Abstract
The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of instructional self-talk on the performance, retention and transfer of dart throwing in late childhood and adolescence. 100 students were randomly selected (50 students from 9-11 years old and 50 students from 15-17 years old) from volunteers for this purpose that none of them were not familiar with the skill of dart throwing. The subjects were randomly divided in to control and experimental groups. The subjects continued practice after 5 sessions of training that every session contained 7 blocks with 3 trails and they participated in the retention and transfer past-tests after 24 hours without practice. The collected data were classified by descriptive statistical methods and were analyzed by non-parametric methods (Wilcoxon and kruskal Wallis) and (α ≤ 0/05). The results showed that the instructional self-talk leaded to facilitate the acquisition, retention and transfer of dart throwing skill at both ages. Therefore, we can conclude that self-talk can be emphasize as a cognitive approach along with physical exercise by the coaches. Also, the results showed that there was a significant difference between performances of control group sand experimental groups in the retention test at both age categories while the control groups in comparison of experimental groups had better performance in transfer test than retention post-test at both age categories. The results of this study were discussed in effect of self-talk like feedback on performance.
Keywords: Instructional self-talk, performance, retention, transfer, dart throwing, childhood, adolescence.
Introduction
Today, the sport psychologists help the athletes' performance remarkable through using the different cognitive techniques Cognitive techniques are one of the appropriate methods of intervention to improve the athletes' performance (Anderon, 1997; Cumming et al., 2006; Klovelonis et al., 2011; Afsanepurak and Bahram, 2012). The researches shows that the using of cognitive strategies can increase the performance in sports (Hardy et al., 1996; Krane and Williams, 2006; Klovelonis et al., 2011) and knowing about the cognitive techniques can be beneficial in interpreting and moderating of the negative perception and thus in the improving of performance (Woods, 2007). Self-talk is one example of these techniques (Williams, 2001; Burton and Raedeke 2008; Hardy et al., 2009). Self-talk is as external and internal dialogues applied to offer instruction and reinforcement to performers during skill execution provides a versatile day-to-day performance aid (Chroni et al., 2007). Therefore, many coaches incorporate self-talk into their programs and theorists consider it as an integral part of psychological skill training programs (Hardy et al., 1996). Two major types of self-talk have been identified, namely instructional and motivational self-talk (Klovelonis et al., 2011). Instructional self-talk refers to statements designed to enhance performance by stimulating desired actions through focus of attention on the technical aspects of a skill. Motivational self-talk refers to statements designed to facilitate performance by building confidence, increasing effort and energy expenditure and creating a positive mood (Hardy et al., 1996; Theodorakis et al., 2000; Weinberg and Gould, 2007). In this regard, the studies have shown that self-talk is effective in the increasing of focus of attention (Landin, 1994; shank, 1986) the changing of focus of attention (Niedfer, 1976), and Shank, 1986 stated that self-talk is effective to focus the attention, the recalling information and encoding new information about the running task (Hardy, 2006; Zinsser et al., 2006). In addition to, selftalk helps the athletes, through the use of appropriate cue-words, to control and organize their thoughts, to focus on basic skill components, or to motivate themselves to exert more effort during practice (Zinsser et al., 2006). Rotella et al., (1980) and Ming & Martin (1996) stated respectively that the elite skiers who were more successful than other opponents had benefited from more self-talk and the using of instructional self-talk can improve the acquisition of the skills on the ice.
Anderson et al., (1999) compared the three approach of teaching: instructional self-talk, traditional and the demonstration-only in the overhand throw in the elementary students and concluded that instructional selftalk is an appropriate strategy for teaching and learning the overhand throw. Also, Kolovelonis et al., (2011) studied the effect of instructional and motivational self-talk on students' motor task performance in physical education of elementary students. They expressed that self-talk is an effective technique for motor task performance enhancement in physical education. Theodorakis et al., (2000) and Chroni et al., (2007) examined the effects of motivational versus instructional self-talk on improving motor performance in adolescents and stated that when the task requires fine motor movements, an instructional self-talk strategy is more effective.
The instructional self-talk is helpful in the acquisition of tennis skill (Ziggler, 1987) and as one effective strategy for the acquisition of skill (Perkos et al., 2002) for the novice performers. Tsiggilis et al., (2003) studied the effect of instructional self-talk on performance of throwing skill in handball on 46 physical education students and expressed that there was no significant difference between control and instructional selftalk group in the performing of skill. Van Raalte et al., (1995) found that the positive self-talk group showed bad performance than control group and negative group in dart throwing. Goudas et al., (2006) investigated the effects of instructional self-talk on talk on standing long jump performance in the physical educations students and stated that physical education students did not improve significantly this performance using self-talk. Dana et al., (2011) examined the effect of instructional and motivational self-talk on performance and retention of discrete and continuous motor tasks in male physical education students that the discrete task was dart throwing in this research that the results showed that instructional self-talk had a significant and higher effect on dart throwing. But there is no significant difference between groups in retention test.
The review of researches shows that the athletes use the interaction of cognitive strategies for improving performance and achieving of success (Hanrahan et al., 1990; Thelwell and Greenlees, 2003; Brent, 2004; Mammasis and Doganis, 2004; Cumming et al., 2006). Today, with special attention to professional orientation process and the increasing of lifetime of athleticism, the analysis and using cognitive strategies such self-talk can be effective in this regard too. Also sports experts and sport teams are trying to improve the performance using different approaches especially using cognitive strategies in addition to physical fitness. Selftalk strategies have been examined in variety of sports performance such as basketball (Perkos et al, 2002; Chroni et al, 2007; Klovelonis et al., 2011), golf (Harvey, 2002), cricket (Holt, 2002; Slogrove et al., 2003), ice hockey (Rogerson and Hrycako, 2002), swimming (Wang et al., 2003), soccer (Papaioannou et al., 2004), tennis (Mamassis & Dogani, 2004), and water polo (Hatzigeorgiadis et al., 2004). The popularity of self-talk would seem to support the belief that it is in deed related to sport performance (Hamilton et al., 2007).
This study aims to investigate the effects of self-talk on the retention and transfer in dart throwing in addition to on performance of it in late of childhood and adolescence. The most of researches about the cognitive strategies were done in the adults, similarly about self-talk is one of the cognitive strategies especially about the effect of instructional self-talk on the retention and transfer of a skill in younger and lower ages. This study has paid to the relationship between the age and effect of instructional self-talk. Is there a question that the self-talk has effect on the children and adolescents' performance as the adult? It is possible that the effects of self-talk will be different on the performance, retention and transfer phase of a skill and the study has not been done in this area. Of course the domain and rang of researches is limited about that the effects of self-talk on the retention and transfer of a skill.
Materials and Methods
Method
The method of research was semi empirical and design of it included pre-test, post test with control group.
Participants
The statistical population of study was children 9-11 years old and adolescents 15-17 years old of girls' elementary schools and high schools in Maragheh city that they had not previous familiarity with the skill of dart throwing. From among the population, 100 students were randomly selected (50 students from 9-11 years old and 50 students from 15-17 years old) from volunteers. All the participants were in physical health and were not familiar with the skill of dart throwing. Also the subjects in each age category were selected close together in terms of weight and tall.
Instruments and tasks
The instruments were used in the study includes standard dart board (made in China, Model JB-D6-1, size: 1.2 18 inch) and standard darts (made in China, Model X-D3 with aluminum body). The scoring procedure is such that the darts hitting Bull's eye receive 100 scores, the darts landing on the circle 9 obtain 90 scores, the hitting the circle 8 receive 8 scores, and so on and finally the darts to hit the circle 1 receive 10 scores. Of course for adolescents the dart board height from floor was set up according to federation rule (173cm) but for elementary students the dart board height from floor was set up according to their tall.
Procedure
The subjects of each age category were divided into control and experimental groups after the holding a training session by two qualified instructor and implementing of pre-test. The acquisition stage was included 5 training sessions and each session 7 blocks and each block 3 dart throwing for the all groups. The experimental groups used instructional self-talk the during of acquisition phase after each trail according to the guidelines set while the control groups did not benefit from instructional self-talk after each trail. In order to prevent any cognitive activities such as self-talk between trails were used the countdown method in all groups. All the subjects performed the retention and transfer post-tests after 48 hours of without training that the conditions of retention test was same with the conditions of acquisition stage but the conditions of transfer test was different in real the experimental groups did not use instructional self-talk but the control groups used it of course both post- test were held with 3 dart throwing. The subjects were asked to say "smooth and straight body" and "look at center of goal".
Data analysis
In this study were used non-parametric methods (Wilcoxon and Kruskal Wallis) for data analysis due to the data was not normal in addition to the descriptive statistics and (α ≤ 0.05). Also was used the SPSS software, version 17 for data analysis.
Results
According to the descriptive statistics mean and standard deviation the subjects' age are 10.12 ± 0.82 for elementary students and 16.08 ± 0.81 for adolescent students. According to table 1, is observed that there was not the significant difference between the experimental and control groups in late of childhood (P = 0.937) and adolescence (P = 0.984) in the pre-test stage as a result, both groups in each age category are at a level in pretest (α = 0.05) and figure 1, shows the mean of points of dart throwing in 4 groups in the pre-test, acquisition, retention and transfer stage.
Figure I. The mean of points of dart throwing in 4 groups in the pre-test, acquisition, retention and transfer stage.
There was observed the significant difference between the experimental and control groups in late of childhood
(P ≤ 0.05) and adolescence (P ≤ 0.05) in all the sessions of the acquisition stage. In other words according figure 1, the experimental groups in both age groups has had better performance than the control groups in the acquisition stage (table 2).
There was the significant difference between the experimental and control groups in late of childhood (P = 0.000) and adolescence (P =0.000) in the retention stage and also between the experimental and control groups in late of childhood (P = 0.000) and adolescence (P = 0.000) in the transfer stage. In other words according figure 1, the experimental groups in both age groups has had better performance than the control groups in the retention and transfer test (Table 3).
There was the significant difference between performance of experimental group in late childhood (P = 0.000) and also between performance of control group and experimental group of adolescent (P = 0.000) in the pre-test and retention test so the cognitive interventions have been effective. But there was no significant difference between performance of control group in the pre-test and retention test (P = 0.315), so instructional self-talk is an effective strategy for learning of dart throwing skill in late of childhood (Table 4).
There was not a significant difference between performance of control groups in the retention and transfer test (P =0.254), while there was the significant difference between performance of control group in late of childhood (P =0.003) and also between performance of control group and experimental group of adolescent (P ≤ 0.05) in the pre-test and retention test. Of course according figure 1, was observed that the control group has been better performance with little difference in comparison with experimental group in transfer test than retention test. Also the control group has been better performance than experimental group in transfer test. These findings can imply that if self-talk was applied very much it can be very dependent such as feedback because the experimental groups did not use instructional self-talk in the transfer test but the control groups used it in the transfer test (Table 5).
Discussion and Conclusion
As you have observed, the effect of instructional self-talk has been useful for dart throwing in late of childhood and adolescence. This finding is consistent with finding of Theodorakis et al., 2000 and Chroni et al., 2007 that stated instructional self-talk is effective for fine tasks (Theodorakis et al., 2000 and Chroni et al., 2007), in this study dart throwing is a fine task and also with finding of Ziegler, 1987 and Perkos et al., 2002 that expressed instructional self-talk is helpful in the acquisition of tennis skill for the novice performers (Ziegler, 1987; Perkos et al., 2002), in this study the subjects were novice too. In addition to, this finding is consistent with the finding of Dana et al.,'s 2011 study that showed instructional self-talk had a significant and higher effect on dart throwing as a discrete task, task of this study is a discrete task (Dana et al., 2011). In addition to, it can expressed that according Landing (1994) , Shank (1986), and Zinsser et al., (2006) instructional self-talk is effective due to dart throwing is a task that requires high focus and attention so using of instructional self-talk might have helped the subjects' focus on task and has improved their performance.
The results showed that the instructional self-talk is essential strategy for learning of dart throwing skill this finding is consistent with finding of Anderson et al., 1999 and Kolovelonis et al., 2011 that stated results of study stated self-talk is an effective technique for the acquisition of skill and motor task performance enhancement in the elementary students (Anderson et al., 1999; Klovelonis et al., 2011).
These findings are conflict with the result of Tsigglis et al.,'s 2003 study that expressed there was no significant difference between control and instructional self-talk group in the performing of skill (Tsiggilis et al., 2003), the finding of Goudas et al., 2006 that stated physical education students did not improve significantly this performance using self-talk (Goudas et al., 2006), and the result of Van Raalte et al.,'s 1995 study that found the positive self-talk group showed bad performance than control group and negative group in dart throwing (Van Raalte et al., 2000).. Perhaps, lake of consistent of Tsiggilis and Goudas's study with this study is due to differences in the subjects' nature, age, and level, the kind of self-talk and the kind and level of skill.
The results of this study showed that the there was the significant difference between the performance of experimental and control groups in the retention test in both age category, This finding is conflict with the finding of Dana et al.,'s 2011 study that showed there was no significant difference between groups in retention test (Dana et al.,'s 2011). Perhaps, lake of consistent of Dana's study with this study is due to differences of the subject's gender, level or age, type of phrases of instructional self-talk and number of sessions of training.
The interesting finding of this study is better performance of control in the transfer test in both age categories. In this study the experimental groups did not use instructional self-talk in the transfer test but the control groups used it. It can be imply that if self-talk was applied very much it can be very dependent such as feedback and the athlete's performance will decline with omitting it. So it is required more researches to examine the effects of kinds of self-talk on other skills for transfers and retention test and the future researches can examine the effects of frequency of self-talk.
According to the results of this study, we can conculude that the instructional self-talk can apply for both age categories as a cognitive strategies. Therefore, the sports and rehabilitation porfesionals and coaches can use it in the planing of training and in the instruction of fine skills like dart throwing along with physical training. Althought it should be noted that the instructional self-talk had more effect in adolescence and perhaps the its reason is due to the adolescents's cognitive capasities.
References
Anderson, A. (1997). Learning strategies in physical education: self-talk, imagery, and goal-setting. Journal of Physical Education Recreation & Dance, 68, 30-35.
Anderson, A., Vogel, P., & Albrecht R. (1999). The effect of instruction self-talk on the overhand throw, The Physical Educator, 56(4).
Afsanepurak, S. A., & Bahram, A. (2012). The Effect of self-talk and imagery on Motor performance in adolescets. International Research Journal of Applied & basic Sciences, 3(3), 601-607.
Berk, L. E. (2006). Development through the Lifespan. Publisher: Allyn & Bacon.
Burton, D., Raedeke, T. (2008). Sport Psychology for Coaches. Publishers: Human Kinetics.
Chroni, S., Perkos, S., & Theodorakis, Y. (2007). Function and preferences of motivational and instructional self-Talk for adolescence Basketball players. Athletic Insight, 9(1), 19-29.
Cumming, J., Nordin S. M., Horton, R., & Reynolds, S. (2006). Examining the direction of imagery and selftalk on dart throwing performance and self-efficacy. The Sport Psychologist, 20, 257-272.
Dana, A., Rezaee, R., Jalili, F., & Zamanian, F. (2011). The effect of instruction and motivational self-talk on performance and retention of discrete and continuous motor tasks. Australian Journal of Applied Sciences, 5(8), 312-315.
Goudas, M., Hatzidimitriou, V., & Kikidi, M. (2006). The effects of self-talk on throwing- and jumping-events performance. Hellenic Journal of Psychology, 3, 105-116.
Hanrahan, S. J., Grove, J. R., & Lockwood, R. J. (1990). Psychological skills training for the blind athlete: A pilot program. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 7, 143-155.
Hamilton, R. A., Scott, D., & MacDougall, M. P. (2007). Assessing the effectiveness of self-talk interventions on endurance performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 19(2), 226-239.
Hardy, J. (2006). Speaking clearly: a critical review of the self-talk literature. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 7, 81-97.
Hardy, L., Jones, G., & Gould, D. (1996). Understanding psychological preparation for Sport: Theory and Practice of Elite Performers. Chichest, uk: Jones Willey & Sons.
Hardy, J., Oliver, E., & Tod, D. (2009). A Framework for the Study and Application of Self-talk within Sport. In S. Mellalieu S. Hanton (Eds.). Advances in Applied Sport Psychology. A Review (pp.37-74). New York: Rout Ledge.
Hatzigeorgiadis, A., Theodorakis, Y., & Zourbanos, N. (2004). Self-talk in the water-polo tasks. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 138-150.
Holt, N. L, & Hogg, J. M. (2002). Perceptions of stress and coping during preparations for the 1999 women's soccer world cup finals. The Sport Psychologist, 16, 251-271.
Klovelonis, A., Goudas, M., & Dermitzaki, I. (2011). The effects of instructional self-Talk on students motor task performance in physical education. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 12, 153-158.
Krane, V., & Williams, J. M. (2006). Psychological Characteristics of Peak Performance, In J. M. Williams (Ed.), Applied Sport Psychology: Personal Growth to Peak Performance. Publishers: McGraw Hill, pp: 207-227.
Landin, D. (1994). The Role of Verbal Cues in Skill Learning. Quest, 46, 299-313.
Mammasis, G., & Doganis, G. (2004). The Effects of a mental training program on juniors pre-competitive anxiety, self-confidence, and tennis performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 118-137.
Ming, S., & Martin, G. L. (1996). Single-subject evaluation of a Self-talk Package for improving figure skating performance. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 227-238.
Niedfer, R. N. (1976). Attention Control Training, In R.N. Singer, M. Murphey, & L.K. Tennant. Handbook of Research on Sport Psychology. Publishers: Macmillan, pp: 127-170.
Papaioannou, A., Ballon, F., Theodorakis, Y., & Auwelle, Y. V. (2004). Combined effect of goal setting and self-talk in performance of a soccer-shooting task. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 98(10), 89-99.
Perkos, S., Theodorakis, Y., & Chroni, S. (2002). Enhancing performance and skill acquisition in novice basketball players with instructional self-talk. The Sport Psychologist, 16, 368-383.
Rogerson, L. J., & Hrycaiko, D. W. (2002). Enhancing competitive performance of ice hockey goaltenders using centering and self-Talk. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 14-26.
Rotella, R., Gansneder, B., Ojala, D., Billing, J. (1980). Cognitions and coping strategies of elite Skiers; An exploratory study of young developing athletes. Journal of Sport Psychology, 1, 350-354.
Slogrove, L., Potgieter, J. R., & Foxcroft, C. D. (2003). "Thought sampling of cricketers during batting". South African Journal for Research in Sport. Physical Education, & Recreation, 25(1), 97-113.
Thelwell, R. C., & Greenlees, I. A. (2003). developing competitive endurance performance using mental skills training. The Sport Psychologist, 17, 318-337.
Theodorakis, Y., Weinberg, R., Natsis, P., Douma, E., & Kazakas, P. (2000). The effects of motivational versus instructional self-Talk on improving motor performance. The Psychologist Sport, 14, 253-272.
Tsiggilis, N., Daroglou, G., Ardamerinos, N., Partemian, S., & Loakimidis, P. (2003). The effect of self-talk on self-efficacy and performance in a handball throwing test. Inquiries in Sport and Physical Education, 1, 189-196.
Van Raalte, J. L., Brewer, A. E., Lewis, B. W., Linder, B.P., Wildman, D. E., & Kozimor, J. (1995). Cork! The effects of positive and negative self-talk on dart throwing performance. Journal of Sport Behavior, 18, 50-57.
Wang, L., S. Huddleston, & Peng, L. (2003). Psychological Skill Use by Chinese Swimmers. International Sports Journal, 7, 48-55.
Weinberg, R. S., Gould, D. (2003). Foundations of Sport and Exercise Psychology (3rdM ed.). Publishers: Human Kinetics.
Williams, J. M., (2001). Applied Sport Psychology: Personal Growth to Peak Performance. Publishers: Mayfield.
Woods, B. (2007). Sport Psychology (translated by Mosayyebi F.A). Publishers: Bam dad Book.
Ziegler S. G. (1987). Effects of stimulus cueing on the acquisition of ground strokes by beginning tennis players. Journal of Applied Behavioral analysis, 20, 405-411.
Zinsser, N., Bunker, L., Williams, J. M. (2006). Cognitive Techniques for Building Confidence and Enhancing Performance, In J.M. Williams (Ed.). Applied Sport Psychology: Personal Growth to Peak Performance. Publishers: McGraw Hill, pp: 349-38.
MOHAMMAD.T AGHDASI1, NASRIN TOUBA2
1Associate Professor, Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, University of Tabriz, IRAN
2M.Sc in Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Payam Noor University, Center of Tehran, IRAN
Published online: September 21, 2012
(Accepted for publication September 10, 2012)
DOI:10.7752/jpes.2012.03058; http://dx.doi.org/10.7752/jpes.2012.03058
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Universitatea din Pitesti Sep 2012
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of instructional self-talk on the performance, retention and transfer of dart throwing in late childhood and adolescence. 100 students were randomly selected (50 students from 9-11 years old and 50 students from 15-17 years old) from volunteers for this purpose that none of them were not familiar with the skill of dart throwing. The subjects were randomly divided in to control and experimental groups. The subjects continued practice after 5 sessions of training that every session contained 7 blocks with 3 trails and they participated in the retention and transfer past-tests after 24 hours without practice. The collected data were classified by descriptive statistical methods and were analyzed by non-parametric methods (Wilcoxon and kruskal Wallis) and (α ≤ 0/05). The results showed that the instructional self-talk leaded to facilitate the acquisition, retention and transfer of dart throwing skill at both ages. Therefore, we can conclude that self-talk can be emphasize as a cognitive approach along with physical exercise by the coaches. Also, the results showed that there was a significant difference between performances of control group sand experimental groups in the retention test at both age categories while the control groups in comparison of experimental groups had better performance in transfer test than retention post-test at both age categories. The results of this study were discussed in effect of self-talk like feedback on performance. [PUBLICATION ABSTRACT]
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer