About the Authors:
Linlin Qiu
Contributed equally to this work with: Linlin Qiu, Lin Tian
Affiliations Institute of Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, People's Republic of China, Key Laboratory for Mental Health, Ministry of Health, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Lin Tian
Contributed equally to this work with: Linlin Qiu, Lin Tian
Affiliations Institute of Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, People's Republic of China, Key Laboratory for Mental Health, Ministry of Health, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Chao Pan
Affiliations Institute of Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, People's Republic of China, Key Laboratory for Mental Health, Ministry of Health, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Risheng Zhu
Affiliations Institute of Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, People's Republic of China, Key Laboratory for Mental Health, Ministry of Health, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Qi Liu
Affiliations Institute of Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, People's Republic of China, Key Laboratory for Mental Health, Ministry of Health, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Jun Yan
Affiliations Institute of Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, People's Republic of China, Key Laboratory for Mental Health, Ministry of Health, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Qiang Zhao
Affiliation: The Department of Radiology, The Third Hospital, Peking University, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Huishu Yuan
Affiliation: The Department of Radiology, The Third Hospital, Peking University, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Yonghua Han
Affiliations Institute of Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, People's Republic of China, Key Laboratory for Mental Health, Ministry of Health, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Weihua Yue
Affiliations Institute of Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, People's Republic of China, Key Laboratory for Mental Health, Ministry of Health, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Hao Yan
* E-mail: [email protected] (DZ); [email protected] (HY)
Affiliations Institute of Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, People's Republic of China, Key Laboratory for Mental Health, Ministry of Health, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Dai Zhang
* E-mail: [email protected] (DZ); [email protected] (HY)
Affiliations Institute of Mental Health, Peking University, Beijing, People's Republic of China, Key Laboratory for Mental Health, Ministry of Health, Beijing, People's Republic of China
Introduction
Eye movement abnormalities are among the most reproducible physiological dysfunctions associated with schizophrenia [1]–[3]. Particularly, exploratory eye movement (EEM) dysfunction appears to be specific to schizophrenia [4], [5]. EEM is a method to examine the participant's eye tracking while viewing stationary S-shaped figures. When performing the EEM task schizophrenic patients showed fewer eye fixations, longer mean duration of fixation and shorter mean scanning length than controls [6], [7]. In most previous EEM studies, the eye tracking patterns of schizophrenic patients were significantly different from those of normal individuals or patients with non-schizophrenic psychosis, and both the sensitivity and specificity of EEM were higher than 70% for discriminating schizophrenics from non-schizophrenics [4], [8]. Moreover, many studies have identified that the dysfunction patterns of EEM did not improve with relieved clinical symptoms of schizophrenia [6], [9]. Additionally, a 10-cM resolution genome-wide linkage analysis has suggested that the schizophrenia-related quantitative EEM trait was associated with chromosome 22q11.2 [10]. Thus, many investigators have proposed that EEM dysfunction appears to be a useful biological marker for schizophrenia [11]–[13].
The EEM task revealed the schizophrenia-related abnormalities by its 5 commonly used parameters obtained from the eye tracking data analysis, including number of eye fixations (NEF), total eye scanning length (TESL), mean eye scanning length (MESL), responsive search score (RSS) and cognitive search score (CSS). Numerous studies have found that the lower RSS was specific to schizophrenia [2], [12], [13]. RSS is obtained according to the pattern of eye fixation points after the final question, ‘Are there any other differences?’ The RSS abnormalities were only found in patients with schizophrenia [2], [12], [13], and the RSS of schizophrenic patients was significantly lower than that of depressed patients or healthy controls irrespective of geographical location, racial and culture influence [8]. Moreover, RSS impairments were also found in the healthy siblings of schizophrenic patients and therefore, it was thought to be an intermediate phenotype and a vulnerability marker for schizophrenia [14].
It is widely accepted that brain structural impairments and functional disability in schizophrenia could be attributed to its pathological substrates [15], [16]. Most studies investigating the neural substrate of eye movement have pointed that abnormal smooth pursuit and saccadic eye movements in schizophrenia were associated with specific brain structural abnormalities [17]–[19]. However, studies on exploring the underlying neuropathological mechanism of the EEM dysfunction in schizophrenia were relatively sparse. A pioneer study exploring the relationship between the EEM and brain morphology has presented that RSS was significantly related to gray matter density (GMD) in the right frontal eye field (rFEF) and right inferior frontal region in schizophrenia-spectrum patients [20]. The study increased the understanding of EEM abnormalities. However, the mechanism of EEM dysfunction in schizophrenia still needs to be further studied. We were wondering what the relationship between the EEM and brain morphology in healthy individuals is and whether or not the brain structural impairments in schizophrenic patients play a role in their EEM dysfunction.
In the current study, we conducted EEM tasks and high-resolution T1-weighted (MP-RAGE) anatomical magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans in schizophrenic patients and healthy controls to investigate the relationship between RSS and brain morphology by using a voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis.
Methods
Ethics statement
The study was approved by the Medical Research Ethics Committee of the Institute of Mental Health, Peking University. All participants enrolled in the study signed written informed consent.
Participants
Thirty-three schizophrenic patients were recruited from the Institute of Mental Health, Peking University. All the patients satisfied ICD-10 diagnostic criteria for research for schizophrenia with paranoid subtype [21] and the diagnoses were made by two trained and experienced psychiatrists. Participants with epilepsy, mental retardation, severe physical disease, and treated with electroconvulsive therapy within the past 6 months were excluded. All patients received antipsychotic medications during the study. Antipsychotic dose was converted into chlorpromazine-equivalent dose [22]–[24]. The clinical symptoms of the patients were assessed by a trained and experienced psychiatrist with Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) [25]. Twenty-nine healthy controls, without a personal history of psychiatric illness or a family history of schizophrenia spectrum disorders, were recruited from community by advertisement. The schizophrenic patients and the healthy controls were matched for age, gender and years of education. All participants were right-handed and were screened to exclude neurologic disease, substance abuse, metal implant and a history of head injury. MRI scans and EEM tasks were conducted at the same day. Table 1 provided detailed demographic and clinical data.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. Demographic and clinical data of schizophrenic patients and healthy controls.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805.t001
Procedures
EEM data acquisition and processing.
The recording procedure followed that used by Kojima et al. [2]. Each participant sat on a chair 1.5 m in front of a screen. A nac 8-B type Eye Mark Recorder (nac, Tokyo, Japan) and a device that detected corneal reflection of infrared light were equipped. Three stationary horizontal S-shaped figures were projected individually onto the screen, including one original target figure (Figure 1a) and two figures that slightly differed from the target (Figure 1b and Figure 1c). The width of each projected figure was 90 cm, and the height was 75 cm (angle of sight was 33° horizontally and 27.5° vertically). First, each participant was shown the original S-shaped figure (Figure 1a) for 15 seconds. Immediately after viewing it, the participant was asked to draw the target figure from memory (a retention task). Second, the participant was shown a slightly different figure with one bump in a different position (Figure 1b) for 15 seconds; Fifteen seconds later while the figure was still being viewed, the participant was asked if it differed from Figure 1a and, if so, how it differed. After the participant had replied, while still viewing the figure, he was then asked, ‘Are there any other differences?’ (This question was repeated until the participant stated there were no differences.) The second step (a comparison task) was repeated for an S-shaped figure without bumps (Figure 1c). Third, the participant was directed to view the projection of Figure 1a again for 15 seconds and to draw it again. The recordings of eye movements were stored into a video tape recording system and were analyzed with a computerized system.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 1. Figures in the exploratory eye movement task.
(a) The original target figure. (b, c) Two figures that slightly differ from the target.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805.g001
The following 5 parameters were obtained:
1. During the first 15 seconds viewing of Figure 1a, number of eye fixations (NEF), total eye scanning length (TESL) and mean eye scanning length (MESL) were extracted. An eye fixation was defined as a gaze held for more than 200 milliseconds.
2. Responsive search score (RSS): Figure 1b and Figure 1c were each divided into seven sections (Figure 2). The participant was shown Figure 1b for 15 seconds and then was asked to tell the difference from Figure 1a and, if so, how it differed. The number of sections upon which the participant's eyes fixed at least once was counted in the 5 seconds immediately after the final question, ‘Are there any other differences?’ was asked. The procedure was repeated for Figure 1c. The possible total maximum score of RSS was 14.
3. Cognitive search score (CSS): When the participant viewed Figure 1b and Figure 1c for comparing to Figure 1a, the frequency of fixation points focused on the important areas on the figure was recorded. One score was got if the participant viewed any one of important areas of the two figures for three or more times within 15 seconds in the second step mentioned above. The possible total maximum score of CSS was 9. The measurements of RSS and CSS followed that conducted by Kojima et al. [12].
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 2. Figures for responsive search score calculation.
Figure 1. b and c were each divided into seven sections.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805.g002
MRI data acquisition and processing.
Structural MRI scans were obtained at the Department of the Radiology, the Third Hospital, Peking University, with a 3.0-Tesla Magnetom Trio MR system (Siemens Medical System, Erlangen, Germany). Three-dimensional T1-weighted images were acquired in a sagittal orientation employing a 3D-MPRAGE sequence with the following parameters: time repetition = 2350 ms, time echo = 3.44 ms, field of view = 256×256 mm2, flip angle = 7°, 192 sagittal slices, slice thickness = 1 mm, matrix size = 256×256, total acquisition time = 363 seconds. The MRI data were processed with the Statistical Parametric Mapping Software (SPM5, Welcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London; available at http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). A VBM analysis was performed using the VBM5 toolbox (available at http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de), which implements segmentation algorithm from SPM5 and extends the core segmentation algorithm by using the Hidden Markov Random Field (HMRF) approach [26]. A detailed description of processing procedure of the VBM5 toolbox can be found elsewhere [27]. This procedure yielded modulated and unmodulated types of tissues images. It has been suggested that in schizophrenic patients with cortical atrophy [28], modulation processing may not be suitable because it could have widespread effects on voxel signal intensities and may result in less consistent spatial localization of gray matter volume differences from sample to sample [29], [30]. Besides, the analysis of unmodulated data in schizophrenia is fertile [29], [31] and has been successfully conducted to explore the relationship between EEM and brain morphology in schizophrenia [20]. Thus we only used unmodulated data for GMD analysis in the present study. Resulting gray matter images were smoothed with a 12 mm full width-half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel.
Statistical analysis.
To test whether EEM were significantly altered in schizophrenic patients, comparisons of each EEM parameter between two groups was performed using t-test or the Mann-Whitney U-test as appropriate, given that MESL, CSS and RSS did not meet the criteria for normality (Shapiro-Wilk test). Pearson's correlation coefficient was used to identify significant relationships between each EEM parameter and duration of illness and medication respectively. Statistical significance was set at p<0.05 (two-tail). Statistical analysis was carried out with SPSS for windows (SPSS 13.0, SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL, USA).
The images were analyzed within the framework of the general linear model implemented in SPM5. First, to examine the GMD changes in schizophrenic patients, we used an analysis of covariance model with diagnostic group as fixed variable, including age and gender as confounding covariates. The significance level (p<0.05) corrected for multiple comparisons was estimated by combing a height threshold (p<0.001, two-tail, uncorrected) and a extent threshold at the cluster level [cluster extent (k)>620] using the random field theory [32]. Second, the relationship between GMD and RSS in schizophrenic patients and controls, respectively, was examined using a multiple regression model with RSS as independent variable. The multiple regression model for patients included antipsychotic dose, duration of illness, age and gender as confounding covariates; and the multiple regression model for controls included age and gender as confounding covariates. The significance level (p<0.05) corrected for multiple comparisons was estimated by combing a height threshold (p<0.01, one-tail, uncorrected) and a extent threshold at the cluster level (k>1683) using the random field theory [32]. Finally, to explore whether the deficits in gray matter was associated with the impaired RSS in schizophrenic patients, an overlap map was generated by calculating the intersection of the two thresholded statistical parameter maps. The results in the overlap mapping were saved to files and imported into the MarsBar toolbox (http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/) and the mean value of GMD of each region was calculated for each participant. Scatter plots of the relationships between RSS and the mean values of GMD were created using SPSS for windows.
Results
EEM results
Compared to healthy controls, schizophrenic patients showed significant decreases in RSS and CSS (see Table 2). There were no significant differences in the other three parameters of EEM (NEF, TESL and MESL) between groups. In addition, no eye movement parameters significantly correlated with antipsychotic dose and duration of illness respectively in schizophrenic patients (see Table S1).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 2. Comparison of eye movement parameters between schizophrenic patients and healthy controls.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805.t002
MRI Results
Morphology Analysis.
Compared to healthy controls, schizophrenic patients showed significant GMD reductions in widespread brain areas such as the right middle frontal cortex, the right precuneus, the right cuneus, the right cerebellum, the left calcarine cortex, the left middle occipital cortex, the left superior temporal cortex, the left inferior temporal cortex, the left superior parietal cortex, the left paracentral lobule, bilateral lingual gyri and fusiform gyri (Figure 3a) (see Table S2). There were no significant GMD increases in schizophrenic patients compared to healthy controls.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 3. Surface renderings representing the results from the analysis of MRI data.
Statistical parametric maps were superimposed on the population, landmarked-and surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex [73] using CARET software (http://brainvis.wustl.edu). Color bar indicates the t value. (a) Voxel-based morphometry analysis. Compared with healthy controls, regions with significantly (p<0.05, corrected) decreased gray matter density (GMD) in schizophrenic patients were shown in cool color. (b) Regression analysis of GMD with responsive search score (RSS) using antipsychotic dose, duration of illness, age and gender as confounding covariates in schizophrenic patients. Regions with significantly (p<0.05, corrected) positive correlations with RSS in schizophrenic patients were shown in warm color. (c) The result of overlap mapping was shown in yellow. Top row, left medial view; second row, left lateral view; third row, right medial view; fourth row, right lateral view.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805.g003
Regression Analysis.
In schizophrenic patients, RSS was positively correlated with GMD in the right middle frontal cortex, the right inferior temporal cortex, the right superior occipital cortex, the right cerebellum (lobule VII b), the left calcrine cortex, the left inferior occipital cortex, bilateral precentral gyri extending to the frontal eye fields (FEF, BA 8), cerebellum (crus I) and supplementary motor areas (Figure 3b) (see Table S3). No significantly positive correlation between RSS and GMD was found in healthy controls.
Overlap Mapping.
In schizophrenic patients, overlap mapping revealed a number of brain regions not only with GMD reductions but also associated with RSS, such as the left calcarine cortex (BA 17), the left cuneus (BA 18), the left superior occipital cortex (BA 18/19), the left superior frontal gyrus (SFG, BA 6), the left cerebellum (crus I), the right lingual cortex (BA 17/18), the right middle occipital cortex (BA19), the right inferior temporal cortex (BA 37), the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (BA 46) and bilateral precentral gyri (BA 6) extending to the FEF (BA8) (Figure 3c, Table 3). Scatter plots of the relationships between RSS and the mean values of GMD for several representative overlap regions were shown in Figure 4.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 4. Representative scatter plots of the gray matter density (GMD) of overlap regions against responsive search score (RSS) in schizophrenic patients (SZ) and healthy controls (HC) with trend lines.
Correlation coefficient (r) was used to assess the significance of a correlation (p<0.05). GMD values of the six overlap regions were significantly correlated with RSS in SZ, whereas there was no significant correlation between RSS and GMD in HC. Red lines for SZ and blue lines for HC. L, left; R, right; ITC, inferior temporal cortex; PC, precentral cortex; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; CC, calcarine cortex.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805.g004
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 3. Overlap mapping in schizophrenic patients.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805.t003
Discussion
The principal finding of this study was that lower RSS in schizophrenic patients was significantly associated with lower GMD in representative components of neural networks implicated in eye movement control and visual information processing, such as the bilateral BA 17, BA 18 and BA 19, bilateral precentral gyri (BA 6) extending to the FEF (BA 8), the right middle occipital cortex, inferior temporal cortex and DLPFC, the left SFG (BA 6) and left cerebellum (crus I). To our knowledge, we firstly reported empirical evidence that gray matter loss in the occipito-tempro-frontal neuroanatomical circuitry of visual processing system was associated with EEM performance in schizophrenia, which may be helpful for the future effort to reveal the underlying neural mechanisms for EEM disturbance in schizophrenia.
Low RSS in schizophrenia
We found that schizophrenic patients showed lower RSS than healthy controls, in line with previous studies [2], [6], [8]. As an important parameter, RSS was based on the frequency of eye fixations on each section of a figure that occurred within 5 seconds immediately after the final question, ‘Are there any other differences?’ This search process could be accommodated within a visuooculomotor model [33]. First, a phase of perception (i.e., visual stimulus of the S-shaped figures), involving both the visual (occipital) and attentional (parietal) areas; Second, a phase of memorization, under the control of the cortical area actually involved in spatial working memory (corresponding to visuospatial integration); third, a phase of eye movement, under the control of the frontal and parietal motor areas. Therefore, the RSS was a measure of the ability for fine discrimination (i.e., figuring out the differences among three S-shaped figures), and, according to the abovementioned three phases, also reflected abilities to handle a series of processes, such as attention, perception, memory and execution. Thus, we speculated that the lower RSS at least partially resulted from the impairments of one or more of the abovementioned abilities in schizophrenia. Furthermore, obtaining RSS implicated a verbal communication during which ability of language comprehension was also needed. Therefore, RSS could also be an objective parameter for assessing the ability of personal communication, and decreased RSS might reflect the specific symptom of poor interpersonal communication in schizophrenics [2], [8]. In addition, consistent with previous reports [9], we also found that RSS showed no significant correlation with antipsychotic dose and duration of illness, which indicated that RSS was stable in schizophrenia and should be a trait-related deficit but not state-related deficit.
Neuroanatomical circuitry underlying EEM
The ventral and dorsal visual-processing pathway.
Our overlap mapping revealed that neuroanatomical deficits and EEM disturbances were associated in schizophrenic patients, i.e. some brain regions with gray matter loss were significantly positively correlated with RSS. Those brain regions included bilateral BA 17, BA 18, BA 19, and the right inferior temporal cortex. Most visual information arrives in the cortex via connections from the lateral geniculate nucleus to BA17 (also known as primary visual cortex, V1), from which two main pathways can be distinguished between areas of visual cortex: a dorsal (occipital-parietal) stream that reaches the posterior parietal cortex and a ventral (occipital-temporal) stream that goes to the inferior temporal region [34], [35]. Our current findings presented decreased GMD in BA17 (V1) and several areas involved in the ventral stream such as BA 18 (V2) and the inferior temporal cortex. Previous studies have reported that V1 is absolutely necessary for most visual functions, and lesions in V1 weakened the capacity for fine visual discrimination [36], [37]. Damage along the ventral visual stream was reported to result in several disturbances in visual recognition and understanding [38]. For instance, lesions in the inferior temporal cortex, which played a critical role in fine object recognition and discrimination tasks [39], [40], could lead to impairment in object shape recognition [41]. The overlap areas also included the right middle occipital cortices (BA19) and the left superior occipital cortex (BA 18/19), which were approximately corresponsive to V2 and V3 of the dorsal stream [35], [38], [42]. The dorsal stream has been documented with functions of visuomotor integration and spatial processing [43]. Furthermore, the middle occipital cortex was documented to associate with pursuit command, spatial position information and eye movement processing [44], [45]. O'Donnell et al. [46] reported that schizophrenia may be accompanied by impaired visual spatial perception and representation and suggested the deficits in trajectory discrimination might reflect a disturbance of the dorsal pathway of the visual system in schizophrenia. In fact, a growing body of evidence has documented deficits in perceptual discrimination of object form and spatial location in schizophrenic patients and, as our findings suggested, these perceptual deficits could be resulted from a relatively specific deficit in the ventral and dorsal visual-processing stream [46]–[49]. Besides, there are numerous interconnections, both forward and backward, between ventral and dorsal pathways, integrating the two pathways into a neural circuitry involved in processing visual information [38]. Thus, the disturbances in the neural circuitry of visual processing system may be associated with the EEM dysfunction in schizophrenia.
There was a concern whether decreased RSS was due to the decreased visual acuity in schizophrenic patients. It should be mentioned that each participant recruited in the present study could meet the basic visual demand of daily life and was able to complete the standard EEM task. Therefore, the changes of visual acuity could not account for decreased RSS in patients in the current study.
The frontal cortices.
Our data clearly demonstrated that some prefrontal cortices, including the right DLPFC (BA 46) and the left SFG (BA 6), were brain areas with GMD reductions associated with RSS decreases. Eye movement dysfunctions in schizophrenia were reportedly concerned with higher-order cognition-related areas [50], [51], among which the prefrontal cortices are likely involved in task-dependent interpretation of visual input to orchestrate cognitive decisions [52]. For instance, Bagary et al. [17] demonstrated that oculomotor dysfunction in schizophrenic patients correlated with abnormal gray matter in DLPFC. Courtney et al. [53] suggested that the SFG was specialized for spatial working memory, which was found to be associated with impaired oculomotor task performance in schizophrenic patients [54]. In addition, a transcranial magnetic stimulation study also showed that the SFG was involved in visual-spatial working memory task, whereas the DLPFC interfered with both visual-object and visual-spatial information in humans [55]. As aforementioned, the procedure of obtaining RSS implicated attention, working memory, as well as eye movement control, during which the DLPFC and SFG may be recruited. Thus, gray matter loss in the right DLPFC and the left SFG may be implicated with the dysfunction of EEM in schizophrenic patients.
The bilateral precentral gyri (BA6) extending to the FEF (BA 8) was another finding in our overlap mapping analysis. The FEF was a principal region involved in oculomotor control and maintenance of eye fixation [56], [57]. Many lesion and functional neuroimaging studies emphasized that the FEF was concerned primarily with the visual attention, the actual triggering and execution of eye movements in human [58], [59]. The FEF lesions have been found to impair the oculomotor tasks requiring exploration or orientation towards visual stimuli [60]. In addition, the FEF may be particularly important in a series of eye movement tasks like smooth pursuit eye movement, saccade eye movement and EEM in schizophrenia [20], [50], [51]. Tsunoda et al. [20] found that the RSS was positively correlated with GMD in the FEF in schizophrenia spectrum patients and suggested that the FEF-related neural network dysfunction may be associated with the systematic intentional exploration of space and contribute to the EEM disturbance in patients. In this study, we replicated their findings on the relationship between the FEF impairment and EEM dysfunction in schizophrenic patients.
Taken together, the cerebral cortical areas observed in our overlap mapping were compatible with a recent study that schizophrenia showed severe functional deficits across a distributed network of sensory and higher-order cognitive brain regions in visual object processing [61]. Although the causal relationship between gray mater changes and EEM abnormalities is still unclear, according to our findings, we speculated that neuroanatomical deficits in the dorsal and ventral pathways, and ‘high-order’ frontal areas might induce a dysintegration of the occipito-tempro-frontal circuitry and dysfunction within this circuitry may underlie the disturbances of EEM in schizophrenia.
The cerebellum.
Our findings also showed the GMD of cerebellum (crusI, lobule VII b) was associated with EEM in schizophrenic patients. The cerebellum is commonly regarded as an organ that subserves coordination, balance, and fine motor control [62]. Moreover, its cortex and deep cerebellar nuclei play a crucial role in supporting the accuracy and adaptation of voluntary eye movements [63]. Previous studies gave evidence that multiple cerebellar structures are involved in the control of eye movements, among which the oculomotor vermis (lobuli VI and VII) are especially well documented [63]. The vermis lesions led to the disruption of the timing, accuracy and dynamics of saccades, and the ability to maintain eye fixation as well [64], [65]. Furthermore, neural cells located in the crus I and II were recorded to discharge related to eye movements and visual stimuli in monkeys during a visually guided eye movement tracking task [66]. In human, lesions of the lateral cerebellar hemisphere may affect the smooth pursuit eye movement [67]. In addition, lateral cerebellar hemisphere (particularly in the crus I and II) involved in human executive network and played an important role in executive functions, such as reasoning and working memory, which were indispensable in EEM as well [68], [69]. Hence, besides the cerebral neuroanatomical circuitry of visual processing system aforementioned, the cerebellum impairment may be also implicated with the EEM dysfunction in schizophrenic patients.
Findings in healthy controls.
Another goal of the present case-control study was to explore the relationship pattern between RSS and GMD in healthy individuals. We found no significantly positive correlation between RSS and GMD in controls, which may be due to the relatively centralized RSS as showed from the lower standard deviation in controls. Previous studies have suggested that multiple cortical areas such as frontal eye fields, the middle temporal, brain stem and cerebellum are all human eye-movement-related structures [63], [70]. Moreover, a PET study indicated a network of brain regions including frontal eye fields, supplementary eye field, V5 and medial cuneus was activated during smooth pursuit and saccade eye movements in healthy individuals [71]. However, the neurobiological pathways implicating EEM in healthy individuals is still unclear. It is required to integrate multiple research techniques, e.g. functional MRI and PET, to address this question in the future studies.
Limitations and considerations
The present study has several limitations and considerations. First, patients in our study had relatively discrete durations of illness (from 8.18 to 74.36 months) and were all exposed to antipsychotic medication. The exposure to antipsychotic medication may impact brain structure [72], which was reflected in our data as well (see Figure S1, Table S4). Although we included antipsychotic dose and duration of illness as nuisance covariates in the regression model for schizophrenic patients, their underlying effects on brain structure cannot be removed thoroughly. Second, the RSS has been found to be correlated positively with the performance Intelligence Quotient (IQ) and the Wechsler's Maze test in schizophrenic patients [2]. However, we did not include IQ or other measurements of cognitive function as control factors in the current study. Though without IQ, a measure of general cognitive function, all participants in the current study were at least junior high school graduates and the education levels were well matched between groups. In addition, patients with mental retardation were excluded from our study. Therefore, we inferred that the intelligence level of all the participants might be normal. Now the effects of the cognitive function on the EEM performance were still unclear. Thus, it will be helpful to elucidate the relationship between the cognitive function and EEM by collecting data from multiple dimensions of cognition and using multiple regression analysis to examine the relationship between the cognitive measurements and EEM performance in future study. Additionally, schizophrenia is a highly genetic disease and EEM is an intermediate phenotype of schizophrenia. Thus, it would be of particular interest to consider the relationship between EEM and brain structure in unaffected relatives of schizophrenic patients in future work.
Conclusions
In summary, decreased RSS and widespread gray matter loss were observed in schizophrenic patients. Moreover, we found that decreased RSS in schizophrenic patients was correlated with decreased GMD in several brain areas in a distributed occipito-tempro-frontal circuitry involved in visual information processing and eye movement control. These findings revealed an association between gray matter reductions and EEM abnormalities, which may help to reveal the underlying neural mechanisms for EEM disturbances in schizophrenia in the future study. The present work may well provide a new underpin for the further research to evaluate and verify the EEM as a biological marker for schizophrenia.
Supporting Information
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure S1.
Surface renderings representing the results from the analysis of MRI data. Statistical parametric maps of regression analysis in schizophrenic patients was superimposed on the population, landmarked-and surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex [73] using CARET software (http://brainvis.wustl.edu). In the regression analysis, chlorpromazine-equivalent antipsychotic dose was used as independent variable and gray matter density within each voxel as dependent variable, including age and gender as confounding covariates. Regions with significantly (p<0.01, corrected) negative correlations with RSS in schizophrenic patients were shown in cool color. Color bar indicates the t value.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805.s001
(TIF)
Table S1.
The relationship between parameters of EEM and antipsychotic dose and duration of illness in schizophrenic patients.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805.s002
(DOC)
Table S2.
Brain regions with decreased gray matter density in schizophrenic patients.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805.s003
(DOC)
Table S3.
Brain regions with a significant positive correlation between responsive search score and gray matter density in schizophrenic patients.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805.s004
(DOC)
Table S4.
Brain regions with a significant negative correlation between antipsychotic medication and gray matter density in schizophrenic patients.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805.s005
(DOC)
Acknowledgments
The authors thank all participants for their cooperation in our study.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: LQ LT WY DZ. Performed the experiments: LQ LT RZ QZ HSY HY JY. Analyzed the data: LQ LT YH DZ HY. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: LQ LT RZ HY CP QL JY WY YH DZ. Wrote the paper: LQ LT HY.
Citation: Qiu L, Tian L, Pan C, Zhu R, Liu Q, Yan J, et al. (2011) Neuroanatomical Circuitry Associated with Exploratory Eye Movement in Schizophrenia: A Voxel-Based Morphometric Study. PLoS ONE6(10): e25805. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025805
1. Matsue Y, Okuma T, Saito H, Aneha S, Ueno T, et al. (1986) Saccadic Eye-Movements in Tracking, Fixation, and Rest in Schizophrenic and Normal Participants. Biol Psychiatry 21: 382–389.Y. MatsueT. OkumaH. SaitoS. AnehaT. Ueno1986Saccadic Eye-Movements in Tracking, Fixation, and Rest in Schizophrenic and Normal Participants.Biol Psychiatry21382389
2. Kojima T, Matsushima E, Ando K, Ando H, Sakurada M, et al. (1992) Exploratory Eye-Movements and Neuropsychological Tests in Schizophrenic-Patients. Schizophr Bull 18: 85–94.T. KojimaE. MatsushimaK. AndoH. AndoM. Sakurada1992Exploratory Eye-Movements and Neuropsychological Tests in Schizophrenic-Patients.Schizophr Bull188594
3. Levy DL, Holzman PS, Matthysse S, Mendell NR (1993) Eye tracking dysfunction and schizophrenia: a critical perspective. Schizophr Bull 19: 461–536.DL LevyPS HolzmanS. MatthysseNR Mendell1993Eye tracking dysfunction and schizophrenia: a critical perspective.Schizophr Bull19461536
4. Suzuki M, Takahashi S, Matsushima E, Tsunoda M, Kurachi M, et al. (2009) Exploratory eye movement dysfunction as a discriminator for schizophrenia: a large sample study using a newly developed digital computerized system. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 259: 186–194.M. SuzukiS. TakahashiE. MatsushimaM. TsunodaM. Kurachi2009Exploratory eye movement dysfunction as a discriminator for schizophrenia: a large sample study using a newly developed digital computerized system.Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci259186194
5. Matsushima E, Kojima T, Ohta K, Obayashi S, Nakajima K, et al. (1998) Exploratory eye movement dysfunctions in patients with schizophrenia: possibility as a discriminator for schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res 32: 289–295.E. MatsushimaT. KojimaK. OhtaS. ObayashiK. Nakajima1998Exploratory eye movement dysfunctions in patients with schizophrenia: possibility as a discriminator for schizophrenia.J Psychiatr Res32289295
6. Kojima T, Matsushima E, Nakajima K, Shiraishi H, Ando K, et al. (1990) Eye-Movements in Acute, Chronic, and Remitted Schizophrenics. Biol Psychiatry 27: 975–989.T. KojimaE. MatsushimaK. NakajimaH. ShiraishiK. Ando1990Eye-Movements in Acute, Chronic, and Remitted Schizophrenics.Biol Psychiatry27975989
7. Matsushima E, Kojima T, Ohbayashi S, Ando H, Ando K, et al. (1992) Exploratory Eye-Movements in Schizophrenic-Patients and Patients with Frontal-Lobe Lesions. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 241: 210–214.E. MatsushimaT. KojimaS. OhbayashiH. AndoK. Ando1992Exploratory Eye-Movements in Schizophrenic-Patients and Patients with Frontal-Lobe Lesions.Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci241210214
8. Kojima T, Matsushima E, Ohta K, Toru M, Han YH, et al. (2001) Stability of exploratory eye movements as a marker of schizophrenia - a WHO multi-center study. Schizophr Res 52: 203–213.T. KojimaE. MatsushimaK. OhtaM. ToruYH Han2001Stability of exploratory eye movements as a marker of schizophrenia - a WHO multi-center study.Schizophr Res52203213
9. Obayashi S, Matsushima E, Okubo Y, Ohkura T, Kojima T, et al. (2001) Relationship between exploratory eye movements and clinical course in schizophrenic patients. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 251: 211–216.S. ObayashiE. MatsushimaY. OkuboT. OhkuraT. Kojima2001Relationship between exploratory eye movements and clinical course in schizophrenic patients.Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci251211216
10. Takahashi S, Ohtsuki T, Yu SY, Tanabe E, Yara K, et al. (2003) Significant linkage to chromosome 22q for exploratory eye movement dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 123B: 27–32.S. TakahashiT. OhtsukiSY YuE. TanabeK. Yara2003Significant linkage to chromosome 22q for exploratory eye movement dysfunction in schizophrenia.Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet123B2732
11. Kojima T, Potkin SG, Kharazmi M, Matsushima E, Herrera J, et al. (1989) Limited Eye-Movement Patterns in Chronic-Schizophrenic Patients. Psychiatr Res 28: 307–314.T. KojimaSG PotkinM. KharazmiE. MatsushimaJ. Herrera1989Limited Eye-Movement Patterns in Chronic-Schizophrenic Patients.Psychiatr Res28307314
12. Matsushima E, Kojima T, Ohta K, Obayashi S, Nakajima K, et al. (1998) Exploratory eye movement dysfunctions in patients with schizophrenia: possibility as a discriminator for schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res 32: 289–295.E. MatsushimaT. KojimaK. OhtaS. ObayashiK. Nakajima1998Exploratory eye movement dysfunctions in patients with schizophrenia: possibility as a discriminator for schizophrenia.J Psychiatr Res32289295
13. Suzuki M, Takahashi S, Matsushima E, Tsunoda M, Kurachi M, et al. (2009) Exploratory eye movement dysfunction as a discriminator for schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 259: 186–194.M. SuzukiS. TakahashiE. MatsushimaM. TsunodaM. Kurachi2009Exploratory eye movement dysfunction as a discriminator for schizophrenia.Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci259186194
14. Takahashi S, Tanabe E, Yara K, Matsuura M, Matsushima E, et al. (2008) Impairment of exploratory eye movement in schizophrenia patients and their siblings. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 62: 487–493.S. TakahashiE. TanabeK. YaraM. MatsuuraE. Matsushima2008Impairment of exploratory eye movement in schizophrenia patients and their siblings.Psychiatry Clin Neurosci62487493
15. Keshavan MS, Tandon R, Boutros NN, Nasrallah HA (2008) Schizophrenia, “just the facts”: what we know in 2008 Part 3: neurobiology. Schizophr Res 106: 89–107.MS KeshavanR. TandonNN BoutrosHA Nasrallah2008Schizophrenia, “just the facts”: what we know in 2008 Part 3: neurobiology.Schizophr Res10689107
16. Wright IC, Rabe-Hesketh S, Woodruff PW, David AS, Murray RM, et al. (2000) Meta-analysis of regional brain volumes in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 157: 16–25.IC WrightS. Rabe-HeskethPW WoodruffAS DavidRM Murray2000Meta-analysis of regional brain volumes in schizophrenia.Am J Psychiatry1571625
17. Bagary MS, Hutton SB, Symms MR, Barker GJ, Mutsatsa SH, et al. (2004) Structural neural networks subserving oculomotor function in first-episode schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 56: 620–627.MS BagarySB HuttonMR SymmsGJ BarkerSH Mutsatsa2004Structural neural networks subserving oculomotor function in first-episode schizophrenia.Biol Psychiatry56620627
18. Katsanis J, Iacono WG (1991) Clinical, Neuropsychological, and Brain Structural Correlates of Smooth-Pursuit Eye Tracking Performance in Chronic-Schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 100: 526–534.J. KatsanisWG Iacono1991Clinical, Neuropsychological, and Brain Structural Correlates of Smooth-Pursuit Eye Tracking Performance in Chronic-Schizophrenia.J Abnorm Psychol100526534
19. Raemaekers M, Jansma JM, Cahn W, Van der Geest JN, van der Linden JA, et al. (2002) Neuronal substrate of the saccadic inhibition deficit in schizophrenia investigated with 3-dimensional event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59: 313–320.M. RaemaekersJM JansmaW. CahnJN Van der GeestJA van der Linden2002Neuronal substrate of the saccadic inhibition deficit in schizophrenia investigated with 3-dimensional event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging.Arch Gen Psychiatry59313320
20. Tsunoda M, Kawasaki Y, Matsui M, Tonoya Y, Hagino H, et al. (2005) Relationship between exploratory eye movements and brain morphology in schizophrenia spectrum patients. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 255: 104–110.M. TsunodaY. KawasakiM. MatsuiY. TonoyaH. Hagino2005Relationship between exploratory eye movements and brain morphology in schizophrenia spectrum patients.Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci255104110
21. World Health Organization (1993) The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva: World Health Organization. World Health Organization1993The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: diagnostic criteria for researchGenevaWorld Health Organizationxiii, 248. xiii, 248.
22. Lehman AF, Steinwachs DM, Project C-IP (1998) At issue: Translating research into practice: The Schizophrenia Patient Outcomes Research Team (PORT) treatment recommendations. Schizophrenia Bulletin 24: 1–10.AF LehmanDM SteinwachsC-IP Project1998At issue: Translating research into practice: The Schizophrenia Patient Outcomes Research Team (PORT) treatment recommendations.Schizophrenia Bulletin24110
23. Woods SW (2003) Chlorpromazine equivalent doses for the newer atypical antipsychotics. J Clin Psychiatry 64: 663–667.SW Woods2003Chlorpromazine equivalent doses for the newer atypical antipsychotics.J Clin Psychiatry64663667
24. Bai YM, Chen TT, Chen JY, Chang WH, Wu B, et al. (2007) Equivalent switching dose from oral risperidone to risperidone long-acting injection: A 48-week randomized, prospective, single-blind pharmacokinetic study. J Clin Psychiatry 68: 1218–1225.YM BaiTT ChenJY ChenWH ChangB. Wu2007Equivalent switching dose from oral risperidone to risperidone long-acting injection: A 48-week randomized, prospective, single-blind pharmacokinetic study.J Clin Psychiatry6812181225
25. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA (1987) The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (Panss) for Schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 13: 261–276.SR KayA. FiszbeinLA Opler1987The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (Panss) for Schizophrenia.Schizophr Bull13261276
26. Cuadra MB, Cammoun L, Butz T, Cuisenaire O, Thiran JP (2005) Comparison and validation of tissue modelization and statistical classification methods in T1-weighted MR brain images. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 24: 1548–1565.MB CuadraL. CammounT. ButzO. CuisenaireJP Thiran2005Comparison and validation of tissue modelization and statistical classification methods in T1-weighted MR brain images.IEEE Trans Med Imaging2415481565
27. Meisenzahl EM, Koutsouleris N, Gaser C, Bottlender R, Schmitt GJE, et al. (2008) Structural brain alterations in participants at high-risk of psychosis: A voxel-based morphometric study. Schizophr Res 102: 150–162.EM MeisenzahlN. KoutsoulerisC. GaserR. BottlenderGJE Schmitt2008Structural brain alterations in participants at high-risk of psychosis: A voxel-based morphometric study.Schizophr Res102150162
28. Coffman JA, Mefferd J, Golden CJ, Bloch S, Graber B (1981) Cerebellar atrophy in chronic schizophrenia. Lancet 1: 666.JA CoffmanJ. MefferdCJ GoldenS. BlochB. Graber1981Cerebellar atrophy in chronic schizophrenia.Lancet1666
29. Eckert MA, Tenforde A, Galaburda AM, Bellugi U, Korenberg JR, et al. (2006) To modulate or not to modulate: differing results in uniquely shaped Williams syndrome brains. Neuroimage 32: 1001–1007.MA EckertA. TenfordeAM GalaburdaU. BellugiJR Korenberg2006To modulate or not to modulate: differing results in uniquely shaped Williams syndrome brains.Neuroimage3210011007
30. Fornito A, Yucel M, Patti J, Wood SJ, Pantelis C (2009) Mapping grey matter reductions in schizophrenia: an anatomical likelihood estimation analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Schizophr Res 108: 104–113.A. FornitoM. YucelJ. PattiSJ WoodC. Pantelis2009Mapping grey matter reductions in schizophrenia: an anatomical likelihood estimation analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies.Schizophr Res108104113
31. Miyata J, Hirao K, Namiki C, Fujiwara H, Shimizu M, et al. (2009) Reduced white matter integrity correlated with cortico-subcortical gray matter deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 111: 78–85.J. MiyataK. HiraoC. NamikiH. FujiwaraM. Shimizu2009Reduced white matter integrity correlated with cortico-subcortical gray matter deficits in schizophrenia.Schizophr Res1117885
32. Hayasaka S, Phan KL, Liberzon I, Worsley KJ, Nichols TE (2004) Nonstationary cluster-size inference with random field and permutation methods. Neuroimage 22: 676–687.S. HayasakaKL PhanI. LiberzonKJ WorsleyTE Nichols2004Nonstationary cluster-size inference with random field and permutation methods.Neuroimage22676687
33. Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Muri RM, Rivaud-Pechoux S, Gaymard B, Ploner CJ (2002) Cortical control of spatial memory in humans: the visuooculomotor model. Ann Neurol 52: 10–19.C. Pierrot-DeseillignyRM MuriS. Rivaud-PechouxB. GaymardCJ Ploner2002Cortical control of spatial memory in humans: the visuooculomotor model.Ann Neurol521019
34. Nassi JJ, Callaway EM (2009) Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 360–372.JJ NassiEM Callaway2009Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system.Nat Rev Neurosci10360372
35. Tootell RBH, Hadjikhani NK, Mendola JD, Marrett S, Dale AM (1998) From retinotopy to recognition: fMRI in human visual cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 2: 174–183.RBH TootellNK HadjikhaniJD MendolaS. MarrettAM Dale1998From retinotopy to recognition: fMRI in human visual cortex.Trends Cogn Sci2174183
36. Zeki S, Ffytche DH (1998) The Riddoch syndrome: insights into the neurobiology of conscious vision. Brain 121: 25–45.S. ZekiDH Ffytche1998The Riddoch syndrome: insights into the neurobiology of conscious vision.Brain1212545
37. Weiskrantz L (1996) Blindsight revisited. Curr Opin Neurobiol 6: 215–220.L. Weiskrantz1996Blindsight revisited.Curr Opin Neurobiol6215220
38. Blumberg J, Kreiman G (2010) How cortical neurons help us see: visual recognition in the human brain. J Clin Invest 120: 3054–3063.J. BlumbergG. Kreiman2010How cortical neurons help us see: visual recognition in the human brain.J Clin Invest12030543063
39. Mishkin M (1954) Visual Discrimination Performance Following Partial Ablations of the Temporal Lobe. 2. Ventral Surface Vs Hippocampus. J Comp Physiol Psychol 47: 187–193.M. Mishkin1954Visual Discrimination Performance Following Partial Ablations of the Temporal Lobe. 2. Ventral Surface Vs Hippocampus.J Comp Physiol Psychol47187193
40. Logothetis NK, Sheinberg DL (1996) Visual object recognition. Ann Rev Neurosci 19: 577–621.NK LogothetisDL Sheinberg1996Visual object recognition.Ann Rev Neurosci19577621
41. Aggleton JP, Mishkin M (1990) Visual impairments in macaques following inferior temporal lesions are exacerbated selectively by additional damage to superior temporal sulcus. Behav Brain Res 39: 262–274.JP AggletonM. Mishkin1990Visual impairments in macaques following inferior temporal lesions are exacerbated selectively by additional damage to superior temporal sulcus.Behav Brain Res39262274
42. Goebel R, Khorram-Sefat D, Muckli L, Hacker H, Singer W (1998) The constructive nature of vision: direct evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of apparent motion and motion imagery. Eur J Neurosci 10: 1563–1573.R. GoebelD. Khorram-SefatL. MuckliH. HackerW. Singer1998The constructive nature of vision: direct evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of apparent motion and motion imagery.Eur J Neurosci1015631573
43. Gegeshidze K, Tsagareli MG (2004) Influence of emotional words on human visual recognition and brain asymmetry. World J Biol Psychiatry 5: 26–32.K. GegeshidzeMG Tsagareli2004Influence of emotional words on human visual recognition and brain asymmetry.World J Biol Psychiatry52632
44. Yan XH, Lin XM, Wang QF, Zhang YC, Chen YM, et al. (2010) Dorsal Visual Pathway Changes in Patients with Comitant Extropia. Plos One 3;5(6): e10931.XH YanXM LinQF WangYC ZhangYM Chen2010Dorsal Visual Pathway Changes in Patients with Comitant Extropia.Plos One3;56e10931
45. Barton JJS, Simpson T, Kiriakopoulos E, Stewart C, Crawley A, et al. (1996) Functional MRI of lateral occipitotemporal cortex during pursuit and motion perception. Ann Neurol 40: 387–398.JJS BartonT. SimpsonE. KiriakopoulosC. StewartA. Crawley1996Functional MRI of lateral occipitotemporal cortex during pursuit and motion perception.Ann Neurol40387398
46. O'Donnell BF, Swearer JM, Smith LT, Nestor PG, Shenton ME, et al. (1996) Selective deficits in visual perception and recognition in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 153: 687–692.BF O'DonnellJM SwearerLT SmithPG NestorME Shenton1996Selective deficits in visual perception and recognition in schizophrenia.Am J Psychiatry153687692
47. Braff DL (1993) Information processing and attention dysfunctions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 19: 233–259.DL Braff1993Information processing and attention dysfunctions in schizophrenia.Schizophr Bull19233259
48. Cadenhead KS, Geyer MA, Butler RW, Perry W, Sprock J, et al. (1997) Information processing deficits of schizophrenia patients: relationship to clinical ratings, gender and medication status. Schizophr Res 28: 51–62.KS CadenheadMA GeyerRW ButlerW. PerryJ. Sprock1997Information processing deficits of schizophrenia patients: relationship to clinical ratings, gender and medication status.Schizophr Res285162
49. Tek C, Gold J, Blaxton T, Wilk C, McMahon RP, et al. (2002) Visual perceptual and working memory impairments in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59: 146–153.C. TekJ. GoldT. BlaxtonC. WilkRP McMahon2002Visual perceptual and working memory impairments in schizophrenia.Arch Gen Psychiatry59146153
50. Tregellas JR, Tanabe JL, Miller DE, Ross RG, Olincy A, et al. (2004) Neurobiology of smooth pursuit eye movement deficits in schizophrenia: an fMRI study. Am J Psychiatry 161: 315–321.JR TregellasJL TanabeDE MillerRG RossA. Olincy2004Neurobiology of smooth pursuit eye movement deficits in schizophrenia: an fMRI study.Am J Psychiatry161315321
51. Levy DL, Sereno AB, Gooding DC, O'Driscoll GA (2010) Eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia: characterization and pathophysiology. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 4: 311–347.DL LevyAB SerenoDC GoodingGA O'Driscoll2010Eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia: characterization and pathophysiology.Curr Top Behav Neurosci4311347
52. Miller EK (2000) The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nat Rev Neurosci 1: 59–65.EK Miller2000The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control.Nat Rev Neurosci15965
53. Courtney SM, Petit L, Maisog JM, Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV (1998) An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex. Science 279: 1347–1351.SM CourtneyL. PetitJM MaisogLG UngerleiderJV Haxby1998An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex.Science27913471351
54. Park S, Holzman PS (1992) Schizophrenics Show Spatial Working Memory Deficits. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49: 975–982.S. ParkPS Holzman1992Schizophrenics Show Spatial Working Memory Deficits.Arch Gen Psychiatry49975982
55. Oliveri M, Turriziani P, Carlesimo GA, Koch G, Tomaiuolo F, et al. (2001) Parieto-frontal interactions in visual-object and visual-spatial working memory: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cereb Cortex 11: 606–618.M. OliveriP. TurrizianiGA CarlesimoG. KochF. Tomaiuolo2001Parieto-frontal interactions in visual-object and visual-spatial working memory: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation.Cereb Cortex11606618
56. Schall JD, Hanes DP (1993) Neural basis of saccade target selection in frontal eye field during visual search. Nature 366: 467–469.JD SchallDP Hanes1993Neural basis of saccade target selection in frontal eye field during visual search.Nature366467469
57. Bense S, Stephan T, Yousry TA, Brandt T, Dieterich M (2001) Multisensory cortical signal increases and decreases during vestibular galvanic stimulation (fMRI). J Neurophysiol 85: 886–899.S. BenseT. StephanTA YousryT. BrandtM. Dieterich2001Multisensory cortical signal increases and decreases during vestibular galvanic stimulation (fMRI).J Neurophysiol85886899
58. Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Rivaud S, Gaymard B, Agid Y (1991) Cortical control of memory-guided saccades in man. Exp Brain Res 83: 607–617.C. Pierrot-DeseillignyS. RivaudB. GaymardY. Agid1991Cortical control of memory-guided saccades in man.Exp Brain Res83607617
59. Schall JD (2004) On the role of frontal eye field in guiding attention and saccades. Vision Res 44: 1453–1467.JD Schall2004On the role of frontal eye field in guiding attention and saccades.Vision Res4414531467
60. Paus T (1996) Location and function of the human frontal eye-field: a selective review. Neuropsychologia 34: 475–483.T. Paus1996Location and function of the human frontal eye-field: a selective review.Neuropsychologia34475483
61. Sehatpour P, Dias EC, Butler PD, Revheim N, Guilfoyle DN, et al. (2010) Impaired visual object processing across an occipital-frontal-hippocampal brain network in schizophrenia: an integrated neuroimaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67: 772–782.P. SehatpourEC DiasPD ButlerN. RevheimDN Guilfoyle2010Impaired visual object processing across an occipital-frontal-hippocampal brain network in schizophrenia: an integrated neuroimaging study.Arch Gen Psychiatry67772782
62. Miall RC (1998) The cerebellum, predictive control and motor coordination. Novartis Found Symp 218: 272–284; discussion 284–290.RC Miall1998The cerebellum, predictive control and motor coordination.Novartis Found Symp218272284; discussion 284–290
63. Krauzlis RJ (2005) The control of voluntary eye movements: New perspectives. Neuroscientist 11: 124–137.RJ Krauzlis2005The control of voluntary eye movements: New perspectives.Neuroscientist11124137
64. Barash S, Melikyan A, Sivakov A, Zhang M, Glickstein M, et al. (1999) Saccadic dysmetria and adaptation after lesions of the cerebellar cortex. J Neurosci 19: 10931–10939.S. BarashA. MelikyanA. SivakovM. ZhangM. Glickstein1999Saccadic dysmetria and adaptation after lesions of the cerebellar cortex.J Neurosci191093110939
65. Takagi M, Zee DS, Tamargo RJ (1998) Effects of lesions of the oculomotor vermis on eye movements in primate: saccades. J Neurophysiol 80: 1911–1931.M. TakagiDS ZeeRJ Tamargo1998Effects of lesions of the oculomotor vermis on eye movements in primate: saccades.J Neurophysiol8019111931
66. Marple-Horvat DE, Stein JF (1990) Neuronal activity in the lateral cerebellum of trained monkeys, related to visual stimuli or to eye movements. J Physiol 428: 595–614.DE Marple-HorvatJF Stein1990Neuronal activity in the lateral cerebellum of trained monkeys, related to visual stimuli or to eye movements.J Physiol428595614
67. Straube A, Scheuerer W, Eggert T (1997) Unilateral cerebellar lesions affect initiation of ipsilateral smooth pursuit eye movements in humans. Ann Neurol 42: 891–898.A. StraubeW. ScheuererT. Eggert1997Unilateral cerebellar lesions affect initiation of ipsilateral smooth pursuit eye movements in humans.Ann Neurol42891898
68. Chen SH, Desmond JE (2005) Cerebrocerebellar networks during articulatory rehearsal and verbal working memory tasks. Neuroimage 24: 332–338.SH ChenJE Desmond2005Cerebrocerebellar networks during articulatory rehearsal and verbal working memory tasks.Neuroimage24332338
69. Monti MM, Osherson DN, Martinez MJ, Parsons LM (2007) Functional neuroanatomy of deductive inference: a language-independent distributed network. Neuroimage 37: 1005–1016.MM MontiDN OshersonMJ MartinezLM Parsons2007Functional neuroanatomy of deductive inference: a language-independent distributed network.Neuroimage3710051016
70. Luna B, Velanova K, Geier CF (2008) Development of eye-movement control. Brain and Cogn 68: 293–308.B. LunaK. VelanovaCF Geier2008Development of eye-movement control.Brain and Cogn68293308
71. O'Driscoll GA, Wolff AL, Benkelfat C, Florencio PS, Lal S, et al. (2000) Functional neuroanatomy of smooth pursuit and predictive saccades. Neuroreport 11: 1335–1340.GA O'DriscollAL WolffC. BenkelfatPS FlorencioS. Lal2000Functional neuroanatomy of smooth pursuit and predictive saccades.Neuroreport1113351340
72. Moncrieff J, Leo J (2010) A systematic review of the effects of antipsychotic drugs on brain volume. Psychol Med 40: 1409–1422.J. MoncrieffJ. Leo2010A systematic review of the effects of antipsychotic drugs on brain volume.Psychol Med4014091422
73. Van Essen DC (2005) A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage 28: 635–662.DC Van Essen2005A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex.Neuroimage28635662
74. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, et al. (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-participant brain. Neuroimage 15: 273–289.N. Tzourio-MazoyerB. LandeauD. PapathanassiouF. CrivelloO. Etard2002Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-participant brain.Neuroimage15273289
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2011 Qiu et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Schizophrenic patients present abnormalities in a variety of eye movement tasks. Exploratory eye movement (EEM) dysfunction appears to be particularly specific to schizophrenia. However, the underlying mechanisms of EEM dysfunction in schizophrenia are not clearly understood. To assess the potential neuroanatomical substrates of EEM, we recorded EEM performance and conducted a voxel-based morphometric analysis of gray matter in 33 schizophrenic patients and 29 well matched healthy controls. In schizophrenic patients, decreased responsive search score (RSS) and widespread gray matter density (GMD) reductions were observed. Moreover, the RSS was positively correlated with GMD in distributed brain regions in schizophrenic patients. Furthermore, in schizophrenic patients, some brain regions with neuroanatomical deficits overlapped with some ones associated with RSS. These brain regions constituted an occipito-tempro-frontal circuitry involved in visual information processing and eye movement control, including the left calcarine cortex [Brodmann area (BA) 17], the left cuneus (BA 18), the left superior occipital cortex (BA 18/19), the left superior frontal gyrus (BA 6), the left cerebellum, the right lingual cortex (BA 17/18), the right middle occipital cortex (BA19), the right inferior temporal cortex (BA 37), the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 46) and bilateral precentral gyri (BA 6) extending to the frontal eye fields (FEF, BA 8). To our knowledge, we firstly reported empirical evidence that gray matter loss in the occipito-tempro-frontal neuroanatomical circuitry of visual processing system was associated with EEM performance in schizophrenia, which may be helpful for the future effort to reveal the underlying neural mechanisms for EEM disturbances in schizophrenia.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer