About the Authors:
Waleed O. Farid
Contributed equally to this work with: Waleed O. Farid, Andrew J. Lawrence, Sarah A. Dunlop
Affiliations Experimental and Regenerative Neurosciences, School of Animal Biology, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia, Australia, Unit for Research and Education in Drugs and Alcohol, School of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia, Australia
Andrew J. Lawrence
Contributed equally to this work with: Waleed O. Farid, Andrew J. Lawrence, Sarah A. Dunlop
Affiliations Florey Neuroscience Institutes, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia, Centre for Neuroscience, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Elena V. Krstew
Affiliation: Florey Neuroscience Institutes, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Robert J. Tait
Affiliations Unit for Research and Education in Drugs and Alcohol, School of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia, Australia, Centre for Mental Health Research, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australian Capital Territory, Australia
Gary K. Hulse
Affiliation: Unit for Research and Education in Drugs and Alcohol, School of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia, Australia
Sarah A. Dunlop
Contributed equally to this work with: Waleed O. Farid, Andrew J. Lawrence, Sarah A. Dunlop
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliations Experimental and Regenerative Neurosciences, School of Animal Biology, The University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia, Australia, The Western Australian Institute for Medical Research, Perth, Western Australia, Australia
Introduction
Naltrexone is a non-selective opioid receptor antagonist [1], used clinically for persons wanting to abstain from opiates [2] and/or alcohol [3], [4]. However, naltrexone is not recommended during pregnancy [5]. Unfortunately, some pregnant opioid-dependent women have received oral naltrexone [6]. Moreover, with the advent of sustained-release naltrexone preparations and their emerging clinical use, inadvertent foetal naltrexone exposure, particularly around conception, is a genuine possibility [7]. To date, 52 women have become pregnant while being treated with an Australian naltrexone implant (GKH, personal communication).
Limited data from humans treated with oral or sustained-release naltrexone suggest no major adverse neonatal outcomes with respect to birth-weights, APGAR scores and head circumferences [6], [8], [9] but long-term follow-up is lacking. However, animal studies, primarily in rat, show adverse neonatal outcomes, but involved daily subcutaneous injections given directly to postnatal offspring rather than clinically relevant maternal (i.e. indirect) and sustained naltrexone exposure. The primary objective of this study was to use a rat model of maternal administration involving sustained-release of naltrexone throughout gestation and lactation at a clinically-relevant plasma concentration of ∼2–10 ng/ml [7], [10] and to determine outcomes in neonates and adult offspring.
Previous studies show that rat neonates directly receiving low-dose naltrexone (1 mg/kg/day) are growth retarded [11], [12] whereas those exposed to high doses (50 mg/kg/day) have increased birth-weights [13], [14]. We therefore assessed the morphometric impact of our sustained-release maternal naltrexone exposure regime hypothesizing that low-dose sustained-release naltrexone exposure would result in reduced birth-weights.
Furthermore, it has been shown that perinatal exposure to naltrexone (1–50 mg/kg/day) can alter aspects of adult offspring behaviour pertaining to emotionality, exploratory drive and analgesic response to morphine [15], [16], [17], [18]. However, changes to locomotor activity are reported not to occur, and to date, no studies are known to have assessed morphine-induced psychomotor sensitization, an indicator of drug-induced plasticity pertinent to aspects of addiction [19], [20]. Moreover, morphine self-administration experiments have not yet been undertaken in naltrexone-exposed offspring. Nonetheless, prenatal exposure to naltrexone can reduce sensitivity to morphine as indicated by decreased open-field locomotion [18]. We therefore assessed morphine-induced sensitization and intravenous morphine self-administration in maternally exposed adult offspring. Given that prenatal naltrexone exposure reduces the density of µ-opioid receptors in offspring with reduced morphine-induced locomotor activity [18], we analysed opioid neurochemistry, namely μ-opioid receptor [21], [22] as well as mRNA for preproenkephalin (PPE) and preprodynorphin (PPD) [23].
Materials and Methods
Animals
Experiments adhered to the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals Act, 1986 and the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council Code of Practice for the Care and Use of Animals for Experimental Purposes in Australia. There were 3 cohorts. Procedures for cohort 1 were approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of the University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia (Approval Number: RA 3 100 423) and the Florey Neuroscience Institutes, Melbourne, Australia (Approval Numbers 05-061). Procedures for cohorts 2 and 3 were approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of the University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia (Approval Number: RA 3 100 618) and the Florey Neuroscience Institutes, Melbourne, Australia (Approval Number: 08-001).
Three cohorts (Figure 1) of Sprague-Dawley rats (Animal Resource Centre, Murdoch University, Australia) were housed in pairs or threes (23°C; 12-h light/dark cycle) with food/water available ad libitum. All females were nulliparous and 9–11 weeks of age upon arrival. In cohort 1, there were 16 dams (180–270 g), in cohort 2 there were 12/treatment group (197–315 g) and in cohort 3 there were 6/treatment group (240–290 g). Ten males (330–460 g) were used for husbandry.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 1. Timelines and overall study design.
Timelines and overall study design is shown for all 3 cohorts, including main procedures (i.e. events) and offspring ages (A and B), experimental outcome measures (A), and sampling of blood/amniotic fluid (B). Each cohort can be identified by their respective arrows (cohort 1, black-dashed; cohort 2, black-filled; cohort 3, gray-filled), which provide a guide for the procedures that were undertaken and the approximate end-point. * maternal implant excision and histopathology data published elsewhere [45]. More detailed timelines for the behavioural sensitization and operant self-administration experiments are presented in Figures 2 and 3, respectively.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.g001
Drugs
Morphine hydrochloride: Glaxo, Australia, in sterile 0.9% saline. Ketamine: Parnell Laboratories, Australia; meloxicam: Boeringher Ingleheim, Germany; neomycin sulfate: Delta Veterinary Laboratories, Australia; and carprofen: Norbrook Laboratories, Australia. Heparinised saline was diluted at 90 IU/ml.
Custom-sized naltrexone implants comprised the same formulation as for humans (naltrexone-loaded poly[trans-3,6-dimethyl-1,4-dioxyane-2,5-dione] (DL-lactide) microspheres, average molecular weight 700,000 Mv, inherent viscosity 0.5 dl/g, specific rotation of –2° to +2°) compressed into a tablet (total weight: 50 mg; 25 mg naltrexone, 25 mg poly[DL-lactide]; 5 mm diameter × 2 mm height) surrounded by a single coat of poly(DL-lactide) [24]. Placebo implants had the same dimensions and contained 50 mg of poly(DL-lactide) microspheres. All implants were sterilised using irradiation (range 25–40 KGy). Previous in vitro (data not shown) and in vivo experiments [24] demonstrate that the custom-sized implant used here has similar release rates (0.4–0.8%/day) and plasma levels (2–10 ng/ml) to that measured during their use in humans [10], [25].
General Procedures
Three cohorts were used in the current study to evaluate the extent of naltrexone exposure in offspring and dams during pregnancy (cohort 1) and before/after pregnancy (cohorts 2 and 3), evaluate gross anatomical outcomes in offspring at birth and adulthood and changes in neurochemistry in adulthood (cohort 2), as well as evaluate behavioural outcomes in adult offspring (cohorts 2 and 3). An overview of the experimental design and timing of events/outcome measures for cohorts 1, 2 and 3 is presented in Figure 1A.
Surgery.
Dams had a single naltrexone or placebo implant inserted subcutaneously under general anaesthesia (2–3% isoflurane). Carprofen (5 mg/kg, s.c.) and neomycin (50 mg/kg, s.c.) were injected post-operatively. Dams in cohort 1 were implanted with naltrexone and those in cohorts 2 and 3 with naltrexone or placebo implants.
Blood sampling and analysis.
Blood was collected into lithium-heparinised tubes and frozen at −80°C. For cohort 1, maternal blood was obtained from a lateral tail vein [26] and samples from foetal/neonatal rats by jugular incision, pooling from each litter. Amniotic fluid was also pooled. All other maternal and offspring samples (cohorts 1, 2 and 3) were obtained by incising the atrium after terminal anaesthesia. The concentrations of free naltrexone and 6,β-naltrexol in blood and amniotic fluid samples were determined (Perth Chemistry Centre, Australia) by liquid-chromatography/mass-spectrometry (LCMS) [27]. Reporting limits were 0.2 ng/ml for naltrexone and 0.1 ng/ml for 6,β-naltrexol.
Timed mating.
Initiation of timed mating took place 12 days after surgical insertion of implants. Timed-matings were undertaken by placement of nulliparous rat dams with 2–3 males overnight; mating was confirmed the following morning by the presence of spermatozoa in vaginal smears. In most cases, conception occurred within 3–5 days of initial presentation to males.
Cohort 1
Naltrexone and 6, β-naltrexol levels during pregnancy.
All 16 dams received a naltrexone implant, were mated, and sampled at either gestation day 13, 15 18 or at birth, following euthanasia (n = 4 per time-point). Offspring blood was collected at gestation day 18 and at birth, and was pooled into one sample per litter. At gestation days 13 and 15, amniotic fluid was collected and also pooled into a single sample for each litter (n = 4 per time-point). Naltrexone and 6,β-naltrexol levels in all samples were measured as above. The sampling time-points for maternal rats and offspring from cohorts 1, 2 and 3 are summarised in Figure 1B.
Cohort 2
Naltrexone and 6, β-naltrexol levels before conception, at birth and at weaning.
Cohort 2 females were randomly allocated to receive either a naltrexone or placebo implant, were impregnated and gave birth (n = 12/treatment group). Blood was obtained from all maternal rats 1 day prior to mating and at birth. Offspring were either euthanised, sampled for blood and examined at birth (from 6 maternal rats per group), or were raised to 4 weeks, weaned, and raised until 10 weeks (using the remaining 6 dams per group), whereupon they were transferred to the Florey Neurosciences Institute, for evaluation of locomotor behaviour and neurochemistry. Maternal rats fostering these offspring had a blood sample taken at weaning. Individual blood samples were collected from one male and one female from each litter at weaning and at 8 weeks.
Body and brain morphometry.
Duration of pregnancy, litter size and gender were recorded. At birth and 8 weeks, offspring body-weight was recorded and heel-toe, occipital-snout and biparietal lengths were measured using vernier callipers. At 8 weeks, one male and female per litter were perfused transcardially with heparinised saline (5000 IU/L), followed by 250 ml 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.2 M phosphate buffer (pH 7.2–7.4) for brain morphometry. Brain weight was recorded and cerebrum length, width, height, as well as brain length (i.e. cerebrum+cerebellum) were measured using vernier callipers, and brain volume was calculated by water displacement as described previously [28], [29], [30].
Behavioural sensitization.
Upon arrival of offspring at the Florey Neuroscience Institutes, animals were acclimatised for 2 weeks and no procedures were undertaken. At 12–14 weeks of age, sensitization to locomotor responses induced by repeated morphine treatment was performed as previously described [31], [32]. Briefly, adult male and female offspring were placed individually in photo-optic locomotor cells (Truscan Photobeam; Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown, PA, USA) in a low luminosity (20 lux), controlled environment, for 60 minutes per day for 3 consecutive days. Vertical (rearing) and horizontal (ambulatory) movements were measured by optic sensor beams.
Testing proceeded as follows. Habituation to the test environment was established using 3 daily 60 min sessions within locomotor chambers. On days 4–8, rats were placed in the locomotor chambers 30 min after an injection of saline (1 ml/kg) or morphine (10 mg/kg, s.c.) and activity recorded for 60 min. From days 9–15, animals underwent a period of home-cage abstinence (i.e. they received no injections). On day 16, rats were challenged with saline or morphine (5 mg/kg) and returned to the locomotor chambers (Figure 2). Treatment groups were: saline-saline, (saline: days 4–8 and 16, SS); saline-morphine (SM); morphine-morphine (MM).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 2. Timeline and paradigm for behavioural sensitization experiment: offspring from cohort 2.
Treatments are shown (habituation, daily injection with morphine [10 mg/kg] or saline, period of abstinence, challenge with morphine [5 mg/kg] or saline injection, brains fresh frozen for neurochemistry experiments), with corresponding day since start of locomotor sensitization testing, and number of days for each treatment (i.e. duration).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.g002
For these experiments, we did not observe differences between males and females. Therefore, for logistical reasons relating to Animal Ethics approval (i.e. minimising animal numbers), only males were used for the subsequent neurochemical and operant self-administration experiments.
Neurochemistry.
At 14 weeks, after behavioural sensitization testing (∼3 h after final treatment), male rats were terminally anaesthetised (pentobarbitone, 80 mg/kg, i.p.) and brains removed and frozen. Serial sagittal brain sections (4.2–3.4 mm lateral to the mid-sagittal sinus, 14 µm; [33] were thaw-mounted onto slides and stored at −80°C.
[125I]DAla2,N-Me-Phe4,Met(O)5-ol enkephalin (FK33,824) was used to label high affinity µ-opioid receptors [34], [35]. Slides were incubated in 50 mM Tris-HCl (pH 7.4; 0.1% bovine serum albumin, 0.1 nM [125I]FK33,824, 2000 Ci/mmol) for 60 min. Non-specific binding was determined by addition of 10 µM naloxone to the incubation medium. Slides were washed, dried and apposed to Biomax AR film (Kodak XAR-5) with [14C]microscales. Films were developed automatically (100 Plus Automatic x-ray film processor: All-Pro Imaging).
Expression of PPE and PPD mRNA has previously been described [36]. Oligonucleotides were 3′-end labelled with [α33P]d-adenosine triphosphate using terminal deoxynucleotidyl transferase. Labelled probes (1 pg/µl, 100 µl) and applied to adjacent sections in hybridisation buffer (50% formamide, 4×saline sodium citrate, pH 7.0, 10% dextran sulfate). Controls used a 100-fold molar excess of unlabelled oligonucleotide. Sections were hybridised in sealed humidified chambers at 42°C overnight, rinsed (1×saline sodium citrate, 1 h, 55°C), dehydrated in ethanol and air-dried. Dry sections were apposed to x-ray film with [14C]microscales and then developed.
Autoradiographs showing binding of radioligand to µ-opioid receptor, and binding of oligonucleotide probes to PPE and PPD mRNA, were captured using a Sony XC-77CE CD Video Camera with an attached Nikon Micro-Nikkor 55 mm lens. Sufficient contrast to background intensity allowed manual delineation of brain regions [33]. Tissue was analysed with an MCID M4 image analysis system under constant illumination; Scion Image software was used to convert optical densities to radioactivity per unit area (disintegrations per minute per millimeter squared; DPM/mm2) by means of a calibration curve derived from autoradiograms of the simultaneously-apposed [14C]microscales.
Cohort 3
Naltrexone and 6, β-naltrexol levels at birth and at weaning.
Dams from cohort 3 were randomly allocated to receive either a naltrexone or placebo implant, were mated and gave birth. At birth, litters were reduced to 10 (5 males and 5 females) and culled neonates from each litter had blood pooled into one sample per litter. Surviving offspring were weaned at 4 weeks, at which point maternal blood was collected, and offspring were reared until 10 weeks of age, whereupon they were transported to the Florey Neuroscience Institutes for assessment for operant self-administration of morphine. Upon arrival, animals were acclimatised for 2 weeks and no procedures undertaken.
Operant self-administration.
Self-administration of intravenous morphine (0.1 or 0.3 mg/kg/infusion) was assessed using operant chambers (Med Associates, USA) with two levers. Rats were connected via a jugular cannula to an intravenous line (polyethylene, inner diameter (ID): 0.6 mm, outer diameter (OD): 1.2 mm) connected to a 22-gauge swivel (Instech Solomon, USA) and a syringe held in an infusion pump (PHM-100SVA; Med Associates) with Bcoex-T22 tubing (Tygon, ID 0.6 mm, OD: 1.6 mm; Instech Soloman). A light (conditioned stimulus, CS) came on for 20 s, contingent with an active lever press and reward delivery. In addition, a drop of vanilla essence provided an olfactory cue (S+) for the location of the active lever. The chambers were housed in sound attenuated boxes and ventilated with fans. Med-PC IV software (Med Associates) was used to record lever presses.
The cannulae consisted of a 10 cm length of silastic tubing (inner diameter 0.635 mm, outer diameter 1.194 mm, Dow Corning Corporation, Midland, MI, USA) sleeved over a 22-gauge cannula (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA, USA). All cannulae were autoclaved. At 12 weeks, rats were anaesthetised (2–3% isoflurane) and cannulae implanted into the jugular vein and flushed with 0.15 ml of antibiotic (1.2 mg trimethoprim and 6 mg sulfadoxin) and heparinised saline. Meloxicam (0.75 mg, i.p.) was given for pain relief. The day after surgery, cannulae were flushed with heparinised antibiotic; thereafter, cannulae were flushed twice daily with heparinised saline. Starting on the day of cannulation, rat daily food intake was restricted to 15 g as this is known to facilitate self-administration of abused drugs [37], [38], [39], [40], [41].
After 3 days of recovery in the home cage, rats underwent operant self-administration training to respond for oral sucrose (5%) at a fixed-ratio of 2 (FR2) schedule. Each of these training sessions were 15 h in duration and took place once daily until lever discrimination was demonstrated (after 3 days in most cases). Subsequently, rats were connected to morphine via the jugular cannula to the intravenous line. The injection volume was 48 µl per infusion and duration of injection was 2.3 s. A maximum of 50 drug infusions was set, with a 20-s timeout period after an infusion. All post-training sessions (except for cue-induced drug-seeking session at the very end) were 2 h in length, unless 50 infusions were obtained, and were held just before the dark phase of the photoperiod. The active lever remained the same for both oral sucrose training and intravenous self-administration of morphine experiments, as did the position of the olfactory cue and contingent light stimulus.
Rats responded for intravenous morphine (0.3 mg/kg/infusion) at a fixed-ratio of 1 (FR1) schedule (3 weeks), with subsequent assessment on a progressive-ratio (PR) 9–4 schedule [42] for 2 days, then PR3-4 [43] for 2 days (Table 1). The following day, the rats were returned to FR1 as before, but using a morphine dose of 0.1 mg/kg/infusion; the order and durations for each phase of testing were the same as for the higher morphine dose. Subsequently, rats remained drug-free in the home cage for 6 weeks. Then, drug-seeking was assessed under extinction conditions (FR1 response resulted in CS but no infusion of morphine) for 1 h. An overview of this experimental paradigm of different responding schedules is presented as a timeline in Figure 3.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 3. Timeline and paradigm for operant self-administration experiment: offspring from cohort 3.
Treatments are shown (oral sucrose training, morphine self-administration at 0.1 mg/kg/infusion dose, then at 0.3 mg/kg/infusion dose, period of abstinence, cue-induced drug-seeking), with corresponding schedule of reinforcement (FR1 and FR2, fixed ratio of 1 and 2, respectively; PR9-4 and PR3-4, progressive ratio 9-4 and 3–4, respectively [see Table 1]; no schedule of reinforcement represented by ‘–’), and corresponding number of days for each treatment schedule (i.e. duration).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.g003
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. Instrumental requirement for PR9-4 and PR3-4 schedules.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.t001
The patency of the cannulae was evaluated periodically using ketamine (5 mg). If loss of righting reflex was not apparent within 3 s of infusion, the rat was excluded. In addition, rats were excluded if any cannula blockage or leakage was identified during daily flushing. During assessment of FR1 responding for morphine, criteria were set at a lever discrimination of ≥65% and <25% variation between the numbers of infusions over 3 days.
Statistical Analyses
Unless otherwise indicated, statistical analysis was undertaken using SPSS for Windows and two-tailed tests. Results for free naltrexone and 6,β-naltrexone, in blood or amniotic fluid, were examined with Pearson Correlation coefficient between maternal and offspring levels, stage of pregnancy and time post-implant. Where data failed tests of normality, a Kruskal-Wallis nonparametric ANOVA was employed. Results are for male and females combined unless mentioned otherwise.
Observational and morphometric data were evaluated by one-way ANOVA because violations of the underlying data prevented the use of multiple analysis of variance; accordingly, to reduce the potential for Type 1 errors [44], stringent alpha levels of p<0.0125 and p<0.008 were used for measures of body and brain morphometry, respectively. Morphometric data are reported separately for male and female offspring (Table 2).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 2. Cohort 2 offspring body and brain morphometric measurements following maternal treatment with naltrexone or placebo implants.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.t002
Data from locomotor studies were analysed using a two-way ANOVA, with drug treatment and exposure duration as factors, and with post-hoc Student-Newman-Keuls analysis. To evaluate overall differences in activity between treatment groups, a generalised linear model for repeated measures was used.
Neurochemistry data (from examination of only male brains) were incorporated into generalised linear mixed models for statistical analysis using SAS; significance and interactions were assessed between the following fixed factors: maternal drug exposure (naltrexone and placebo); duration of morphine exposure; and ligand ([125I]FK33,824)/probe ([33P]preproenkephalin, [33P]preprodynorphin).
Operant self-administration data were obtained for male offspring only. For time course analyses (PR and drug-seeking cumulative responses), and analyses of the effect of implant treatment and dose on morphine self-administration, a two-way ANOVA was performed. Comparisons of the PR data between the offspring groups were analysed using an unpaired t-test. Drug-seeking data were analysed by one-way ANOVA, with Student-Newman-Keuls post-tests. Differences were deemed significant if p<0.05, and for pair-wise comparisons, if p<0.05 after adjustments using the Dunn-Sidák procedure.
Results
General Observations
Data from cohort 2 offspring were combined for analysis of litter size, length of gestation and gender ratio. In cohort 2, there were 10 naltrexone-exposed litters and 10 placebo-exposed litters which, respectively, produced 144, and 117 live pups. There were ∼23% more pups per litter following maternal naltrexone treatment compared to placebo (F(1,19) = 13.53; p<0.01; Table 3). There was no significant effect of drug treatment on term of pregnancy (F(1,19) = 2.70; p>0.05; Table 3), nor in gender ratio at birth (χ2(1,19) = 3.29; p>0.05; Table 3).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 3. The effects of maternal naltrexone, or placebo, implants on offspring.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.t003
Naltrexone and 6, β-naltrexol Levels in Dams and Offspring
In both dams and offspring, free 6,β-naltrexol blood-levels were low with many below the limit of detection (>0.1 ng/mg) and a maximum blood concentration of 0.3 ng/ml found in two maternal samples. Nevertheless, a positive correlation existed between naltrexone and 6,β-naltrexol maternal levels: r <0.01, N = 94 (data not shown).
Free naltrexone was quantified in maternal and offspring blood samples, as well as in amniotic fluid (cohort 1). The weight-adjusted naltrexone concentration in maternal blood was 1.19±0.19 ng/ml; raw offspring naltrexone concentration in blood or amniotic fluid was 1.23±0.25 ng/ml. Further analysis revealed that offspring levels correlated with maternal levels: r <0.001, N = 15 (Figure 4A). Furthermore, both maternal (p<0.01) and offspring (p<0.05) levels correlated with time post-implant (Figure 4A).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 4. Free naltrexone levels in dams and offspring with naltrexone implant.
Temporal profiles of free naltrexone concentration are shown in offspring blood or amniotic fluid (A, cohort 1) or maternal blood (A and B, cohorts 1, 2 & 3.) after maternal administration with a naltrexone implant (25 mg, s.c.). A: Temporal profiles of free naltrexone concentration in offspring blood (GD18 [purple] and Birth [blue]) or amniotic fluid (GD13 [yellow] and GD15 [green]) or maternal blood (black) after maternal administration with a naltrexone implant (25 mg, s.c.). Cohort 1 maternal and offspring rats were sampled throughout gestation. Exponential lines of best fit are shown for maternal rats (y = 6.76e−0.07x, filled line) and offspring (y = 9.45e−0.09x, dashed line). Offspring levels correlated with maternal levels (Pearson Correlation [ = 0.75] significant at 0.001 level; N = 15); and both maternal (p<0.01; R2 = 0.62) and offspring (p<0.05; R2 = 0.55) levels correlated with the number of days post-implant. To allow appropriate comparison with offspring, unadjusted maternal data are presented here. C: conception; GD: gestation day; B: birth. B: Maternal naltrexone-blood levels prior to conception, at birth and at weaning from cohorts 1 (closed circles), 2 (open circles) and 3 (crosses) were associated with days post-implant (p<0.01, N = 54; mean ± SEM: 2.86±0.62 ng/ml). A polynomial line of best fit indicated a moderate level of association (R2 = 0.68). C: conception, B: birth, W: weaning.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.g004
For cohort 2, naltrexone was detected in maternal and offspring blood at birth and at weaning (postnatal day 28). Naltrexone was not detected at postnatal day 56 (adulthood). In some offspring at weaning, naltrexone was not detected (n = 8) and these data were excluded from the calculation of means. Unadjusted maternal naltrexone-blood levels are reported elsewhere [45]. Here, we report weight-adjusted maternal mean ± SEM, which declined from 9.74±1.59 ng/ml (n = 12) at 1 day prior to conception to 1.18±0.50 ng/ml (n = 5) at <24 h of birth, 1.18±0.33 ng/ml (n = 5) at postnatal day 3 and 0.30±0.15 ng/ml (n = 5) at weaning. Offspring levels (mean ± S.E.M) were 0.99±0.27 (n = 5) at <24 h of birth and 0.34±0.00 ng/ml (n = 3) at weaning.
At weaning, the weight-adjusted mean ± SEM for free naltrexone in blood from implanted females (cohort 3) was 0.95±0.32 ng/ml (n = 6). Raw data for naltrexone in blood from offspring culled at birth (pooled into one sample per litter) was 3.17±1.65 ng/ml. Weight-adjusted maternal values for free naltrexone-blood concentration with respect to the number of days post-implant are represented for all mothers (cohorts 1, 2 and 3) in Figure 4B. Maternal blood levels sampled from all 3 cohorts correlated with the number of days post-implant (r <0.01, N = 54; Figure 4B). Levels after the initial sample taken at preconception (12 days post-implant) also correlated with days post implant (r <0.01, N = 42; mean ± SEM: 0.90±0.12 ng/ml; Figure 4B).
Body and Brain Morphometry
Total litter size, as well as maternal weights from each group, were used as covariates. Covariates appearing in the model of offspring at birth were evaluated at 239.42 (mothers’ weight) and 12.66 (litter size). For offspring evaluated in adulthood, values were 272.22 mothers’ weight and 13.67 for litter size.
After adjusting for covariates, naltrexone had a significant effect on offspring body weight at birth (F(1,129) = 14.93; p<0.001; Table 2). Estimated marginal means for body weight were 10.9% less in the naltrexone-group compared to that of placebo offspring. There was also a trend for gender to impact on offspring birth-weight (F(1,129) = 3.81; p = 0.05; Table 2), with males 3.9% heavier than females. Both male and female pups in the naltrexone group had lower birth-weights than placebo counterparts.
In adult offspring, there were no effects from drug treatment on body weight (F(1,130) = 1.73; p>0.05; Table 2). There was, however, a significant effect of gender on body weight (F(1,130) = 14.26; p<0.001; Table 2). Naltrexone-exposed adult offspring did not differ significantly from those exposed to placebo with respect to occipital-to-snout length, although planned comparison revealed a trend for naltrexone to affect head size, with a 2.3% reduction in the estimated marginal mean of occipital-to-snout length in naltrexone offspring when compared to placebo (F(1,130) = 3.28; p = 0.04; Table 2).
For brain morphometry, only adult offspring were examined. Each variable was assessed separately with drug group and gender as fixed factors. None of the measures showed a significant main effect of drug group or a significant group by gender interaction. Each variable showed a significant main effect of gender at the adjusted alpha level except for brain width (F(1,19) = 8.10; p = 0.01; Table 2), and cerebrum length (F(1,19) = 7.70; p = 0.01; Table 2); male offspring had larger and heavier brains (Table 2).
Locomotor Activity and Behavioural Sensitization
We did not observe differences between males and females; accordingly, we combined them into a single data set. After initial placement in locomotor chambers, naltrexone-exposed offspring (males and females combined) exhibited fewer vertical plane entries (rearing) compared to placebo-exposed offspring (F(1,88) = 5.70; p<0.05; 32.6%; Figure 5A) and continued to display significantly less rearing while habituating over days 2 (F(1,88) = 6.00; p<0.05; 52.3%; Figure 5A) and 3 (F(1,88) = 12.90; p<0.001; 49.1%; Figure 5A), translating to an overall reduction of basal activity in the vertical plane (F(1,88) = 20.45; p<0.001; 42.6%). Horizontal plane activity (ambulation) did not differ on day 1 (F(1,88) = 1.80; p>0.05; Figure 5B), although in naltrexone-exposed offspring, total horizontal activity was significantly attenuated compared to placebo offspring by day 3 for males and females combined (F(1,88) = 6.00; p<0.05; 25.3%; Figure 5B).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 5. Comparison of locomotor activity between offspring groups in response to a novel environment.
Locomotor activity is shown on days 1–3, after saline or morphine administration on days 4–8 (10 mg/kg, s.c.), and after a final morphine challenge on day 16 (5 mg/kg, s.c.). A: Total activity scores in the vertical plane and B: in the horizontal plane, reveal that habituation was greater among naltrexone-exposed offspring. C: Total horizontal locomotor activity did not differ between groups treated with saline and challenged with morphine (saline-morphine). D: Total horizontal locomotor activity did not differ between groups treated with morphine, although sensitization was expressed in both groups after challenge with morphine (morphine-morphine). E: Analysis of data expressing total morphine-induced locomotor activity as a function of basal locomotor activity on day 3 revealed significantly greater activity values for naltrexone-exposed offspring, indicating an increased development of sensitization. Data are expressed as vertical plane entries (A) or horizontal distance moved in centimetres (B, C, D and E) and represent the mean ± SEM (one-way ANOVA: * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001; days 1–3 (A and B): placebo: n = 37, naltrexone: n = 53; days 4–16 (C): placebo: n = 12, naltrexone: n = 19; days 4–16 (D and E): placebo: n = 20, naltrexone: n = 26).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.g005
Given the difference in horizontal activity between treatment groups following habituation by day 3, subsequent data for morphine treatments were calculated as a function of basal activity. While there was no difference between groups on the first day of morphine treatment, from day 4 to 5 (i.e. morphine injection day 1 to 2) there was a significantly greater increase in ambulation of naltrexone-exposed offspring which was 11.5 fold greater than placebo-exposed offspring (726.8±248.6 cm compared to 37.0±204.6 cm; F(1,44) = 4.19; p<0.05; Figure 5D). From day 5 until the completion of testing (day 16), naltrexone-exposed offspring continued to exhibit significantly greater development of sensitization as a function of basal activity (Figure 5E). This was further validated using a generalised linear model for repeated measures which confirmed 42.7% greater development of sensitization during the induction phase and greater overall ambulation of naltrexone-exposed offspring, in males (F(1,20) = 5.68; p<0.05), females (F(1,22) = 9.12; p<0.01), and males and females combined (F(1,44) = 14.98; p<0.001; Figure 5E). There was no effect of saline injection on locomotor activity with respect to time and, likewise, no difference between groups in response to morphine challenge (day 16) following saline pre-treatment (saline-morphine; F(1,29) = 0.29, p>0.05; Figure 5C).
Opioid Neurochemistry in Offspring Brains
Analysis of brains from male offspring (cohort 2), revealed a two-way interaction between maternal treatment group (i.e. naltrexone or placebo), treatment received during behavioural testing (i.e. saline-saline, saline-morphine, morphine-morphine) and PPE mRNA expression. Namely, reduced expression was associated with placebo offspring given morphine, either as saline-morphine (20.0%; p = 0.018) or morphine-morphine (21.4%; p = 0.005) compared to expression in morphine-naïve counterparts (saline-saline) (Figure 6A and B). However, no reduction in expression was found in naltrexone-exposed offspring, although striatal PPE mRNA expression was greater in those given morphine (saline-morphine: 29.6%; p = 0.002 or morphine-morphine: 31.3%; p<0.001) compared to placebo-exposed counterparts (Figure 6A and B). This effect on PPE mRNA expression was not found in other brain regions examined (Table 4). No significant differences were found with respect to expression of PPD mRNA or μ-opioid receptor expression (Table 4).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 6. Opioid neurochemistry in brains of offspring.
Evaluation of [33P]preproenkephalin (PPE) mRNA binding in the striatum (Str) by in situ hybridisation histochemistry (ISHH), with comparison between offspring groups (placebo; naltrexone) and drug treatments received during behavioural sensitization (saline-saline, SS; saline-morphine, SM; morphine-morphine, MM). A: Representative autoradiograms showing localisation of [33P]PPE mRNA binding sites within midsagittal sections of whole rat brain at approximately 4.0 mm from the midline. Relative PPE mRNA expression within Str presented in accordance with quantitative ISHH (A), which was performed as described in Materials and Methods. As indicated by the density scale, the highest binding levels appear as black. B: Quantitative data for PPE mRNA binding in Str are expressed in disintegrations per minute per millimetre squared (DPM/mm2) and represent the mean ± SEM. Using generalised linear mixed modelling: * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001, naltrexone versus placebo by SS versus SM versus MM, naltrexone: n = 14; placebo: n = 17; n≈5/column.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.g006
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 4. Mean ± SEM values (DPM/mm2) for in situ hybridisation histochemistry and autoradiography binding.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.t004
Propensity to Self-administer Morphine
Fixed-Ratio responding.
Both naltrexone- and placebo-exposed offspring reliably self-administered morphine (Figure 7). Analysis of mean total data for FR1 responding revealed significant discrimination between the two levers at 0.1 mg/kg/infusion (F(3,20) = 35.94, p<0.001; Figure 7A). Student-Neuman-Keuls post hoc comparisons revealed significant preference for the active lever over the inactive lever in placebo- (F(1,10) = 5.97, p = 0.035) and naltrexone-exposed rats (F(1,10) = 13.54, p = 0.004). Significant discrimination was also evident at 0.3 mg/kg/infusion (F(3,38) = 18.68, p<0.001; Figure 7B) with post hoc comparisons revealing significant preference for the active lever over the inactive lever in both placebo- (F(1,22) = 17.58, p<0.001) and naltrexone-exposed rats (F(1,16) = 33.68, p<0.001).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 7. Comparison of naltrexone- and placebo-exposed offspring to fixed ratio morphine self-administration.
Morphine self-administration under a fixed-ratio of 1 lever press and acquisition of a stable response. Data are expressed as the number of active versus inactive lever responses per 2 h session over 6 days for dose of 0.1 mg/kg/infusion (A), and over 7 days for 0.3 mg/kg/infusion (B), and represent the mean ± SEM (two-way ANOVA, * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001, compared to the active lever, 0.1 mg/kg/infusion: placebo: n = 6, naltrexone n = 6; 0.3 mg/kg/infusion: placebo: n = 12, naltrexone: n = 9). Data are expressed as number of infusions and amount of morphine (mg/kg) obtained each day over 6 days for dose of 0.1 mg/kg/infusion (C) and over 7 days for 0.3 mg/kg/infusion (D) represent the mean ± SEM (no difference between groups using two-way ANOVA).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.g007
Analysis of the number of morphine infusions received over 7 days revealed an overall significantly augmented response by naltrexone-exposed offspring at 0.3 mg/kg/infusion over this period (F(1,15) = 6.17, p = 0.025). This difference between groups was not observed over a similar period at 0.1 mg/kg/infusion (F(1,7) = 0.90, p>0.05). At 0.3 mg/kg/infusion, there was a significant escalation in morphine self-administration by naltrexone-offspring, with greater consumption on day 7 compared to day 1 (t(14.63) = 2.57, p = 0.022; Figure 7D). However, as there was a lack of escalation in self-administration at 0.1 mg/kg/infusion (t(16.16) = 0.75, p>0.05; Figure 7C), FR1 testing at the lower dose was undertaken for 6 days. The number of timeout responses did not differ between placebo- and naltrexone-exposed offspring at either 0.1 (F(1,10) = 0.47, p>0.05) or 0.3 mg/kg/infusion (F(1,19) = 3.50, p>0.05) (data not shown).
Progressive-ratio responding.
No difference was seen between placebo- and naltrexone-exposed offspring for 0.1 mg/kg/infusion assessed on the PR3-4 schedule with respect to active lever presses (t(10) = 0.83, p>0.05), breakpoint (t(10) = 0.85, p>0.05), drug infusions (t(10) = 0.83, p>0.05), or overall cumulative response (F(1,10) = 0.38, p>0.05) (data not shown). However, at 0.3 mg/kg/infusion, naltrexone-exposed rats displayed a higher breakpoint compared to placebo offspring on a PR3-4 schedule. Unpaired t-tests revealed a significant difference between offspring groups with respect to the final ratio obtained (breakpoint) (t(12) = 2.91, p = 0.013; Figure 8B), total number of lever presses for the session (t(12) = 2.82, p = 0.015; Figure 8A) and number of infusions (t(12) = 3.01, p = 0.011; Figure 8C). Furthermore, when assessing the proportion of rats that reached a ‘true breakpoint’ (when no reinforcer is earned for a 60 min period), none of the 7 naltrexone-exposed rats reached a ‘true breakpoint’ in the 2-h session, whereas 4 of the 7 placebo-exposed rats (57%) did. This is apparent as the clear plateau in the responding of placebo- but not for the naltrexone-exposed offspring when examining cumulative responses (Figure 8D).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 8. Comparison of naltrexone- and placebo-exposed offspring to progressive ratio morphine self-administration.
Morphine self-administration under a progressive ratio (PR) schedule of 3–4 (see Table 1 for response requirement) at a dose of 0.3 mg/kg/infusion. Data from the PR3–4 session are expressed as total active lever presses (A), breakpoint, defined as the final ratio completed within the 2 h session (B), and number of drug infusions (C) and represent the mean ± SEM (unpaired t-test, * p<0.05, naltrexone versus placebo (n = 7). D: Cumulative response record for the PR3-4 session divided into 10 minute time bins. Data are expressed as number of active lever presses and represent mean ± SEM (two-way ANOVA, * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, naltrexone versus placebo (n = 7) for each time bin).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.g008
Analysis of cumulative responding revealed a significant effect of offspring group (F(1,13) = 9.24; p = 0.012; Figure 8D) and time (F(11,66) = 44.06; p<0.001), and a significant interaction between the two factors (F(1.36,16.28) = 6.89, p = 0.012). Dunn-Sidák post test comparisons revealed no difference between placebo- and naltrexone-exposed offspring in the first 10 min time bin (p>0.05; Figure 8D) but cumulative responses were significantly greater among naltrexone-exposed offspring, compared to those exposed to placebo, in all remaining time bins (20–120 min: t = 7.97, 12.42, 17.61, 22.14, 27.19, 31.58, 32.11, 33.94, 36.64, 39.53 and 39.50, respectively, p<0.05 for all except from 60 to 70 min time bins, p<0.01; Figure 8D).
For the PR9-4 schedule (data not shown) at 0.3 mg/kg/infusion, groups differed with respect to breakpoint (t(11) = 2.29, p = 0.043) and infusions (t(11) = 2.30, p = 0.042). Cumulative responses were significantly greater in naltrexone-, compared to placebo-exposed rats, in the 30, 40 and 90 min time bins (t = 2.37, 2.20 and 2.23, respectively, with p≤0.05).
Cue-induced drug-seeking.
Both groups exhibited robust cue-conditioned drug-seeking behaviour after a period of abstinence (Figure 9). Nonetheless, overall cumulative responses for the active lever were greater among naltrexone-exposed offspring (F(1.69,23.68) = 4.77, p = 0.023; Figure 9A). Dunn-Sidák post hoc test comparisons revealed no difference between placebo- and naltrexone-exposed offspring in the first 10 min time bin (t(14) = 1.41, p>0.05; Figure 9A) but cumulative responses were significantly greater among naltrexone-exposed offspring, compared to placebo, in all remaining time bins (20–60 min: t = 2.68, 2.94, 2.73, 2.73 and 2.70, respectively, p<0.05; Figure 9A). A plateau in responding was evident for both placebo- (40 to 60 min) and naltrexone-exposed offspring (50 to 60 min) (Figure 9A).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 9. Comparison of naltrexone- and placebo-exposed offspring to cue-induced drug-seeking.
Cue-induced drug seeking (cues present but no drug infusions;1 h) after an abstinence period of 6 weeks with comparison between offspring groups (placebo, naltrexone). A: Cumulative response record for cue-induce drug-seeking divided into 10 minute time bins. Data are expressed as number of active lever presses and represent mean ± SEM (two-way ANOVA, * p<0.05, naltrexone (n = 9) versus placebo (n = 7) for each time bin). B: Total active and inactive lever presses during the drug-seeking session (represented by white bars, placebo: n = 9, naltrexone: n = 7) as compared to lever presses during 7 days of stable responding under a fixed-ratio 1 (represented by black bars, placebo: n = 12, naltrexone: n = 9). Data expressed as number of lever presses and represent mean ± SEM (one-way ANOVA: * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001, naltrexone versus placebo).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812.g009
Active lever presses were significantly higher on test day (1-h session) compared to baseline responding (2-h session), despite the absence of drug infusions during the session. This significant cue-conditioned drug-seeking was revealed by two-way ANOVA (F(7,66) = 16.62, p<0.001; Figure 9B), and Student-Newman–Keuls post hoc analyses for placebo- (p = 0.018; 130%) and naltrexone-exposed offspring (p<0.001; 163%), confirming enhanced responding in naltrexone-exposed offspring on the active lever on test day compared to the FR1 schedule. Moreover, compared to placebo, there was augmented active lever responding by naltrexone-exposed offspring on the cue-induced drug-seeking test day versus baseline day (F(1,14) = 11.48, p = 0.005; 73%; Figure 9B). Furthermore, during the cue-induced drug-seeking session, naltrexone-exposed offspring exhibited significantly greater active lever responding relative to inactive lever presses (p<0.001; 124%; Figure 9B); conversely, among placebo rats, responding was comparable between active and inactive levers. There was no difference in latency to first active lever press between naltrexone- and placebo-exposed offspring (F(1,14) = 0.046, p>0.05).
Discussion
Maternal Naltrexone Exposure Induces Behavioural Changes in Adult Offspring
Previous animal studies have shown that prenatal exposure to cocaine, cannabis and morphine leads to an increased propensity to self-administer drugs in adulthood [46], [47], [48] although no effect was observed following prenatal morphine exposure in another study [49]. Nonetheless, the current study is the first to suggest that maternal exposure to sustained-release naltrexone during gestation and lactation confers increased opioid abuse risk in adulthood. The enhanced morphine self-administration in naltrexone-exposed offspring suggests that morphine may have an increased ‘reward value’. Furthermore, increases in breakpoint on a PR schedule suggest increased motivation to obtain the drug [50]. Although both groups showed robust morphine-seeking after 6 weeks of abstinence, naltrexone-exposed offspring exhibited significantly more responses, supporting enhanced relapse-like drug-seeking in naltrexone-treated offspring.
Increased drug-seeking behaviour reflects neuroadaptations that occur with repeated drug exposure [51], [52]. Our findings suggest that developmental opioid receptor antagonism may cause pathophysiological changes that contribute to compulsive drug-seeking behaviour. One possible explanation is that in utero naltrexone exposure may negatively impact upon circuits necessary for extinction learning which would also explain an apparently increased motivation to obtain morphine under PR. Interestingly, the endogenous opioid system has been implicated in goal-directed learning as opioid receptor antagonism facilitates the transition from goal-oriented to habitual responding [53]. The present data, therefore, suggest that developmental opioid receptor antagonism may impact upon the pathways implicated in habit-forming. Clearly, a definitive answer for the mechanism behind increased self-administration of opiates and relapse propensity in adult rats exposed to naltrexone in utero requires further investigation. One possibility is that striatal enkephalin signalling is altered following naltrexone exposure, as implicated by the neurochemical data. Another possibility is that, rather than naltrexone exposure underlying an increase in reward, the observed increase in operant responding for morphine and cue-induced drug seeking may be a pharmacodynamic effect from naltrexone-induced changes in opioid G-protein receptor coupling or other signalling pathways [54], [55]. Regardless of the mechanism, we suggest that our data argue for long-term follow-up of human cases of foetal naltrexone exposure. For example, the present data provide a strong argument for interrogation of the epigenetic effects of in utero naltrexone exposure that may underpin some of the behavioural differences noted in our current study.
Significant differences between naltrexone- and placebo-exposed offspring occurred at the 0.3 mg/kg but not the 0.1 mg/kg morphine dose, consistent with an inverted U-shaped dose-response [56], [57], highlighting the importance of using different drug doses. Moreover, the collective data suggest an increased reinforcing strength of morphine in naltrexone-exposed rats that may relate to the relative resistance of striatal enkephalin expression to morphine-induced down-regulation compared to placebo rats.
Naltrexone-exposed offspring exhibited lower vertical activity (rearing) compared to placebos, when first introduced to a novel environment. During subsequent re-exposure to the same environment, less rearing (i.e. habituation) occurred in both groups and remained lower in the naltrexone-exposed group, consistent with other studies [12], [58]. In addition, although similar levels of overall ambulation (horizontal activity) are consistent with other studies [17], [58], [59], we show apparently increased habituation in this plane. Reduced locomotor activity in a novel environment, particularly rearing, could indicate reduced exploratory drive [58] and/or altered motivational systems [60], [61].
Moderate morphine doses in rats produce a biphasic locomotor response [62], [63], which was observed acutely in both naltrexone- and placebo-exposed offspring. Tolerance, the attenuation of a response over a period of chronic drug treatment [64], and sensitization, the increase in a response that arises from repeated drug treatment [62], [65], are different neuroadaptive processes that can occur concomitantly. Repeated morphine treatment produces a decrease in its sedative effects (tolerance) and an increase in its stimulatory effects on locomotion (sensitization) [63], [66]. Changes in vertical and horizontal activity can be used respectively to assess tolerance and sensitization [67]. Drug-induced locomotor sensitization is a robust and well-established indicator of plasticity that may relate to aspects of addiction [19], [20]. Although the expression of sensitization did not differ between groups, naltrexone-exposed offspring showed a greater transition from an initial morphine-induced depressant response to a stimulant response after repeated morphine treatment, suggesting enhanced development of sensitization.
Interestingly, this was mirrored by resistance of striatal enkephalin mRNA expression to down-regulation following morphine treatment compared to placebos. Enkephalin appears to mediate morphine-induced tolerance and sensitization [21], [22], [23] via networks known to regulate motor behaviour. Treatment of placebo-exposed offspring with morphine was associated with decreased striatal PPE mRNA expression relative to saline-treated placebo rats. However, this effect did not occur in naltrexone-exposed offspring, indicating that morphine-induced regulation of enkephalin was a normal process [68], [69] not evident following naltrexone-exposure during development. Importantly, this occurred in adult rats that had been free of naltrexone since weaning; indicating that developmental exposure to naltrexone can result in enduring behavioural changes and neurochemical alterations to central opioid systems.
Although an effect was observed for PPE mRNA, no such changes were seen for PPD mRNA or μ-opioid receptor expression. Other studies have shown changes to opioid receptors following naltrexone exposure [70], [71]. For example, in adult mice implanted with 15 mg naltrexone pellets, up-regulation of μ- and δ-opioid receptors was observed after 7 days of naltrexone pre-treatment [72]. Given that up-regulation of μ- and δ-opioid receptors correlate with potency changes of morphine [71], [73] further studies in our model would be interesting to examine whether changes to κ- and/or δ-opioid receptors play a role in the behavioural effects we observed.
Gross Morphological Changes Following Maternal Naltrexone Exposure are Minimal
Persistent behavioural effects occurred in the absence of substantial morphological changes in adult offspring but were preceded by decreased birth-weights. Because litter size impacts on growth and nutritional intake [74], [75], we incorporated the naltrexone-associated increase in litter size as a covariate and showed that decreases in neonatal body weight occurred independently of litter size. Consistent with our findings, offspring body weight in mice is reduced following low-dose maternal naltrexone [76] and is associated with intermittent postnatal opioid receptor antagonism [77], [78]. Reduced birth-weight and head-size following low maternal and foetal naltrexone exposure (<2 ng/ml) is also consistent with the general inhibitory effects of low-dose opioid receptor antagonism [13], [17], [77], [78], [79], [80], [81], [82]. The trend toward reduced adult offspring head-size, not evident at birth, highlights the need for follow-up in naltrexone-exposed human newborns who appear to be developmentally unaffected [6], [8], [9], [83].
A Pregnant Rat Model for Sustained-release Naltrexone
The sustained-release naltrexone preparation comprised the same formulation used in humans and yielded comparable release- (∼0.4 mg/kg/day) and blood-levels (∼2–10 ng/ml) [7], [10]. Following intraperitoneal injection in rat, naltrexone undergoes placental transfer [84] but does not remain in pre-weaning offspring blood for more than 2 days after the last injection [85]. Using our customised implant, we have shown transfer of naltrexone to the amniotic fluid, blood and lactate. Humans and rats are also similar with respect to naltrexone’s affinity for opioid receptors [86], [87], plasma protein binding [88] and rate of elimination [89], [90], with a half-life range of 2–14 hours in humans [91], [92], and 4.6–11.4 hours in rats [93], [94].
In relative terms, brain development in rats and humans occurs to a common timetable defined by the time between conception and eye opening (“caecal period” rat: 36 days; human: 182 days). When expressed as a percentage of the caecal period, neurodevelopmental milestones start and finish at approximately the same value [95], [96], [97]. Based on caecal period calculations, blood-naltrexone levels were sustained above 1 ng/ml, comparable to levels in humans, for a clinically comparable amount of time (>30 days in rat ≈ 5.5 months in humans). Moreover, the decline in naltrexone levels in maternal rat plasma was similar to the profiles in non-pregnant humans [10], [98] and a single case study of a pregnant woman [7].
Future animal studies investigating the developmental effects of sustained-release naltrexone, particularly on addiction-related behaviours, would provide greater insight in the clinical context if maternal rats were opioid-dependent prior to treatment with naltrexone. Nonetheless, the current study still has much to offer as a clinically-relevant model in that there are a number of potential therapeutic uses for naltrexone in the non-opioid dependent patient, including in the management of alcoholism [3], [4], [99], compulsive gambling [100], [101], [102], [103], multiple sclerosis [104], [105], [106] and obesity [107], [108], [109].
The current study highlights the vulnerability of the developing brain to drug exposure. Indeed, factors such as “imprinting” may be critical at the time of birth. For example, in humans, obstetric administration of opiates, barbiturates or nitrous oxide (>1 hour) during labour and within 10 hours of birth increased the relative risk of offspring subsequently becoming addicted to opiates in adulthood [110], [111], [112]. Our model examined continuous naltrexone exposure during pregnancy and lactation but, in future studies, may be of value to determine whether much shorter critical period/s of exposure also produce effects in adult offspring.
Conclusion
The current study demonstrates that chronic, low-dose maternal naltrexone delivered via a sustained-release implant impacts both behaviour and neurochemistry in adult offspring, but without obvious morphological effects. Our data are consistent with perturbations that are reported to result from naltrexone exposure during development with significant implications for alterations in morphine-induced neuroplasticity, and increased risk of opioid-abuse later in life. Our findings highlight the need for greater information on the safety of using naltrexone during human pregnancy.
Acknowledgments
We thank Ms Carole Bartlett for assistance with surgery, Ms Tammy Esmaili and Dr Andy Symonds for assistance with blood acquisition, Mr Vince Clark for assistance with densitometry analysis, Ms Louise Goodes for final figure preparation and Professor Lyn Beazley and Matthew Martin-Iverson for useful discussion. We also extend our gratitude to Go Medical Industries for the production and supply of custom-sized implants, and to the Perth Chemistry Centre for undertaking liquid chromatography and mass spectrometry analysis of blood samples.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: SAD AJL GKH. Performed the experiments: WOF EVK RJT. Analyzed the data: WOF RJT. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: AJL GKH SAD. Wrote the paper: WOF SAD AJL GKH RJT. Provided contact with Perth Chemistry Centre for blood analyses: GKH.
Citation: Farid WO, Lawrence AJ, Krstew EV, Tait RJ, Hulse GK, Dunlop SA (2012) Maternally Administered Sustained-Release Naltrexone in Rats Affects Offspring Neurochemistry and Behaviour in Adulthood. PLoS ONE 7(12): e52812. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052812
1. Kirchmayer U, Davoli M, Verster AD, Amato L, Ferri A, et al. (2002) A systematic review on the efficacy of naltrexone maintenance treatment in opioid dependence. Addiction 97: 1241–1249.
2. Johansson BA, Berglund M, Lindgren A (2006) Efficacy of maintenance treatment with naltrexone for opioid dependence: a meta-analytical review. Addiction 101: 491–503.
3. Garbutt JC, Kranzler HR, O’Malley SS, Gastfriend DR, Pettinati HM, et al. (2005) Efficacy and tolerability of long-acting injectable naltrexone for alcohol dependence: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 293: 1617–1625.
4. Kranzler HR, Wesson DR, Billot L (2004) Naltrexone depot for treatment of alcohol dependence: a multicenter, randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 28: 1051–1059.
5. Caswell A (2005) MIMS Bi-Monthly. Sydney: CMPMedica Australia.
6. Hulse GK, O’Neil G, Pereira C, Brewer C (2001) Obstetric and neonatal outcomes associated with maternal naltrexone exposure. Aust N Z J Obstet Gynaecol 41: 424–428.
7. Hulse GK, Arnold-Reed DE, O’Neil G, Hansson RC (2003) Naltrexone implant and blood naltrexone levels over pregnancy. Aust N Z J Obstet Gynaecol 43: 386–388.
8. Hulse GK, O’Neil G (2002) A possible role for implantable naltrexone in the management of the high-risk pregnant heroin user. Aust N Z J Obstet Gynaecol 42: 104–105.
9. Hulse GK, O’Neil G, Arnold-Reed DE (2004) Methadone maintenance vs. implantable naltrexone treatment in the pregnant heroin user. Int J Gynaecol Obstet 85: 170–171.
10. Ngo HT, Arnold-Reed DE, Hansson RC, Tait RJ, Hulse GK (2008) Blood naltrexone levels over time following naltrexone implant. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32: 23–28.
11. McLaughlin PJ, Tobias SW, Lang CM, Zagon IS (1997) Chronic exposure to the opioid antagonist naltrexone during pregnancy: maternal and offspring effects. Physiol Behav 62: 501–508.
12. McLaughlin PJ, Tobias SW, Lang CM, Zagon IS (1997) Opioid receptor blockade during prenatal life modifies postnatal behavioral development. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 58: 1075–1082.
13. Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1986) Opioid antagonist (naltrexone) modulation of cerebellar development: histological and morphometric studies. J Neurosci 6: 1424–1432.
14. Zagon IS, Wu Y, McLaughlin PJ (1999) Opioid growth factor and organ development in rat and human embryos. Brain Res 839: 313–322.
15. de Cabo de la Vega C, Pujol A, Paz Viveros M (1995) Neonatally administered naltrexone affects several behavioral responses in adult rats of both genders. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 50: 277–286.
16. Harry GJ, Rosecrans JA (1979) Behavioral effects of perinatal naltrexone exposure: a preliminary investigation. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 11 Suppl: 19–22
17. Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1985) Naltrexone’s influence on neurobehavioral development. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 22: 441–448.
18. Zagon IS, Tobias SW, Hytrek SD, McLaughlin PJ (1998) Opioid receptor blockade throughout prenatal life confers long-term insensitivity to morphine and alters mu opioid receptors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 59: 201–207.
19. Robinson TE, Berridge KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 18: 247–291.
20. Robinson TE, Browman KE, Crombag HS, Badiani A (1998) Modulation of the induction or expression of psychostimulant sensitization by the circumstances surrounding drug administration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 22: 347–354.
21. Georges F, Stinus L, Bloch B, Le Moine C (1999) Chronic morphine exposure and spontaneous withdrawal are associated with modifications of dopamine receptor and neuropeptide gene expression in the rat striatum. Eur J Neurosci 11: 481–490.
22. Mansour A, Fox CA, Akil H, Watson SJ (1995) Opioid-receptor mRNA expression in the rat CNS: anatomical and functional implications. Trends Neurosci 18: 22–29.
23. Steiner H, Gerfen CR (1998) Role of dynorphin and enkephalin in the regulation of striatal output pathways and behavior. Exp Brain Res 123: 60–76.
24. Liu Y, Sunderland VB, O’Neil AG (2006) In vitro and in vivo release of naltrexone from biodegradable depot systems. Drug Dev Ind Pharm 32: 85–94.
25. Hulse GK, Arnold-Reed DE, O’Neil G, Chan CT, Hansson R, et al. (2004) Blood naltrexone and 6-beta-naltrexol levels following naltrexone implant: comparing two naltrexone implants. Addict Biol 9: 59–65.
26. Frank P, Schoenhard GL, Burton E (1991) A method for rapid and frequent blood collection from the rat tail vein. J Pharmacol Methods 26: 233–238.
27. Bugge CJL, Grun I, Stanfor K, Tan MS, Garcia DB (1996) Determination of Naltrexone and 6-b-Naltrexol in human plasma by LC-MS-MS. Austin, TX: CEDRA Corporation.
28. DiResta GR, Lee JB, Arbit E (1991) Measurement of brain tissue specific gravity using pycnometry. J Neurosci Methods 39: 245–251.
29. Huang WL, Beazley LD, Quinlivan JA, Evans SF, Newnham JP, et al. (1999) Effect of corticosteroids on brain growth in fetal sheep. Obstet Gynecol 94: 213–218.
30. Owoeye O, Farombi EO, Onwuka SK (2011) Gross morphometric reduction of rats’ cerebellum by gamma irradiation was mitigated by pretreatment with Vernonia amygdalina leaf extract. Rom J Morphol Embryol 52: 81–88.
31. McPherson CS, Lawrence AJ (2006) Exposure to amphetamine in rats during periadolescence establishes behavioural and extrastriatal neural sensitization in adulthood. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 9: 377–392.
32. Brown RM, Short JL, Cowen MS, Ledent C, Lawrence AJ (2009) A differential role for the adenosine A2A receptor in opiate reinforcement vs opiate-seeking behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 34: 844–856.
33. Paxinos G, Watson C (2005) The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. Amsterdam: Elsevier Academic Press.
34. Cowen MS, Rezvani AH, Jarrott B, Lawrence AJ (1999) Ethanol consumption by Fawn-Hooded rats following abstinence: effect of naltrexone and changes in mu-opioid receptor density. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 23: 1008–1014.
35. Djouma E, Lawrence AJ (2002) The effect of chronic ethanol consumption and withdrawal on mu-opioid and dopamine D(1) and D(2) receptor density in Fawn-Hooded rat brain. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 302: 551–559.
36. Cowen MS, Lawrence AJ (2001) Alterations in central preproenkephalin mRNA expression after chronic free-choice ethanol consumption by fawn-hooded rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 25: 1126–1133.
37. Carroll ME, Campbell UC, Heideman P (2001) Ketoconazole suppresses food restriction-induced increases in heroin self-administration in rats: sex differences. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 9: 307–316.
38. Carroll ME, France CP, Meisch RA (1979) Food deprivation increases oral and intravenous drug intake in rats. Science 205: 319–321.
39. Carroll ME, Meisch RA (1981) Determinants of increased drug self-administration due to food deprivation. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 74: 197–200.
40. Lang WJ, Latiff AA, McQueen A, Singer G (1977) Self administration of nicotine with and without a food delivery schedule. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 7: 65–70.
41. Singer G, Simpson F, Lang WJ (1978) Schedule induced self injections of nicotine with recovered body weight. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 9: 387–389.
42. Grasing K, Li N, He S, Parrish C, Delich J, et al. (2003) A new progressive ratio schedule for support of morphine self-administration in opiate dependent rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 168: 387–396.
43. Roberts DCS, Richardson NR (1992) Self-administration of psychomotor stimulants using progressive ratio schedules of reinforcement. In: Boulton AA, Baker GB, Wu PH, editors. Animal models of drug addiction. New York, NY: Humana Press. 233–269.
44. Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.
45. Farid WO, McCallum D, Tait RJ, Dunlop SA, Hulse GK (2009) Minor pathological changes are induced by naltrexone-poly(DL-lactide) implants in pregnant rats. J Biomed Mater Res A 91: 964–974.
46. Narita M, Miyagawa K, Mizuo K, Yoshida T, Suzuki T (2006) Prenatal and neonatal exposure to low-dose of bisphenol-A enhance the morphine-induced hyperlocomotion and rewarding effect. Neurosci Lett 402: 249–252.
47. Rocha BA, Mead AN, Kosofsky BE (2002) Increased vulnerability to self-administer cocaine in mice prenatally exposed to cocaine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 163: 221–229.
48. Spano MS, Ellgren M, Wang X, Hurd YL (2007) Prenatal cannabis exposure increases heroin seeking with allostatic changes in limbic enkephalin systems in adulthood. Biol Psychiatry 61: 554–563.
49. Riley MA, Vathy I (2006) Mid- to late gestational morphine exposure does not alter the rewarding properties of morphine in adult male rats. Neuropharmacology 51: 295–304.
50. Arnold JM, Roberts DC (1997) A critique of fixed and progressive ratio schedules used to examine the neural substrates of drug reinforcement. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 57: 441–447.
51. Vanderschuren LJ, Kalivas PW (2000) Alterations in dopaminergic and glutamatergic transmission in the induction and expression of behavioral sensitization: a critical review of preclinical studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 151: 99–120.
52. White FJ, Kalivas PW (1998) Neuroadaptations involved in amphetamine and cocaine addiction. Drug Alcohol Depend 51: 141–153.
53. Wassum KM, Cely IC, Maidment NT, Balleine BW (2009) Disruption of endogenous opioid activity during instrumental learning enhances habit acquisition. Neuroscience 163: 770–780.
54. Chen F, Lawrence AJ (2000) Effect of chronic ethanol and withdrawal on the mu-opioid receptor- and 5-Hydroxytryptamine(1A) receptor-stimulated binding of [(35)S]Guanosine-5′-O-(3-thio)triphosphate in the fawn-hooded rat brain: A quantitative autoradiography study. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 293: 159–165.
55. Chen F, Lawrence AJ (2004) Chronic antidepressant treatment causes a selective reduction of mu-opioid receptor binding and functional coupling to G Proteins in the amygdala of fawn-hooded rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 310: 1020–1026.
56. Bickel WK, Marsch LA, Carroll ME (2000) Deconstructing relative reinforcing efficacy and situating the measures of pharmacological reinforcement with behavioral economics: a theoretical proposal. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 153: 44–56.
57. Shabat-Simon M, Levy D, Amir A, Rehavi M, Zangen A (2008) Dissociation between rewarding and psychomotor effects of opiates: differential roles for glutamate receptors within anterior and posterior portions of the ventral tegmental area. J Neurosci 28: 8406–8416.
58. Meyerson BJ, Berg M, Johansson B (1988) Neonatal naltrexone treatment: effects on sexual and exploratory behavior in male and female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 31: 63–67.
59. Paul L, Diaz J, Bailey B (1978) Behavioral effects of chronic narcotic antagonist administration of infant rats. Neuropharmacology 17: 655–657.
60. Willig F, Palacios A, Monmaur P, M’Harzi M, Laurent J, et al. (1987) Short-term memory, exploration and locomotor activity in aged rats. Neurobiol Aging 8: 393–402.
61. Klemenhagen KC, Gordon JA, David DJ, Hen R, Gross CT (2006) Increased fear response to contextual cues in mice lacking the 5-HT1A receptor. Neuropsychopharmacology 31: 101–111.
62. Babbini M, Davis WM (1972) Time-dose relationships for locomotor activity effects of morphine after acute or repeated treatment. Br J Pharmacol 46: 213–224.
63. Kalinichev M, Easterling KW, Holtzman SG (2003) Long-lasting changes in morphine-induced locomotor sensitization and tolerance in Long-Evans mother rats as a result of periodic postpartum separation from the litter: a novel model of increased vulnerability to drug abuse? Neuropsychopharmacology 28: 317–328.
64. Koob GF, Bloom FE (1988) Cellular and molecular mechanisms of drug dependence. Science 242: 715–723.
65. Lett BT (1989) Repeated exposures intensify rather than diminish the rewarding effects of amphetamine, morphine, and cocaine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 98: 357–362.
66. Kalinichev M, Easterling KW, Holtzman SG (2002) Early neonatal experience of Long-Evans rats results in long-lasting changes in reactivity to a novel environment and morphine-induced sensitization and tolerance. Neuropsychopharmacology 27: 518–533.
67. Kalinichev M, White DA, Holtzman SG (2004) Individual differences in locomotor reactivity to a novel environment and sensitivity to opioid drugs in the rat. I. Expression of morphine-induced locomotor sensitization. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 177: 61–67.
68. Olive MF, Maidment NT (1998) Opioid regulation of pallidal enkephalin release: bimodal effects of locally administered mu and delta opioid agonists in freely moving rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 285: 1310–1316.
69. Tempel A, Espinoza K (1992) Morphine-induced downregulation of mu-opioid receptors and peptide synthesis in neonatal rat brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci 654: 529–530.
70. Yoburn BC, Goodman RR, Cohen AH, Pasternak GW, Inturrisi CE (1985) Increased analgesic potency of morphine and increased brain opioid binding sites in the rat following chronic naltrexone treatment. Life Sci 36: 2325–2332.
71. Yoburn BC, Luke MC, Pasternak GW, Inturrisi CE (1988) Upregulation of opioid receptor subtypes correlates with potency changes of morphine and DADLE. Life Sci 43: 1319–1324.
72. Lesscher HM, Bailey A, Burbach JP, Van Ree JM, Kitchen I, et al. (2003) Receptor-selective changes in mu-, delta- and kappa-opioid receptors after chronic naltrexone treatment in mice. Eur J Neurosci 17: 1006–1012.
73. Lee SC, Yoburn BC (2000) The effect of nimodipine on opioid antagonist-induced upregulation and supersensitivity. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 66: 347–351.
74. Grota LJ (1973) Effects of litter size, age of young, and parity on foster mother behaviour in Rattus norvegicus. Anim Behav 21: 78–82.
75. Nowosielski-Slepowron BJ, Park AW (1974) The criteria of litter size in relation to growth of the rat. I. The influence of numbers. Acta Morphol Neerl Scand 12: 299–316.
76. D’Amato FR, Castellano C, Ammassari-Teule M, Oliverio A (1988) Prenatal antagonism of stress by naltrexone administration: early and long-lasting effects on emotional behaviors in mice. Dev Psychobiol 21: 283–292.
77. Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1984) Naltrexone modulates body and brain development in rats: a role for endogenous opioid systems in growth. Life Sci 35: 2057–2064.
78. Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1985) Opioid antagonist-induced regulation of organ development. Physiol Behav 34: 507–511.
79. Hauser KF, McLaughlin PJ, Zagon IS (1989) Endogenous opioid systems and the regulation of dendritic growth and spine formation. J Comp Neurol 281: 13–22.
80. Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1983) Naltrexone modulates growth in infant rats. Life Sci 33: 2449–2454.
81. Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1986) Opioid antagonist-induced modulation of cerebral and hippocampal development: histological and morphometric studies. Brain Res 393: 233–246.
82. Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ (1987) Endogenous opioid systems regulate cell proliferation in the developing rat brain. Brain Res 412: 68–72.
83. Hulse G, O’Neil G (2002) Using naltrexone implants in the management of the pregnant heroin user. Aust N Z J Obstet Gynaecol 42: 569–573.
84. Zagon IS, Hurst WJ, McLaughlin PJ (1997) Transplacental transfer of naltrexone in rats. Life Sci 61: 1261–1267.
85. Zagon IS, Hurst WJ, McLaughlin PJ (1998) Naltrexone is not detected in preweaning rats following transplacental exposure: implications for growth modulation. Life Sci 62: 221–228.
86. Bhargava HN, Rahmani NH, Villar VM, Larsen AK (1993) Effects of naltrexone on pharmacodynamics and pharmacokinetics of intravenously administered morphine in the rat. Pharmacology 46: 66–74.
87. Wang HY, Frankfurt M, Burns LH (2008) High-affinity naloxone binding to filamin a prevents mu opioid receptor-Gs coupling underlying opioid tolerance and dependence. PLoS One 3: e1554.
88. Ludden TM, Malspeis L, Baggot JD, Sokoloski TD, Frank SG, et al. (1976) Tritiated naltrexone binding in plasma from several species and tissue distribution in mice. J Pharm Sci 65: 712–716.
89. Meyer MC, Straughn AB, Lo MW, Schary WL, Whitney CC (1984) Bioequivalence, dose-proportionality, and pharmacokinetics of naltrexone after oral administration. J Clin Psychiatry 45: 15–19.
90. Wall ME, Brine DR, Perez-Reyes M (1981) Metabolism and disposition of naltrexone in man after oral and intravenous administration. Drug Metab Dispos 9: 369–375.
91. Bertolotti M, Ferrari A, Vitale G, Stefani M, Trenti T, et al. (1997) Effect of liver cirrhosis on the systemic availability of naltrexone in humans. J Hepatol 27: 505–511.
92. Wall ME, Perez-Reyes M, Brine DR, Cook CE (1984) Naltrexone disposition in man after subcutaneous administration. Drug Metab Dispos 12: 677–682.
93. Misra AL, Bloch R, Vardy J, Mule SJ, Verebely K (1976) Disposition of (15,16–3H)naltrexone in the central nervous system of the rat. Drug Metab Dispos 4: 276–280.
94. Yoburn BC, Cohen AH, Inturrisi CE (1986) Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of subcutaneous naltrexone pellets in the rat. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 237: 126–130.
95. Darlington RB, Dunlop SA, Finlay BL (1999) Neural development in metatherian and eutherian mammals: variation and constraint. J Comp Neurol 411: 359–368.
96. Robinson SR, Dreher B (1990) The visual pathways of eutherian mammals and marsupials develop according to a common timetable. Brain Behav Evol 36: 177–195.
97. Finlay BL, Darlington RB (1995) Linked regularities in the development and evolution of mammalian brains. Science 268: 1578–1584.
98. Hulse GK, Arnold-Reed DE, O’Neil G, Chan CT, Hansson RC (2004) Achieving long-term continuous blood naltrexone and 6-beta-naltrexol coverage following sequential naltrexone implants. Addict Biol 9: 67–72.
99. O’Brien CP, Gastfriend DR, Forman RF, Schweizer E, Pettinati HM (2011) Long-term opioid blockade and hedonic response: preliminary data from two open-label extension studies with extended-release naltrexone. Am J Addict 20: 106–112.
100. Dannon PN, Lowengrub K, Musin E, Gonopolski Y, Kotler M (2005) Sustained-release bupropion versus naltrexone in the treatment of pathological gambling: a preliminary blind-rater study. J Clin Psychopharmacol 25: 593–596.
101. Kim SW (1998) Opioid antagonists in the treatment of impulse-control disorders. J Clin Psychiatry 59: 159–164.
102. Kim SW, Grant JE, Adson DE, Shin YC (2001) Double-blind naltrexone and placebo comparison study in the treatment of pathological gambling. Biol Psychiatry 49: 914–921.
103. Lahti T, Halme JT, Pankakoski M, Sinclair D, Alho H (2010) Treatment of pathological gambling with naltrexone pharmacotherapy and brief intervention: a pilot study. Psychopharmacol Bull 43: 35–44.
104. Agrawal YP (2005) Low dose naltrexone therapy in multiple sclerosis. Med Hypotheses 64: 721–724.
105. Cree BA, Kornyeyeva E, Goodin DS (2010) Pilot trial of low-dose naltrexone and quality of life in multiple sclerosis. Ann Neurol 68: 145–150.
106. Gironi M, Martinelli-Boneschi F, Sacerdote P, Solaro C, Zaffaroni M, et al. (2008) A pilot trial of low-dose naltrexone in primary progressive multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler 14: 1076–1083.
107. Lee MW, Fujioka K (2009) Naltrexone for the treatment of obesity: review and update. Expert Opin Pharmacother 10: 1841–1845.
108. Plodkowski RA, Nguyen Q, Sundaram U, Nguyen L, Chau DL, et al. (2009) Bupropion and naltrexone: a review of their use individually and in combination for the treatment of obesity. Expert Opin Pharmacother 10: 1069–1081.
109. Witkamp RF (2011) Current and future drug targets in weight management. Pharm Res 28: 1792–1818.
110. Jacobson B, Nyberg K, Gronbladh L, Eklund G, Bygdeman M, et al. (1990) Opiate addiction in adult offspring through possible imprinting after obstetric treatment. BMJ 301: 1067–1070.
111. Nyberg K, Allebeck P, Eklund G, Jacobson B (1992) Socio-economic versus obstetric risk factors for drug addiction in offspring. Br J Addict 87: 1669–1676.
112. Nyberg K, Buka SL, Lipsitt LP (2000) Perinatal medication as a potential risk factor for adult drug abuse in a North American cohort. Epidemiology 11: 715–716.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2012 Farid et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Naltrexone is not recommended during pregnancy. However, sustained-release naltrexone implant use in humans has resulted in cases of inadvertent foetal exposure. Here, we used clinically relevant dosing to examine the effects of maternally administered sustained-release naltrexone on the rat brain by examining offspring at birth and in adulthood. Maternal treatment (naltrexone or placebo implant) started before conception and ceased during gestation, birth or weaning. Morphometry was assessed in offspring at birth and adulthood. Adult offspring were evaluated for differences in locomotor behaviour (basal and morphine-induced, 10 mg/kg, s.c.) and opioid neurochemistry, propensity to self-administer morphine and cue-induced drug-seeking after abstinence. Blood analysis confirmed offspring exposure to naltrexone during gestation, birth and weaning. Naltrexone exposure increased litter size and reduced offspring birth-weight but did not alter brain morphometry. Compared to placebo, basal motor activity of naltrexone-exposed adult offspring was lower, yet they showed enhanced development of psychomotor sensitization to morphine. Developmental naltrexone exposure was associated with resistance to morphine-induced down-regulation of striatal preproenkephalin mRNA expression in adulthood. Adult offspring also exhibited greater operant responding for morphine and, in addition, cue-induced drug-seeking was enhanced. Collectively, these data show pronounced effects of developmental naltrexone exposure, some of which persist into adulthood, highlighting the need for follow up of humans that were exposed to naltrexone in utero.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer