About the Authors:
Jonathan S. Adelstein
Affiliation: Phyllis Green and Randolph Cōwen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience at the NYU Child Study Center, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America
Zarrar Shehzad
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
Maarten Mennes
Affiliation: Phyllis Green and Randolph Cōwen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience at the NYU Child Study Center, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America
Colin G. DeYoung
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota, United States of America
Xi-Nian Zuo
Affiliations Phyllis Green and Randolph Cōwen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience at the NYU Child Study Center, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America, Laboratory for Functional Connectome and Development, Key Laboratory of Behavioral Sciences, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
Clare Kelly
Affiliation: Phyllis Green and Randolph Cōwen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience at the NYU Child Study Center, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America
Daniel S. Margulies
Affiliations Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany, Mind and Brain Institute, Humboldt University, Berlin, Germany
Aaron Bloomfield
Affiliation: Phyllis Green and Randolph Cōwen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience at the NYU Child Study Center, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America
Jeremy R. Gray
Affiliation: Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America
F. Xavier Castellanos
Affiliations Phyllis Green and Randolph Cōwen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience at the NYU Child Study Center, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York, New York, United States of America, Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg, New York, United States of America
Michael P. Milham
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliations Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg, New York, United States of America, Center for the Developing Brain, Child Mind Institute, New York, New York, United States of America
Introduction
Despite the varied and dynamic nature of human environments, the patterns of behavior and cognition that constitute personality tend to be enduring and broadly predictable. A fundamental challenge to neuroscience is uncovering how personality is encoded in the brain [1], [2].
The predominant approach to dimensionalizing personality traits [2], [3] assesses five domains: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness [4], [5]. Studies of the neurobiological substrates of personality traits have largely focused on the most long-standing domains: Neuroticism and Extraversion [2], [6]. The unevenness of coverage of the five principal personality domains is partly ascribable to the constraints inherent in task-based imaging approaches, which require effective cognitive, behavioral or emotional probes that target specific psychological constructs. Consequentially, task-based studies are limited in the breadth of neural systems and cognitive-behavioral constructs that can be effectively probed in a given experiment. Investigating the relationship between personality and brain structure is one method for simultaneously delineating brain systems potentially relevant to all five trait domains [7], but interpretations of structure-behavior relationships remain ambiguous.
Here, we use resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) analyses to directly examine the brain's functional architecture [8], [9] in relation to each of the five-factor personality traits quantified by the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO PI-R; [4]). RSFC offers a means to characterize inter-individual differences in intrinsic brain activity while avoiding the constraints of task-based approaches. Recent work has successfully related inter-individual differences in trait measures—such as social competence [10], risk-taking [11], working memory [12], episodic memory [13], aggression [14] and cognitive efficiency [15]—to patterns of RSFC. Much like personality traits, patterns of RSFC observed in these studies are stable across time [5], [16], [17], [18], [19]. In addition, these networks are strikingly similar to the networks activated by a broad spectrum of cognitive-behavioral tasks [20]. In fact, coordinated brain activity at rest has been shown to predict task-evoked activity and behavior [21], [22]. Together these studies suggest that the circuits revealed by analyses of RSFC represent intrinsically organized functional brain networks [23] that persist across tasks, and which appear to serve as the neural foundation on which task-evoked activity, and therefore behavior, is based.
Accordingly, we employed RSFC analyses to identify potentially dissociable intrinsic functional networks associated with each of the five domains of personality quantified by the NEO PI-R: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. We chose to examine RSFC with respect to two functionally heterogeneous brain areas involved in diverse aspects of cognition—such as integration of multidimensional information and higher-order executive control—that are commonly investigated in RSFC studies: the anterior cingulate cortex [24], [25] and the precuneus [26]. These regions are thought to be cortical “hubs” with connections spanning the majority of the brain [27], [28], [29]. Based on the neuroimaging literature on personality, we hypothesized that inter-individual variations in personality measures would predict RSFC between our chosen regions of interest and regions implicated in cognitive functions related to each trait. Specifically, we expected that Neuroticism would predict connectivity with regions involved in self- and other-evaluation, such as the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex [7]; Extraversion would predict connectivity with regions implicated in reward and motivation, including the orbitofrontal cortex, insula and the amygdala [7], [30]; Openness to Experience would predict connectivity with regions involved in cognitive flexibility, such as the anterior cingulate cortex [31] and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [32]; Agreeableness would predict connectivity with regions subserving altruism and social information processing, including the occipital cortex and posterior temporal cortex [33]; and Conscientiousness would predict connectivity with regions involved in planning and self-discipline, such as the lateral prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe [2], [7]. Additionally, since the five personality domains have been shown to be relatively independent and to describe non-overlapping traits [4], we expected to observe unique neural correlates for each domain.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Resting-state scans were acquired for 39 right-handed adults (18 males, mean age 30±8 years) who completed the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO PI-R; [4]). The NEO PI-R was designed to measure normal variations of personality in terms of five stable, heritable [34] domains, and it possesses strong reliability and validity [3], [4], [35], [36]. Each participant completed between 1 to 5 resting-state fMRI scans. The first scan session (Scan 1) took place 5–16 months prior to a second session during which two additional resting-state scans were acquired ∼45 minutes apart (Scans 2 and 3). A small number of participants attended a third scanning session 1–2 weeks later, during which two further resting-state scans were acquired ∼45 minutes apart (Scans 4 and 5). For each subject, functional connectivity maps of all scans with less than 3 mm maximum head displacement were averaged to derive the best estimate of that individual's RSFC. Importantly, the number of resting state scans in each participant's RSFC estimates was included as a nuisance covariate for all group-level analyses to avoid introduction of a possible confound. In addition, group-level connectivity maps obtained when all available scans for a subject were used to assess RSFC measures were highly similar to those maps derived from a single resting scan, and both maps showed high Kendall's W concordance as shown in Supporting Figure S1. As expected, the results were more robust when all available scans were included, owing to the fact that the inclusion of multiple scans for a given subject improves our estimate of that subject's RSFC (Supporting Figure S1).
In total, data from five resting-state scans (Scans 1–5) were available for eight participants, data from four resting-state scans (Scans 1–4) were available for one participant, data from three resting-state scans (Scans 1, 2 and 3) were available for six participants, from two scans five months apart (Scans 1 and 2 or 3) for four participants, from two scans 45 minutes apart (Scans 2 and 3) for two participants, and from one scan only (Scan 1 or 2) for 18 participants. Fifteen of these scans were eliminated due to motion as above: five scans from session 1, two from session 2, six from session 3, zero from session 4, and two scans from session 5. Following completion of all scan sessions, participants were asked to return for an additional visit to complete the NEO PI-R. These visits were scheduled at the participants' convenience, and all occurred within one year of each participant's final scan session.
Participants had no history of psychiatric or neurological illness as confirmed by psychiatric clinical assessment. Signed informed consent was obtained prior to participation, and this study was approved by the institutional review boards of New York University (NYU) and the NYU Langone School of Medicine. Data from Scans 1–3 have been reported in several previous studies [10], [17], [18], [21], [24], [37] and are publically available for download at http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/.
Assessment
The NEO PI-R was designed by Costa and McCrae [4] (supplanting the original 1985 version). The NEO PI-R form S (self-report) consists of 240 questions answered on a 5-point scale. These questions measure personality across five domains: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness. Each domain is subdivided into six facets, and is intended to be orthogonal to all other domains. Examples of questions include “I can handle myself pretty well in a crisis,” (domain: Neuroticism, facet: Vulnerability) and “I enjoy parties with lots of people” (domain: Extraversion, facet: Gregariousness).
Data acquisition
For each participant, 6.5-minute resting state functional MRI scans were collected on a 3.0 Tesla Siemens Allegra MRI scanner (197 EPI volumes; TR = 2000 ms; TE = 25 ms; flip angle = 90°; 39 slices; matrix = 64×64; FOV = 192 mm; acquisition voxel size = 3×3×3 mm). During each scan, participants were instructed to rest with their eyes open while the word “Relax” was projected onto the center of the display screen. A high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical image was also acquired using a magnetization prepared gradient echo sequence (MPRAGE, TR = 2500 ms; TE = 4.35 ms; TI = 900 ms; flip angle = 8; 176 slices; FOV = 256 mm).
Image preprocessing
As detailed in our prior studies [22], [38], data were processed using both AFNI (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/afni) and FSL (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk). Specific commands can be found in the preprocessing scripts available for download at http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/. Preprocessing using AFNI consisted of 1) slice time correction for interleaved acquisitions using Fourier interpolation, 2) motion correction using least squares alignment of each volume to the eighth image using Fourier interpolation, 3) despiking of extreme time series outliers using a continuous transformation function, 4) temporal band-pass filtering between 0.009–0.1 Hz using Fourier transformation, and 5) removal of linear and quadratic trends. Additional preprocessing using FSL consisted of 1) spatial smoothing (Gaussian kernel FWHM = 6 mm), and 2) mean-based intensity normalization of all volumes by the same factor (10,000). Next, each participant's preprocessed volume was regressed on nine nuisance signals (global mean, white matter, and CSF signals and six motion parameters). The output of these preprocessing steps was a 4D residual functional volume in each participant's native functional space.
Transformations from native functional and structural space to the Montreal Neurological Institute MNI152 template with 2×2×2 mm resolution were computed using FLIRT and FNIRT [39]. Each participant's high-resolution structural image was registered to the MNI152 template by computing a 12-degree-of-freedom linear affine transformation that was further refined using FNIRT nonlinear registration. Registration of each participant's functional data to their high-resolution structural image was carried out using a linear transformation with 6 degrees of freedom. The structural-to-standard nonlinear warp parameters were then applied to obtain a functional volume in MNI152 standard space.
Nuisance signal regression
Consistent with common practice in the resting-state fMRI literature, nuisance signals were removed from the data via multiple regression before functional connectivity analyses were performed. This step is designed to control for the effects of physiological processes, such as fluctuations related to motion and cardiac and respiratory cycles [40]. Specifically, each individual's 4D time series data were regressed on nine predictors: white matter (WM), cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), the global signal, and six motion parameters. The global signal regressor was generated by averaging across the time series of all voxels in the brain mask. The WM and CSF covariates were generated by segmenting each individual's high-resolution structural image (using FAST in FSL). The resulting segmented WM and CSF images were thresholded to ensure 80% tissue type probability. These thresholded masks were then applied to each individual's time series, and a mean time series was calculated by averaging across time series of all voxels within each mask. The six motion parameters were calculated in the motion-correction step during preprocessing. Movement in each of the three cardinal directions (X, Y, and Z) and rotational movement around three axes (pitch, yaw, and roll) were included for each individual.
Individual seed-based functional connectivity analysis
For seed placement, we selected the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the precuneus (PCU), two functionally heterogeneous brain areas known to be involved in diverse aspects of cognition—such as integration of information and higher-order executive control—and that are commonly investigated in RSFC studies. We created spherical seed regions of interest (diameter = 8 mm) centered at each of these coordinates in both the left and right hemispheres for use in our RSFC analyses: five in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; [25]) and four in the precuneus (PCU; [26]). Seed locations are shown in Figure 1 and coordinates are listed in Supporting Table S1. As detailed in prior studies [22], each individual's residual 4D time series data were spatially normalized by applying the previously computed transformation to the MNI152 standard space. Then the time series for each seed was extracted from these data. Time series were averaged across all voxels in each seed region of interest (ROI). For each individual dataset, the correlation between the time series of the seed ROI and that of each voxel in the brain was determined. This analysis was implemented using 3dfim+ in AFNI to produce individual-level correlation maps of all voxels that were positively or negatively correlated with the seed's time series. Finally, these individual-level correlation maps were converted to Z-value maps using Fisher's r-to-z transformation for subsequent group-level analyses.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 1. Seed locations.
General location of the nine seeds: five within the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; seeds s1, s3, s5, s7 and i9) and four within the precuneus (PCU; seeds p4, p6, p14, p17). Also shown are associated functions of each of these regions [24], [25], [26]. Seed coordinates are listed in Supporting Table S2.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633.g001
Group-level analyses
Group-level mixed-effects analyses were carried out using ordinary least squares, as implemented in FSL FEAT. Demeaned personality domain scores were included as simultaneous covariates of interest in one model, as well as analyzed independently in separate models. Across all seeds and domains, Kendall's W was calculated between the models in which the domains were included simultaneously and the models in which they were included separately to determine the correspondence between the two types of group-level modeling (Supporting Figure S2). We chose to focus on the results of the model in which all five personality domain scores were included as simultaneous covariates of interest, because that model design reveals the associations between RSFC and personality that are unique to each personality domain. Correlations between personality domain scores and the number of resting-state scans obtained per participant—that were included in the final analysis—were negligible (ranging from r = 0.019 for Neuroticism to r = −0.303 for Agreeableness). Nevertheless, we covaried the number of resting state scans included per participant to minimize artifactual contributions. Nuisance covariates for age and sex were included as well. Gaussian random field theory was used to correct for multiple comparisons at the cluster-level (Z>2.3; p<0.05, corrected).
For each seed region, group-level analyses produced the following two types of thresholded z-statistic maps: 1) maps of voxels exhibiting significant positive and negative functional connectivity with the seed across all individuals and 2) maps of voxels whose positive or negative functional connectivity with the seed exhibited significant variation in association with the personality domain scores (i.e., regions in which connectivity with the seed was predicted by score). Regions whose RSFC with the relevant seed ROI exhibited a significant relationship with personality scores were sorted according to the valence of their RSFC—that is, whether the region was significantly (i.e., consistently) positively correlated (“invariant positive”), significantly negatively correlated (“invariant negative”), or not significantly correlated (“variable”) with the relevant seed ROI, across participants. Finally, we conducted conjunction analyses to quantify the number of voxels exhibiting relationships with all five personality domains. This was accomplished by binarizing group-level thresholded maps of positive, negative and variable RSFC across all seeds and then summing them to create a conjunction map. The resultant map was then thresholded to identify areas that were common or unique to all RSFC maps.
Confirmatory analyses for personality-RSFC relationships
To ensure the robustness of our findings relating personality scores to specific functional connections, we verified our findings using both a split-half analysis and non-parametric testing. Specifically, we 1) randomly split the sample into two halves, and 2) for each functional connection identified as having a significant relationship with personality score in the entire sample (i.e., in the primary analyses), we verified the presence of the RSFC/personality score relationship in each of the two split groups. This analysis minimizes the likelihood that the observed relationships were driven by a subset of participants. Then, in a separate confirmatory analysis, we employed non-parametric testing to verify the presence of significant RSFC/personality score relationships emerging from our primary analyses. Specifically, for each of the significant functional connection/personality score relationships identified in the primary analysis, we 1) generated 5000 random pairings between the strength of RSFC for that specific connection and personality scores across subjects, allowing us to build a null distribution of r-values, and 2) calculated the p-value for each relationship examined based upon where the true RSFC/personality score correlation fell within that null distribution (e.g., if the true r-value ranked in the 97.5th percentile of the distribution, the p-value would be 0.025 [1−.975]). The confirmatory nature of this analysis justified forgoing corrections for multiple comparisons. Results for both split-half and non-parametric confirmatory analyses are shown in Supporting Table S1.
Results
Personality domain scores
In our sample, participant scores on Neuroticism (N; mean ± SD: 78±28; range: 12–142), Extraversion (E; 119±20; range: 79–168), Openness to Experience (O; 128±21; range: 92–166), Agreeableness (A; 125±15; range: 88–151), and Conscientiousness (C; 122±22; range: 70–176) closely matched population norms (N: 79±21; E: 109±18; O: 111±17; A: 124±16; C: 123±18) [4]. Between-domain score correlations are shown in Supporting Figure S3.
Personality domain scores predicted RSFC
For all five personality domains, we detected significant RSFC-personality relationships between our a priori seeds (Figure 1) and expected cognitive and affective processing regions (Figures 2, 3, 4 and 5). The majority of regions whose RSFC with the ACC seeds (Figure 1) was predicted by personality were located in the medial prefrontal cortex, paracingulate gyrus and anterior/central precuneus (Figures 3, 4 and 5). The majority of regions whose RSFC with the PCU seeds (Figure 1) was predicted by personality were located in and surrounding the precuneus, the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and the posterior cingulate gyrus, as well as the primary motor and visual cortices (Figures 3, 4 and 5). For example, Neuroticism scores predicted positive RSFC between PCU seed p4 (Figure 1) and the central precuneus and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (Figure 3). Regions whose RSFC with ACC seeds (all except ACC s5; Figure 1) was invariantly positive were located at long distances from the seed region of interest or were located in distinct functional areas (i.e., the connections were long-range; Figure 3). By contrast, regions whose RSFC with PCU seeds was invariably positive were primarily located proximal to the seed region of interest or were located in the same anatomical or functional region (i.e., the connections were local; Figure 3). For ACC seeds, this pattern was also evident for RSFC that was both invariantly negative and variably present across participants (Figures 4, 5). The above results are summarized in Supporting Table S2, and surface maps of all personality-RSFC relationships can be seen in Figures 3, 4 and 5. Supplementary single-scan, split-half and non-parametric analyses confirmed the robustness of our results, as shown in Supporting Figure S1 and Supporting Table S1.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 2. Personality trait measures predicted RSFC.
Connections identified as having a relationship with personality, grouped by color according to the personality domain that predicted their RSFC. For the purpose of illustration, significant findings were collapsed across seed regions and RSFC/personality score relationship valence (i.e., whether the correlation was significantly positive or negative). Individual seed region findings are presented separately in Figures 3–5. White represents overlap of findings for multiple (one or more) personality domains predicting RSFC. LH = left hemisphere; RH = right hemisphere.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633.g002
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 3. Functional connections predicted by positive RSFC.
Surface maps of regions whose RSFC with ACC and PCU seeds was predicted by personality. These maps illustrate positive RSFC only. Colors on the surface maps represent RSFC with the seeds shown at the top in corresponding colors. “All domains” refers to all five personality domains combined into a single map. POS = positive relationships (stronger RSFC relationships with higher personality score); NEG = negative relationships (stronger RSFC relationships with lower personality score). N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness to Experience; A = Agreeableness; C = Conscientiousness.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633.g003
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 4. Functional connections predicted by negative RSFC.
Surface maps of regions whose RSFC with ACC and PCU seeds was predicted by personality. These maps illustrate negative RSFC only. Colors on the surface maps represent RSFC with the seeds shown at the top in corresponding colors. “All domains” refers to all five personality domains combined into a single map. POS = positive relationships (stronger RSFC relationships with higher personality score); NEG = negative relationships (stronger RSFC relationships with lower personality score). N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness to Experience; A = Agreeableness; C = Conscientiousness.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633.g004
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 5. Functional connections predicted by variable RSFC.
Surface maps of regions whose RSFC with ACC and PCU seeds was predicted by personality. These maps illustrate variable RSFC only. Colors on the surface maps represent RSFC with the seeds shown at the top in corresponding colors. “All domains” refers to all five personality domains combined into a single map. POS = positive relationships (stronger RSFC relationships with higher personality score); NEG = negative relationships (stronger RSFC relationships with lower personality score). N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness to Experience; A = Agreeableness; C = Conscientiousness.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633.g005
Brain regions whose RSFC was predicted by personality did not overlap
Using a voxel-wise conjunction analysis, we determined that there were no voxels common to all five personality-RSFC relationships. With minor exceptions (the white areas in Figure 2 illustrate voxels common to more than one but fewer than five domains), each domain of personality predicted RSFC between seed ROIs (ACC and PCU; Figure 1) and unique sets of brain regions (Figure 2). Across all domains, the pattern of regions whose connectivity was predicted by personality corresponded with functional subsystems in the brain, particularly default-mode network fractionations (Figure 2; [18], [41]). For example, Neuroticism predicted RSFC in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex subsystem known to be involved in self-referential processing [41] and emotional regulation [7], [42]. Functional connections identified as having a significant relationship with Neuroticism were also located in the middle temporal gyrus and temporal pole, consistent with activation studies of this trait during fearful anticipation and negative emotions [43], [44]. Extraversion predicted RSFC in the lateral paralimbic group implicated in motivation and reward [33], [42], [45], [46]. Functional connections identified as having a significant relationship with Extraversion were also located in the fusiform gyrus, consistent with prior studies [47] and the area's role in social attention and face recognition [48]. Openness to Experience predicted RSFC with the midline core “hubs” of the default mode network, known to be involved in integration of the self and the environment [31], [41]. Functional connections identified as having a significant relationship with Openness to Experience were also located in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, a region associated with working memory, intelligence, creativity and the intellect facet of Openness to Experience [31], [32], [49]. Agreeableness predicted RSFC with posteromedial extrastriate regions as well as some primary sensorimotor areas, the combination of which is reported to be involved in social and emotional attention [7], [33]. Finally, Conscientiousness predicted RSFC with the medial temporal lobe subsystem involved in future-oriented episodic judgment and planning [41]. Although our model design was organized to maximize independence among the domain-RSFC patterns, this independence persisted when the five personality domains were analyzed in separate models. This is demonstrated by the high Kendall's W concordance across all seeds and domains between 1) maps generated by the group analysis model where all five domains were analyzed simultaneously, and 2) maps generated when all five domains were analyzed independently in separate group analysis models (Supporting Figure S2).
Unpredicted personality-RSFC relationships
Unexpected relationships also emerged: all five domains except Openness to Experience predicted RSFC between ACC seeds and the cerebellar vermis. Openness to Experience predicted RSFC between PCU seeds and the right cerebellar hemisphere. Additionally, all five domains predicted RSFC between at least one seed and the visual cortex (Figures 3, 4 and 5). All relationships are listed in Supporting Table S2.
Functional connections predicted by personality were inconsistently present across participants
Across all personality domains, the majority of functional connections found to be related to personality scores were variably present across participants (Figure 6). In other words, these connections did not exhibit statistically significant positive or negative RSFC with the seed regions across the sample. Of note, these connections were frequently located on the boundaries of regions whose RSFC with relevant seed ROIs was consistently significantly positive or negative across the sample.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 6. Functional connections predicted by personality are variable across participants.
Regions whose RSFC with ACC and PCU seeds was predicted by each of the five personality domains, grouped by color according to the valence of their RSFC—that is, whether the region was significantly (i.e., consistently) positively correlated (“invariant positive”; pos), significantly negatively correlated (“invariant negative”; neg), or not significantly correlated (“variable”; var) with the relevant seed region of interest, across all ACC and PCU seeds. Results are segregated by the relevant personality domain that predicted RSFC. Histograms to the right of the surface maps quantify the number of voxels identified as having a relationship with personality score, grouped according to their RSFC valence. As illustrated at the top of the figure, for both brain regions and histogram categories, blue corresponds to “positive” RSFC valence, green corresponds to “negative” RSFC valence, and red corresponds to “variable” RSFC valence. L = left hemisphere; R = right hemisphere; N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness to Experience; A = Agreeableness; C = Conscientiousness.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633.g006
Discussion
Unique patterns in the brain's intrinsic functional architecture reflected each of the five personality domains assessed by the NEO PI-R. RSFC patterns of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and precuneus (PCU), regions commonly implicated in the regulation and integration of higher order information [24], [25], [26], [27], [28], [29], were correlated with personality domain scores. These results highlight the utility of examining the brain's intrinsic functional architecture to identify neural markers of complex traits, such as in the study of psychiatric or personality disorders.
Personality-RSFC network functions matched psychological qualities associated with each trait
Cognitive and psychological functions associated with the regions whose RSFC with ACC and PCU seeds was predicted by personality were consistent with known qualities about each relevant personality domain [7], [35]. For instance, Neuroticism predicted RSFC with brain areas involved in self-evaluation and fear [41], [43], [44], and is known to be associated with anxiety and self-consciousness [35], [50]. Extraversion predicted RSFC with brain areas involved in reward and motivation [33], [42], [45], [46], and is implicated in gregariousness and excitement-seeking [35], [51]. Openness to Experience predicted RSFC with brain areas involved in cognitive flexibility and imagination [31], [32], [41], and is associated with fantasy, intellectual curiosity and exploration [2], [35]. Agreeableness predicted RSFC with brain areas involved in empathy and social information processing [7], [33], and is linked with compassion and friendliness [35]. Finally, Conscientiousness predicted RSFC with brain areas involved in planning and self-discipline [41], and is implicated in carefulness, industriousness and organization [35], [52].
Personality relates to a network of functional connections
Our RSFC findings extend prior studies that linked personality traits to regional differences in brain structure [7], [53], [54], [55] or function [56], [57], [58], [59], [60], [61]. By contrast, the present findings emphasize the importance of considering functional relationships between regions in order to map complex brain-behavior relationships, rather than being limited to volumetric differences or the momentary responsivity of individual brain regions or sets of regions.
For example, Neuroticism predicted positive RSFC between PCU seed p4—a region involved in limbic processing (Figure 1; [26])—and the surrounding precuneus (Figure 3). This region of the precuneus is implicated in social [62] and emotional [63] functions, especially among individuals high in Neuroticism [44], [64], [65], [66], who tend to be more socially dysfunctional [67] and reactive to negative emotional experiences [35]. Yet the precuneus is a large, functionally heterogeneous region [26], and it cannot be assumed to be solely responsible for Neuroticism. Instead, it is only when we consider the functional relationship between the seed and additional regions that we can interpret how these areas interact in unique ways to produce a framework for modulating behavioral responses to environmental stimuli.
In this example, individuals scoring high on Neuroticism exhibit a more tightly connected “limbic” precuneus, as reflected in the increased local connectivity in this area (Figure 3). But these individuals also demonstrated increased connectivity with the central “cognitive” precuneus (Figure 3; [26]). As the seed region (p4; Figure 1) is involved in limbic processing [26] and the central precuneus is involved in higher-order cognitive function [68], [69], [70], this connection suggests that Neuroticism involves increased integration of social and emotional information [7], [35], [71] and may relate to increased sensitivity to social-emotional cognitive conflicts [72], [73]. Yet this same seed (p4; Figure 1) is also connected to the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (Figure 3), a region involved in self-evaluation [41] and social interaction [74]. A relationship between these cognitive processes (i.e., self-evaluation and integration of social and emotional information) is highly consistent with the psychological qualities inherent to this personality trait [35]. Moreover, the distributed pattern of connectivity shown here between seed p4 (Figure 1), multiple regions of the precuneus, and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex implies the existence of a network of inter-related regions underlying Neuroticism that are each individually identified by task-based studies, but captured entirely by RSFC. The networks of interconnected brain regions delineated by RSFC studies would otherwise be appreciated piecemeal through the lens of specific task contrasts [75]. Thus, resting-state fMRI is well-suited to address complex constructs such as personality. These results set the stage for complementary task-based studies that can be particularly useful in parsing and supporting the interpretation of resting-state fMRI results.
Contributions of unpredicted brain regions to personality
We also detected less intuitive results. For example, all five personality domains predicted RSFC between numerous seeds and primary motor and sensory regions (e.g., occipital cortex; Figures 3, 4 and 5). Task-based studies have also found relationships between the occipital cortex and higher-order behavioral traits, such as word [76] and food picture [77] recognition, risky decision-making [78] and auditory expectation [79]. Typically these findings are attributed to the visual components inherent to the task paradigms employed in the particular study. But Kober et al. [33] suggested that visual cortex activity may contribute to attentional processing of emotionally-valenced stimuli, rather than being limited to low-level sensory processing. As some of these seeds (e.g., seed s1 and Openness to Experience; Figures 1 and 5) also demonstrated a connection with prefrontal regions mediating higher-order cognitive function, this suggests that dynamic interactions of large-scale networks including low-level sensory and high-order cognitive brain regions subserve complex thoughts and behavior [80].
Of particular interest is the ubiquitous relationship demonstrated between personality domain scores and the cerebellum, especially the cerebellar vermis. Previous studies implicated the cerebellum in non-motor [81], higher cognitive functions [82], [83], and cerebellar lesions have been shown to produce personality changes [84], [85]. This suggests that full coverage of the cerebellum should be a priority in future neuroimaging studies of personality.
Significance of variable RSFC
In analyses of RSFC data from over 1000 participants, a “core” intrinsic, functional architecture was found to be consistent across individuals and imaging centers [38], [86]. However, despite striking similarities, substantial inter-individual variations could also be appreciated. In a previous study, we found that autistic traits were related to functional connections that were variably present across participants (as opposed to relatively invariant positive or negative) [10]. Here, we also found that the majority of functional connections exhibiting relationships with personality had inconsistent patterns of connectivity across participants (Figure 6). This suggests that although a fundamental, core functional architecture is preserved across individuals, variable connections outside of that core may underlie the inter-individual differences in personality that motivate diverse responses.
Clinical implications
Prior efforts to use behavior-RSFC relationships to target clinically-relevant neural circuits [10], [11], [12], [13], [14], [15] are continued in the present study, as differences in five-factor personality scores have been linked to a range of clinical pathology including personality disorders [87], [88], mood and anxiety disorders [89], [90] and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder [91]. As our knowledge of the neural circuitry of personality improves, so will too our ability to identify abnormalities in these personality networks relevant to neuropsychopathology. Future investigations of the neural circuits identified by these studies will enhance our understanding of their functional significance, ultimately improving our ability to diagnose and treat a wide range of neuropsychiatric disorders.
Limitations
The scope of the present work is limited to the identification of markers of inter-individual variation in personality traits within the brain's intrinsic functional architecture. While our findings hold great potential for the identification of markers related to personality pathology, their correlational nature constrains the interpretations they can support. Specifically, we cannot yet conclude that our results represent the cortical embodiment of inter-individual differences in personality traits nor the mechanisms through which such differences manifest in individuals.
An important methodological limitation of seed-based RSFC studies, including ours, is the a priori selection of regions of interest. A potential strategy would have been to select regions of interest based on previous neuroimaging studies of personality [7], [43], [54], [73], [92]. However, the sheer number of published results and the diversity of task paradigms employed across studies did not make this a practical option. Instead, we focused on the brain's cortical “hubs”: regions that are widely connected with the rest of the brain and are of central importance to diverse cognitive, affective, and motivational processes [27], [28], [29]. By examining the relationship of these hubs with the areas to which they are functionally connected, this approach emphasizes the degree to which personality traits are associated with unique distributed networks of regions, rather than being localized in a few specific regions. Future work using RSFC can systematically interrogate other regions and networks such as the striatum [93], [94], amygdala [95], [96], [97] and insula [10], [98], [99] to gain further insights into the functional specialization of personality traits.
Our study was also limited by its modest sample size. An exhaustive mapping of RSFC/personality relationships in the functional connectome would require levels of statistical correction beyond what is practical for the present sample. Though less comprehensive, seed-based correlation analysis limits the scope of statistical interrogation, making exploration of modestly powered samples feasible. Future studies incorporating large-scale, data-driven methods [100] are needed to examine the neural correlates of personality more comprehensively.
Conclusion
Consistent with the neural network model of personality [101], our results suggest that distinct personality domains are encoded by dissociable patterns of functional connectivity among specific brain regions, despite the presence of modest inter-domain score correlations (Supporting Figure S3). Further appreciation of how personality is encoded within RSFC patterns will integrate with previous multimodal approaches [7], [100], [102] and inform future studies of personality, mood and anxiety disorders, and their development over the first several decades of life.
Supporting Information
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure S1.
Comparison of results using single- and multiple-scan session data. Comparison of surface maps of regions whose RSFC with PCU seeds was predicted by personality between a representative single-scan analysis (i.e., data from Scan 1 only; SINGLE) and the analysis used in the main text (i.e., data averaged across all scan sessions; MULTIPLE). Maps are not sorted according to their RSFC valence (i.e., positive, negative, or variable). Kendall's W concordance between the SINGLE and MULTIPLE maps for both positive (i.e., stronger RSFC relationships with higher personality score; POS) and negative (i.e., stronger RSFC relationships with lower personality score; NEG) behavior relationships is listed in the third column. Colors are consistent with labeling in Figure 3, 4 and 5.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633.s001
(TIF)
Figure S2.
Concordance between models in which personality domains are included simultaneously and models in which domains are analyzed separately. Kendall's W comparison between group maps when all five personality domains are included simultaneously in the group analysis model, and when all five personality domains are analyzed in separate models. Kendall's W concordance between these two models was calculated for every seed (Y axis) and every personality domain (X axis). Seed locations are shown in Figure 1 and coordinates are listed in Supporting Table S2. N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness to Experience; A = Agreeableness; C = Conscientiousness.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633.s002
(EPS)
Figure S3.
Correlations between personality domain scores across subjects. Correlations between all possible pairs of personality domain scores across all subjects. Numeric values are shown in the upper triangle; corresponding colors are shown in the lower triangle. Red colors represent positive correlations; blue colors represent negative correlations. Darker colors correspond to higher absolute values. N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness to Experience; A = Agreeableness; C = Conscientiousness.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633.s003
(TIF)
Table S1.
Confirmatory split-half and non-parametric analyses. Table of results from confirmatory split-half and non-parametric analyses. Results are only shown for seeds demonstrating significant RSFC relationships predicted by personality. Results are listed for each personality domain and for both positive (i.e., stronger RSFC relationships with higher personality score; POS) and negative (i.e., stronger RSFC relationships with lower personality score; NEG) behavior relationships. Results include: 1) for the split-half analysis, r-values for the full sample and both sample halves, and 2) for the non-parametric analysis, p-values. The two P-values greater than 0.05 (i.e., those connections for which the true RSFC/personality score correlation as determined our primary analysis did not fall outside of the 95% confidence interval of the null distribution of r-values) are highlighted in yellow. Seed labels are consistent with Figure 1. R = right-sided seed; L = left-sided seed.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633.s004
(XLS)
Table S2.
Details of all functional connections predicted by personality. Table of local peaks associated with each RSFC relationship for each domain of personality. Coordinates are in standard MNI152 space. Seed locations are shown in Figure 1. Behavior refers to behavioral relationships: positive relationships are stronger with higher personality domain scores, negative relationships are stronger with lower personality domain scores. pos = positive relationship; neg = negative relationship. N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness; A = Agreeableness; C = Conscientiousness.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633.s005
(DOC)
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Dr. Adriana Di Martino, Dr. Amy Krain Roy and Dr. Christine Cox for their helpful editorial suggestions. The authors also thank the Stavros Niarchos Foundation, the Leon Levy Foundation, and Phyllis Green and Randolph Cōwen for their generous support of the NYU Child Study Center, and all participants for their time and cooperation.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: JSA ZS MM CK FXC MPM. Performed the experiments: JSA ZS CK. Analyzed the data: JSA ZS MM XNZ CK AB FXC MPM. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: JSA ZS MM XNZ CK FXC MPM. Wrote the paper: JSA ZS MM CGD XNZ CK DSM JRG FXC MPM. Figure creation: DSM.
Citation: Adelstein JS, Shehzad Z, Mennes M, DeYoung CG, Zuo X-N, Kelly C, et al. (2011) Personality Is Reflected in the Brain's Intrinsic Functional Architecture. PLoS ONE6(11): e27633. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027633
1. Canli T, Amin Z (2002) Neuroimaging of emotion and personality: scientific evidence and ethical considerations. Brain Cogn 50: 414–431.T. CanliZ. Amin2002Neuroimaging of emotion and personality: scientific evidence and ethical considerations.Brain Cogn50414431
2. DeYoung CG, Gray JR (2009) Personality neuroscience: Explaining individual differences in affect, behavior and cognition. In: Corr PJ, Matthews G, editors. The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press. CG DeYoungJR Gray2009Personality neuroscience: Explaining individual differences in affect, behavior and cognition.PJ CorrG. MatthewsThe Cambridge handbook of personality psychologyNew YorkCambridge University Press
3. Digman JM (1997) Higher-order factors of the Big Five. J Pers Soc Psychol 73: 1246–1256.JM Digman1997Higher-order factors of the Big Five.J Pers Soc Psychol7312461256
4. Costa PT Jr, McCrae RR (1992) NEO PI-R Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. PT Costa JrRR McCrae1992NEO PI-R Professional ManualOdessa, FLPsychological Assessment Resources
5. John OP, Naumann LP, Soto CJ (2008) Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of personality: Theory and research. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 114–156.OP JohnLP NaumannCJ Soto2008Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues.OP JohnRW RobinsLA PervinHandbook of personality: Theory and researchNew YorkGuilford Press114156
6. Canli T (2004) Functional brain mapping of extraversion and neuroticism: learning from individual differences in emotion processing. J Pers 72: 1105–1132.T. Canli2004Functional brain mapping of extraversion and neuroticism: learning from individual differences in emotion processing.J Pers7211051132
7. DeYoung CG, Hirsh JB, Shane MS, Papademetris X, Rajeevan N, et al. (2010) Testing Predictions From Personality Neuroscience: Brain Structure and the Big Five. Psychol Sci 21: 820–828.CG DeYoungJB HirshMS ShaneX. PapademetrisN. Rajeevan2010Testing Predictions From Personality Neuroscience: Brain Structure and the Big Five.Psychol Sci21820828
8. Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SA, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, et al. (2006) Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103: 13848–13853.JS DamoiseauxSA RomboutsF. BarkhofP. ScheltensCJ Stam2006Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A1031384813853
9. Raichle ME (2010) The brain's dark energy. Sci Am 302: 44–49.ME Raichle2010The brain's dark energy.Sci Am3024449
10. Di Martino A, Shehzad Z, Kelly C, Roy AK, Gee DG, et al. (2009) Relationship between cingulo-insular functional connectivity and autistic traits in neurotypical adults. Am J Psychiatry 166: 891–899.A. Di MartinoZ. ShehzadC. KellyAK RoyDG Gee2009Relationship between cingulo-insular functional connectivity and autistic traits in neurotypical adults.Am J Psychiatry166891899
11. Cox CL, Gotimer K, Roy AK, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, et al. (2010) Your resting brain CAREs about your risky behavior. PLoS One 5: e12296.CL CoxK. GotimerAK RoyFX CastellanosMP Milham2010Your resting brain CAREs about your risky behavior.PLoS One5e12296
12. Hampson M, Driesen NR, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Constable RT (2006) Brain connectivity related to working memory performance. J Neurosci 26: 13338–13343.M. HampsonNR DriesenP. SkudlarskiJC GoreRT Constable2006Brain connectivity related to working memory performance.J Neurosci261333813343
13. Ystad M, Eichele T, Lundervold AJ, Lundervold A (2010) Subcortical functional connectivity and verbal episodic memory in healthy elderly–a resting state fMRI study. Neuroimage 52: 379–388.M. YstadT. EicheleAJ LundervoldA. Lundervold2010Subcortical functional connectivity and verbal episodic memory in healthy elderly–a resting state fMRI study.Neuroimage52379388
14. Hoptman MJ, D'Angelo D, Catalano D, Mauro CJ, Shehzad ZE, et al. (2009) Amygdalofrontal Functional Disconnectivity and Aggression in Schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 36: 1020–1028.MJ HoptmanD. D'AngeloD. CatalanoCJ MauroZE Shehzad2009Amygdalofrontal Functional Disconnectivity and Aggression in Schizophrenia.Schizophr Bull3610201028
15. Andrews-Hanna JR, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Lustig C, Head D, et al. (2007) Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron 56: 924–935.JR Andrews-HannaAZ SnyderJL VincentC. LustigD. Head2007Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging.Neuron56924935
16. Allemand M, Zimprich D, Hendriks AA (2008) Age differences in five personality domains across the life span. Dev Psychol 44: 758–770.M. AllemandD. ZimprichAA Hendriks2008Age differences in five personality domains across the life span.Dev Psychol44758770
17. Shehzad Z, Kelly AM, Reiss PT, Gee DG, Gotimer K, et al. (2009) The resting brain: unconstrained yet reliable. Cereb Cortex 19: 2209–2229.Z. ShehzadAM KellyPT ReissDG GeeK. Gotimer2009The resting brain: unconstrained yet reliable.Cereb Cortex1922092229
18. Zuo XN, Kelly C, Adelstein JS, Klein DF, Castellanos FX, et al. (2010) Reliable intrinsic connectivity networks: test-retest evaluation using ICA and dual regression approach. Neuroimage 49: 2163–2177.XN ZuoC. KellyJS AdelsteinDF KleinFX Castellanos2010Reliable intrinsic connectivity networks: test-retest evaluation using ICA and dual regression approach.Neuroimage4921632177
19. Van Dijk KR, Hedden T, Venkataraman A, Evans KC, Lazar SW, et al. (2009) Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization. J Neurophysiol 103: 297–321.KR Van DijkT. HeddenA. VenkataramanKC EvansSW Lazar2009Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization.J Neurophysiol103297321
20. Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, et al. (2009) Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 13040–13045.SM SmithPT FoxKL MillerDC GlahnPM Fox2009Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A1061304013045
21. Kelly AM, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2008) Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability. Neuroimage 39: 527–537.AM KellyLQ UddinBB BiswalFX CastellanosMP Milham2008Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability.Neuroimage39527537
22. Mennes M, Kelly C, Zuo XN, Di Martino A, Biswal BB, et al. (2010) Inter-individual differences in resting-state functional connectivity predict task-induced BOLD activity. Neuroimage 50: 1690–1701.M. MennesC. KellyXN ZuoA. Di MartinoBB Biswal2010Inter-individual differences in resting-state functional connectivity predict task-induced BOLD activity.Neuroimage5016901701
23. Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS (1995) Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 34: 537–541.B. BiswalFZ YetkinVM HaughtonJS Hyde1995Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI.Magn Reson Med34537541
24. Margulies DS, Kelly AM, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, et al. (2007) Mapping the functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroimage 37: 579–588.DS MarguliesAM KellyLQ UddinBB BiswalFX Castellanos2007Mapping the functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex.Neuroimage37579588
25. Kelly AM, Di Martino A, Uddin LQ, Shehzad Z, Gee DG, et al. (2009) Development of anterior cingulate functional connectivity from late childhood to early adulthood. Cereb Cortex 19: 640–657.AM KellyA. Di MartinoLQ UddinZ. ShehzadDG Gee2009Development of anterior cingulate functional connectivity from late childhood to early adulthood.Cereb Cortex19640657
26. Margulies DS, Vincent JL, Kelly C, Lohmann G, Uddin LQ, et al. (2009) Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 20069–20074.DS MarguliesJL VincentC. KellyG. LohmannLQ Uddin2009Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A1062006920074
27. Achard S, Salvador R, Whitcher B, Suckling J, Bullmore E (2006) A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs. J Neurosci 26: 63–72.S. AchardR. SalvadorB. WhitcherJ. SucklingE. Bullmore2006A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs.J Neurosci266372
28. Hagmann P, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Meuli R, Honey CJ, et al. (2008) Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol 6: e159.P. HagmannL. CammounX. GigandetR. MeuliCJ Honey2008Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex.PLoS Biol6e159
29. Buckner RL, Sepulcre J, Talukdar T, Krienen FM, Liu H, et al. (2009) Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci 29: 1860–1873.RL BucknerJ. SepulcreT. TalukdarFM KrienenH. Liu2009Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer's disease.J Neurosci2918601873
30. Depue RA, Collins PF (1999) Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behav Brain Sci 22: 491–517; discussion 518–469.RA DepuePF Collins1999Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion.Behav Brain Sci22491517; discussion 518–469
31. Jung RE, Segall JM, Jeremy Bockholt H, Flores RA, Smith SM, et al. (2010) Neuroanatomy of creativity. Hum Brain Mapp 31: 398–409.RE JungJM SegallH. Jeremy BockholtRA FloresSM Smith2010Neuroanatomy of creativity.Hum Brain Mapp31398409
32. DeYoung CG, Shamosh NA, Green AE, Braver TS, Gray JR (2009) Intellect as distinct from Openness: differences revealed by fMRI of working memory. J Pers Soc Psychol 97: 883–892.CG DeYoungNA ShamoshAE GreenTS BraverJR Gray2009Intellect as distinct from Openness: differences revealed by fMRI of working memory.J Pers Soc Psychol97883892
33. Kober H, Barrett LF, Joseph J, Bliss-Moreau E, Lindquist K, et al. (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-subcortical interactions in emotion: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage 42: 998–1031.H. KoberLF BarrettJ. JosephE. Bliss-MoreauK. Lindquist2008Functional grouping and cortical-subcortical interactions in emotion: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies.Neuroimage429981031
34. Bouchard TJ Jr, McGue M (2003) Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences. J Neurobiol 54: 4–45.TJ Bouchard JrM. McGue2003Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences.J Neurobiol54445
35. Costa PT Jr, McCrae RR (1995) Domains and facets: hierarchical personality assessment using the revised NEO personality inventory. J Pers Assess 64: 21–50.PT Costa JrRR McCrae1995Domains and facets: hierarchical personality assessment using the revised NEO personality inventory.J Pers Assess642150
36. Jang KL, McCrae RR, Angleitner A, Riemann R, Livesley WJ (1998) Heritability of facet-level traits in a cross-cultural twin sample: support for a hierarchical model of personality. J Pers Soc Psychol 74: 1556–1565.KL JangRR McCraeA. AngleitnerR. RiemannWJ Livesley1998Heritability of facet-level traits in a cross-cultural twin sample: support for a hierarchical model of personality.J Pers Soc Psychol7415561565
37. Zuo XN, Di Martino A, Kelly C, Shehzad ZE, Gee DG, et al. (2010) The oscillating brain: complex and reliable. Neuroimage 49: 1432–1445.XN ZuoA. Di MartinoC. KellyZE ShehzadDG Gee2010The oscillating brain: complex and reliable.Neuroimage4914321445
38. Biswal BB, Mennes M, Zuo XN, Gohel S, Kelly C, et al. (2010) Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107: 4734–4739.BB BiswalM. MennesXN ZuoS. GohelC. Kelly2010Toward discovery science of human brain function.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A10747344739
39. Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith SM (2007) TR07JA2: Non-linear registration, aka Spatial normalisation. FMRIB Analysis Group Technical Reports. JLR AnderssonM. JenkinsonSM Smith2007TR07JA2: Non-linear registration, aka Spatial normalisation.FMRIB Analysis Group Technical Reports
40. Fox MD, Zhang D, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME (2009) The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. J Neurophysiol 101: 3270–3283.MD FoxD. ZhangAZ SnyderME Raichle2009The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks.J Neurophysiol10132703283
41. Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R, Buckner RL (2010) Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain's default network. Neuron 65: 550–562.JR Andrews-HannaJS ReidlerJ. SepulcreR. PoulinRL Buckner2010Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain's default network.Neuron65550562
42. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, et al. (2007) Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 27: 2349–2356.WW SeeleyV. MenonAF SchatzbergJ. KellerGH Glover2007Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control.J Neurosci2723492356
43. Kumari V, ffytche DH, Das M, Wilson GD, Goswami S, et al. (2007) Neuroticism and brain responses to anticipatory fear. Behav Neurosci 121: 643–652.V. KumariDH ffytcheM. DasGD WilsonS. Goswami2007Neuroticism and brain responses to anticipatory fear.Behav Neurosci121643652
44. Jimura K, Konishi S, Miyashita Y (2009) Temporal pole activity during perception of sad faces, but not happy faces, correlates with neuroticism trait. Neuroscience Letters 453: 45–48.K. JimuraS. KonishiY. Miyashita2009Temporal pole activity during perception of sad faces, but not happy faces, correlates with neuroticism trait.Neuroscience Letters4534548
45. Mobbs D, Hagan CC, Azim E, Menon V, Reiss AL (2005) Personality predicts activity in reward and emotional regions associated with humor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102: 16502–16506.D. MobbsCC HaganE. AzimV. MenonAL Reiss2005Personality predicts activity in reward and emotional regions associated with humor.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America1021650216506
46. Cohen MX, Young J, Baek JM, Kessler C, Ranganath C (2005) Individual differences in extraversion and dopamine genetics predict neural reward responses. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 25: 851–861.MX CohenJ. YoungJM BaekC. KesslerC. Ranganath2005Individual differences in extraversion and dopamine genetics predict neural reward responses.Brain Res Cogn Brain Res25851861
47. Amin Z, Constable RT, Canli T (2004) Attentional bias for a valenced stimuli as a function of personality in the dot-probe task. Journal of Research in Personality 38: 15–23.Z. AminRT ConstableT. Canli2004Attentional bias for a valenced stimuli as a function of personality in the dot-probe task.Journal of Research in Personality381523
48. Sabatini E, Della Penna S, Franciotti R, Ferretti A, Zoccolotti P, et al. (2009) Brain structures activated by overt and covert emotional visual stimuli. Brain Res Bull 79: 258–264.E. SabatiniS. Della PennaR. FranciottiA. FerrettiP. Zoccolotti2009Brain structures activated by overt and covert emotional visual stimuli.Brain Res Bull79258264
49. DeYoung CG, Peterson JB, Higgins DM (2005) Sources of openness/intellect: cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. J Pers 73: 825–858.CG DeYoungJB PetersonDM Higgins2005Sources of openness/intellect: cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality.J Pers73825858
50. Uliaszek AA, Hauner KK, Zinbarg RE, Craske MG, Mineka S, et al. (2009) An Examination of Content Overlap and Disorder-Specific Predictions in the Associations of Neuroticism with Anxiety and Depression. J Res Pers 43: 785–794.AA UliaszekKK HaunerRE ZinbargMG CraskeS. Mineka2009An Examination of Content Overlap and Disorder-Specific Predictions in the Associations of Neuroticism with Anxiety and Depression.J Res Pers43785794
51. Canli T, Zhao Z, Desmond JE, Kang E, Gross J, et al. (2001) An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behav Neurosci 115: 33–42.T. CanliZ. ZhaoJE DesmondE. KangJ. Gross2001An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli.Behav Neurosci1153342
52. Friedman HS, Kern ML, Reynolds CA (2010) Personality and health, subjective well-being, and longevity. J Pers 78: 179–216.HS FriedmanML KernCA Reynolds2010Personality and health, subjective well-being, and longevity.J Pers78179216
53. Omura K, Todd Constable R, Canli T (2005) Amygdala gray matter concentration is associated with extraversion and neuroticism. Neuroreport 16: 1905–1908.K. OmuraR. Todd ConstableT. Canli2005Amygdala gray matter concentration is associated with extraversion and neuroticism.Neuroreport1619051908
54. Wright CI, Williams D, Feczko E, Barrett LF, Dickerson BC, et al. (2006) Neuroanatomical correlates of extraversion and neuroticism. Cereb Cortex 16: 1809–1819.CI WrightD. WilliamsE. FeczkoLF BarrettBC Dickerson2006Neuroanatomical correlates of extraversion and neuroticism.Cereb Cortex1618091819
55. Sollberger M, Stanley CM, Wilson SM, Gyurak A, Weiner MW, et al. (2009) Neural Basis of Interpersonal Traits in Neurodegenerative Diseases. Neuropsychologia 47: 2812–2827.M. SollbergerCM StanleySM WilsonA. GyurakMW Weiner2009Neural Basis of Interpersonal Traits in Neurodegenerative Diseases.Neuropsychologia4728122827
56. Johnson DL, Wiebe JS, Gold SM, Andreasen NC, Hichwa RD, et al. (1999) Cerebral blood flow and personality: a positron emission tomography study. Am J Psychiatry 156: 252–257.DL JohnsonJS WiebeSM GoldNC AndreasenRD Hichwa1999Cerebral blood flow and personality: a positron emission tomography study.Am J Psychiatry156252257
57. Tran Y, Craig A, Boord P, Connell K, Cooper N, et al. (2006) Personality traits and its association with resting regional brain activity. Int J Psychophysiol 60: 215–224.Y. TranA. CraigP. BoordK. ConnellN. Cooper2006Personality traits and its association with resting regional brain activity.Int J Psychophysiol60215224
58. Jausovec N, Jausovec K (2007) Personality, gender and brain oscillations. Int J Psychophysiol 66: 215–224.N. JausovecK. Jausovec2007Personality, gender and brain oscillations.Int J Psychophysiol66215224
59. Kim SH, Hwang JH, Park HS, Kim SE (2008) Resting brain metabolic correlates of neuroticism and extraversion in young men. Neuroreport 19: 883–886.SH KimJH HwangHS ParkSE Kim2008Resting brain metabolic correlates of neuroticism and extraversion in young men.Neuroreport19883886
60. Sutin AR, Beason-Held LL, Resnick SM, Costa PT (2009) Sex Differences in Resting-State Neural Correlates of Openness to Experience among Older Adults. Cereb Cortex 19: 2797–2802.AR SutinLL Beason-HeldSM ResnickPT Costa2009Sex Differences in Resting-State Neural Correlates of Openness to Experience among Older Adults.Cereb Cortex1927972802
61. Sheng T, Gheytanchi A, Aziz-Zadeh L (2010) Default network deactivations are correlated with psychopathic personality traits. PLoS One 5: e12611.T. ShengA. GheytanchiL. Aziz-Zadeh2010Default network deactivations are correlated with psychopathic personality traits.PLoS One5e12611
62. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR (2006) The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129: 564–583.AE CavannaMR Trimble2006The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates.Brain129564583
63. Olson IR, Plotzker A, Ezzyat Y (2007) The Enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain 130: 1718–1731.IR OlsonA. PlotzkerY. Ezzyat2007The Enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing.Brain13017181731
64. Fletcher PC, Frith CD, Baker SC, Shallice T, Frackowiak RS, et al. (1995) The mind's eye–precuneus activation in memory-related imagery. Neuroimage 2: 195–200.PC FletcherCD FrithSC BakerT. ShalliceRS Frackowiak1995The mind's eye–precuneus activation in memory-related imagery.Neuroimage2195200
65. Saxe R, Kanwisher N (2003) People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. Neuroimage 19: 1835–1842.R. SaxeN. Kanwisher2003People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”.Neuroimage1918351842
66. Jimura K, Konishi S, Asari T, Miyashita Y (2010) Temporal pole activity during understanding other persons' mental states correlates with neuroticism trait. Brain Res 1328: 104–112.K. JimuraS. KonishiT. AsariY. Miyashita2010Temporal pole activity during understanding other persons' mental states correlates with neuroticism trait.Brain Res1328104112
67. Hopwood CJ, Morey LC, Ansell EB, Grilo CM, Sanislow CA, et al. (2009) The convergent and discriminant validity of five-factor traits: current and prospective social, work, and recreational dysfunction. J Pers Disord 23: 466–476.CJ HopwoodLC MoreyEB AnsellCM GriloCA Sanislow2009The convergent and discriminant validity of five-factor traits: current and prospective social, work, and recreational dysfunction.J Pers Disord23466476
68. Dosenbach NU, Fair DA, Miezin FM, Cohen AL, Wenger KK, et al. (2007) Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104: 11073–11078.NU DosenbachDA FairFM MiezinAL CohenKK Wenger2007Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A1041107311078
69. Fransson P, Marrelec G (2008) The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: Evidence from a partial correlation network analysis. Neuroimage 42: 1178–1184.P. FranssonG. Marrelec2008The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: Evidence from a partial correlation network analysis.Neuroimage4211781184
70. Vincent JL, Kahn I, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME, Buckner RL (2008) Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 100: 3328–3342.JL VincentI. KahnAZ SnyderME RaichleRL Buckner2008Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity.J Neurophysiol10033283342
71. Kristensen AS, Mortensen EL, Mors O (2009) The structure of emotional and cognitive anxiety symptoms. J Anxiety Disord 23: 600–608.AS KristensenEL MortensenO. Mors2009The structure of emotional and cognitive anxiety symptoms.J Anxiety Disord23600608
72. Greene JD, Nystrom LE, Engell AD, Darley JM, Cohen JD (2004) The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron 44: 389–400.JD GreeneLE NystromAD EngellJM DarleyJD Cohen2004The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment.Neuron44389400
73. Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD, Satpute AB (2005) Personality from a controlled processing perspective: an fMRI study of neuroticism, extraversion, and self-consciousness. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 5: 169–181.NI EisenbergerMD LiebermanAB Satpute2005Personality from a controlled processing perspective: an fMRI study of neuroticism, extraversion, and self-consciousness.Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci5169181
74. Iacoboni M, Lieberman MD, Knowlton BJ, Molnar-Szakacs I, Moritz M, et al. (2004) Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline. Neuroimage 21: 1167–1173.M. IacoboniMD LiebermanBJ KnowltonI. Molnar-SzakacsM. Moritz2004Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline.Neuroimage2111671173
75. Fox MD, Raichle ME (2007) Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 8: 700–711.MD FoxME Raichle2007Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging.Nat Rev Neurosci8700711
76. Woollams AM, Silani G, Okada K, Patterson K, Price CJ (2010) Word or Word-like? Dissociating Orthographic Typicality from Lexicality in the Left Occipito-temporal Cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 23: 992–1002.AM WoollamsG. SilaniK. OkadaK. PattersonCJ Price2010Word or Word-like? Dissociating Orthographic Typicality from Lexicality in the Left Occipito-temporal Cortex.J Cogn Neurosci239921002
77. Frank S, Laharnar N, Kullmann S, Veit R, Canova C, et al. (2010) Processing of Food Pictures: Influence of Hunger, Gender and Calorie Content. Brain Res 1350: 159–166.S. FrankN. LaharnarS. KullmannR. VeitC. Canova2010Processing of Food Pictures: Influence of Hunger, Gender and Calorie Content.Brain Res1350159166
78. Jollant F, Lawrence NS, Olie E, O'Daly O, Malafosse A, et al. (2010) Decreased activation of lateral orbitofrontal cortex during risky choices under uncertainty is associated with disadvantageous decision-making and suicidal behavior. Neuroimage 51: 1275–1281.F. JollantNS LawrenceE. OlieO. O'DalyA. Malafosse2010Decreased activation of lateral orbitofrontal cortex during risky choices under uncertainty is associated with disadvantageous decision-making and suicidal behavior.Neuroimage5112751281
79. Bueti D, Macaluso E (2010) Auditory temporal expectations modulate activity in visual cortex. Neuroimage 51: 1168–1183.D. BuetiE. Macaluso2010Auditory temporal expectations modulate activity in visual cortex.Neuroimage5111681183
80. Bressler SL, Menon V (2010) Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci 14: 277–290.SL BresslerV. Menon2010Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles.Trends Cogn Sci14277290
81. Krienen FM, Buckner RL (2009) Segregated fronto-cerebellar circuits revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Cereb Cortex 19: 2485–2497.FM KrienenRL Buckner2009Segregated fronto-cerebellar circuits revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity.Cereb Cortex1924852497
82. Middleton FA, Strick PL (1994) Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognitive function. Science 266: 458–461.FA MiddletonPL Strick1994Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognitive function.Science266458461
83. Berquin PC, Giedd JN, Jacobsen LK, Hamburger SD, Krain AL, et al. (1998) Cerebellum in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a morphometric MRI study. Neurology 50: 1087–1093.PC BerquinJN GieddLK JacobsenSD HamburgerAL Krain1998Cerebellum in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a morphometric MRI study.Neurology5010871093
84. Marien P, Baillieux H, De Smet HJ, Engelborghs S, Wilssens I, et al. (2009) Cognitive, linguistic and affective disturbances following a right superior cerebellar artery infarction: a case study. Cortex 45: 527–536.P. MarienH. BaillieuxHJ De SmetS. EngelborghsI. Wilssens2009Cognitive, linguistic and affective disturbances following a right superior cerebellar artery infarction: a case study.Cortex45527536
85. Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD (2010) Evidence for topographic organization in the cerebellum of motor control versus cognitive and affective processing. Cortex 46: 831–844.CJ StoodleyJD Schmahmann2010Evidence for topographic organization in the cerebellum of motor control versus cognitive and affective processing.Cortex46831844
86. Tomasi D, Volkow ND (2010) Functional connectivity density mapping. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107: 9885–9890.D. TomasiND Volkow2010Functional connectivity density mapping.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A10798859890
87. Brieger P, Sommer S, Bloink F, Marneros AA (2000) The relationship between five-factor personality measurements and ICD-10 personality disorder dimensions: results from a sample of 229 subjects. J Pers Disord 14: 282–290.P. BriegerS. SommerF. BloinkAA Marneros2000The relationship between five-factor personality measurements and ICD-10 personality disorder dimensions: results from a sample of 229 subjects.J Pers Disord14282290
88. Trull TJ, Durrett CA (2005) Categorical and dimensional models of personality disorder. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 1: 355–380.TJ TrullCA Durrett2005Categorical and dimensional models of personality disorder.Annu Rev Clin Psychol1355380
89. Cuijpers P, van Straten A, Donker M (2005) Personality traits of patients with mood and anxiety disorders. Psychiatry Res 133: 229–237.P. CuijpersA. van StratenM. Donker2005Personality traits of patients with mood and anxiety disorders.Psychiatry Res133229237
90. Spinhoven P, de Rooij M, Heiser W, Smit JH, Penninx BW (2009) The role of personality in comorbidity among anxiety and depressive disorders in primary care and specialty care: a cross-sectional analysis. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 31: 470–477.P. SpinhovenM. de RooijW. HeiserJH SmitBW Penninx2009The role of personality in comorbidity among anxiety and depressive disorders in primary care and specialty care: a cross-sectional analysis.Gen Hosp Psychiatry31470477
91. Nigg JT, John OP, Blaskey LG, Huang-Pollock CL, Willcutt EG, et al. (2002) Big five dimensions and ADHD symptoms: links between personality traits and clinical symptoms. J Pers Soc Psychol 83: 451–469.JT NiggOP JohnLG BlaskeyCL Huang-PollockEG Willcutt2002Big five dimensions and ADHD symptoms: links between personality traits and clinical symptoms.J Pers Soc Psychol83451469
92. Canli T, Amin Z, Haas B, Omura K, Constable RT (2004) A double dissociation between mood states and personality traits in the anterior cingulate. Behav Neurosci 118: 897–904.T. CanliZ. AminB. HaasK. OmuraRT Constable2004A double dissociation between mood states and personality traits in the anterior cingulate.Behav Neurosci118897904
93. Di Martino A, Scheres A, Margulies DS, Kelly AM, Uddin LQ, et al. (2008) Functional connectivity of human striatum: a resting state FMRI study. Cereb Cortex 18: 2735–2747.A. Di MartinoA. ScheresDS MarguliesAM KellyLQ Uddin2008Functional connectivity of human striatum: a resting state FMRI study.Cereb Cortex1827352747
94. Cohen MX, Schoene-Bake JC, Elger CE, Weber B (2009) Connectivity-based segregation of the human striatum predicts personality characteristics. Nat Neurosci 12: 32–34.MX CohenJC Schoene-BakeCE ElgerB. Weber2009Connectivity-based segregation of the human striatum predicts personality characteristics.Nat Neurosci123234
95. Canli T, Sivers H, Whitfield SL, Gotlib IH, Gabrieli JD (2002) Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of extraversion. Science 296: 2191.T. CanliH. SiversSL WhitfieldIH GotlibJD Gabrieli2002Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of extraversion.Science2962191
96. Haas BW, Omura K, Constable RT, Canli T (2007) Emotional conflict and neuroticism: personality-dependent activation in the amygdala and subgenual anterior cingulate. Behav Neurosci 121: 249–256.BW HaasK. OmuraRT ConstableT. Canli2007Emotional conflict and neuroticism: personality-dependent activation in the amygdala and subgenual anterior cingulate.Behav Neurosci121249256
97. Roy AK, Shehzad Z, Margulies DS, Kelly AM, Uddin LQ, et al. (2009) Functional connectivity of the human amygdala using resting state fMRI. Neuroimage 45: 614–626.AK RoyZ. ShehzadDS MarguliesAM KellyLQ Uddin2009Functional connectivity of the human amygdala using resting state fMRI.Neuroimage45614626
98. Paulus MP, Rogalsky C, Simmons A, Feinstein JS, Stein MB (2003) Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism. Neuroimage 19: 1439–1448.MP PaulusC. RogalskyA. SimmonsJS FeinsteinMB Stein2003Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism.Neuroimage1914391448
99. Krain AL, Wilson AM, Arbuckle R, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2006) Distinct neural mechanisms of risk and ambiguity: a meta-analysis of decision-making. Neuroimage 32: 477–484.AL KrainAM WilsonR. ArbuckleFX CastellanosMP Milham2006Distinct neural mechanisms of risk and ambiguity: a meta-analysis of decision-making.Neuroimage32477484
100. Kunisato Y, Okamoto Y, Okada G, Aoyama S, Nishiyama Y, et al. (2011) Personality traits and the amplitude of spontaneous low-frequency oscillations during resting state. Neurosci Lett 492: 109–113.Y. KunisatoY. OkamotoG. OkadaS. AoyamaY. Nishiyama2011Personality traits and the amplitude of spontaneous low-frequency oscillations during resting state.Neurosci Lett492109113
101. Read SJ, Monroe BM, Brownstein AL, Yang Y, Chopra G, et al. (2010) A neural network model of the structure and dynamics of human personality. Psychol Rev 117: 61–92.SJ ReadBM MonroeAL BrownsteinY. YangG. Chopra2010A neural network model of the structure and dynamics of human personality.Psychol Rev1176192
102. Terracciano A, Sanna S, Uda M, Deiana B, Usala G, et al. (2010) Genome-wide association scan for five major dimensions of personality. Mol Psychiatry 15: 647–656.A. TerraccianoS. SannaM. UdaB. DeianaG. Usala2010Genome-wide association scan for five major dimensions of personality.Mol Psychiatry15647656
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2011 Adelstein et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Personality describes persistent human behavioral responses to broad classes of environmental stimuli. Investigating how personality traits are reflected in the brain's functional architecture is challenging, in part due to the difficulty of designing appropriate task probes. Resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) can detect intrinsic activation patterns without relying on any specific task. Here we use RSFC to investigate the neural correlates of the five-factor personality domains. Based on seed regions placed within two cognitive and affective ‘hubs’ in the brain—the anterior cingulate and precuneus—each domain of personality predicted RSFC with a unique pattern of brain regions. These patterns corresponded with functional subdivisions responsible for cognitive and affective processing such as motivation, empathy and future-oriented thinking. Neuroticism and Extraversion, the two most widely studied of the five constructs, predicted connectivity between seed regions and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and lateral paralimbic regions, respectively. These areas are associated with emotional regulation, self-evaluation and reward, consistent with the trait qualities. Personality traits were mostly associated with functional connections that were inconsistently present across participants. This suggests that although a fundamental, core functional architecture is preserved across individuals, variable connections outside of that core encompass the inter-individual differences in personality that motivate diverse responses.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer