About the Authors:
Paola Marangolo
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliations Facoltà di Medicina, Università Politecnica Marche, Ancona, Italy, IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Roma, Italy
Susanna Cipollari
Affiliation: IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Roma, Italy
Valentina Fiori
Affiliation: IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Roma, Italy
Carmela Razzano
Affiliation: IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Roma, Italy
Carlo Caltagirone
Affiliations IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Roma, Italy, Università di Roma, Tor Vergata, Roma, Italy
Introduction
It is well known that in aphasic patients word-finding difficulties are the most pervasive symptom of language breakdown. Different rehabilitation therapies based either on the simple use of gesture [1]–[3] or on gestures paired with verbal production [4]–[8] have been proposed. In a work by Hanlon et al. [1], the effect of different unilateral gestural movements on naming to confrontation were examined. Results showed that activating the hemiplegic right arm to execute a communicative but non-representational pointing gesture facilitated aphasics' naming abilities. Raimer et al. [6] examined the effect of gestural treatments using pantomimes paired with verbal training for noun and verb retrieval in a group of aphasic patients. Results showed a specific improvement in naming trained nouns and verbs but not in untrained words.
According to these data, gestures and speech are two separate domains. Gestures might interact either at an early stage, when the message to be conveyed is being prepared for linguistic formulation (“conceptual gestures”) or during later stages, when the retrieval of lexical items momentary fails (“lexical gestures”) [1], [9]–[10].
More recently, a different interpretation has been proposed [11]. In agreement with an embodied cognition viewpoint [12]–[13], some authors have suggested that gesture and speech are strongly connected to the same conceptual representation. Words whose retrieval is facilitated by gestures are more likely to be analogically encoded in a multimodal representation including sensory-motor features [11], [14]. The more a word is grounded in sensory-motor features, the more the real execution of the corresponding gesture will facilitate its retrieval [11]. Very recently, Marangolo et al. [15] presented data in favour of the embodied representation proposal, which did not confirm that intentionally performing a gesture prior to name [11] is a necessary prerequisite for enhancing naming. The authors [15] investigated whether the “observation of semantically congruent actions” and/or “the observation and execution of semantically congruent actions” would improve verb-finding difficulties in a group of six aphasic patients. Differently from most of the previous reports [4]–[8], neither treatment was combined with verbal cues. Results showed a significant improvement in verb retrieval not only when subjects, prior to naming, were required to observe and then to execute the action performed by the examiner (“action observation and execution”), but also when they were required to simply observe the action (“action observation”). In both conditions, this improvement was still present two months after the two treatments ended. No significant effects were found in the control condition in which patients first observed the action performed by the examiner and then had to execute a meaningless movement. In short, results clearly showed that the simple observation of a semantically congruent action has reinforced verb retrieval in the same way as the actual execution of the action. Current views assume that a shared motor representation for the execution and observation of actions, the so-called “Mirror Neuron System”, is implicated in understanding others' actions by means of an automatic matching process that links observed and performed actions [16]–[20] In line with the Mirror Neuron hypothesis and the multimodal concept representation proposal [14], the authors argued that in their work the observation of the performed action was sufficient to activate in the semantic system its corresponding sensory-motor representation, which served as input at the lexical level facilitating verb retrieval [15].
The role of action observation, as an effective strategy in neurorehabilitation, has been yet supported by several recent studies showing that action observation has a positive impact on recovery of motor deficits after stroke [21]–[24].
Ertelt et al. [22] combined observation of daily actions with concomitant physical training of the observed actions in eight stroke patients with moderate, chronic motor deficit of the upper limb. A control group of eight participants post-stroke paired motor training with observation of geometric symbols and letters. Significant functional improvement on standard scales occurred for combined action observation and motor training compared with controls despite a stable pre-training baseline. Very recently, these results were replicated in a larger group of twenty-eight participants with chronic upper limb motor deficits, a group affected by Parkinson's disease [21], [23] and post-surgical orthopedic patients [24].
However, it is still an open question as to what extent the Action Observation approach is really effective. It has been suggested that the Mirror Neuron System is indeed more strongly activated by actions that can be reproduced and are part of the motor repertoire of the observer. In an fMRI study, Buccino et al. [25] investigated whether the observation of actions performed by nonconspecifics (e.g. monkey and dog) would activate the same cortical areas that are active when subjects observe actions made by humans. Results showed that when the observed action is common to animals and humans (e.g. biting), there is a clear overlap between the activated areas. In contrast, during the observation of an action that does not belong to humans (e.g. barking), there was a clear difference in the distribution and extent of activations. Similar results were obtained comparing familiar versus unfamiliar actions [26]–[28] and human agents versus robotic agents [29]–[30], even when all actions were matched for kinematics [30]–[32]. Actions that are not part of the motor repertoire of the observer and which therefore cannot be reproduced appear to be recognized in non motor terms.
To further evaluate the proposal that action observation might be an effective rehabilitation approach in stroke patients [15], [21]–[24], in this study we wanted to further investigate as to what extent action observation might be a useful tool in language rehabilitation. In particular, we contrasted the effects induced by observing human video-clips of actions (e.g., dancing, biting, pointing, kicking) versus the results obtained by observing non human video-clips of actions (e.g., barking, printing) in seven patients with lexical verb retrieval disturbances.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Seven chronic aphasic patients (5 females and 2 males) classified as right-handed according to the Edinburgh Inventory [33] were included in the study. Inclusion criteria were the presence of a single left cerebrovascular accident (CVA) at least six months prior to the investigation (see Table 1) with no previous neurological, psychiatric, or substance abuse history. All were native Italian speakers.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. Sociodemographic and Clinical data of the seven aphasic subjects.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038610.t001
All patients were classified as nonfluent aphasics because of their reduced spontaneous speech with short sentences and frequent word-finding difficulties. They had no articulatory deficits with preserved word repetition. In a task requiring the ability to match an auditory presented verb to one of the two semantically related pictures (Verb Comprehension task), their comprehension was intact. As regards commands and auditory sentences, their comprehension was still severely (patient 3 and 6) to mildly impaired (patient 1, 2, 4, 5, 7; 29/36 cut-off score, Token test) [34]. In a naming task, all patients had verb retrieval deficits (the Battery for the analysis of aphasic disorders, BADA test) [35] (see Table 1). On the ideative, ideomotor, bucco-facial tests and on the Gait Apraxia test, no patient revealed an apraxia disorder [36]–[37].
Ethics Statement
The Institutional Review Board of the IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy specifically approved this study. The data analyzed in the current study were collected in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration and our institutional review board.
Materials
Before the training, a list of 115 human (N = 78) and non human (N = 37) videotaped actions matched for frequency of use and length were selected (human actions mean frequency = 23, SEM = 3; non human actions mean frequency = 22; SEM = 3, t = 0.21, p = .83; human action mean length = 8, SEM = 0.2; non human actions: mean length = 8, SEM = 0.2; t = 0.68, p = .50). Human actions were divided into four categories depending on the motor effector performing the action (hand action (N = 21), e.g., cutting; mouth action (N = 20), e.g., drinking; foot action (N = 16), e.g., kicking; body action (N = 21), e.g., dancing). Non human actions were divided into two categories of natural (N = 14, e.g., barking, raining) and mechanical actions (N = 23, e.g., printing, digging). All categories were matched for length and frequency of use (for all t-test comparison → p = n.s.).
To measure the patient's response consistency to each item, the 115 videotaped actions were presented to the patients on a desktop once a day for three consecutive days and they had to respond within 15 seconds. The actions which were correctly named three times out of three would be excluded from rehabilitation. Neither verb was correctly named three times out of three but all patients gave inconsistent responses to all stimuli either producing an incorrect answer or an omission. Therefore, for each patient, all the 115 items were selected for the training.
Treatment procedure
Each participant was asked to participate in an intensive language training which included five daily sessions over two consecutive weeks. In each session, each patient was asked to carefully observe the 115 video-clips of actions projected, one at a time, on a desktop with a 100×200 cm screen. Each action remained on the screen for 15 seconds. After observing the action, he/she was asked to produce the corresponding name.
During the session, the therapist did not facilitate the patient with verbal cues but simply reminded him/her to pay attention to the video clips and manually recorded the answers. If the patient failed to produce an answer or produce an incorrect verb, after 15 seconds the therapist presented the subsequent action. Over sessions, all actions were randomly presented to the subject. The treatment was carried out in a quiet room with the patient sitting comfortably in front of the screen.
At the end of the treatment, to control for a possible transfer of rehabilitation effects also in other tasks, all patients were again administered the language examination tasks.
Data Analysis
The subjects' performance was evaluated by taking into account the mean percentage of accuracy rates (number of stimuli correctly named divided by the number of stimuli presented in each block). First, pre-treatment (baseline performance) and post-treatment mean percentage of correct responses (after two weeks) on the 115 videotaped actions were compared within group by using a 2×2 repeated-measures ANOVA. Two within-subject factors were included: Time (two levels, baseline (T1) vs. end of treatment (T10)) and Condition (two levels, human vs. non-human actions). Interaction was explored using the Scheffè post-hoc test.
In order to control for differences in the mean percentage of correct responses between categories within each group of actions (human and non human), two separate repeated-measures ANOVAs were also performed. In each analysis, two within-subject factors were included: Time (two levels, baseline (T1) vs. end of treatment (T10)) and Condition (four levels: mouth, feet, body and hand category for human actions; and, two levels: natural and mechanical category for non human actions). Interaction was explored using the Scheffè post-hoc test.
In order to measure long-lasting beneficial effects, a repeated-measures ANOVA was also run on three follow-up sessions carried out at one week, one month and two months after the end of the treatment. Two within-subject factors were included: End-Post Treatment (four levels, tenth day vs. first follow-up vs. second follow-up vs. third follow-up) and Condition (two levels, human vs. non-human actions). Interaction was explored using the Scheffè post-hoc test.
Finally, to regard for a possible transfer of verb treatment effects in the language examination, different chi square tests were performed to compare the patient's percentage of correct responses before and after the treatment in the verb naming and in the description task of the BADA examination [35].
Results
The analysis showed a significant effect of Time [two levels, baseline (T1) vs. end of treatment (T10), F (1,6) = 31.77; p = .00) and Condition (two levels, human vs. non-human, F (1, 6) = 63.28; p = .00). Subjects' performance significantly improved at the end of training with respect to baseline performance (mean = 51%, SEM = 7 (T10) vs. mean = 33%, SEM = 5 (T1), p = .0001). Moreover, the mean percentage of correct responses for human actions was significantly greater than for non-human actions (mean = 51%, SEM = 7 (human actions) vs. mean = 33%, SEM = 5 (non human actions) p = .000). The interaction of Time × Condition (F (1,6) = 54.83; p = .00) was also significant.
The Scheffè post-hoc test revealed that, while the mean percentage of correct responses for non human actions at the end of training did not significantly differ from baseline performance (mean = 33%, SEM = 6 (T10), vs. mean = 29%, SEM = 6 (T1), p = .23), a significant improvement was observed between the end of treatment and baseline performance for human actions (mean = 69%, SEM = 8 (T10) vs. mean = 36%, SEM = 8 (T1), p = .000). Moreover, while no significant differences emerged in the mean percentage of correct responses between human and non human actions at baseline (mean = 33%, SEM = 6 (human actions) vs. mean = 29, SEM = 6 (non human actions), p = .6), the mean percentage of response accuracy was greater for human actions than for non human actions at the end of training (mean = 69 %, SEM = 8 (human actions) vs. mean = 36%, SEM = 8 (non human actions), p = .000) (see Figure 1).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 1. Mean percentage of correct responses for human and non human actions at T1 and T10, respectively (*p = .000).
Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038610.g001
The two separate repeated-measures ANOVA performed within each group of actions (human vs. non humans) confirmed the above results. While the effect of Time was significant for human actions [two levels, baseline (t1) vs. end of treatment (t10), F (1,6) = 60.43; p = .000) revealing, as before, a significant improvement in the mean percentage of correct responses at end of treatment with respect to baseline performance (mean = 66%, SEM = 4 (T10) vs. mean = 33%, SEM = 3 (T1), p = .000), no significant effects were found between the two time conditions for non human actions (mean = 38%, SEM = 6 (T10) vs. mean = 32%, SEM = 5 (T1), p = .2). Moreover, for both group of actions (human and non human), no significant effect emerged between the different categories.
It might be argued that, since in the non human natural category we mixed both living (e.g, barking) and non-living (e.g., raining) actions, some possible dissociations in the patients' performance between the two subcategories might have been blinded not analyzing the data separately. Right now, a large amount of literature argues in favour of dissociable mechanisms in the production of living vs. non living things in aphasic patients [38]. In order to control for differences in the mean percentage of correct responses between the two classes, one separate repeated-measures 2×2 ANOVA was performed. Two within-subject factors were included: Time (two levels, baseline (T1) vs. end of treatment (T10)) and Condition (two levels: living vs. non living actions). The analysis revealed no significant effect between the two groups neither for Time [F(1,6) = 0.73; p = .42], nor for Condition [F(1,6) = 2.11; p = .20].
Follow-ups
The analysis showed a significant effect of Condition (two levels, human vs. non-human, F (1, 6) = 116.1; p = .000). The mean percentage of correct responses for human actions was significantly greater than for non human actions (mean = 65%, SEM = 4 (human actions) vs. mean = 36%, SEM = 4 (non human actions) p = .000). The interaction of Time × Condition (F (3,18) = 4.93; p = .01) was also significant.
The Scheffè post-hoc test revealed that the greater amount in the mean percentage of correctly named actions between human and non human actions observed at the end of treatment, was still present at the first (mean = 62% (human actions), SEM = 9 vs. mean = 38%, SEM = 9 (non human actions), p = .000), the second (mean = 64% (human actions), SEM = 7 vs. mean = 36%, SEM = 8 (non human actions), p = .000) and the third follow-up (mean = 62% (human actions), SEM = 8 vs. mean = 35%, SEM = 8 (non human actions), p = .000) (see Figure 2 and Table 2).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 2. Mean percentage of correct responses for human and non human actions at end of treatment (T 10) vs. first follow-up (F/U, one week) vs. second follow-up (one month) vs. third follow-up (two months) (*p = .000).
Error bars represent standard error of the mean (SEM).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038610.g002
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 2. Mean percentage of correct responses (± SEM).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038610.t002
Transfer of verb treatment effects in the language examination tasks
Although only one patient made significant improvement in the verb naming task of the BADA test after the treatment (Pz 3, χ2 = 8.84, p = .003), we found significant differences in the description task of the Language Examination test (see Table 3). For six out of seven patients (one patient had already reached the maximum score before the treatment) chi square test indicated a significant difference in the percentage of correct responses before and after the therapy (p<.000).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 3. For each subject, the percentage of correct responses in the description task of the Language Examination are reported before and after the Observation Therapy.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038610.t003
Discussion
The present study demonstrates that improvement in verb production in chronic aphasia after stroke can be achieved by intensive Action Observation Treatment over two consecutive weeks. Consistent with previous results [15], all patients showed a significant recovery of verb naming by observing video-clips of actions which still persisted after 2 months the treatment ended. This result allows us to confirm that Action Observation Treatment can be an useful strategy to enhance verb production. Most importantly, results clearly indicated that the amount of verb recovery was not equal across the two groups but increased significantly only for human actions.
The fact that the human brain exhibits such a great amount of plasticity and that language can improve late after stroke in such a short period of time may have important implications for future therapeutic interventions in aphasia. The present study suggests that the same basic principles relevant to enhance motor performance [21]–[24] may also be efficacious to improve language functions. These principles include 1) the use of massed practice for short time intervals that can be as effective as the use of long-term but less frequent training [39]–[41] 2) observation of actions that determines a strong impact on the recovery of verb production which persists over time 3) the fact that the therapy works only for actions belonging to the sensory-motor repertoire of the observer.
The choice to use such an intensive training was in accordance with recent proposals which suggest that, for stroke patients with motor deficits, intensive therapy over a short amount of time has greater impact on recovery than less intensive therapy over a long period of time [39]–[40]. Similar results were obtained in the language domain [41]. We found that such an intensive training exerts its influence when subjects are simply asked to observe video-clips of actions. As stated in the introduction, the embodied cognition viewpoint [12]–[13] suggests that the representation of a concept is composed not only of stored information about the features defining that concept, such as its typical form, colour and motion, but also of the motor movement associated with its use. Words whose retrieval is facilitated by gestures are more likely represented in sensory-motor features. The more a word is grounded in sensory-motor features, the more the actual execution of gestures will accompany its retrieval [11], [14]. Therefore, differently from previous reports, we have shown that not only action execution is an unnecessary prerequisite to enhance naming [9]–[11], but also that recovery might occur without verbal facilitation [6]–[8]. In agreement with a multimodal semantic representation proposal, we believe that together with the real execution of an action, the observation of the action directly interacts with the semantic system which enhances the activation of its corresponding sensory-motor representation. This activation serves as input at lexical level, and facilitates the retrieval of the word form. Although our data are strictly behavioural, we could speculate that the mechanism underlying this effect involves the Mirror Neuron System, which is equally active when actions are actually carried out and/or are simply observed [16]–[20]. Earlier studies have proposed that the Mirror Neuron System responds most robustly when watching familiar, executable actions made by conspecifics [25]. Brain regions associated with the Mirror System show stronger responses to human actions than to animal actions [25] and to actions made by robots [29]–[30]. These findings are consistent with the notion that observing actions with familiar kinematic features, which are within one's motor repertoire, result in greater Mirror Neuron involvement than observing less familiar actions. In line with most of the prior reports, we found that only human actions belonging to the sensory-motor experiential repertoire of the observer (e.g, dancing, eating) enhance verb recovery. These effects were consistently present even two months after the end of the treatment independently of the motor effectors (mouth, hand, foot, body) performing the action. We hypothesize that the observed familiar action exerts its influence at the semantic level because the sensory-motor representation, on which it is matched, produces an outcome that is known to the acting individual. Actions that are not part of the motor repertoire of the observer, such as natural (e.g, barking, raining) or mechanical actions (e.g., printing, digging) cannot make use of this matching process.
One final point regards a possible transfer of verb treatment effects in the language examination which would indicate a generalization of the recovery also in other tasks. Although only one patient made significant improvement in the verb naming task of the BADA test after the treatment, more interestingly, six out of seven patients showed a significant change in the description task with an increase in the use of verbs before and after the treatment. These different results might be due to the fact that while the verb naming task is administered using static and non-contextualised picture of actions, in the description task, verb production is more contextually-driven (e.g., the verb “to read” is presented in the context of a man who is reading a newspaper seated in an armchair) and therefore, in some ways, it better duplicates the situation used during the treatment (the video-clips presented were all contextually-driven).
In conclusion, our data clearly confirm that Action Observation Treatment is an useful rehabilitation strategy to promote a long-lasting recovery in verb production in aphasic patients. However, since the number of patients treated was small, the facilitation found between human and non human actions and their corresponding neural correlates deserve further investigations. We believe that these new findings open future directions for planning new therapeutic interventions for language rehabilitation.
Supporting Information
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Appendix S1.
List of human and non human videotaped actions presented in the language training.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038610.s001
(DOC)
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: PM SC VF CR. Performed the experiments: SC VF. Analyzed the data: VF. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SC VF CR. Wrote the paper: PM. Editing and critical revision of the manuscript: PM CC.
Citation: Marangolo P, Cipollari S, Fiori V, Razzano C, Caltagirone C (2012) Walking but Not Barking Improves Verb Recovery: Implications for Action Observation Treatment in Aphasia Rehabilitation. PLoS ONE7(6): e38610. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038610
1. Hanlon RE, Brown JW, Gerstman LJ (1990) Enhancement of naming in nonfluent aphasia through gesture. Brain Lang 38: 298–314.RE HanlonJW BrownLJ Gerstman1990Enhancement of naming in nonfluent aphasia through gesture.Brain Lang38298314
2. Richards K, Singletary F, Koehler S, Crosson B, Rothi LJG (2002) Treatment of nonfluent aphasia through the pairing of a non-symbolic movement sequence and naming. J Rehabil Res Dev 39: 7–16.K. RichardsF. SingletaryS. KoehlerB. CrossonLJG Rothi2002Treatment of nonfluent aphasia through the pairing of a non-symbolic movement sequence and naming.J Rehabil Res Dev39716
3. Pashek GV (1998) Gestural facilitation of noun and verb retrieval in aphasia: A case study. Brain Lang 65: 177–180.GV Pashek1998Gestural facilitation of noun and verb retrieval in aphasia: A case study.Brain Lang65177180
4. PasheK GV (1997) A case study of gesturally cued naming in aphasia: dominant versus nondominant hand training. J Comm Dis 30: 349–366.GV PasheK1997A case study of gesturally cued naming in aphasia: dominant versus nondominant hand training.J Comm Dis30349366
5. Richards K, Singletary F, Rothi LJG, Koehler S, Crosson B (2002) Activation of intentional mechanisms through utilization of nonsymbolic movements in aphasia rehabilitation. J Rehabil Res Dev 39(4): 7–16.K. RichardsF. SingletaryLJG RothiS. KoehlerB. Crosson2002Activation of intentional mechanisms through utilization of nonsymbolic movements in aphasia rehabilitation.J Rehabil Res Dev39(4)716
6. Raimer AM, Singletary F, Rodriquez A, Ciampitti M, Heilman KM, et al. (2006) Effects of gesture + verbal treatment for noun and verb retrieval in aphasia. J Int Neuropsych Soc 12: 867–882.AM RaimerF. SingletaryA. RodriquezM. CiampittiKM Heilman2006Effects of gesture + verbal treatment for noun and verb retrieval in aphasia.J Int Neuropsych Soc12867882
7. Rodriquez A, Raimer AM, Rothi LJG (2006) Effects of gestural + verbal and semantic-phonologic treatments for verb retrieval in aphasia. Aphasiology 20: 286–297.A. RodriquezAM RaimerLJG Rothi2006Effects of gestural + verbal and semantic-phonologic treatments for verb retrieval in aphasia.Aphasiology20286297
8. Rose M, Douglas J, Matyas T (2011) The comparative effectiveness of gesture and verbal treatments for specific phonologic naming impairment. Aphasiology 16: 1001–1030.M. RoseJ. DouglasT. Matyas2011The comparative effectiveness of gesture and verbal treatments for specific phonologic naming impairment.Aphasiology1610011030
9. Hadar U, Wenkert-Olenik D, Krauss RM, Soroker N (1998) Gesture and the processing of speech: Neuropsychological evidence. Brain Lang 62: 107–26.U. HadarD. Wenkert-OlenikRM KraussN. Soroker1998Gesture and the processing of speech: Neuropsychological evidence.Brain Lang6210726
10. Krauss RM, Hadar U (1999) The role of speech related-arm/hand gestures in word retrieval. In R. Campbell, & L. Messing (Eds), Gesture, speech, and sign (93-116). Oxford: Oxford University Press. RM KraussU. Hadar1999The role of speech related-arm/hand gestures in word retrieval. In R. Campbell, & L. Messing (Eds), Gesture, speech, and sign (93-116).Oxford: Oxford University Press
11. Krauss RM, Chen Y, Gottesman RF (2000) Lexical gestures and lexical access: a process model. In D. McNeill (Ed), Language and gesture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. RM KraussY. ChenRF Gottesman2000Lexical gestures and lexical access: a process model. In D. McNeill (Ed), Language and gesture.Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
12. Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci 22: 560–577.LW Barsalou1999Perceptual symbol systems.Behav Brain Sci22560577
13. Gallese V, Lakoff G (2005) The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cogn Neuropsychol 22(3): 455–79.V. GalleseG. Lakoff2005The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge.Cogn Neuropsychol22(3)45579
14. Martin A, Wiggs CL, Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV (2000) Category specificity and the brain: the sensory/motor model of semantic representations of objects. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed), The new cognitive neurosciences, second Edition (1023–1036). Cambridge, MA: the MIT Press. A. MartinCL WiggsLG UngerleiderJV Haxby2000Category specificity and the brain: the sensory/motor model of semantic representations of objects. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed), The new cognitive neurosciences, second Edition (1023–1036).Cambridge, MA: the MIT Press
15. Marangolo P, Bonifazi S, Tomaiuolo F, Craighero L, Coccia M, et al. (2010) Improving Language without words: first evidence from aphasia. Neuropsychologia 48: 3824–33.P. MarangoloS. BonifaziF. TomaiuoloL. CraigheroM. Coccia2010Improving Language without words: first evidence from aphasia.Neuropsychologia48382433
16. Binkofski F, Buccino G (2006) The role of ventral premotor cortex in action execution and action understanding. J Physiol-Paris 99: 396–405.F. BinkofskiG. Buccino2006The role of ventral premotor cortex in action execution and action understanding.J Physiol-Paris99396405
17. Fadiga L, Craighero L, Buccino G, Rizzolatti G (2002) Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: A TMS study. Eur J Neurosci 15: 399–402.L. FadigaL. CraigheroG. BuccinoG. Rizzolatti2002Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: A TMS study.Eur J Neurosci15399402
18. Hauk O, Pulvermuller F (2004) Neurophysiological distinction of Action Words in the Fronto-Central Cortex. Hum Brain Mapp 21: 191–201.O. HaukF. Pulvermuller2004Neurophysiological distinction of Action Words in the Fronto-Central Cortex.Hum Brain Mapp21191201
19. Pulvermuller F, Hauk O, Nikulin VV, Ilmoniemi RJ (2005) Functional links between motor and language systems. Eur J Neurosci 21: 793–797.F. PulvermullerO. HaukVV NikulinRJ Ilmoniemi2005Functional links between motor and language systems.Eur J Neurosci21793797
20. Rizzolatti G, Fabbri-Destro M, Cattaneo L (2009) Mirror neurons and their clinical relevance. Nat Clin Pract Neuro 5: 24–34.G. RizzolattiM. Fabbri-DestroL. Cattaneo2009Mirror neurons and their clinical relevance.Nat Clin Pract Neuro52434
21. Franceschini M, Agosti M, Cantagallo A, Sale P, Mancuso , et al. (2010) Mirror neurons: action observation treatment as a tool in stroke rehabilitation. Eur J Phys Rehab Med 46: 517–23.M. FranceschiniM. AgostiA. CantagalloP. SaleMancuso2010Mirror neurons: action observation treatment as a tool in stroke rehabilitation.Eur J Phys Rehab Med4651723
22. Ertelt D, Small S, Solodkin A, Dettmers C, McNamara A, et al. (2007) Action observation has a positive impact on rehabilitation of motor deficits after stroke. Neuroimage 36: 164–173.D. ErteltS. SmallA. SolodkinC. DettmersA. McNamara2007Action observation has a positive impact on rehabilitation of motor deficits after stroke.Neuroimage36164173
23. Buccino G, Gatti R, Giusti MC, Negrotti A, Rossi A, et al. (2011) Action observation treatment improves autonomy in daily activities in Parkinson's disease patients: Results from a pilot study. Mov Disord 15: 1963–64.G. BuccinoR. GattiMC GiustiA. NegrottiA. Rossi2011Action observation treatment improves autonomy in daily activities in Parkinson's disease patients: Results from a pilot study.Mov Disord15196364
24. Bellelli G, Buccino G, Bernardini B, Padovani A, Trabucchi M (2010) Action observation treatment improves recovery of postsurgical orthopedic patients: evidence for a top-down effect? Arch Phys Med Rehab 91: 1489–94.G. BellelliG. BuccinoB. BernardiniA. PadovaniM. Trabucchi2010Action observation treatment improves recovery of postsurgical orthopedic patients: evidence for a top-down effect?Arch Phys Med Rehab91148994
25. Buccino G, Lui F, Canessa N, Patteri I, Lagravinese G, et al. (2004) Neural circuits involved in the recognition of actions performed by nonconspecifics: An FMRI study. J Cognitive Neurosci 16: 114–126.G. BuccinoF. LuiN. CanessaI. PatteriG. Lagravinese2004Neural circuits involved in the recognition of actions performed by nonconspecifics: An FMRI study.J Cognitive Neurosci16114126
26. Calvo-Merino B, Glaser DE, Grezes J, Passingham RE, Haggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motor skills: An FMRI study with expert dancers. Cereb Cortex 15: 1243–1249.B. Calvo-MerinoDE GlaserJ. GrezesRE PassinghamP. Haggard2005Action observation and acquired motor skills: An FMRI study with expert dancers.Cereb Cortex1512431249
27. Cross ES, Hamilton AF, Kraemer DJ, Kelley WM, Grafton ST (2009a) Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human action observation network. Eur J Neurosci 30: 1383–1392.ES CrossAF HamiltonDJ KraemerWM KelleyST Grafton2009aDissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human action observation network.Eur J Neurosci3013831392
28. Cross ES, Kraemer DJ, Hamilton AF, Kelley WM, Grafton ST (2009b) Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. Cereb Cortex 19: 315–326.ES CrossDJ KraemerAF HamiltonWM KelleyST Grafton2009bSensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning.Cereb Cortex19315326
29. Tai YF, Scherfler C, Brooks DJ, Sawamoto N, Castiello U (2004) The human premotor cortex is ‘mirror’ only for biological actions. Curr Biol 14: 117–120.YF TaiC. ScherflerDJ BrooksN. SawamotoU. Castiello2004The human premotor cortex is ‘mirror’ only for biological actions.Curr Biol14117120
30. Shimada S (2010) Deactivation in the sensorimotor area during observation of a human agent performing robotic actions. Brain Cognition 72: 394–399.S. Shimada2010Deactivation in the sensorimotor area during observation of a human agent performing robotic actions.Brain Cognition72394399
31. Chaminade T, Zecca M, Blakemore SJ, Takanishi A, Frith CD, et al. (2010) Brain response to a humanoid robot in areas implicated in the perception of human emotional gestures. PLoS One 21: e11577.T. ChaminadeM. ZeccaSJ BlakemoreA. TakanishiCD Frith2010Brain response to a humanoid robot in areas implicated in the perception of human emotional gestures.PLoS One21e11577
32. Miura N, Sugiura M, Takahashi M, Sassa Y, Miyamoto A, et al. (2010) Effect of motion smoothness on brain activity while observing a dance: An fMRI study using a humanoid robot. Soc Neurosci 5: 40–58.N. MiuraM. SugiuraM. TakahashiY. SassaA. Miyamoto2010Effect of motion smoothness on brain activity while observing a dance: An fMRI study using a humanoid robot.Soc Neurosci54058
33. Odfield RC (1971) The Assessment and Analysis of Handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 9: 97–113.RC Odfield1971The Assessment and Analysis of Handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory.Neuropsychologia997113
34. De Renzi E, Faglioni P (1978) Normative data and screening power of a shortened version of Token Test. Cortex 14: 41–49.E. De RenziP. Faglioni1978Normative data and screening power of a shortened version of Token Test.Cortex144149
35. Miceli G, Laudanna A, Burani C, Capasso R (1994) Batteria per l'analisi dei deficit afasici. BADA. Cepsag. Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore Policlinico Gemelli. G. MiceliA. LaudannaC. BuraniR. Capasso1994Batteria per l'analisi dei deficit afasici. BADA. Cepsag.Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore Policlinico Gemelli
36. De Renzi E, Motti F, Nichelli P (1980) Imitating gestures. A quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia. Arch Neurol 37: 6–10.E. De RenziF. MottiP. Nichelli1980Imitating gestures. A quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia.Arch Neurol37610
37. Della Sala S, Spinnler H, Venneri A (2005) Walking difficulties in patients with Alzheimer's disease might originate from gait apraxia. J Neurol Neurosur Ps 75: 196–201.S. Della SalaH. SpinnlerA. Venneri2005Walking difficulties in patients with Alzheimer's disease might originate from gait apraxia.J Neurol Neurosur Ps75196201
38. Capitani E, Laiacona M, Mahon B, Caramazza A (2003) What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits? A critical review of the clinical evidence. Cog Neuropsychol 20: 213–61.E. CapitaniM. LaiaconaB. MahonA. Caramazza2003What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits? A critical review of the clinical evidence.Cog Neuropsychol2021361
39. Moore JL, Roth EJ, Killian C, Hornby TG (2010) Locomotor training improves daily stepping activity and gait efficiency in individuals post-stroke who have reached a “plateau” in recovery. Stroke 41: 129–35.JL MooreEJ RothC. KillianTG Hornby2010Locomotor training improves daily stepping activity and gait efficiency in individuals post-stroke who have reached a “plateau” in recovery.Stroke4112935
40. Hornby TG, Straube DS, Kinnaird CR, Holleran CL, Echaur AJ (2011) Importance of specificity, amount and intensity of locomotor training to improve ambulatory function in patients poststroke. Top Stroke Rehabil 18: 293–307.TG HornbyDS StraubeCR KinnairdCL HolleranAJ Echaur2011Importance of specificity, amount and intensity of locomotor training to improve ambulatory function in patients poststroke.Top Stroke Rehabil18293307
41. Bhogal SK, Teasell R, Speechley M, Albert ML (2003) Intensity of Aphasia Therapy, Impact on Recovery* Aphasia Therapy Works! Stroke 34: 987–993.SK BhogalR. TeasellM. SpeechleyML Albert2003Intensity of Aphasia Therapy, Impact on Recovery* Aphasia Therapy Works! Stroke34987993
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2012 Marangolo et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Recent studies have shown that action observation treatment without concomitant verbal cue has a positive impact on the recovery of verb retrieval deficits in aphasic patients. In agreement with an embodied cognition viewpoint, a hypothesis has been advanced that gestures and language form a single communication system and words whose retrieval is facilitated by gestures are semantically represented through sensory-motor features. However, it is still an open question as to what extent this treatment approach works. Results from the recovery of motor deficits have suggested that action observation promotes motor recovery only for actions that are part of the motor repertoire of the observer. The aim of the present experiment was to further investigate the role of action observation treatment in verb recovery. In particular, we contrasted the effects induced by observing human actions (e.g. dancing, kicking, pointing, eating) versus non human actions (e.g. barking, printing). Seven chronic aphasic patients with a selective deficit in verb retrieval underwent an intensive rehabilitation training that included five daily sessions over two consecutive weeks. Each subject was asked to carefully observe 115 video-clips of actions, one at a time and, after observing them, they had to produce the corresponding verb. Two groups of actions were randomly presented: humans versus nonhuman actions. In all patients, significant improvement in verb retrieval was found only by observing video-clips of human actions. Moreover, follow-up testing revealed long-term verb recovery that was still present two months after the two treatments had ended. In support of the multimodal concept representation's proposal, we suggest that just the observation of actions pertaining to the human motor repertoire is an effective rehabilitation approach for verb recovery.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer