About the Authors:
Jun Zhong
Contributed equally to this work with: Jun Zhong, Shih-Chieh Chuang
* E-mail: [email protected] (HT); [email protected] (JZ)
Affiliation: Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, The Robert F. Furchgott Center for Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America
Shih-Chieh Chuang
Contributed equally to this work with: Jun Zhong, Shih-Chieh Chuang
Affiliations Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, The Robert F. Furchgott Center for Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America, Program in Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America
Riccardo Bianchi
Affiliations Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, The Robert F. Furchgott Center for Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America, Program in Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America
Wangfa Zhao
Affiliations Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, The Robert F. Furchgott Center for Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America, Program in Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America
Geet Paul
Affiliation: Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, The Robert F. Furchgott Center for Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America
Punam Thakkar
Affiliation: Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, The Robert F. Furchgott Center for Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America
David Liu
Affiliation: Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, The Robert F. Furchgott Center for Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America
André A. Fenton
Current address: Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York, United States of America
¶These authors also contributed equally to this work.
Affiliations Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, The Robert F. Furchgott Center for Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America, Program in Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America
Robert K. S. Wong
¶These authors also contributed equally to this work.
Affiliations Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, The Robert F. Furchgott Center for Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America, Program in Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America, Department of Neurology, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America
Henri Tiedge
* E-mail: [email protected] (HT); [email protected] (JZ)
¶These authors also contributed equally to this work.
Affiliations Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, The Robert F. Furchgott Center for Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America, Program in Neural and Behavioral Science, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America, Department of Neurology, State University of New York Health Science Center at Brooklyn, Brooklyn, New York, United States of America
Introduction
A key determinant in the experience-dependent interpretation of genetic information is provided by local translational control of gene expression at neuronal synapses. The regulated translation of select mRNAs at synaptic sites is now recognized as a core mechanism in the long-term modulation of neuronal interactions [1]–[4]. Several types of translational regulators have been identified in neurons, including the fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP) [5]–[7] and regulatory BC RNAs [8], [9].
Functional absence of FMRP gives rise to the fragile X syndrome (FXS) [10], a common inherited form of mental retardation that is characterized by cognitive impairments, behavioral abnormalities and, in subpopulations of FXS patients, by epileptic and/or autistic phenotypes [11]. FMRP is an RNA-binding protein that interacts with a subset of neuronal RNAs [12]–[17]. The protein operates as a translational repressor [17]–[19], most likely via association with neuronal polyribosomes [20]–[23] [but see 19], [24].
Regulatory BC RNAs repress translation at the initiation level [25]–[28]. Absence of dendritic BC1 RNA results in neuronal hyperexcitability and epileptogenesis [29]. The apparent phenotypical commonalities between impaired BC1 RNA and FMRP translational control [see 29,30,31] prompt the question whether the two systems are functionally intersecting. Is the overlapping phenotypical output the result of pathway modulations that are implemented sequentially (and thus likely in independent fashion) or concomitantly (and thus possibly in mutually interdependent fashion)? Answers to these questions will be directly relevant to our understanding of the molecular basis of FXS and associated disorders. Therefore, in the present work, we undertook a functional dissection of FMRP and BC1 RNA translational repression pathway interactions using Fmr1−/−, BC1−/−, and BC1−/− Fmr1−/− animal model systems.
Results
Exacerbated Hyperexcitability of the CA3 Neuronal Network
We examined synaptic excitability and epileptogenic susceptibility using electrophysiological approaches. Intracellular recordings were performed in CA3 glutamatergic principal neurons of hippocampal slice preparations from BC1−/−, Fmr1−/−, BC1−/− Fmr1−/−, and WT animals. Epileptiform discharges were elicited by application of bicuculline, a GABAA receptor antagonist [31]. This disinhibition causes short (<1.5 s) synchronized discharges that are stable over the recording period in WT animals; they result in elevated synaptic release of glutamate from principal neurons [31].
In all three mutant preparations, but not in WT preparations, such short discharges over time transformed into recurrent prolonged synchronized discharges (duration>1.5 s) that are similar to ictal events in epilepsy [Fig. 1; see also 29,31]. This transition of discharge duration from a unimodal phase (short bursts only) to a bimodal phase (long bursts in addition to short bursts) was significantly accelerated in hippocampal slices prepared from BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO animals. Prolonged discharges appeared after only 20 min in dKO preparations (Fig. 1A and 1B, right columns), whereas they did not begin to occur until after 30–40 min in sKO preparations (Fig. 1, left and center columns). In addition, at time points 20 min or more after bicuculline application, epileptiform discharges in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO slices were significantly longer (Fig. 1B, 20 and 40 min) and occurred more frequently (Fig. 1B, right column) than in either type of sKO slices. Thus, prolonged discharges (i) appeared earlier, (ii) were of longer average duration, and (iii) occurred at a higher relative frequency in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO preparations than in either BC1−/− or Fmr1−/− sKO preparations. The data indicate that the concurrent absence of BC1 RNA and FMRP precipitates a significantly higher level of neuronal hyperexcitability than the absence of one repressor alone.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 1. Concurrent absence of BC1 RNA and FMRP gives rise to exacerbated synaptic hyperexcitability of CA3 pyramidal cells.
(A) Prolonged epileptiform discharges induced by bicuculline (Bic) in hippocampal slices occurred earlier (20 min) in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− slice preparations (right column) than in BC1−/− (left column) or Fmr1−/− (middle column) preparations. (B) Summary frequency histograms from each of the three groups of preparations (n, number of slices; one slice per animal) with second-order Gaussian function fits. The appearance of a second distinct peak of burst duration distribution indicates burst prolongation. Bicuculline-elicited burst prolongation occurred in all the three groups but was most prominent in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− preparations, evidenced as follows. (i) Between 10 and 20 min of bicuculline, burst durations significantly increased in the BC1−/− Fmr1−/− group (10 min: 0.213±0.008 s; n = 22; 20 min: 1.316±0.228 s; n = 29; two-way ANOVA followed by Newman-Keuls post-hoc test: P<0.01), whereas burst durations did not change in the BC1−/− (10 min: 0.216±0.005 s; n = 24; 20 min: 0.287±0.016 s; n = 30) or Fmr1−/− (10 min: 0.230±0.013 s; n = 13; 20 min: 0.616±0.026 s; n = 35) groups. (ii) Beginning at 20 min of bicuculline, burst durations were significantly longer in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− slices than in BC1−/− or in Fmr1−/− slices (P<0.01 in all cases). At 40 min bicuculline, the mean duration of long bursts was greater in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− (4.413±0.279 s; n = 13) than in BC1−/− (2.992±0.164 s; n = 11; P<0.001) or in Fmr1−/− slices (2.705±0.264 s; n = 9; P<0.001). (iii) The relative frequency of long bursts (as percentage of total number of bursts) was significantly higher in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− (Bic 30 min: 67.9%; Bic 40 min: 68.4%) than in BC1−/− (Bic 30 min: 21.9%; χ2 test, P<0.001; Bic 40 min: 29.7%; P<0.01) or in Fmr1−/− preparations (Bic 30 min: 15.1%; P<0.001; Bic 40 min: 29.0%; P<0.01).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015509.g001
Heightened Epileptogenic Susceptibility
To investigate epileptogenic vulnerability in vivo, WT animals and BC1−/−, Fmr1−/−, and BC1−/− Fmr1−/− mutant animals were subjected to auditory stimulation. When exposed to a 120 dB sound, animals of the three mutant mouse strains, but not WT animals, typically initiated excessive motor activity in the form of wild, uncontrolled running and jumping, as has previously been described for BC1−/− and Fmr1−/− sKO animals [29], [32], [33]. Such wild running was followed, within less than 2 minutes, by generalized tonic-clonic seizures (Fig. 2; Movie S1). The percentage of animals undergoing convulsive seizures was high for all three mutant strains (>75%; Fig. 2A). However, while most sKO animals recovered from such seizures within less than 2 minutes, a remarkably high percentage of BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO animals died while undergoing seizures (>86% lethality within <90 s after onset of alarm) (Fig. 2B). Thus, BC1-/− Fmr1−/− dKO animals are acutely susceptible to audiogenic epileptogenesis, and epileptic lethality is significantly increased in comparison to BC1−/− or Fmr1−/− sKO animals (Fig. 2).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 2. Severe epileptogenic susceptibility of BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO animals results in high lethality.
(A) Lack of BC1 RNA, FMRP, or both, significantly increased propensity for audiogenic seizures (Generalized Linear Model, followed by post-hoc tests of pairs of groups using exact logistic regression stratified by litter; P<0.0001 for each group when compared to WT). (B) Rate of lethality resulting from audiogenic seizures was significantly higher in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− animals than in BC1−/− animals (P<0.0001) or in Fmr1−/− animals (P = 0.0007). All mutant animals had a significantly higher audiogenic lethality rate compared with WT (BC1−/− Fmr1−/−, P<0.0001; BC1−/−, P = 0.008; Fmr1−/−, P<0.0001). BC1−/− animals did not significantly differ from Fmr1−/− animals in audiogenic lethality (P = 0.0719). Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. WT, n = 30; BC1−/−, n = 31; BC1−/− Fmr1−/−, n = 37; Fmr1−/−, n = 29.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015509.g002
These results are corroborated by data indicating that initial epileptogenic responses (uncontrolled running) were triggered faster in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO animals than in either BC1−/− animals or Fmr1−/− animals. The percentages of animals in uncontrolled running at 10 s, 15 s, and 20 s after start of the alarm were significantly higher in the BC1−/− Fmr1−/− group than in either the BC1−/− or the Fmr1−/− group (Fig. S1).
The combined results indicate that concomitant absence of BC1 RNA and FMRP results in severely heightened susceptibility to hyperexcitability and epileptogenesis, in comparison with animal models that lack only one type of translational repressor.
Impaired Place Learning
We used an active place avoidance paradigm [34] to assess place learning in sKO and dKO animals. As shown in Fig. 3A, WT animals quickly learned to avoid entering a rotating shock zone, reaching their performance asymptote by the third trial on the first day of training. Learning in BC1−/− and Fmr1−/− sKO animals was also robust but retarded in comparison to the WT animals. However, in clear contrast to learning in sKO animals, active place avoidance in dKO animals did not improve at all, even over 3 days of training (Fig. 3).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 3. Place learning is mildly impaired in BC1−/− and Fmr1−/− sKO mice but severely impaired in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO mice.
(A) Exploration, measured as the number of entrances into a 60° zone (gray sector on the circle in the schematic), did not differ amongst the groups on the pre-training trial when the shock was off (F3,26 = 0.38; P = 0.8). When the shock was turned on (red sector), all groups except the dKO learned to avoid the shock zone. The effects of genotype and trial across place training were significant (genotype: F3,234 = 141.81; P = 10−52; F8,234 = 4.96; P = 10−5) and the interaction was not (F24,234 = 0.70; P = 0.85). Newman-Keuls post-hoc tests confirmed that the overall number of entrances significantly differed between all groups (WT<Fmr1−/−<BC1−/−<dKO). The failure of the dKO to learn persisted across 3 days of training (3 trials/day) and this was confirmed by 1-way ANOVA on the factor trial (F8,45 = 1.02; P = 0.43). (B) Summary of behavior on the pre-training and active avoidance trials illustrates the significant group differences during place learning. Error bars indicate S.E.M., *P<0.05, ***P<0.001. BC1−/−, n = 6; Fmr1−/−, n = 6; dKO, n = 6; WT, n = 12.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015509.g003
Thus, while place learning was preserved although retarded in both groups of sKO animals, a severe learning deficit was apparent in dKO animals.
Discussion
We used three phenotypical readouts -— neuronal network excitability, epileptogenesis, and place learning -— to evaluate the relative contributions of two translational repressors, BC1 RNA and FMRP, to brain function. We report that in all three cases, phenotypical deficiencies were significantly exacerbated in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO animals, relative to BC1−/− or Fmr1−/− sKO animals. As will be discussed in the following, these results have critical implications concerning the mode of functional interactions between the two repressors, BC1 RNA and FMRP.
Neuronal excitability was examined in hippocampal CA3 pyramidal cells. Previous work has shown that in BC1−/− and Fmr1−/− preparations, synaptic glutamate release elicits ictal-like prolonged epileptiform discharges [29], [31]. We now observe that the severity of such hyperexcitability is significantly heightened in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO preparations. This phenotypical exacerbation in the absence of two translational repressors was mirrored in aggravated epileptogenesis in vivo. Sensitivity to audiogenic seizures, as previously described for BC1−/− and Fmr1−/− animals [29], [32], [33] was also observed in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO animals. However, in contrast to sKO animals in which lethality from audiogenic seizures is in the range of 20–30%, audiogenic lethality in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO animals was found approaching 90%. It appears that absence of the translational repressors BC1 RNA and FMRP contribute in modular fashion to phenotypical impairments.
In the active place avoidance paradigm, BC1−/− and Fmr1−/− sKO animals exhibited only mild place learning deficits, results that are in agreement with previous observations that such animals showed no or only mild learning impairments in the Morris water maze and other maze tasks [35]–[37]. In contrast, BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO animals were severely impaired in their place learning ability. We note that the active place avoidance task goes beyond testing spatial and navigation ability [38] because optimal performance requires the mouse to segregate the useful spatial information from the stationary spatial frame from the irrelevant spatial information from the rotating spatial frame. Indeed, the ability to segregate spatial information has been dissociated from the ability to form spatial associations [39]. These properties may make the active place avoidance task exquisitely sensitive to even mild hippocampal dysfunction [40], [41], certainly more than the water maze [39], [42], and possibly more than other place learning tasks. In summary, data obtained in three readouts of brain function indicate that concurrent lack of both BC1 RNA and FMRP significantly compounds the phenotypical consequences that are observed in the absence of only one repressor.
The results presented here indicate that the repressors BC1 RNA and FMRP operate sequentially in the translation pathway (Fig. 4). In this scenario, the repressors act in series and independently of each other. Therefore, lack of one repressor will leave the respective other repressor unaffected and functional, and phenotypical output will be less severely impacted than in the case of concurrent absence of both repressors. This model is also compatible with recent results showing that defects of striatal mGluR5-endocannabinoid signaling were significantly exacerbated in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO preparations [43] [but see 44]. Conversely, our results are not compatible with a model in which BC1 RNA and FMRP operate in interdependent fashion [24] as in this case, lack of both repressors should precipitate the same phenotypical consequences as lack of either one repressor. We conclude that the mode of action of BC1 RNA and FMRP is sequential-independent.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 4. BC1 RNA and FMRP act as repressors on activity-stimulated translation.
In this model, a balance of power is maintained in the postsynaptic translation pathway by the functional interplay between two opposing forces: (i) the stimulatory consequences of receptor activation, and (ii) translational repression by BC1 RNA and FMRP. It is suggested that BC1 RNA and FMRP, operating in series in the same translational pathway, target overlapping but non-identical sets of mRNAs. We posit that multiple repressors are needed at the synapse to ensure adequate stimulation-repression homeostasis and to allow for differential derepression options.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015509.g004
Protein-synthetic capacity in synapto-dendritic domains allows a neuron to respond to external stimuli in an input-specific, experience-dependent manner [2], [45]. However, such gains in neuronal plastic responsiveness come at a price: a protein synthetic machinery that, unless properly controlled, may engage in premature or inappropriate translation of locally available mRNAs, resulting in neuronal hyperexcitability. We suggest that effective local translational repression is vital for a neuron to ensure that proteins are only synthesized when and where needed.
BC RNAs and FMRP represent two types of neuronal translational repressors. Previous work has established a balance of power between receptor activation on one hand and translational repression by BC1 RNA and FMRP on the other [7], [29], [46]. Translational repressors thus operate as “brakes” that counteract receptor-mediated translational stimulation (Fig. 4), establishing a stimulation-repression balance that controls translational output in accordance with the physiological status of the synapse. Such brakes have been in place early during phylogenetic brain development as an FMRP ortholog exists in Drosophila [47]. In contrast, dendritic BC RNAs are mammalian-specific, with BC1 RNA restricted to rodents (Rodentia), BC200 RNA restricted to simian primates (Anthropoidea) [48], [49]. It is therefore likely that these RNAs were independently recruited into their current repression function at a later time in mammalian evolution during which increasing brain complexity required more stringent translational control mechanisms. Multiple, mutually independent translational control mechanisms in neurons may also allow for more discrete regulation by upstream signals. The evolution of neuronal RNA-coding genes thus appears to be linked to increasing nervous system complexity in eukaryotes [50], [51].
Materials and Methods
Animals
Work with vertebrate animals was in accordance with the Public Health Service Policy on Humane Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and was approved by the State University of New York Brooklyn Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Institutional Assurance Number A3260-01, Protocol Number 10-074-09).
BC1−/− mice (lines 13 and 15) were established from independent mutant ES cell lines [52] and used as described [53]. Both lines have a mixed C57BL6/sv129 background. Lines 13 and 15 were used (with equivalent results) for experiments shown in Figs. 1 and 2, line 15 was used for experiments shown in Fig. 3. Fmr1−/− mice carrying the Fmr1tm1Cgr allele were obtained from Jackson Laboratories (Bar Harbor, ME), and have a mixed C57BL6/FVB background. BC1−/− mice were crossed with Fmr1−/− mice to generate BC1−/− Fmr1−/− mice which have a mixed C57BL6/FVB/sv129 background. We used animals at 18–21 days of age except for place learning tasks in which animals at 2–4 months were used.
Hippocampal Slice Preparations and Electrophysiological Recordings
Transverse hippocampal slices (400 µm) were prepared as described [54]. Slices were allowed to recover from the isolation procedure for at least 1.5 h. Intracellular recordings were carried out in CA3 pyramidal cells using an Axoclamp 2A amplifier (Molecular Devices, Palo Alto, CA) as described [29].
Auditory Stimulation
Epileptogenic susceptibility to auditory stimuli was tested as described [29]. Animals were subjected to auditory stimulation for 15 minutes. Video recordings were analyzed by a person who was not informed of the animals' genotypes. Recorded parameters included: time to onset of uncontrolled running, time to onset of seizure, percentage of animals undergoing seizures, and lethality.
Place Learning
We used an active place avoidance paradigm to examine place learning [34]. All animals were trained in a task that requires intact hippocampal function for learning, consolidation and recall [34] as well as hippocampal LTP maintenance by persistent activation of PKMζ for long-term memory storage [42], [55]. The mice were habituated to a rotating arena during a 10-min pre-training session. Active avoidance training began on the following trials by activating a stationary 60° sector as a shock zone.
Data Analysis and Statistical Evaluation were performed as described [29].
Supporting Information
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure S1.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015509.s001
Initial epileptogenic responses were triggered significantly faster in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO animals. Percentages of animals in uncontrolled running after 10 s, 15 s, and 20 s were significantly higher in the BC1−/− Fmr1−/− group than in either the BC1−/− or the Fmr1−/− group (Generalized Linear Model, followed by post-hoc tests of pairs of groups using exact logistic regression stratified by litter, P<0.0001 for 10 s, 15 s, and 20 s groups). The BC1−/− and the Fmr1−/− groups did not significantly differ from each other. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. (EPS)
Movie S1.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015509.s002
(MP4)
Acknowledgments
We thank colleagues at the Robert F. Furchgott Center for advice and discussion. Statistical consultation was provided by Dr. Jeremy Weedon at the SUNY Brooklyn Scientific Computing Center.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: JZ AAF RKSW HT. Performed the experiments: JZ SCC GP PT DL. Analyzed the data: JZ SCC RB WZ GP PT AAF. Wrote the paper: AAF RKSW HT. Edited the manuscript, wrote figure legends, and helped with the outline of the paper: JZ SCC RB WZ.
Citation: Zhong J, Chuang S-C, Bianchi R, Zhao W, Paul G, Thakkar P, et al. (2010) Regulatory BC1 RNA and the Fragile X Mental Retardation Protein: Convergent Functionality in Brain. PLoS ONE5(11): e15509. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015509
1. Grossman AW, Aldridge GM, Weiler IJ, Greenough WT (2006) Local protein synthesis and spine morphogenesis: Fragile X syndrome and beyond. Journal of Neuroscience 26: 7151–7155.AW GrossmanGM AldridgeIJ WeilerWT Greenough2006Local protein synthesis and spine morphogenesis: Fragile X syndrome and beyond.Journal of Neuroscience2671517155
2. Kindler S, Wang H, Richter D, Tiedge H (2005) RNA transport and local control of translation. Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology 21: 223–245.S. KindlerH. WangD. RichterH. Tiedge2005RNA transport and local control of translation.Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology21223245
3. Miyashiro KY, Bell TJ, Sul JY, Eberwine J (2009) Subcellular neuropharmacology: the importance of intracellular targeting. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences 30: 203–211.KY MiyashiroTJ BellJY SulJ. Eberwine2009Subcellular neuropharmacology: the importance of intracellular targeting.Trends in Pharmacological Sciences30203211
4. Pfeiffer BE, Huber KM (2006) Current advances in local protein synthesis and synaptic plasticity. Journal of Neuroscience 26: 7147–7150.BE PfeifferKM Huber2006Current advances in local protein synthesis and synaptic plasticity.Journal of Neuroscience2671477150
5. Bassell GJ, Warren ST (2008) Fragile X syndrome: loss of local mRNA regulation alters synaptic development and function. Neuron 60: 201–214.GJ BassellST Warren2008Fragile X syndrome: loss of local mRNA regulation alters synaptic development and function.Neuron60201214
6. Oostra BA, Willemsen R (2009) FMR1: a gene with three faces. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta 1790: 467–477.BA OostraR. Willemsen2009FMR1: a gene with three faces.Biochimica et Biophysica Acta1790467477
7. Bear MF, Huber KM, Warren ST (2004) The mGluR theory of fragile X mental retardation. Trends in Neurosciences 27: 370–377.MF BearKM HuberST Warren2004The mGluR theory of fragile X mental retardation.Trends in Neurosciences27370377
8. Wang H, Tiedge H (2009) Dendrites: localized translation. Encyclopedia of Neuroscience 3: 431–435.H. WangH. Tiedge2009Dendrites: localized translation.Encyclopedia of Neuroscience3431435
9. Iacoangeli A, Bianchi R, Tiedge H (2010) Regulatory RNAs in brain function and disorders. Brain Research 1338: 36–47.A. IacoangeliR. BianchiH. Tiedge2010Regulatory RNAs in brain function and disorders.Brain Research13383647
10. Nelson DL (1995) The fragile X syndromes. Seminars in Cell Biology 6: 5–11.DL Nelson1995The fragile X syndromes.Seminars in Cell Biology6511
11. Jacquemont S, Hagerman RJ, Hagerman PJ, Leehey MA (2007) Fragile-X syndrome and fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome: two faces of FMR1. Lancet Neurology 6: 45–55.S. JacquemontRJ HagermanPJ HagermanMA Leehey2007Fragile-X syndrome and fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome: two faces of FMR1.Lancet Neurology64555
12. Ashley CT Jr, Wilkinson KD, Reines D, Warren ST (1993) FMR1 protein: conserved RNP family domains and selective RNA binding. Science 262: 563–566.CT Ashley JrKD WilkinsonD. ReinesST Warren1993FMR1 protein: conserved RNP family domains and selective RNA binding.Science262563566
13. Darnell JC, Fraser CE, Mostovetsky O, Stefani G, Jones TA, et al. (2005) Kissing complex RNAs mediate interaction between the Fragile-X mental retardation protein KH2 domain and brain polyribosomes. Genes and Development 19: 903–918.JC DarnellCE FraserO. MostovetskyG. StefaniTA Jones2005Kissing complex RNAs mediate interaction between the Fragile-X mental retardation protein KH2 domain and brain polyribosomes.Genes and Development19903918
14. Darnell JC, Jensen K, Jin P, Brown V, Warren S, et al. (2001) Fragile X mental retardation protein targets G quartet mRNAs important for neuronal function. Cell 107: 489–499.JC DarnellK. JensenP. JinV. BrownS. Warren2001Fragile X mental retardation protein targets G quartet mRNAs important for neuronal function.Cell107489499
15. Brown V, Jin P, Ceman S, Darnell JC, O'Donnell WT, et al. (2001) Microarray identification of FMRP-associated brain mRNAs and altered mRNA translational profiles in fragile X syndrome. Cell 107: 477–487.V. BrownP. JinS. CemanJC DarnellWT O'Donnell2001Microarray identification of FMRP-associated brain mRNAs and altered mRNA translational profiles in fragile X syndrome.Cell107477487
16. Miyashiro K, Beckel-Mitchener A, Purk T-P, Becker K, Barret T, et al. (2003) RNA cargoes associating with FMRP reveal deficits in cellular functioning in Fmr1 null mice. Neuron 37: 417–431.K. MiyashiroA. Beckel-MitchenerT-P PurkK. BeckerT. Barret2003RNA cargoes associating with FMRP reveal deficits in cellular functioning in Fmr1 null mice.Neuron37417431
17. Sung YJ, Dolzhanskaya N, Nolin SL, Brown T, Currie JR, et al. (2003) The fragile X mental retardation protein FMRP binds elongation factor 1A mRNA and negatively regulates its translation in vivo. Journal of Biological Chemistry 278: 15669–15678.YJ SungN. DolzhanskayaSL NolinT. BrownJR Currie2003The fragile X mental retardation protein FMRP binds elongation factor 1A mRNA and negatively regulates its translation in vivo.Journal of Biological Chemistry2781566915678
18. Li Z, Zhang Y, Ku L, Wilkinson KD, Warren ST, et al. (2001) The fragile X mental retardation protein inhibits translation via interacting with mRNA. Nucleic Acids Research 29: 2276–2283.Z. LiY. ZhangL. KuKD WilkinsonST Warren2001The fragile X mental retardation protein inhibits translation via interacting with mRNA.Nucleic Acids Research2922762283
19. Laggerbauer B, Ostareck D, Keidel EM, Ostareck-Lederer A, Fischer U (2001) Evidence that fragile X mental retardation protein is a negative regulator of translation. Human Molecular Genetics 10: 329–338.B. LaggerbauerD. OstareckEM KeidelA. Ostareck-LedererU. Fischer2001Evidence that fragile X mental retardation protein is a negative regulator of translation.Human Molecular Genetics10329338
20. Stefani G, Fraser CE, Darnell JC, Darnell RB (2004) Fragile X mental retardation protein is associated with translating polyribosomes in neuronal cells. Journal of Neuroscience 24: 7272–7276.G. StefaniCE FraserJC DarnellRB Darnell2004Fragile X mental retardation protein is associated with translating polyribosomes in neuronal cells.Journal of Neuroscience2472727276
21. Khandjian EW, Huot ME, Tremblay S, Davidovic L, Mazroui R, et al. (2004) Biochemical evidence for the association of fragile X mental retardation protein with brain polyribosomal ribonucleoparticles. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 101: 13357–13362.EW KhandjianME HuotS. TremblayL. DavidovicR. Mazroui2004Biochemical evidence for the association of fragile X mental retardation protein with brain polyribosomal ribonucleoparticles.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA1011335713362
22. Ceman S, O'Donnell WT, Reed M, Patton S, Pohl J, et al. (2003) Phosphorylation influences the translation state of FMRP-associated polyribosomes. Human Molecular Genetics 12: 3295–3305.S. CemanWT O'DonnellM. ReedS. PattonJ. Pohl2003Phosphorylation influences the translation state of FMRP-associated polyribosomes.Human Molecular Genetics1232953305
23. Weiler IJ, Irwin SA, Klintsova AY, Spencer CM, Brazelton AD, et al. (1997) Fragile X mental retardation protein is translated near synapses in response to neurotransmitter activation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 94: 5395–5400.IJ WeilerSA IrwinAY KlintsovaCM SpencerAD Brazelton1997Fragile X mental retardation protein is translated near synapses in response to neurotransmitter activation.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA9453955400
24. Zalfa F, Giorgi M, Primerano B, Moro A, Di Penta A, et al. (2003) The fragile X syndrome protein FMRP associates with BC1 RNA and regulates the translation of specific mRNAs at synapses. Cell 112: 317–327.F. ZalfaM. GiorgiB. PrimeranoA. MoroA. Di Penta2003The fragile X syndrome protein FMRP associates with BC1 RNA and regulates the translation of specific mRNAs at synapses.Cell112317327
25. Wang H, Iacoangeli A, Lin D, Williams K, Denman RB, et al. (2005) Dendritic BC1 RNA in translational control mechanisms. Journal of Cell Biology 171: 811–821.H. WangA. IacoangeliD. LinK. WilliamsRB Denman2005Dendritic BC1 RNA in translational control mechanisms.Journal of Cell Biology171811821
26. Wang H, Iacoangeli A, Popp S, Muslimov IA, Imataka H, et al. (2002) Dendritic BC1 RNA: functional role in regulation of translation initiation. Journal of Neuroscience 22: 10232–10241.H. WangA. IacoangeliS. PoppIA MuslimovH. Imataka2002Dendritic BC1 RNA: functional role in regulation of translation initiation.Journal of Neuroscience221023210241
27. Lin D, Pestova TV, Hellen CU, Tiedge H (2008) Translational control by a small RNA: dendritic BC1 RNA targets the eukaryotic initiation factor 4A helicase mechanism. Molecular and Cellular Biology 28: 3008–3019.D. LinTV PestovaCU HellenH. Tiedge2008Translational control by a small RNA: dendritic BC1 RNA targets the eukaryotic initiation factor 4A helicase mechanism.Molecular and Cellular Biology2830083019
28. Kondrashov AV, Kiefmann M, Ebnet K, Khanam T, Muddashetty RS, et al. (2005) Inhibitory effect of naked neural BC1 RNA or BC200 RNA on eukaryotic in vitro translation systems is reversed by poly(A)-binding protein (PABP). J Mol Biol 353: 88–103.AV KondrashovM. KiefmannK. EbnetT. KhanamRS Muddashetty2005Inhibitory effect of naked neural BC1 RNA or BC200 RNA on eukaryotic in vitro translation systems is reversed by poly(A)-binding protein (PABP).J Mol Biol35388103
29. Zhong J, Chuang SC, Bianchi R, Zhao W, Lee H, et al. (2009) BC1 regulation of metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated neuronal excitability. Journal of Neuroscience 29: 9977–9986.J. ZhongSC ChuangR. BianchiW. ZhaoH. Lee2009BC1 regulation of metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated neuronal excitability.Journal of Neuroscience2999779986
30. Yan QJ, Asafo-Adjei PK, Arnold HM, Brown RE, Bauchwitz RP (2004) A phenotypic and molecular characterization of the fmr1-tm1Cgr fragile X mouse. Genes, Brain and Behavior 3: 337–359.QJ YanPK Asafo-AdjeiHM ArnoldRE BrownRP Bauchwitz2004A phenotypic and molecular characterization of the fmr1-tm1Cgr fragile X mouse.Genes, Brain and Behavior3337359
31. Chuang SC, Zhao W, Bauchwitz R, Yan Q, Bianchi R, et al. (2005) Prolonged epileptiform discharges induced by altered group I metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated synaptic responses in hippocampal slices of a fragile X mouse model. Journal of Neuroscience 25: 8048–8055.SC ChuangW. ZhaoR. BauchwitzQ. YanR. Bianchi2005Prolonged epileptiform discharges induced by altered group I metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated synaptic responses in hippocampal slices of a fragile X mouse model.Journal of Neuroscience2580488055
32. Musumeci SA, Bosco P, Calabrese G, Bakker C, De Sarro GB, et al. (2000) Audiogenic seizures susceptibility in transgenic mice with fragile X syndrome. Epilepsia 41: 19–23.SA MusumeciP. BoscoG. CalabreseC. BakkerGB De Sarro2000Audiogenic seizures susceptibility in transgenic mice with fragile X syndrome.Epilepsia411923
33. Chen L, Toth M (2001) Fragile X mice develop sensory hyperreactivity to auditory stimuli. Neuroscience 103: 1043–1050.L. ChenM. Toth2001Fragile X mice develop sensory hyperreactivity to auditory stimuli.Neuroscience10310431050
34. Cimadevilla JM, Wesierska M, Fenton AA, Bures J (2001) Inactivating one hippocampus impairs avoidance of a stable room-defined place during dissociation of arena cues from room cues by rotation of the arena. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 98: 3531–3536.JM CimadevillaM. WesierskaAA FentonJ. Bures2001Inactivating one hippocampus impairs avoidance of a stable room-defined place during dissociation of arena cues from room cues by rotation of the arena.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA9835313536
35. D'Hooge R, Nagels G, Franck F, Bakker CE, Reyniers E, et al. (1997) Mildly impaired water maze performance in male Fmr1 knockout mice. Neuroscience 76: 367–376.R. D'HoogeG. NagelsF. FranckCE BakkerE. Reyniers1997Mildly impaired water maze performance in male Fmr1 knockout mice.Neuroscience76367376
36. Dutch-Belgian Fragile X Consortium (1994) Fmr1 knockout mice: a model to study fragile X mental retardation. Cell 78: 23–33.Dutch-Belgian Fragile X Consortium1994Fmr1 knockout mice: a model to study fragile X mental retardation.Cell782333
37. Lewejohann L, Skryabin BV, Sachser N, Prehn C, Heiduschka P, et al. (2004) Role of a neuronal small non-messenger RNA: behavioural alterations in BC1 RNA-deleted mice. Behavioural Brain Research 154: 273–289.L. LewejohannBV SkryabinN. SachserC. PrehnP. Heiduschka2004Role of a neuronal small non-messenger RNA: behavioural alterations in BC1 RNA-deleted mice.Behavioural Brain Research154273289
38. Wesierska M, Dockery C, Fenton AA (2005) Beyond memory, navigation, and inhibition: behavioral evidence for hippocampus-dependent cognitive coordination in the rat. J Neurosci 25: 2413–2419.M. WesierskaC. DockeryAA Fenton2005Beyond memory, navigation, and inhibition: behavioral evidence for hippocampus-dependent cognitive coordination in the rat.J Neurosci2524132419
39. Kubik S, Fenton AA (2005) Behavioral evidence that segregation and representation are dissociable hippocampal functions. J Neurosci 25: 9205–9212.S. KubikAA Fenton2005Behavioral evidence that segregation and representation are dissociable hippocampal functions.J Neurosci2592059212
40. Cimadevilla JM, Wesierska M, Fenton AA, Bures J (2001) Inactivating one hippocampus impairs avoidance of a stable room-defined place during dissociation of arena cues from room cues by rotation of the arena. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98: 3531–3536.JM CimadevillaM. WesierskaAA FentonJ. Bures2001Inactivating one hippocampus impairs avoidance of a stable room-defined place during dissociation of arena cues from room cues by rotation of the arena.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A9835313536
41. Olypher AV, Klement D, Fenton AA (2006) Cognitive disorganization in hippocampus: a physiological model of the disorganization in psychosis. J Neurosci 26: 158–168.AV OlypherD. KlementAA Fenton2006Cognitive disorganization in hippocampus: a physiological model of the disorganization in psychosis.J Neurosci26158168
42. Serrano P, Friedman EL, Kenney J, Taubenfeld SM, Zimmerman JM, et al. (2008) PKMzeta maintains spatial, instrumental, and classically conditioned long-term memories. PLoS Biol 6: 2698–2706.P. SerranoEL FriedmanJ. KenneySM TaubenfeldJM Zimmerman2008PKMzeta maintains spatial, instrumental, and classically conditioned long-term memories.PLoS Biol626982706
43. Maccarrone M, Rossi S, Bari M, De Chiara V, Rapino C, et al. (2010) Abnormal mGlu 5 receptor/endocannabinoid coupling in mice lacking FMRP and BC1 RNA. Neuropsychopharmacology 35: 1500–1509.M. MaccarroneS. RossiM. BariV. De ChiaraC. Rapino2010Abnormal mGlu 5 receptor/endocannabinoid coupling in mice lacking FMRP and BC1 RNA.Neuropsychopharmacology3515001509
44. Centonze D, Rossi S, Mercaldo V, Napoli I, Ciotti MT, et al. (2008) Abnormal striatal GABA transmission in the mouse model for the fragile X syndrome. Biological Psychiatry 63: 963–973.D. CentonzeS. RossiV. MercaldoI. NapoliMT Ciotti2008Abnormal striatal GABA transmission in the mouse model for the fragile X syndrome.Biological Psychiatry63963973
45. Cao X, Yeo G, Muotri AR, Kuwabara T, Gage FH (2006) Noncoding RNAs in the mammalian central nervous system. Annu Rev Neurosci 29: 77–103.X. CaoG. YeoAR MuotriT. KuwabaraFH Gage2006Noncoding RNAs in the mammalian central nervous system.Annu Rev Neurosci2977103
46. Volk LJ, Pfeiffer BE, Gibson JR, Huber KM (2007) Multiple Gq-coupled receptors converge on a common protein synthesis-dependent long-term depression that is affected in fragile X syndrome mental retardation. Journal of Neuroscience 27: 11624–11634.LJ VolkBE PfeifferJR GibsonKM Huber2007Multiple Gq-coupled receptors converge on a common protein synthesis-dependent long-term depression that is affected in fragile X syndrome mental retardation.Journal of Neuroscience271162411634
47. Wan L, Dockendorff TC, Jongens TA, Dreyfuss G (2000) Characterization of dFMR1, a Drosophila melanogaster homolog of the fragile X mental retardation protein. Molecular and Cellular Biology 20: 8536–8547.L. WanTC DockendorffTA JongensG. Dreyfuss2000Characterization of dFMR1, a Drosophila melanogaster homolog of the fragile X mental retardation protein.Molecular and Cellular Biology2085368547
48. Martignetti JA, Brosius J (1993) Neural BC1 RNA as an evolutionary marker: guinea pig remains a rodent. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 90: 9698–9702.JA MartignettiJ. Brosius1993Neural BC1 RNA as an evolutionary marker: guinea pig remains a rodent.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA9096989702
49. Martignetti JA, Brosius J (1993) BC200 RNA: a neural RNA polymerase III product encoded by a monomeric Alu element. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 90: 11563–11567.JA MartignettiJ. Brosius1993BC200 RNA: a neural RNA polymerase III product encoded by a monomeric Alu element.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA901156311567
50. Mattick JS (2004) RNA regulation: a new genetics? Nature Reviews: Genetics 5: 316–323.JS Mattick2004RNA regulation: a new genetics?Nature Reviews: Genetics5316323
51. Taft RJ, Pheasant M, Mattick JS (2007) The relationship between non-protein-coding DNA and eukaryotic complexity. Bioessays 29: 288–299.RJ TaftM. PheasantJS Mattick2007The relationship between non-protein-coding DNA and eukaryotic complexity.Bioessays29288299
52. Skryabin BV, Sukonina V, Jordan U, Lewejohann L, Sachser N, et al. (2003) Neuronal untranslated BC1 RNA: targeted gene elimination in mice. Mol Cell Biol 23: 6435–6441.BV SkryabinV. SukoninaU. JordanL. LewejohannN. Sachser2003Neuronal untranslated BC1 RNA: targeted gene elimination in mice.Mol Cell Biol2364356441
53. Zhong J, Chuang SC, Bianchi R, Zhao W, Lee H, et al. (2009) BC1 regulation of metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated neuronal excitability. J Neurosci 29: 9977–9986.J. ZhongSC ChuangR. BianchiW. ZhaoH. Lee2009BC1 regulation of metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated neuronal excitability.J Neurosci2999779986
54. Lee AC, Wong RK, Chuang SC, Shin HS, Bianchi R (2002) Role of synaptic metabotropic glutamate receptors in epileptiform discharges in hippocampal slices. Journal of Neurophysiology 88: 1625–1633.AC LeeRK WongSC ChuangHS ShinR. Bianchi2002Role of synaptic metabotropic glutamate receptors in epileptiform discharges in hippocampal slices.Journal of Neurophysiology8816251633
55. Pastalkova E, Serrano P, Pinkhasova D, Wallace E, Fenton AA, et al. (2006) Storage of spatial information by the maintenance mechanism of LTP. Science 313: 1141–1144.E. PastalkovaP. SerranoD. PinkhasovaE. WallaceAA Fenton2006Storage of spatial information by the maintenance mechanism of LTP.Science31311411144
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2010 Zhong et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Background
BC RNAs and the fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP) are translational repressors that have been implicated in the control of local protein synthesis at the synapse. Work with BC1 and Fmr1 animal models has revealed that phenotypical consequences resulting from the absence of either BC1 RNA or FMRP are remarkably similar. To establish functional interactions between BC1 RNA and FMRP is important for our understanding of how local protein synthesis regulates neuronal excitability.
Methodology/Principal Findings
We generated BC1−/− Fmr1−/− double knockout (dKO) mice. We examined such animals, lacking both BC1 RNA and FMRP, in comparison with single knockout (sKO) animals lacking either one repressor. Analysis of neural phenotypical output revealed that at least three attributes of brain functionality are subject to control by both BC1 RNA and FMRP: neuronal network excitability, epileptogenesis, and place learning. The severity of CA3 pyramidal cell hyperexcitability was significantly higher in BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO preparations than in the respective sKO preparations, as was seizure susceptibility of BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO animals in response to auditory stimulation. In place learning, BC1−/− Fmr1−/− dKO animals were severely impaired, in contrast to BC1−/− or Fmr1−/− sKO animals which exhibited only mild deficits.
Conclusions/Significance
Our data indicate that BC1 RNA and FMRP operate in sequential-independent fashion. They suggest that the molecular interplay between two translational repressors directly impacts brain functionality.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer