About the Authors:
Sang Joon Son
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry, Ajou University School of Medicine, 164 Worldcup-ro, Yeongtong-gu, Suwon-si, Gyeonggi-do 443–380, Republic of Korea
Kang Soo Lee
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry, CHA Gangnam Medical Center, CHA University, Nonhyon-ro, Gangnam-gu, Seoul 135–081, Republic of Korea
Ji Hyung Chung
Affiliation: Department of Applied Bioscience, College of Life Science, CHA University, Yatap-dong, Bundang-gu, Seongnam-si, Gyeonggi-do 463–400, Republic of Korea
Ki Jung Chang
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry, Ajou University School of Medicine, 164 Worldcup-ro, Yeongtong-gu, Suwon-si, Gyeonggi-do 443–380, Republic of Korea
Hyun Woong Roh
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry, Ajou University School of Medicine, 164 Worldcup-ro, Yeongtong-gu, Suwon-si, Gyeonggi-do 443–380, Republic of Korea
Soo Hyun Kim
Affiliation: Graduate Program in Science for Aging, Yonsei University, 50 Yonsei-ro, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 120–752, Republic of Korea
Taewon Jin
Affiliation: Graduate Program in Science for Aging, Yonsei University, 50 Yonsei-ro, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 120–752, Republic of Korea
Joung Hwan Back
Affiliation: Health Insurance Policy Research Institute, National Health Insurance Service, 311 Dongmak-ro, Mapo-gu, Seoul 121–749, Republic of Korea
Hyun Jung Kim
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry, National Medical Center, 245 Eulji-ro, Jung-gu, Seoul 100–799, Republic of Korea
Yunhwan Lee
Affiliation: Department of Preventive Medicine and Public Health, Ajou University School of Medicine, 164 Worldcup-ro, Yeongtong-gu, Suwon-si, Gyeonggi-do 443–380, Republic of Korea
Seong Hye Choi
Affiliation: Department of Neurology, Inha University College of Medicine, 27 Inhang-ro, Jung-gu, Incheon 400–711, Republic of Korea
Jai Sung Noh
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry, Ajou University School of Medicine, 164 Worldcup-ro, Yeongtong-gu, Suwon-si, Gyeonggi-do 443–380, Republic of Korea
Ki Young Lim
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry, Ajou University School of Medicine, 164 Worldcup-ro, Yeongtong-gu, Suwon-si, Gyeonggi-do 443–380, Republic of Korea
Young Ki Chung
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry, Ajou University School of Medicine, 164 Worldcup-ro, Yeongtong-gu, Suwon-si, Gyeonggi-do 443–380, Republic of Korea
Chang Hyung Hong
* E-mail: [email protected] (CHH); [email protected] (BHO)
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry, Ajou University School of Medicine, 164 Worldcup-ro, Yeongtong-gu, Suwon-si, Gyeonggi-do 443–380, Republic of Korea
Byoung Hoon Oh
* E-mail: [email protected] (CHH); [email protected] (BHO)
Affiliation: Department of Psychiatry and Institute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, 50 Yonsei-ro, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 120–752, Republic of Korea
Introduction
Heat shock proteins (HSPs) are expected to be critically involved in the progression of neurodegeneration and potential therapeutic targets for cognitive decline in the elderly [1,2]. HSPs are molecular chaperones providing a first line of defense against misfolded proteins, a characteristic pathology of neurodegenerative dementia [3]. HSPs also interfere with detrimental processes that occur during neurodegeneration, including oxidative stress and abnormal activation of signaling pathways [4–6]. In that context, early synaptic and axonal abnormalities in neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) may therefore be early detected or partially reversed by HSPs [7,8].
Among HSPs, the expression of two HSPs, namely, the 70-kDa HSP (HSP 70) and 27-kDa HSP (HSP 27), in the brain is notable because both are highly induced in glial cells and neurons following various of noxious stimuli in the early neurodegeneration stage [9]. Several studies reported that HSP 70 and HSP 27 levels are increased in subjects with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), a transitional stage between normal aging and dementia. Both of these HSPs are known to have neuroprotective effects in the brain due to their anti-apoptotic and chaperoning activities [2,10].
However, it remains unclear whether circulating concentrations of these HSPs change as a person gets older, and whether there are differences in the HSPs plasma levels according to the presence of cognitive impairment. In addition, to our knowledge, there is no published data on HSPs level-related cognitive phenotypes even though the association between HSPs and cognitive impairment in the elderly has been reported [9]. When considering early diagnostic and therapeutic applications for HSP 70 and HSP 27, it is important to note the specific response of HSPs according to time and cognitive phenotype which reflect the progression and severity of regional brain pathology. Thus, we investigated the longitudinal associations among plasma HSP 70/27 levels, the conversion from no cognitive impairment (NCI) to MCI, and cognitive phenotype in the community-dwelling elderly.
Materials and Methods
1. Subjects
This study was based on data derived from a community-based cohort study called the Gwangju Dementia and Mild Cognitive Impairment Study (GDEMCIS), which was designed to assess the occurrence and risk factors of dementia. We recruited elderly subjects over the age of 60 who resided within a well-defined geographic region in a small city with agricultural districts. Detailed descriptions of GDEMCIS have been provided in prior literature [10,11]. Of the GDEMCIS participants, we retrospectively studied 66 subjects who had 1) normal cognitive function in baseline assessment, 2) normal cognitive function or MCI at a follow-up assessment 3) complete baseline/follow-up cognitive test data, 4) baseline/follow-up blood samples, 5) no newly developed physical illness to be treated from baseline status and 6) no history of taking saclicylate prophylaxis which is a known inducer of HSP 70 in several tissues [12]. All subjects included in this study had clinical data of two time points; at baseline and at the follow-up assessment, and were classified into a non-conversion group (NCI to NCI) or conversion group (NCI to MCI). The NCI group was defined as the one with each cognitive domain (memory, language, visuospatial, and frontal/executive function) test score higher than the cutoff values. The cutoff values for each factor score were 1.0 standard deviation (SD) of the age-adjusted mean for neuropsychological tests assessing cognitive domains. All NCI subjects were classified as Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) 0. The diagnostic criteria of MCI included the following: 1) subjective cognitive decline that were corroborated by an informant, 2) objective impairments that were 1.0 SD below the age-adjusted mean for at least one of the neuropsychological tests assessing the four cognitive domains, 3) activities of daily living were preserved, and 4) CDR was 0.5. None of our subjects met any of the following exclusion criteria during each visit: 1) a history of significant hearing or visual impairment hindering participation in the interview, 2) neurological disorders (e.g., stroke, Parkinson’s disease or active epilepsy), 3) psychiatric disorders (e.g., schizophrenia, mental retardation, severe depression or, mania), 4) history of use of psychotropic medications, or a history of use of psychoactive substances other than alcohol, and 5) physical illnesses or disorders that could interfere with the clinical study such as severe cardiac disorders, severe respiratory illnesses, uncontrolled diabetes, uncontrolled hypertension, malignancy, and hepatic or renal disorders. All subjects provided their written, informed consent after providing a complete description of the study to the subjects and their relatives. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Severance Mental Hospital.
2. Assessments and measurements
The clinical evaluation form was filled out by informants after they had received appropriate instructions. The clinical evaluation form included questions related to: 1) basic demographic characteristics and history of cognitive decline, 2) Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Examination [13], 3) CDR [14], 4) the 15–item geriatric depression scale [15], and 5) a medical history including vascular risk factors such as hypertension and diabetes. Medical diseases were regarded as positive if the patient had either previously been diagnosed with the associated disease or was currently under medical treatment for the disease.
A standardized neuropsychological battery, the Seoul Neuropsychological Screening Battery-Dementia Version (SNSB-D) was used to assess all of the participants [16]. The SNSB-D includes tests of verbal memory, language, visuospatial function, and frontal/executive function domains. These domains were assessed by performing the following tests: the Seoul Verbal Learning Test-delayed recall (SVLT-delayed recall) for verbal memory, the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT), the Stroop Test-color reading for frontal/executive function, the Korean short version of the Boston Naming Test (K-BNT) for language, and the Rey-Complex Figure Test-copy (RCFT-copy) for visuospatial function.
All blood samples were drawn in the morning from consenting subjects after an eight-hour fast. Blood samples were not obtained from subjects who were in the acute phase of an illness or had a history of fever, alcohol drinking, exercise, or severe stress within the previous week. Plasma samples obtained from all study subjects were aliquoted and stored at-80°C until analysis. Plasma HSP 70 and HSP 27 levels were analyzed using commercially available enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) kits (StressGen, Victoria, Canada). The average intra-assay coefficient of variation was 6% and the average inter-assay coefficient of variation was 9% for HSP 70 ELISA assay. The average intra-assay coefficient of variation was 7% and the average inter-assay coefficient of variation was 8% for HSP 27 ELISA assay.
3. Statistical analyses
The general characteristics of the participants were examined according to subject groups (the NCI-to-NCI non-conversion group vs. the NCI-to-MCI conversion group) using the chi-square tests and independent t-test as appropriate. Then, a repeated measures analysis of variance (repeated measures ANOVA) was conducted in order to examine the interaction between group and time with respect to the change in plasma HSPs level throughout the follow-up period. To minimize the potential confounding effects, the following covariates were entered into the adjusted model: age, gender, years of education, duration of follow-up, cerebrovascular risk factors (hypertension and diabetes), and APOE e4 allele status.
Linear mixed models were used to estimate the association between the plasma HSP level and cognitive change during the follow-up period. These models were applied separately based on plasma HSP 70 and HSP 27 levels. The linear mixed model included terms for time, plasma HSPs levels, age, gender, years of education, and the duration of follow-up. This model was subsequently expanded to include cerebrovascular risk factors (hypertension and diabetes) and APOE e4 allele status as covariates. A value of p<0.05 was considered statistically significant. SPSS software, version 18.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used for all analyses.
Results
1. Baseline clinical characteristics
Clinical characteristics of the subjects according to the presence of conversion from NCI to MCI are described in Table 1. No significant differences were noted in the demographic and clinical characteristics between the conversion (N = 30) and non-conversion (N = 36) groups except years of education and follow-up MMSE scores (years of education: conversion group<non-conversion group, t = 2.18, p = 0.033; follow-up MMSE score: conversion group<non-conversion group, t = 4.72, p<0.001).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of subjects. †
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119180.t001
2. Interaction between group and time on change in plasma HSPs level
The conversion and non-conversion groups showed no significant differences in plasma HSP 70 and 27 levels at baseline. On the other hand, there was a significant difference in HSP 70 level between the two groups at follow-up (t = -3.43, p = 0.002) (Table 2).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 2. Plasma heat shock protein 70 and 27 levels at baseline and follow-up. †
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119180.t002
Using repeated measure ANOVA, the interactive effects between the group and time on the change in plasma HSPs levels were tested after adjusting for confounding variables. We observed that plasma HSPs levels tended to increase with age, even though the change was not statistically significant (HSP 70: F = 0.07, p = 0.792; HSP 27: F = 0.25, p = 0.622). However, when considering the effect of the different groups, a significant interaction between group and time was observed in the plasma HSP 70 level (HSP 70: F = 7.59, p = 0.008; HSP 27: F = 0.67, p = 0.416) (Fig. 1).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 1. Interactive effects between time and group on change in plasma HSP levels.
Repeated measure ANOVA was tested. Adjusted for age, gender, years of education, follow-up duration, APOE e4, hypertension, and diabetes (HSP70: F = 7.59 p = 0.008; HSP27: F = 0.67, p = 0.416). * p = 0.008.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119180.g001
3. Association between the change in HSPs levels and cognitive function during the follow-up period
Linear mixed model analyses showed that plasma HSP 70 level was negatively correlated with cognitive performance on the cognitive tests (K-BNT:—Coefficient [95% CI] -0.443 [-0.804, -0.081], p = 0.016; SVLT-delayed recall: -0.093 [-0.163, 0.022], p = 0.010; RCFT-copy: -0.174 [-0.707, 0.359], p = 0.522; COWAT: -0.171 [-0.329, -0.012], p = 0.035; Stroop: -0.278 [-0.447, -0.109], p = 0.001) after adjusting for age at baseline, gender, years of education, and follow-up duration. Even when the confounding effects of the APOE e4 allele, hypertension, and diabetes were considered together, the overall results were unchanged (K-BNT: -0.426 [-0.781, -0.071], p = 0.019; SVLT-delayed recall: -0.063 [-0.138, 0.012], p = 0.101; RCFT-copy: -0.041 [-0.569, 0.486], p = 0.877; COWAT: -0.176 [-0.328, -0.023], p = 0.024; Stroop: -0.304 [-0.458, -0.150], p<0.001). However, these associations were not statistically significant in relation to the plasma HSP 27 level. These findings are summarized in Table 3.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 3. Association between the change in heat shock protein level and cognitive function during follow-up period. †
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119180.t003
Discussion
In this study, we investigated the relationship between plasma HSP 70 and 27 levels and cognitive decline in the community-dwelling elderly. The results from our study showed that only the plasma HSP 70 level was associated with a conversion from NCI to MCI after adjustments for age, gender, years of education, follow-up duration, APOE e4 status, hypertension, and diabetes. Furthermore, an increased plasma HSP 70 level was associated with cognitive decline in language and executive functions.
An underlying mechanism of the linkage between increasing plasma HSP 70 level and the conversion from NCI to MCI has not yet been clearly identified, but the contribution of a cytoprotective mechanism seems plausible. Increasing oxidative stress markers have been reported in subjects with MCI [17–20]. Oxidative stress could be an early event of cognitive decline in the elderly [21,22]. The protein structure changes caused by oxidative damage generally result in impaired function and lead to the formation of protein aggregates in the cytosol [2]. HSP 70 is involved in the ubiquitin—proteasome pathway and is induced to defend against oxidative stress in its cytoprotective capacity [2,23]. For instance, HSP 70 was identified as a protector against Aβ accumulation and its overexpression rescued neurons from Aβ-mediated neurotoxicity [24,25].
Longitudinal analyses demonstrated increasing plasma HSP 70 levels were associated with a cognitive decline in language and executive functions, even after adjusting for confounding factors. A similar tendency was also observed in memory function before adjusting for the APOE e4 factor (SVLT-delayed recall: -0.093 [-0.163, 0.022], p = 0.010). However, the statistical significance of this association disappeared after adjustment for the presence of APOE e4 (SVLT-delayed recall: -0.063 [-0.138, 0.012], p = 0.101). The aggravation of cerebrovascular pathology in the elderly was known to be more closely related to language and executive functions than memory function [26–29]. Several studies have indicated that the expression of HSP 70 reduced ischemic injury, induced the inflammatory response, and protected both neurons and glial cells [10,30–34]. Taken together, our data present the possibility that an increase of plasma HSP 70 level may reflect functional declines in the cognitive domains known to be more closely associated with cerebrovascular injury.
Further, our results revealed that the plasma Hsp27 level did not show a significant relationship with the conversion to MCI. However, inverse correlation tendency between the plasma HSP 27 level and cognitive decline was observed, even though it was not statistically significant (K-BNT: -0.014 [-0.028, 0.000], p = 0.051; COWAT: -0.006 [-0.013, 0.001], p = 0.078). This may be due to the characteristics of HSP 27 function, which are modulated by its phosphorylation status. Several studies showed that HSP 27 directly bound to hyperphosphorylated tau without ubiquitination. Increased HSP 27 levels might be favored by the occurrence of hyperphosphorylated tau, which could protect the subsequent formation of the neurofibrillary tangle (NFT) [8,35–38]. NFT formation shows a tendency to increase gradually throughout the course of the illness unlike other neurodegenerative pathology, such as amyloid deposition which reaches a threshold early in the disease process [39]. Given that our subjects were in the relatively early phase of cognitive decline, the pathological state of our subjects might not have been enough to reach the conditions for HSP 27 overexpression.
There are several limitations to this study. First, the small sample size limits the predictive power of the study. First, the small sample size limits the predictive power of the study. GDEMCIS was a large scale community study but the current study only involved 66 subjects. To increase the certainty of diagnosis and exclude confounding factors in relation between HSPs level and early phage of cognitive declines in the elderly, we could include only a small number of subjects who had complete blood sample data and clinical follow-ups from the whole cohort. Therefore, these aspects might limit the representativeness of our sample as a community-based population. Second, there was only one follow-up measurements reported in this study. MCI has not been known as a confirmative condition. As people with MCI would improve or remain stable in cognition over time, it would be difficult to state any firm conclusion based on the findings in converters to MCI at only one point of follow up assessment. Third, the change in follow up HSPs levels might reflect a nonspecific disease state. Although we adjusted for the possible confounding factors, it should be noted that the observed interrelationship might not be a direct cause—effect relationship due to complex interactive factors, such as medications and age-related physical illnesses. Fourth, it is conceivable that plasma levels do not adequately reflect tissue levels or tissue responses. Fifth, analyses on association with different cognitive domains alone may not give direct evidences for the effect of HSPs on the degenerative process. The function of HSPs may be related nonspecific physiological effects on homeostasis and neuroprotection. So, it is necessary to carry out further investigations to prove out this relationship. Finally, it is possible that our subjects with MCI are a pathologically heterogeneous group. However, multiple pathologies might be more common than a single pure pathology in older adults with dementia [40].
Nonetheless, our study has several strengths. To our knowledge, this is the first study to report the temporal relationship between plasma HSP 70 levels and the conversion from NCI to MCI. We also uniquely demonstrated that language and executive function declines were associated more strongly with the change in plasma HSP 70 level in the elderly. This study raises the need for further investigations about HSPs involvement in the pathogenesis and progression of cognitive decline in the elderly.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: SJS KSL CHH JHC BHO YL SHC JSN KYL YKC. Performed the experiments: SJS KSL HJK SHK TJ KJC HWR. Analyzed the data: SJS JWB. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SJS CHH JHC SHK TJ. Wrote the paper: SJS CHH.
Citation: Son SJ, Lee KS, Chung JH, Chang KJ, Roh HW, Kim SH, et al. (2015) Increased Plasma Levels of Heat Shock Protein 70 Associated with Subsequent Clinical Conversion to Mild Cognitive Impairment in Cognitively Healthy Elderly. PLoS ONE 10(3): e0119180. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119180
1. Brown IR. Heat shock proteins and protection of the nervous system. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007; 1113: 147–158. pmid:17656567
2. Di Domenico F, Sultana R, Tiu GF, Scheff NN, Perluigi M, Cini C, et al. Protein levels of heat shock proteins 27, 32, 60, 70, 90 and thioredoxin-1 in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: an investigation on the role of cellular stress response in the progression of Alzheimer disease. Brain Res. 2010; 1333: 72–81. pmid:20362559
3. Mayer MP, Bukau B. Hsp70 chaperones: cellular functions and molecular mechanism. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2005; 62: 670–684. pmid:15770419
4. Selkoe DJ. Cell biology of protein misfolding: the examples of Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Nat Cell Biol. 2004; 6: 1054–1061. pmid:15516999
5. Morimoto RI, Kline MP, Bimston DN, Cotto JJ. The heat-shock response: regulation and function of heat-shock proteins and molecular chaperones. Essays Biochem. 1997; 32: 17–29. pmid:9493008
6. Becker J, Craig EA. Heat-shock proteins as molecular chaperones. Eur J Biochem. 1994; 219: 11–23. pmid:8306977
7. Arawaka S, Machiya Y, Kato T. Heat shock proteins as suppressors of accumulation of toxic prefibrillar intermediates and misfolded proteins in neurodegenerative diseases. Curr Pharm Biotechnol. 2010; 11: 158–166. pmid:20170473
8. Cohen FE, Kelly JW. Therapeutic approaches to protein-misfolding diseases. Nature. 2003; 426: 905–909. pmid:14685252
9. Franklin TB, Krueger-Naug AM, Clarke DB, Arrigo AP, Currie RW. The role of heat shock proteins Hsp70 and Hsp27 in cellular protection of the central nervous system. Int J Hyperthermia. 2005; 21: 379–392. pmid:16048836
10. Lee KS, Chung JH, Oh BH, Hong CH. Increased plasma levels of heat shock protein 70 in patients with vascular mild cognitive impairment. Neurosci Lett. 2008; 436: 223–226. pmid:18394800
11. Kim E, Lee SH, Lee KS, Cheong HK, Namkoong K, Hong CH, et al. AMPK gamma2 subunit gene PRKAG2 polymorphism associated with cognitive impairment as well as diabetes in old age. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012; 37: 358–365. pmid:21813245
12. Amici C, Rossi A, Santoro MG. Aspirin enhances thermotolerance in human erythroleukemic cells: an effect associated with the modulation of the heat shock response. Cancer Res. 1995; 55: 4452–4457. pmid:7671259
13. Kang Y, Na DL, Hanhn S. A validity study on the Korean mini-mental state examination (K-MMSE) in dementia patients. Journal of the Korean Neurological Association. 1997; 15: 300–307.
14. Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993; 43: 2412–2414. pmid:8232972
15. Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. In Brink TL, editor. Clinical Gerontology: A Guide to Assessment and Intervention. New York: Haworth Press; 1986. pp. 165–173.
16. Kang Y, Na DL. Seoul Neuropsychological Screening Battery (SNSB). 1st ed. Seoul: Human Brain Research & Consulting Co; 2003.
17. Butterfield DA, Lauderback CM. Lipid peroxidation and protein oxidation in Alzheimer’s disease brain: potential causes and consequences involving amyloid beta-peptide-associated free radical oxidative stress. Free Radic Biol Med. 2002; 32: 1050–1060. pmid:12031889
18. Butterfield DA, Poon HF, St Clair D, Keller JN, Pierce WM, Klein JB, et al. Redox proteomics identification of oxidatively modified hippocampal proteins in mild cognitive impairment: insights into the development of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Dis. 2006; 22: 223–232. pmid:16466929
19. Reed T, Perluigi M, Sultana R, Pierce WM, Klein JB, Turner DM, et al. Redox proteomic identification of 4-hydroxy-2-nonenal-modified brain proteins in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: insight into the role of lipid peroxidation in the progression and pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Dis. 2008; 30: 107–120. pmid:18325775
20. Keller JN, Schmitt FA, Scheff SW, Ding Q, Chen Q, Butterfield DA, et al. Evidence of increased oxidative damage in subjects with mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2005; 64: 1152–1156. pmid:15824339
21. Sultana R, Butterfield DA. Oxidatively modified, mitochondria-relevant brain proteins in subjects with Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. J Bioenerg Biomembr. 2009; 41: 441–446. pmid:19777328
22. Muchowski PJ, Wacker JL. Modulation of neurodegeneration by molecular chaperones. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005; 6: 11–22. pmid:15611723
23. Calabrese V, Scapagnini G, Ravagna A, Fariello RG, Giuffrida Stella AM, Abraham NG. Regional distribution of heme oxygenase, HSP70, and glutathione in brain: relevance for endogenous oxidant/antioxidant balance and stress tolerance. J Neurosci Res. 2002; 68: 65–75. pmid:11933050
24. Evans CG, Wisen S, Gestwicki JE. Heat shock proteins 70 and 90 inhibit early stages of amyloid beta-(1–42) aggregation in vitro. J Biol Chem. 2006; 281: 33182–33191. pmid:16973602
25. Magrane J, Smith RC, Walsh K, Querfurth HW. Heat shock protein 70 participates in the neuroprotective response to intracellularly expressed beta-amyloid in neurons. J Neurosci. 2004; 24: 1700–1706. pmid:14973234
26. Maineri Nde L, Xavier FM, Berleze MC, Moriguchi EH. Risk factors for cerebrovascular disease and cognitive function in the elderly. Arq Bras Cardiol. 2007; 89: 142–146, 158–162. pmid:17906814
27. Tervo S, Kivipelto M, Hanninen T, Vanhanen M, Hallikainen M, Mannermaa A, et al. Incidence and risk factors for mild cognitive impairment: a population-based three-year follow-up study of cognitively healthy elderly subjects. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2004; 17: 196–203. pmid:14739544
28. Kramer JH, Reed BR, Mungas D, Weiner MW, Chui HC Executive dysfunction in subcortical ischaemic vascular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002; 72: 217–220. pmid:11796772
29. Son SJ, Lee KS, Lee Y, Baek JH, Choi SH, Na DL, et al. Association between white matter hyperintensity severity and cognitive impairment according to the presence of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) epsilon4 allele in the elderly: retrospective analysis of data from the CREDOS study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2012; 73: 1555–1562. pmid:23146133
30. Giffard RG, Xu L, Zhao H, Carrico W, Ouyang Y, Qiao Y, et al. Chaperones, protein aggregation, and brain protection from hypoxic/ischemic injury. J Exp Biol. 2004; 207: 3213–3220. pmid:15299042
31. Rajdev S, Hara K, Kokubo Y, Mestril R, Dillmann W, Weinstein , et al. Mice overexpressing rat heat shock protein 70 are protected against cerebral infarction. Ann Neurol. 2000; 47: 782–791. pmid:10852544
32. Pockley AG. Heat shock proteins, inflammation, and cardiovascular disease. Circulation. 2002; 105: 1012–1017. pmid:11864934
33. Wright BH, Corton JM, El-Nahas AM, Wood RF, Pockley AG. Elevated levels of circulating heat shock protein 70 (Hsp70) in peripheral and renal vascular disease. Heart Vessels. 2000; 15: 18–22. pmid:11001481
34. Njemini R, Bautmans I, Onyema OO, Van Puyvelde K, Demanet C, Mets T. Circulating heat shock protein 70 in health, aging and disease. BMC Immunol. 2011; 12: 24. pmid:21443787
35. Bjorkdahl C, Sjogren MJ, Zhou X, Concha H, Avila J, Winblad B, et al. Small heat shock proteins Hsp27 or alphaB-crystallin and the protein components of neurofibrillary tangles: tau and neurofilaments. J Neurosci Res. 2008; 86: 1343–1352. pmid:18061943
36. Charette SJ, Landry J. The interaction of HSP27 with Daxx identifies a potential regulatory role of HSP27 in Fas-induced apoptosis. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2000; 926: 126–131. pmid:11193028
37. Renkawek K, Bosman GJ, de Jong WW. Expression of small heat-shock protein hsp 27 in reactive gliosis in Alzheimer disease and other types of dementia. Acta Neuropathol. 1994; 87: 511–519. pmid:8059604
38. Shimura H, Miura-Shimura Y, Kosik KS. Binding of tau to heat shock protein 27 leads to decreased concentration of hyperphosphorylated tau and enhanced cell survival. J Biol Chem. 2004; 279: 17957–17962. pmid:14963027
39. Ingelsson M, Fukumoto H, Newell KL, Growdon JH, Hedley-Whyte ET, Frosch MP, et al. Early Abeta accumulation and progressive synaptic loss, gliosis, and tangle formation in AD brain. Neurology. 2004; 62: 925–931. pmid:15037694
40. Wang BW, Lu E, Mackenzie IR, Assaly M, Jacova C, Lee PE, et al. Multiple pathologies are common in Alzheimer patients in clinical trials. Can J Neurol Sci. 2012; 39: 592–599. pmid:22931699
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2015 Son et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Background and Aims
Heat shock proteins (HSPs) have been regarded as cytoprotectants that protect brain cells during the progression of neurodegenerative diseases and from damage resulting from cerebral ischemia. In this study, we assessed the association between plasma HSP 70/27 levels and cognitive decline.
Methods
Among participants in the community-based cohort study of dementia called the Gwangju Dementia and Mild Cognitive Impairment Study, subjects without cognitive impairment at baseline, who then either remained without impairment (non-conversion group), or suffered mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (conversion group) (non-conversion group, N = 36; conversion group, N = 30) were analyzed.
Results
After a five to six year follow-up period, comparison of the plasma HSP 70 and HSP 27 levels of the two groups revealed that only the plasma HSP 70 level was associated with a conversion to MCI after adjustments for age, gender, years of education, follow-up duration, APOE e4, hypertension, and diabetes (repeated measure analysis of variance: F = 7.59, p = 0.008). Furthermore, an increase in plasma HSP 70 level was associated with cognitive decline in language and executive function (linear mixed model: Korean Boston Naming Test, -0.426 [-0.781, -0.071], p = 0.019; Controlled Oral Word Association Test, -0.176 [-0.328, -0.023], p = 0.024; Stroop Test, -0.304 [-0.458, -0.150], p<0.001).
Conclusions
These findings suggest that the plasma HSP 70 level may be related to cognitive decline in the elderly.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer