible that alcohol conversations and drinking may be mutually reinforcing. For example, positively discussing alcohol could lead to increased drinking, while drinking may stimulate more positive conversations about alcohol, thereby creating a feedback loop. However, in our data, we observed a unidirectional pattern: neural responses to peers specifically moderated how conversations about alcohol relate to next-day drinking, but the reverse was not supported. That is, neural responses to peers did not significantly moderate how drinking behavior correlates with next-day conversations about alcohol. This asymmetry is consistent across both hypothesized brain systems—the reward and mentalizing systems. Specifically, individuals with varying neural responses to peers did not show different associations between alcohol use and next-day conversations about alcohol in either the reward system [OR = 1.49, 95% CI [0.36–6.11], p =.583] or the mentalizing system [OR = 1.34, 95% CI [0.50–3.64], p =.562]. These findings further underscore the correlational nature of the observed effects. To isolate causal effects between alcohol conversations and subsequent drinking behaviors, future experimental studies are needed that manipulate and monitor the content of these discussions. Future studies may also employ hyperscanning paradigms during peer conversations to validate the brain mechanisms engaged in near-real time.
Notably, in our data, individual differences in neural responses to the faces of drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers moderated the incidence of drinking episodes but not the specific amount consumed when drinking, as shown in Supplement B Tables S8 and S9. One possibility is that neural responses to drinking vs. non-drinking peers may be more strongly correlated with the likelihood of initiating drinking episodes than with the amount consumed during those episodes—which may be subject to additional factors not considered here. Another possibility is in our current sample of social drinkers, we had insufficient variability in the number of drinks per occasion to detect differences. As such, future work could fruitfully explore how individual differences in brain activity to different peers relates to health decision making across various behavioral contexts.
In sum, the present study, is the first to our knowledge, to combine fMRI, with information about real social groups, and EMA to examine how individual differences in neural responses to peers relate to associations between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking. Individuals whose brains showed stronger responses to faces of drinking vs. non-drinking peers—in systems related reward and mentalizing—showed a more positive association between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking. This work highlights that the ways individuals’ brains gauge motivational relevance of social connections may be associated with drinking following alcohol conversations, or may provide a protective buffer, depending on mental representations of different peers. Specifically, the present data suggest that neural responses to the faces of peers with varying drinking interactions may serve as indicators of broader susceptibility to alcohol conversations and drinking among this sample of college students. Future work may validate this neural index across more representative samples and integrate it in interventions that embed positive peer influence to promote healthy behaviors.
Methods
Data
We use data from the Social Health Impact of Network Effects Study (SHINE), a multimodal, multisite project designed to provide insight into mind, body, and community relationships among social groups of young adults (see Refs65, 66, 67, 68, 69, 70–71). All research, methods, and study protocols were approved by the Human Subjects Electronic Research Application (HSERA) Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the University of Pennsylvania and were acknowledged by the Human Research Protection Office of the Department of Defense. All research, methods, and study protocols were conducted in accordance with the IRB at the University of Pennsylvania and the Human Research Protection Office of the Department of Defense.
Participant recruitment and sample characteristics.
Students in two urban universities who belonged to on-campus groups (e.g., sports teams, arts groups, Greek life, etc.) were invited to participate. We recruited groups where more than 80% of the group expressed interest to participate, with the goal of facilitating data collection from many social partners in the group. At baseline, each participant was invited to upload a photo of themselves to build a pool of stimuli for the face viewing fMRI task. We collected 588 photos of peer faces across 24 social groups.
The current study includes 104 participants (Mage=20.56 years, SDage=1.72), from the SHINE study, who completed a baseline online survey, an fMRI visit and a post-scan survey, and a 28-day EMA assessment. The sample comprised students from 10 social groups (mean group size = 42.36 students; SD = 21.38, median = 32). Participants reported the following gender and racial/ethnic identities: 63 women, 40 men, 1 other; 57 white, 32 Asian, 2 Black, 5 Latino/a, 8 Other/or more than one identity. Among the participants, 32% participated as part of a sports team, 48% as part of a performing arts group, 17% as part of Greek life, and 3% as another type of group. From these students, 24% identified as freshmen, 17% as sophomores, 28% as juniors, 29% as seniors, and 0.2% as other. Only 8% reported being international students. Regarding family income, 34% of participants reported a family income of $200,000 or more; 28% reported a family income between $100,000 and $199,999; 14% reported $75,000 to $99,999; 11% reported $50,000 to $74,999; 6% reported $20,000 to $34,999; 5% reported $35,000 to $49,999; and 3% indicated an income below $19,999. In terms of maternal education, 33% of mothers held a Bachelor’s degree; 35% a Master’s degree; 15% a Ph.D. or equivalent (M.D., J.D., etc.); 6% associate or professional degree; 4% attended some college; 7% completed high school or GED; and 1% had some high school education. For paternal education, 29% of fathers held a Master’s degree; 28% a Ph.D. or equivalent; 21% a Bachelor’s degree; 10% completed high school or GED; 6% an associate degree; 4% attended some college; and 2% had some high school education.
Among the participants, 32% participated as part of a sports team, 48% as part of a performing arts group, 17% as part of Greek life, and 3% as another type of group. From these students, 24% identified as freshmen, 17% as sophomores, 28% as juniors, 29% as seniors, and 0.2% as other. Only 8% reported being international students. Regarding family income, 34% of participants reported a family income of $200,000 or more; 28% reported a family income between $100,000 and $199,999; 14% reported $75,000 to $99,999; 11% reported $50,000 to $74,999; 6% reported $20,000 to $34,999; 5% reported $35,000 to $49,999; and 3% indicated an income below $19,999. In terms of maternal education, 33% of mothers held a Bachelor’s degree; 35% a Master’s degree; 15% a Ph.D. or equivalent (M.D., J.D., etc.); 6% associate or professional degree, 4% attended some college, 7% completed high school or GED, and 1% had some high school education. For paternal education, 29% of fathers held a Master’s degree, 28% a Ph.D. or equivalent, 21% a Bachelor’s degree, 10% completed high school or GED, 6% had an associate degree, 4% attended some college, and 2% had some high school education. See ‘Supplement A Recruitment’ for more details and Fig. S1 for participant exclusion and see Ref65. for further details on study protocol and procedures. All participants provided written informed consent and were paid for their participation.
Measures and tasks
fMRI face-viewing task
To measure neural responses to peer faces, we used a task adapted from Refs17,50,51. In the scanner, participants viewed photographs of peers from their on-campus social group. The task stimuli were prepared from peer photographs collected during the baseline survey. Group members were asked to face the camera, with a neutral expression, and to have no objects in the background. Photographs were inspected manually by researchers for quality and cropped and converted to grayscale with equal luminance, for standardization. See Ref65. for details on task development and stimuli selection.
The face-viewing task implemented a rapid event-related design across two runs. During each run, participants viewed three trial types: faces of peers who were part of their on-campus social group (M = 25 unique peer faces; SD = 3; range = 18–27), their own face, and a red dot in the center of the screen (control images), appearing one at a time. Each face appeared on the screen 6 times (3 times per run) in a randomized order. All trials were presented for one second, followed by a jittered fixation cross (M = 5.5s, SD = 2.8). To ensure engagement during the task, participants were instructed to press a button each time they saw a red dot on the screen (∼10% of total presentations), using a five-button box. See ‘Supplement A Fig. S2’ for task visualization. The task was presented using PsychoPy (Version v3.0.0b1172. Following the scan, participants reported on how frequently they drank with each of their peers featured in the face-viewing task using a 9-point scale (1–9) in addition to other measures that probed dimensions of individual variation that were beyond the scope of the current report65.
Ecological assessment of conversations and drinking
Throughout the 28-day EMA period, participants received two survey prompts per day via the LifeData mobile app (www.lifedatacorp.com): in the morning (8am) and in the evening (6pm). Surveys assessed alcohol-related behaviors in addition to other measures such as craving and mood not reported here65.
Alcohol conversations and drinking occasions in situ
To measure alcohol conversations, participants were asked: “Since the (EVENING/MORNING) survey, have you talked to someone about alcohol?” Participants answered using a “No/Yes” response option. To measure alcohol use, participants were asked: “Since the EVENING/MORNING survey, have you consumed any alcohol?” Participants answered using a “No” or “Yes” response option. Both items were measured twice a day, during the morning and evening EMA. For follow-up alcohol use measures, see ‘Supplement A Follow-up measures’ and for analyses with these measures see Supplement B Tables S8 and S9 and Supplement B (pp. 22–23).
Baseline measures
During the initial online survey, participants reported on demographics including age, gender, and race/ethnicity, in addition to typical drinking frequency and drinking amount in the past 6 months prior to the study. The race/ethnicity variable indicated Asian, Black, Latino/a, white, and Other status. For wording and measurement of the baseline drinking measures see ‘Supplement A Alcohol use baseline measures’.
fMRI data acquisition, modeling, and ROI analysis
fMRI data collection
Neuroimaging data were acquired on 3 Tesla Siemens Prisma scanners equipped with a 64-channel head coil. High-resolution T1-weighted structural images were collected using an MPRAGE sequence (TI = 1,100ms, voxel size = 0.9 × 0.9 × 1 mm, 160 slices, field of view [FOV] = 256, repetition time [TR] = 1850ms, echo time [TE] = 3.91ms, flip angle = 8°). T2*-weighted functional images were also collected (voxel size = 3 × 3 × 3 mm, 42 slices, FOV = 70, TR = 1,000ms, TE = 30, flip angle = 62°). Additional, fMRI data pre-processing, modeling, and ROI analysis are reported in detail in Supplement A.
fMRI task modeling
Since we aimed to compute a neural index of responses to peer faces with varying drinking interactions, we extracted activity to peer faces as modulated by drinking frequency ratings, or how often each participant reported drinking with each individual peer. As such, we modeled the task using a parametric modulation design as implemented in SPM 1273. We constructed first level models for each participant that regressed periods of exposure to peer faces on mean centered, trial-by-trial differences in how frequently they reported drinking with each peer (range: 1–9). Models also included nuisance regressors that were modeled separately. These included periods of exposure to an individual’s own face, red dot, and five motion regressors described above. Thus, to compute a brain index that captures differences in responses to peer faces with varying drinking interactions, we extracted activity to peer faces as modulated by drinking frequency ratings, or how often each participant reported drinking with each of the peers whose face they viewed in the scanner (~ 25 peers).
ROI analysis
To define targeted regions of interest, we extracted two functionally defined maps from Neurosynth74. For reward-related activity, we used the search term ‘reward’ (922 studies; 30418 activations, p <.01, corrected). For mentalizing-related activity, we used the search term ‘mentalizing’ (151 studies; 6824 activations, p <.01, corrected). Next, to create a neural index of activity to peers with whom one drinks more vs. less frequently, we extracted mean parameter estimates from the parametric modulated viewing of peer faces within the (a) reward ROI and the (b) mentalizing (ROI). Greater values on these neural indices correspond to stronger activity to faces of peers with whom one engages in more (vs. less) frequent drinking activities (i.e., drinking vs. non-drinking peers). This procedure resulted in two values per participant that were used as individual difference measures of ‘reward’ and ‘mentalizing’ activity to faces of drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers, as a function of peer drinking interactions.
Data preparation
We took several steps to prepare the data prior to modeling. We aimed to model how alcohol conversations relate to prospective alcohol use, and we slid forward the alcohol use variable by one day (by two observations), as the questions were phrased to measure alcohol use since the previous survey, twice a day. That is, we considered whether today’s alcohol conversation correlated with tomorrow’s alcohol use9. Further, to attenuate the influence of outliers in the brain data, we winsorized activity in reward regions and mentalizing regions +/- 2 standard deviations from the mean, following outlier inspection. This cutoff applied to seven participants’ activity in reward-related regions and four participants’ activity in mentalizing regions. As a sensitivity test, we repeated all analyses including the outliers in the brain data and observed parallel results. See ‘Supplement B Tables S1 and S2’ for analyses including outliers.
Analysis plan
To examine whether individual differences in neural activity in reward and mentalizing regions to drinking vs. non-drinking peers moderates the association between talking about alcohol and next-day drinking, we estimated mixed-effects models separately for each brain system. The main predictor of interest was an interaction term of (a) engaging in a conversation about alcohol (Yes/No) and (b) a between-subject neural index (for either reward or mentalizing systems) that captured activity to drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers. The alcohol conversation variable was split into a within and between components following standards in the field (See Ref75. for example). Specifically, we created a person-level conversation variable by computing (a) the overall proportion of alcohol conversations that occurred across the EMA protocol and (b) a within-person daily variable indicating whether (1) or not (0) an alcohol conversation occurred at a given prompt. Our primary outcome was the likelihood of alcohol use, indicating whether (1) or not (0) alcohol was consumed at a given prompt68,76.
We controlled for the following covariates: baseline drinking frequency and amount in the past six months (to account for possible individual differences in overall drinking), age, gender, race/ethnicity, group membership, response rates, social weekend, time in study—as on average, participants reported drinking less over the study period, and condition effects as part of a larger intervention study62. Social weekend is defined as Thursday-Sunday (relative to rest of the weekdays) given higher drinking levels among college students during these days77. Two intervention variables were specified to include (1) between-person randomized condition assignment (mindfulness, perspective-taking, and control) and (2) within-person condition assignment (active vs. inactive week). Group membership is defined as belonging to one of ten on-campus social groups (i.e., sports clubs or fraternities). Results are robust to both the inclusion and exclusion of covariates (Supplement B tables S3 and S4). All numeric variables were grand-mean centered, and intercepts were allowed to vary randomly across people. Models did not converge when a random effect for alcohol conversations was included and, as such, a simpler model without this random effect estimate is presented.
To quantify the likelihood of a binary future drinking episode, we conducted multilevel binary logistic regression using the ‘glmer’ function from the lme4 package78. We specified the “bobyqua” algorithm to optimize model convergence, with “optCtrl = list(maxfun = 100000)”. As a sensitivity test, we repeated the same analyses using more complex multi-level hurdle models using glmmTMB79 and speficied a truncated negative binomial distribution. The zero inflated multi-level hurdle models are commonly used to model alcohol use with an excess of zeros and a skewed distribution of non-zero counts and to separately model two aspects of alcohol use: (1) whether alcohol was used at all (a binary outcome: yes or no) and (2) the number of alcohol use occasions among those who did consume alcohol (a count outcome)18. We observed parallel results using both analytical approaches. See Supplement B Tables S8 and S9 for results from multi-level hurdle models. All analyses were conducted in RStudio version 3.6.280.
Table 1. Reward-related responses to drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers moderate associations between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking.
Alcohol conversation*reward activity and next-day drinking | |||
---|---|---|---|
Fixed Effects | OR | 95%CI | P |
Intercept Alcohol conversation Reward activity Baseline drinking amount Baseline drinking frequency Gender Age Race Social group Social weekend (vs. weekdays) Proportion of alcohol conversations Alcohol responses Time in study Condition mindful (vs. control) Condition perspective (vs. control) Active week (vs. inactive) Alcohol conversation*reward activity | 0.08 1.44 0.81 1.11 1.29 1.14 1.19 1.15 1.02 1.23 10.05 0.98 0.99 1.02 0.98 1.17 2.69 | 0.04, 0.17 1.17, 1.77 0.43, 1.54 0.04, 1.46 1.16–1.44 0.85, 1.52 1.10–1.29 1.02, 1.29 0.96, 1.08 1.04, 1.46 3.95, 25.58 0.96, 1.00 0.98, 0.99 0.72, 1.44 0.68, 1.40 0.94, 1.45 1.15, 7.26 | < 0.001*** 0.001** 0.523 0.018 < 0.001*** 0.387 0.021* 0.557 0.557 0.018* < 0.001*** 0.102 < 0.001*** 0.920 0.896 0.166 0.024* |
ICC | SD | ||
Intercept Participant ID | 0.07 | 0.50 |
Note. 4760 observations. *p ≤.05, **p ≤.01, ***p ≤.001.
Table 2. Mentalizing-related responses to drinking (vs. non-drinking) peers moderate associations between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking.
Alcohol conversation*mentalizing activity and next-day drinking | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Fixed Effects | OR | 95%CI | P | |
Intercept Alcohol conversation Mentalizing activity Baseline drinking amount Baseline drinking frequency Gender Age Race Social group Social weekend (vs. weekdays) Proportion of alcohol conversations Alcohol responses Time in study Condition mindful (vs. control) Condition perspective (vs. control) Active week (vs. inactive) Alcohol conversation*mentalizing activity | 0.08 1.45 0.84 1.11 1.29 1.13 1.19 1.15 1.02 1.23 10.46 0.98 0.99 1.01 0.97 1.17 2.53 | 0.04, 0.18 1.18, 1.78 0.52, 1.37 1.04–1.19 1.16–1.44 0.85, 1.51 1.10, 1.29 1.02, 1.29 0.96, 1.08 1.04, 1.45 4.09, 26.79 0.96, 1.00 0.98, 0.99 0.71, 1.43 0.67, 1.40 0.94, 1.46 1.25, 5.11 | < 0.001*** < 0.001*** 0.457 0.003 < 0.001*** 0.402 < 0.001*** 0.020 0.580 0.019* < 0.001*** 0.102 < 0.001*** 0.947 0.874 0.154 0.010* | |
ICC | SD | |||
Intercept Participant ID | 0.07 | 0.51 |
Note. 4760 observations. *p ≤.05, **p ≤.01, ***p ≤.001.
Author contributions
Conceptualization: M.J, O.S, E. B. F, K.N.O, B.D., Formal Analysis: M.J, O.S, D.C., Funding Acquisition: E.B.F, K.N.O, D.S.B, P.M., Supervision: D.C, N.C, D.L.S, K.N.O, Y.K, E.B.F.; Investigation: O.S, Y.K; Z.B., P.J.M., Methodology: M.J, B.D, C.S, O.S, K.N.O., Writing – original draft: M.J., Writing – editing and revising: M.J, O.S., C.S, B.D, E. B. F, P.J.M, D.M.L, K.N.O, F.C, Y.K, N.C, Z.B, D.C.
Funding statement
Open access funding provided by Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich. Research was sponsored by the Army Research Office and was accomplished under Grant Number W911NF-18-1-0244. M.J. is affiliated with the Centre for Digital Health Interventions (CDHI), a joint initiative of the Department of Management, Technology, and Economics at ETH Zurich, the Institute for Implementation Science in Health Care at the University of Zurich, and the Institute of Technology Management and School of Medicine at the University of St. Gallen; and acknowledges funding support by CSS, a Swiss health insurer. D.M.L. acknowledges support from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (K01 DA047417) and the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation. D.S.B. acknowledges support from the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, the Swartz Foundation, the Paul G. Allen Family Foundation, the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation and the NSF (IIS-1926757). D.C. and E.B.F. acknowledge support from Hopelab and the Lentic fund. K.N.O. acknowledges support from the National Institute on Alcoholism and Alcohol Abuse (R01 AA023653). M.J. acknowledges support from CSS. The views and conclusions contained in this document are those of the authors and should not be interpreted as representing the official policies, either expressed or implied, of the Army Research Office or the U.S. Government. The U.S. Government is authorized to reproduce and distribute reprints for Government purposes notwithstanding any copyright notation herein.
Data availability
De-identified data and code to reproduce the main analyses are available on: https://github.com/miajov/brain-responses---peer-influence.
Declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Citation statement
We sought to proactively consider choosing references that reflect the diversity of the field in thought, form of contribution, gender, race, ethnicity, and other factors. First, we obtained the predicted gender of the first and last author of each reference (excluding software package citations) by using databases that store the probability of a first name being carried by a woman (https://github.com/dalejn/cleanBib). By this measure (and excluding self-citations to the first and last authors of our current paper), our non-software references contain 28% woman(first)/woman(last), 27% man/woman, 14% woman/man, and 31% man/man. This method is limited in that names used to construct the databases may not, in every case, be indicative of gender identity and cannot account for intersex, non-binary, or transgender people.
Positionality statement
Mindful that our identities can influence our approach to science, the authors wish to provide the reader with information about our backgrounds. With respect to gender, when the manuscript was drafted, eight authors self-identified as women, one as non-binary, and four as men. With respect to race, 12 authors self-identified as white and one as Asian. With respect to engagement with college students, when this study was conducted, two authors were doctoral students who teach and/or mentor other students, three authors were postdoctoral researchers or research scientists who teach and/or mentor students, and eight authors were professors who teach and/or mentor students.
Ethics declaration
All research and experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the Human Subjects Electronic Research Application (HSERA) Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the University of Pennsylvania and acknowledged by the Human Research Protection Office of the Department of Defense. All participants provided informed consent before taking part in the study and all participants were financially compensated.
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
1. Cialdini, RB; Goldstein, NJ. Social influence: compliance and conformity. Ann. Rev. Psychol.; 2004; 55, pp. 591-621.
2. Higgs, S; Thomas, J. Social influences on eating. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci.; 2016; 9, pp. 1-6.
3. Wolske, KS; Gillingham, KT; Schultz, P. W. Peer influence on household energy behaviours. Nat. Energy; 2020; 5, pp. 202-212.2020NatEn..5.202W
4. Cline, R. J. W. Everyday interpersonal communication and health. In The Routledge Handbook of Health Communication (2nd edn) (eds Thompson, T. L., Parrott, R. & Nussbaum, J. F.) 21–42 (Routledge, 2011).
5. Jeong, M; Bae, RE. The effect of Campaign-Generated interpersonal communication on Campaign-Targeted health outcomes: A Meta-Analysis. Health Commun.; 2018; 33, pp. 988-1003. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28622003]
6. Borsari, B; Carey, KB. How the quality of peer relationships influences college alcohol use. Drug Alcohol Rev.; 2006; 25, pp. 361-370. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16854663][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2635065]
7. Hendriks, H; van den Putte, B; de Bruijn, GJ; de Vreese, CH. Predicting health: the interplay between interpersonal communication and health campaigns. J. Health Commun.; 2014; 19, pp. 625-636. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24446759]
8. Real, K; Rimal, RN. Friends talk to friends about drinking: exploring the role of peer communication in the theory of normative social behavior. Health Commun.; 2007; 22, pp. 169-180. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17668996]
9. Scholz, C; Doré, BP; Cooper, N; Falk, EB. Neural valuation of antidrinking campaigns and risky peer influence in daily life. Health Psychol.; 2019; 38, pp. 658-667. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31008644][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6653645]
10. Centola, D. An experimental study of homophily in the adoption of health behavior. Science; 2011; 334, pp. 1269-1272.2011Sci..334.1269C1:CAS:528:DC%2BC3MXhsFCms7jM [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22144624]
11. Dillard, JP; Li, SS; Cannava, K. Talking about Sugar-Sweetened beverages: causes, processes, and consequences of Campaign-Induced interpersonal communication. Health Commun.; 2022; 37, pp. 316-326. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33124466]
12. Zhang, J et al. Support or competition? How online social networks increase physical activity: A randomized controlled trial. Prev. Med. Rep.; 2016; 4, pp. 453-458. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27617191][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5008041]
13. Jeong, M; Tan, ASL; Brennan, E; Gibson, L; Hornik, RC. Talking about quitting: interpersonal communication as a mediator of campaign effects on smokers’ quit behaviors. J. Health Commun.; 2015; 20, pp. 1196-1205. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26147367][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4703310]
14. Ackerson, LK; Viswanath, K. The social context of interpersonal communication and health. J. Health Commun.; 2009; 14,
15. Falk, E; Scholz, C; Persuasion,. Influence, and value: perspectives from communication and social neuroscience. Annu. Rev. Psychol.; 2018; 69, pp. 329-356. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28961060]
16. McGowan, AL et al. Dense sampling approaches for psychiatry research: combining scanners and smartphones. Biol. Psychiatry; 2023; 93, pp. 681-689. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36797176]
17. Zerubavel, N., Bearman, P. S., Weber, J. & Ochsner, K. N. Neural mechanisms tracking popularity in real-world social networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 112, 15072–15077 (2015).
18. Jovanova, M et al. Brain system integration and message consistent health behavior change. Health Psychol.; 2022; 41, pp. 611-620. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36006700][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10152515]
19. Shiffman, S. Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in studies of substance use. Psychol. Assess.; 2009; 21, pp. 486-497. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19947783][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2846437]
20. Jernigan, DH; Trangenstein, PJ. What’s next for who’s global strategy to reduce the harmful use of alcohol?. Bull. World Health Organ.; 2020; 98, pp. 222-223. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32132758][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7047030]
21. Mallett, KA et al. An update of research examining college student alcohol-related consequences: new perspectives and implications for interventions. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res.; 2013; 37, pp. 709-716. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23241024]
22. Campbell-Meiklejohn, D. K., Bach, D. R., Roepstorff, A., Dolan, R. J. & Frith, C. D. How the Opinion of Others Affects Our Valuation of Objects. Current Biology vol. 20 1165–1170 Preprint at (2010). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.04.055
23. Zaki, J; Schirmer, J; Mitchell, JP. Social influence modulates the neural computation of value. Psychol. Sci.; 2011; 22, pp. 894-900. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21653908]
24. Cascio, CN; O’Donnell, MB; Bayer, J; Tinney, FJ; Falk, EB. Neural correlates of susceptibility to group opinions in online Word-of-Mouth recommendations. J. Mark. Res.; 2015; 52, pp. 559-575.
25. Welborn, BL et al. Neural mechanisms of social influence in adolescence. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci.; 2016; 11, pp. 100-109. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26203050]
26. Zaki, J; Ochsner, KN. The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise. Nat. Neurosci.; 2012; 15, pp. 675-680.1:CAS:528:DC%2BC38XlsFWhs7k%3D [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22504346]
27. Ruff, CC; Fehr, E. The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.; 2014; 15, pp. 549-562.1:CAS:528:DC%2BC2cXhtVyhs7rN [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24986556]
28. Telzer, EH; Rogers, CR; Van Hoorn, J. Neural correlates of social influence on risk taking and substance use in adolescents. Curr. Addict. Rep.; 2017; 4, pp. 333-341. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29242768][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5724782]
29. Pei, R et al. Neural processes during adolescent risky decision making are associated with conformity to peer influence. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci.; 2020; 44, 100794. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32716849][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7281781]
30. Nook, EC; Zaki, J. Social norms shift behavioral and neural responses to foods. J. Cogn. Neurosci.; 2015; 27, pp. 1412-1426. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25671502]
31. Baek, EC et al. Activity in the brain’s valuation and mentalizing networks is associated with propagation of online recommendations. Sci. Rep.; 2021; 11, 11196.2021NatSR.1111196B1:CAS:528:DC%2BB3MXhsFOhtL%2FK [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34045543][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8160140]
32. Lieberman, MD. Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annu. Rev. Psychol.; 2007; 58, pp. 259-289. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17002553]
33. Duerler, P; Vollenweider, FX; Preller, K. H. A Neurobiological perspective on social influence: serotonin and social adaptation. J. Neurochem; 2022; 162, pp. 60-79.1:CAS:528:DC%2BB38Xosl2hsbs%3D [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35274296][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9322456]
34. Cascio, CN; Scholz, C; Falk, EB. Social influence and the brain: persuasion, susceptibility to influence and retransmission. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci.; 2015; 3, pp. 51-57.
35. Borsari, B; Carey, KB. Peer influences on college drinking: a review of the research. J. Subst. Abuse; 2001; 13, pp. 391-424.1:STN:280:DC%2BD38%2FkvVeluw%3D%3D [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11775073]
36. Durkin, KF; Wolfe, TW; Clark, GA. College students and binge drinking: an evaluation of social learning theory. Sociol. Spectr.; 2005; 25, pp. 255-272.
37. O’Donnell, R et al. Ecological momentary assessment of drinking in young adults: an investigation into social context, affect and motives. Addict. Behav.; 2019; 98, 106019. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31247534]
38. Frith, CD; Frith, U. The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron; 2006; 50, pp. 531-534.1:CAS:528:DC%2BD28XltF2itbc%3D [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16701204]
39. Saxe, R; Powell, LJ. It’s the thought that counts: specific brain regions for one component of theory of Mind. Psychol. Sci.; 2006; 17, pp. 692-699. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16913952]
40. Falk, EB et al. Neural responses to exclusion predict susceptibility to social influence. J. Adolesc. Health; 2014; 54, pp. S22-31. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24759437][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4144831]
41. Zhang, L; Gläscher, J. A brain network supporting social influences in human decision-making. Sci. Adv.; 2020; 6, eabb4159.2020SciA..6.4159Z [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32875112][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7438106]
42. Van Hoorn, J; Van Dijk, E; Güroğlu, B; Crone, EA. Neural correlates of prosocial peer influence on public goods game donations during adolescence. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci.; 2016; 11, pp. 923-933. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26865424][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4884312]
43. Centifanti, LCM; Modecki, KL; MacLellan, S; Gowling, H. Driving under the influence of risky peers: an experimental study of adolescent risk taking. J. Res. Adolesc.; 2016; 26, pp. 207-222.
44. Smith, ER; Mackie, DM. Representation and incorporation of close others’ responses: the RICOR model of social influence. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev.; 2016; 20, pp. 311-331. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26238964]
45. Teunissen, HA et al. Friends’ drinking norms and male adolescents’ alcohol consumption: the moderating role of performance-based peer influence susceptibility. J. Adolesc.; 2016; 53, pp. 45-54. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27622919]
46. Cullum, J; O’Grady, M; Armeli, S; Tennen, H. Change and stability in active and passive social influence dynamics during natural drinking events: A longitudinal Measurement-Burst study. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol.; 2012; 31, pp. 51-80. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22661826][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3361969]
47. Telzer, EH; Jorgensen, NA; Prinstein, MJ; Lindquist, KA. Neurobiological sensitivity to social rewards and punishments moderates link between peer norms and adolescent risk taking. Child. Dev.; 2021; 92, pp. 731-745. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33030267]
48. Berkowitz An overview of the social norms approach. Changing the culture of college drinking: A (2005).
49. Fiske, S. T. & Neil Macrae, C. The SAGE Handbook of Social Cognition (SAGE, 2012).
50. Zerubavel, N; Hoffman, MA; Reich, A; Ochsner, KN; Bearman, P. Neural precursors of future liking and affective reciprocity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A; 2018; 115, pp. 4375-4380.2018PNAS.115.4375Z1:CAS:528:DC%2BC1cXitlWrtb3N [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29632195][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5924932]
51. Morelli, SA; Leong, YC; Carlson, RW; Kullar, M; Zaki, J. Neural detection of socially valued community members. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A; 2018; 115, pp. 8149-8154.2018PNAS.115.8149M1:CAS:528:DC%2BC1cXitVanurfM [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30038007][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6094096]
52. Rinker, DV; Krieger, H; Neighbors, C. Social network factors and addictive behaviors among college students. Curr. Addict. Rep.; 2016; 3, pp. 356-367. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28580226][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5450660]
53. Jennison, KM. The short-term effects and unintended long-term consequences of binge drinking in college: a 10-year follow-up study. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse; 2004; 30, pp. 659-684. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15540499]
54. Vrieze, SI. Model selection and psychological theory: a discussion of the differences between the Akaike information criterion (AIC) and the bayesian information criterion (BIC). Psychol. Methods; 2012; 17, pp. 228-243. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22309957][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3366160]
55. Parkinson, C; Kleinbaum, AM; Wheatley, T. Similar neural responses predict friendship. Nat. Commun.; 2018; 9, 332.2018NatCo..9.332P [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29382820][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5790806]
56. Martin, RE; Villanueva, Y; Stephano, T; Franz, PJ; Ochsner, KN. Social influence shifts valuation of appetitive cues in early adolescence and adulthood. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.; 2018; 147, pp. 1521-1530. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30272465][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6544394]
57. Beard, SJ; Yoon, L; Venticinque, JS; Shepherd, NE; Guyer, AE. The brain in social context: A systematic review of substance use and social processing from adolescence to young adulthood. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci.; 2022; 57, 101147. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36030675][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9434028]
58. Telzer, EH; Fuligni, AJ; Lieberman, MD; Miernicki, ME; Galván, A. The quality of adolescents’ peer relationships modulates neural sensitivity to risk taking. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci.; 2015; 10, pp. 389-398. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24795443]
59. Hendriks, H; Scholz, C; Larsen, H; De Bruijn, GJ. Van Den putte, B. Intervening through conversations: how instructions influence conversational Valence and binge drinking determinants. Health Commun.; 2021; [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33789549][DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2020.1712524]
60. Jovanova, M et al. Psychological distance intervention reminders reduce alcohol consumption frequency in daily life. Sci. Rep.; 2023; 13, 12045.2023NatSR.1312045J1:CAS:528:DC%2BB3sXhsFCkt7fE [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37491371][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10368637]
61. Bae, S; Chung, T; Ferreira, D; Dey, AK; Suffoletto, B. Mobile phone sensors and supervised machine learning to identify alcohol use events in young adults: implications for just-in-time adaptive interventions. Addict. Behav.; 2018; 83, pp. 42-47. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29217132]
62. Brügger, V., Kowatsch, T. & Jovanova, M. Personalizing dietary interventions by predicting individual vulnerability to glucose excursions. MedRxiv2024.08.07.24311591https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.08.07.24311591 (2024).
63. Beckjord, E; Shiffman, S. Background for Real-Time monitoring and intervention related to alcohol use. Alcohol Res.; 2014; 36, pp. 9-18. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26258996][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4432861]
64. Bryan, CJ; Tipton, E; Yeager, DS. Behavioural science is unlikely to change the world without a heterogeneity revolution. Nat. Hum. Behav.; 2021; 5, pp. 980-989. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34294901][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8928154]
65. Cosme, D et al. Study protocol: social health impact of network effects (SHINE) study. PsyArXiv; 2022; [DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/cj2nx]
66. Zhou, D. et al. Mindful attention promotes control of brain network dynamics for self-regulation and discontinues the past from the present. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 120, e2201074119 (2023).
67. Kang, Y et al. Frontoparietal functional connectivity moderates the link between time spent on social media and subsequent negative affect in daily life. Sci. Rep.; 2023; 13, 20501.2023NatSR.1320501K1:CAS:528:DC%2BB3sXisVertL7N [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37993522][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10665348]
68. Jovanova, M. A person-specific Approach To Predict Health Behaviors: a Proof of Concept (University of Pennsylvania, 2023).
69. Kang, Y. et al. Purpose in life, neural alcohol cue reactivity and daily alcohol use in social drinkers. Addiction (2022). https://doi.org/10.1111/add.16012
70. McGowan, AL et al. Controllability of structural brain networks and the waxing and waning of negative affect in daily life. Biol. Psychiatry Glob Open. Sci.; 2022; 2, pp. 432-439. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36324655]
71. Stanoi, O. A. et al. Staying connected: How close friendshipssupported emotional well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rfh63_v1.
72. Peirce, JW. PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python. J. Neurosci. Methods; 2007; 162, pp. 8-13. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17254636][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2018741]
73. Penny, W. D., Friston, K. J., Ashburner, J. T., Kiebel, S. J. & Nichols, T. E. Statistical Parametric Mapping: the Analysis of Functional Brain Images (Elsevier, 2011).
74. Yarkoni, T; Poldrack, RA; Nichols, TE; Van Essen, DC; Wager, T. D. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat. Methods; 2011; 8, pp. 665-670.1:CAS:528:DC%2BC3MXotV2gs7w%3D [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21706013][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3146590]
75. Drake, A et al. Daily Stressor-Related negative mood and its associations with flourishing and daily curiosity. J. Happiness Stud.; 2022; 23, pp. 423-438. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/40641736]
76. Soyster, PD; Ashlock, L; Fisher, AJ. Pooled and person-specific machine learning models for predicting future alcohol consumption, craving, and wanting to drink: A demonstration of parallel utility. Psychol. Addict. Behav.; 2022; 36, pp. 296-306. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35041441]
77. Finlay, AK; Ram, N; Maggs, JL; Caldwell, LL. Leisure activities, the social weekend, and alcohol use: evidence from a daily study of first-year college students. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs; 2012; 73, pp. 250-259. [PubMed: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22333332][PubMedCentral: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3281983]
78. Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models using lme4. arXiv [stat.CO] (2014).
79. Brooks, K. & Van Benthem. glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. R J.; 9: 378–400. doi: 10.32614. RJ-2017-066.[CrossRef][Google (2017). (2017).
80. Kronthaler, F. & Zöllner, S. Data Analysis with RStudio. (Springer, Berlin Heidelberg, 2014).
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© The Author(s) 2025. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Conversations shape future behaviors, particularly among young adults. However, young adults vary widely in their susceptibility to peer influence. What neural processes relate to this susceptibility? We examined whether activity in brain regions associated with social rewards and making sense of others’ minds relates to a common health behavior—drinking, following conversations about alcohol. We studied ten social groups of college students (N = 104 students; 4760 total observations) across two university campuses. We collected whole-brain fMRI data while participants viewed photographs of peers with whom they tended to drink at varying frequencies. Next, using ecological momentary assessment, we tracked alcohol conversations and drinking twice daily for 28 days. On average, talking about alcohol was associated with a higher likelihood of next-day drinking. Controlling for baseline drinking, participants who responded more strongly to peers with whom they drank alcohol more frequently—in brain regions associated with social rewards and mentalizing—showed a stronger, positive association between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking. Conversely, stronger neural responses to peers with whom they drank less frequently decoupled associations between alcohol conversations and next-day drinking. We conceptually replicate prior findings linking conversations and drinking in an observational, longitudinal setting and provide new evidence that neural responses to peers moderate links between alcohol conversations and drinking behavior among young adults.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details












1 University of St. Gallen, School of Medicine, St. Gallen, Switzerland (GRID:grid.15775.31) (ISNI:0000 0001 2156 6618); ETH, Department of Management, Technology, and Economics, Zurich, Switzerland (GRID:grid.5801.c) (ISNI:0000 0001 2156 2780)
2 University of Pennsylvania, Annenberg School for Communication, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972); University of Pennsylvania, Annenberg Public Policy Center, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972)
3 University of Amsterdam, Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Amsterdam, The Netherlands (GRID:grid.7177.6) (ISNI:0000 0000 8499 2262)
4 McGill University, Desautels Faculty of Management, Montreal, Canada (GRID:grid.14709.3b) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8649)
5 The State University of New Jersey, Department of Psychology, Camden, New Jersey, USA (GRID:grid.430387.b) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8796)
6 University of Pennsylvania, Annenberg School for Communication, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972)
7 Brigham Young University, Department of Mathematics, Provo, Utah, USA (GRID:grid.253294.b) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 9115)
8 University of Pennsylvania, Department of Bioengineering, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972); University of Pennsylvania, Department of Electrical & Systems Engineering, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972); University of Pennsylvania, Department of Neurology, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972); University of Pennsylvania, Pennsylvania, Department of Psychiatry, Philadelphia, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972); University of Pennsylvania, Department of Physics & Astronomy, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972); The Santa Fe Institute, Santa Fe, New Mexico, USA (GRID:grid.209665.e) (ISNI:0000 0001 1941 1940)
9 Dartmouth College, Department of Mathematics, Hanover, New Hampshire, USA (GRID:grid.254880.3) (ISNI:0000 0001 2179 2404)
10 University of Pennsylvania, Annenberg School for Communication, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972); University of Pennsylvania, Department of Bioengineering, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972); University of Pennsylvania, Leonard Davis Institute of Health Economics, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972)
11 Columbia University, Department of Psychology, New York City, USA (GRID:grid.21729.3f) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8729)
12 University of Pennsylvania, Annenberg School for Communication, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972); University of Pennsylvania, Annenberg Public Policy Center, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972); University of Pennsylvania, Wharton Marketing Department, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972); University of Pennsylvania, Wharton Operations, Information and Decisions Department, Philadelphia,Pennsylvania, USA (GRID:grid.25879.31) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8972)