1. Introduction
Over the past years, naturopathic treatment strategies, such as acupuncture or meditation, have received increasing attention with regard to the treatment of various health conditions, especially pain [1–4]. However, there is still a substantial lack of established experimental pain models for the investigation of acupuncture or meditation that can be utilized in the laboratory. Moreover, experimental studies examining a possible interaction of both the techniques are currently not available although both the methods are often recommended and practiced simultaneously for the treatment of chronic pain syndromes in a multimodal integrative or complementary medicine setting. It can be speculated that a combination of these methods is most effective if they utilize, at least in part, different neurophysiological pathways to exhibit their analgesic effects. If the simultaneous application of meditation and acupuncture fails to show additive effects, then this could be interpreted as an evidence demonstrating that the same descending pain-modulating pathways are involved (e.g., at the level of the dorsal horn).
Meditation effects are often compared with other cognitive manipulations, such as hypnosis or expectancy. These interventions are known to influence the subjective experience of pain and the associated neuronal activity [5–8], particularly with regard to the emotional and functional aspects of pain. Overall, meditation states are comparatively well described neurobiologically [9–13]. Moreover, recent investigations examined the analgesic effect of meditation on experimental, laboratory paradigms, such as the cold pressure test [14, 15], thermal pain [11, 16], or noxious laser stimulation [10]. Nonetheless, clear evidence for a functional relationship between meditation practice and pain relief in experimental, laboratory pain models is still lacking. At the same time, experimental pain paradigms such as thermal pain tests (e.g., [17, 18]) or electrical stimulation [16, 19, 20] have been utilized in laboratory acupuncture research to investigate acupuncture or acupuncture-like TENS (transcutaneous nerve stimulation) analgesia. In the light of the German large-scale acupuncture trials, the results are equivocal [21–24].
In the presented study, the standard paradigm of placebo research, the submaximum effort tourniquet technique (SETT, [25]), was used as experimental pain stimulus to investigate both EA- and meditation-induced analgesia.
2. Methods
2.1. Subjects
The study was approved by the institutional review board of the Medical Institutions of the University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany (no. 07-3499). Twenty-three healthy young men (20–44 years) were recruited. Among them, 10 participants (6 males, mean age: 37.3
2.2. Study Design
On their first visit to the laboratory, the study participants completed the experimental pain procedure (SETT) without any further treatment while on day 2 and 3, they received EA on the arm or leg in randomized order, in addition to SETT. Each examination was separated by at least 48 hours.
2.3. Pain Induction
The SETT induces ischemic pain by inflating a blood pressure cuff on the arm for a prolonged period of time [25]. The SETT was performed according to the standard procedure in placebo research [26–28]. Figure 1 gives an overview of the design used. The subjects were asked to relax for 20 minutes by lying comfortably on an examination couch. Subsequently, they were asked to expose their nondominant arms above the bulk of the biceps/triceps. A standard blood pressure cuff was applied up to a point approximately 5 cm above the elbow crease, then the arm was elevated straight to the ceiling for 30 s, and afterwards, the cuff was rapidly inflated to 250 mmHg. The subjects were asked to lower their arms immediately after complete inflation and were instructed to perform 12 gripping exercises using maximal grip strength. The exercises were performed in a standardized manner by maintaining the grip for 1 s and relaxing for 1 s. The subjects were prompted by a standardized beep tone delivered by an mp3 player.
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]The study participants were prompted every 3 minutes to rate their pain on a numeric rating scale from 0 to 10 with 0 corresponding to “no pain” and 10 corresponding to “worst imaginable pain.” The first rating was given while bringing the arm back in the horizontal position. SETT time was limited to 30 minutes or a pain rating of 10. At the end of pain induction, the cuff was deflated slowly over a 2-minute period. On completion of the procedure, the cuff was carefully removed and the skin examined for any evidence of trauma. No evidence of such trauma or other side effects occurred throughout the study.
2.4. Acupuncture Procedure
Acupuncture was carried out by a physician licensed as an acupuncturist who was involved in neither data collection nor analysis. Common analgesic acupoints were selected for acupuncture treatment (leg condition: ST 36 and LV3, arm condition: LI4 and LI10). All acupoints were located contralaterally to the SETT. Before needle insertion, the acupoints’ surroundings were pressed by the nonpuncturing index finger and thumb to exactly locate the acupoints. Afterwards, needling was performed with 0.25
2.5. Meditation
Different styles of meditation have proved to deliver “improvements in the functioning of mind and brain…consistent with those observed in mental health” [30]. A very old and strict form of meditation is the mainly concentrative Vipassana practice after the tradition of Goenka, which has its origin in Buddhism and is one of India’s most ancient techniques of meditation [31]. Vipassana focuses on the connection between body and mind and is supposed to force a highly disciplined attention to the physical sensations that “instantly form the spiritual sensations.” Experienced Vipassana meditators were selected for the study because of the explicit distance to bodily and emotional experience in this meditation technique. All the meditators were asked to meditate throughout the whole experimental session on all three occasions.
2.6. Statistics
The results were analyzed using analysis of variance (ANOVA) for repeated measures with Greenhouse-Geisser correction when necessary. Post hoc comparisons were made by Bonferroni
3. Results
No subject withdrew from the study. Additionally, no side effects such as skin trauma owing to pain induction occurred during the study.
3.1. Numeric Rating Scale (NRS) Ratings
In case the participants gave a rating of “10” prior to the time limit of 30 minutes, numeric pain ratings interpolated to 10 for the remaining pain rating points. The average rating over time was calculated by dividing the cumulative rating by 11. Figure 4 summarizes the averaged NRS ratings for the two groups at the three pain assessment days.
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]Greenhouse-Geisser corrected ANOVA showed a significant interaction between group and treatment condition with F(
Table 1
Test statistics for two-sample-t-test for sampled groups.
Group | Tested variables | Mean | SD |
|
df |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Controls | Mean rating base and mean rating with arm acupuncture | 1.15035 | .78211 | 5.303 | 12 | .000* |
Mean rating base and mean rating with leg acupuncture | .88811 | .79807 | 4.012 | 12 | .002* | |
Mean rating with arm and leg acupuncture |
|
.69163 |
|
12 | .197 | |
|
||||||
Meditators | Mean rating base and mean rating with arm acupuncture |
|
1.4915 |
|
9 | .226 |
Mean rating base and mean rating with leg acupuncture |
|
1.79403 |
|
9 | .232 | |
Mean rating with arm and leg acupuncture |
|
.58525 |
|
9 | .480 |
A two-sample t-test for independent groups revealed a trend (see Figure 5 for the course of the pain rating during baseline session) but without any significant difference between the groups during baseline with t(21) = 1.99 and
3.2. Break-Off Times
Greenhouse-Geisser corrected ANOVA showed no significant interaction between the group and treatment condition with F(21,1) = 2.572,
3.3. Pain Tolerance
Owing to the strong ceiling effect concerning the break-off times, pain tolerance index scores (defined as the point of break-off divided through the rating at that point) were calculated to derive a measure that takes rating and time into account. ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between the group factor and treatment condition with F(21,1) = 4.120 and
Bonferroni α-adjusted (
Table 2
Test statistics for two-sample-t-test for sampled groups.
Group | Tested variables | Mean | SD |
|
df |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Controls | Pain tolerance base and pain tolerance with arm acupuncture |
|
.80883 |
|
12 | .003* |
Pain tolerance base and pain tolerance with leg acupuncture |
|
.79011 |
|
12 | .024 | |
Pain tolerance with arm and leg acupuncture |
|
.69163 |
|
12 | .197 | |
|
||||||
Meditators | Pain tolerance base and pain tolerance with arm acupuncture | .44000 | 1.50569 | .924 | 9 | .380 |
Pain tolerance base and pain tolerance with leg acupuncture | .40529 | 7.75281 | .731 | 9 | .483 | |
Pain tolerance with arm and leg acupuncture |
|
.58525 | .131 | 9 | .480 |
A two-sample
4. Discussion
Meditation as well as EA was shown to substantially control SETT-induced pain while no synergetic effect of both the techniques was observed (Figure 8). As EA was similarly effective at both the arm and leg, it is likely that supraspinal mechanisms, such as the placebo effect (e.g., [32, 33]) or the spinomedullary Diffuse Noxious Inhibitory Controls (DNICs, [34]) were involved.
[figure omitted; refer to PDF]SETT-induced pain was chosen because (i) the test is opiate-sensitive [26–28], and acupuncture analgesia was previously shown to be partially mediated through endogenous opiate-dependent pathways [35–42], (ii) it has been suggested that acupuncture analgesia may represent a placebo mechanism and the SETT presents a well-established paradigm out of placebo research, and (iii) acupuncture analgesia needs time to unfold its effects. Although there is a foremost evidence [43] demonstrating that analgesic acupuncture effects can also be observed in a threshold paradigm, we expected a more robust effect using a tolerance paradigm. Furthermore, pain tolerance is more similar to patients’ painful experiences.
Clinical acupuncture effects have been discussed to be placebo effects, because “verum” and “sham” acupuncture were similarly effective in the German acupuncture studies (e.g., [21–24]). However, even though it was a standard procedure to utilize minimal or non-point-specific acupuncture as a control, there are now substantial doubts whether these conditions are really inactive for the treatment of pain [44–47]. Support for the notion that the German acupuncture trials differ from other trials on pain comes from a recently updated Cochrane review [48]. The authors were unable to reveal a placebo effect across a wide variety of conditions, including pain. The effect on pain was particularly variable, also among trials with low risk of bias. Among these, the four similarly designed German acupuncture trials reported the largest effects whereas three other pain trials reported low or no effect. This is a strong hint to the fact that the assumed sham control condition in these trials was active. In the light of a possible misinterpretation of “sham” acupuncture to be an inactive condition, former attempts to investigate acupuncture analgesia with the SETT, revealing no significant difference between true and sham acupuncture [49], have to be re-evaluated. In the study by Barlas et al., both the conditions showed a substantial reduction in pain ratings. Moreover, a study from our own group showed that EA was as effective as a single dose of an orally administered opiate in reducing SETT pain while there was no effect of a placebo pill or Ibuprofen [50]. In summary, we conclude that placebo effects in acupuncture trials are largely overestimated and that the acupuncture effects seen in our study are not owing to placebo effects.
Meditation-induced analgesia is in line with numerous findings showing that other cortically mediated or “top-down” mechanisms, such as expectation, emotion, or attention, affect the neuronal activity of brain regions involved in descending pain inhibition [5, 51–53]. People suffering from pain are active in trying multiple treatments, including self-care strategies [54], and often use them in combination.
The lack of synergetic effects of meditation and EA in this study is interesting and contradicts the theoretical assumption that hypnosis, another top-down process, and acupuncture are expected to reveal synergistic effects [8]. However, our findings are in line with the recent exciting data from the study by Eippert et al. showing that the cortically induced antinociceptive placebo response utilizes a spinal pathway [55]. “Top-down” mechanisms could therefore induce presynaptic inhibition at the level of the dorsal horn [56] and thus have a direct influence on the incoming sensory information.
It is possible to observe a physiological “ceiling” effect in our data owing to the fact that both the interventions utilize a final common pathway. Furthermore, the lack of a synergetic effect of EA and meditation in our study may even reflect a reciprocal inhibition. It has been shown in humans that expectation with regard to pain can change the intensity of spinal nociceptive responses. In particular, the expectation of hyperalgesia can completely block the normal analgesic response induced by DNIC [5]. As DNIC is a likely mechanism in the mode of action of acupuncture analgesia, an antianalgesic interaction at the level of the periaqueductal gray and the rostroventral medulla must also be considered. Nonetheless, this hypothesis still remains tentative and has to be investigated further.
The results presented here are based on a rather small sample, and there are some other limitations of the study. As a complex intervention, acupuncture treatment is almost impossible to blind. Therefore, as in almost all acupuncture trials, EA was blinded neither to participants nor to the acupuncturist. However, this holds true for the group of meditators as well, who did not show any benefit from acupuncture. Furthermore, factors other than the actual meditative state, for example, meditators’ lifestyle, could have influenced the results. The fact that meditators exhibit permanent brain alterations in the brainstem [13] also and that DNIC is mediated through spinomedullary circuits [34], may have contributed to the group differences.
A further limitation of the study was the ceiling effect observed for the break-off times owing to a rather strict time limit. When compared with the ratings that are likely to represent the emotional aspects of pain processing, the break-off times correspond more to the behavioral aspects of pain, for example, two subjects giving the same pain rating might still have different break-off times. As there is no consensus on how to analyze SETT data if ceiling effects occur, we decided to calculate a pain tolerance index score that takes both the pain ratings as well as the break-off times into account. Thus, the sensational and behavioral aspects of pain processing were expressed in one index.
In conclusion, the standard pain paradigm of placebo research, the SETT, has been shown to be a valid tool in experimental acupuncture as well as meditation research. EA on the leg increased pain tolerance in healthy controls to 25.6% and EA on the arm increased pain tolerance to 38.4% while meditators reached a 69.7% higher pain tolerance when compared with the baseline condition of the healthy controls. These are substantial effects supporting the effectiveness of both the procedures in pain control. To our knowledge, this is the first investigation directly comparing the effect of acupuncture and meditation as two commonly recommended methods of therapeutic pain control in an experimental, laboratory pain procedure.
Acknowledgments
The authors express their gratitude to all the subjects for their participation in the study. This work was supported by a grant of the “Karl and Veronica Carstens Foundation” and by the Chair of Internal and Integrative Medicine, University of Duisburg Essen, Germany. All the authors have disclosed that they have no commercial associations that might create a conflict of interest in connection with the submitted paper. There is particularly no competing financial interest.
[1] S. Birch, "Clinical research on acupuncture: part 2. Controlled clinical trials, an overview of their methods," Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 10 no. 3, pp. 481-498, 2004.
[2] L. Eshkevari, "Acupuncture and pain: a review of the literature," Journal of the American Association of Nurse Anesthetists, vol. 71 no. 5, pp. 361-370, 2003.
[3] J. Kabat-Zinn, A. O. Massion, J. Kristeller, L. G. Peterson, K. E. Fletcher, L. Pbert, W. R. Lenderking, S. F. Santorelli, "Effectiveness of a meditation-based stress reduction program in the treatment of anxiety disorders," American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 149 no. 7, pp. 936-943, 1992.
[4] N. E. Morone, C. S. Lynch, C. M. Greco, H. A. Tindle, D. K. Weiner, "“I felt like a new person.” The effects of mindfulness meditation on older adults with chronic pain: qualitative narrative analysis of diary entries," Journal of Pain, vol. 9 no. 9, pp. 841-848, DOI: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.04.003, 2008.
[5] P. Goffaux, W. J. Redmond, P. Rainville, S. Marchand, "Descending analgesia—when the spine echoes what the brain expects," Pain, vol. 130 no. 1-2, pp. 137-143, DOI: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.11.011, 2007.
[6] J. A. Grant, P. Rainville, "Pain sensitivity and analgesic effects of mindful states in zen meditators: a cross-sectional study," Psychosomatic Medicine, vol. 71 no. 1, pp. 106-114, DOI: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818f52ee, 2009.
[7] T. Koyama, J. G. McHaffie, P. J. Laurienti, R. C. Coghill, "The subjective experience of pain: where expectations become reality," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 102 no. 36, pp. 12950-12955, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0408576102, 2005.
[8] E. Schiff, S. Gurgevich, O. Caspi, "Potential synergism between hypnosis and acupuncture—is the whole more than the sum of its parts?," Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 4 no. 2, pp. 233-240, DOI: 10.1093/ecam/nel069, 2007.
[9] B. R. Cahn, J. Polich, "Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies," Psychological Bulletin, vol. 132 no. 2, pp. 180-211, DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180, 2006.
[10] R. Kakigi, H. Nakata, K. Inui, N. Hiroe, O. Nagata, M. Honda, S. Tanaka, N. Sadato, M. Kawakami, "Intracerebral pain processing in a Yoga Master who claims not to feel pain during meditation," European Journal of Pain, vol. 9 no. 5, pp. 581-589, DOI: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.12.006, 2005.
[11] D. W. Orme-Johnson, R. H. Schneider, Y. D. Son, S. Nidich, Z.-H. Cho, "Neuroimaging of meditation's effect on brain reactivity to pain," NeuroReport, vol. 17 no. 12, pp. 1359-1363, DOI: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000233094.67289.a8, 2006.
[12] E. B. Short, S. Kose, Q. Mu, J. Borckardt, A. Newberg, M. S. George, F. A. Kozel, "Regional brain activation during meditation shows time and practice effects: an exploratory FMRI study," Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 7 no. 1, pp. 121-127, DOI: 10.1093/ecam/nem163, 2010.
[13] P. Vestergaard-Poulsen, M. van Beek, J. Skewes, C. R. Bjarkam, M. Stubberup, J. Bertelsen, A. Roepstorff, "Long-term meditation is associated with increased gray matter density in the brain stem," NeuroReport, vol. 20 no. 2, pp. 170-174, DOI: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328320012a, 2009.
[14] J. Kingston, P. Chadwick, D. Meron, T. C. Skinner, "A pilot randomized control trial investigating the effect of mindfulness practice on pain tolerance, psychological well-being, and physiological activity," Journal of Psychosomatic Research, vol. 62 no. 3, pp. 297-300, DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.10.007, 2007.
[15] W. W. Mills, J. T. Farrow, "The transcendental meditation technique and acute experimental pain," Psychosomatic Medicine, vol. 43 no. 2, pp. 157-164, 1981.
[16] N. Danziger, S. Rozenberg, P. Bourgeois, G. Charpentier, J. C. Willer, "Depressive effects of segmental and heterotopic application of transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation and piezo-electric current on lower limb nociceptive flexion reflex in human subjects," Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, vol. 79 no. 2, pp. 191-200, DOI: 10.1016/S0003-9993(98)90299-4, 1998.
[17] J. Kong, D. T. Fufa, A. J. Gerber, I. S. Rosman, M. G. Vangel, R. H. Gracely, R. L. Gollub, "Psychophysical outcomes from a randomized pilot study of manual, electro, and sham acupuncture treatment on experimentally induced thermal pain," Journal of Pain, vol. 6 no. 1, pp. 55-64, DOI: 10.1016/j.jpain.2004.10.005, 2005.
[18] A. Leung, B. Khadivi, J.-R. Duann, Z. H. Cho, T. Yaksh, "The effect of Ting point (tendinomuscular meridians) electroacupuncture on thermal pain: a model for studying the neuronal mechanism of acupuncture analgesia," Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 11 no. 4, pp. 653-661, DOI: 10.1089/acm.2005.11.653, 2005.
[19] F. L. Cramp, G. Noble, A. S. Lowe, D. M. Walsh, J. C. Willer, "A controlled study on the effects of transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation and interferential therapy upon the RIII nociceptive and H- reflexes in humans," Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, vol. 81 no. 3, pp. 324-333, DOI: 10.1053/apmr.2000.0810324, 2000.
[20] D. M. Walsh, G. Noble, G. D. Baxter, J. M. Allen, "Study of the effects of various transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) parameters upon the RIII nociceptive and H-reflexes in humans," Clinical Physiology, vol. 20 no. 3, pp. 191-199, DOI: 10.1046/j.1365-2281.2000.00246.x, 2000.
[21] B. Brinkhaus, C. M. Witt, S. Jena, K. Linde, A. Streng, S. Wagenpfeil, D. Irnich, H.-U. Walther, D. Melchart, S. N. Willich, "Acupuncture in patients with chronic low back pain: a randomized controlled trial," Archives of Internal Medicine, vol. 166 no. 4, pp. 450-457, DOI: 10.1001/.450, 2006.
[22] K. Linde, A. Streng, S. Jürgens, A. Hoppe, B. Brinkhaus, C. Witt, S. Wagenpfeil, V. Pfaffenrath, M. G. Hammes, W. Weidenhammer, S. N. Willich, D. Melchart, "Acupuncture for patients with migraine: a randomized controlled trial," Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 293 no. 17, pp. 2118-2125, DOI: 10.1001/jama.293.17.2118, 2005.
[23] D. Melchart, A. Streng, A. Hoppe, B. Brinkhaus, C. Witt, S. Wagenpfeil, V. Pfaffenrath, M. Hammes, J. Hummelsberger, D. Irnich, W. Weidenhammer, S. N. Willich, K. Linde, "Acupuncture in patients with tension-type headache: randomised controlled trial," British Medical Journal, vol. 331 no. 7513, pp. 376-379, DOI: 10.1136/bmj.38512.405440.8F, 2005.
[24] C. Witt, B. Brinkhaus, S. Jena, K. Linde, A. Streng, S. Wagenpfeil, J. Hummelsberger, H. U. Walther, D. Melchart, S. N. Willich, "Acupuncture in patients with osteoarthritis of the knee: a randomised trial," Lancet, vol. 366 no. 9480, pp. 136-143, DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)66871-7, 2005.
[25] G. M. Smith, L. D. Egbert, R. A. Markowitz, F. Mosteller, H. K. Beecher, "An experimental pain method sensitive to morphine in man: the submaximum effort tourniquet technique," Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, vol. 154 no. 2, pp. 324-332, 1966.
[26] F. Benedetti, "The opposite effects of the opiate antagonist naloxone and the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumide on placebo analgesia," Pain, vol. 64 no. 3, pp. 535-543, DOI: 10.1016/0304-3959(95)00179-4, 1996.
[27] A. Pertovaara, P. Kemppainen, G. Johansson, S. L. Karonen, "Ischemic pain nonsegmentally produces a predominant reduction of pain and thermal sensitivity in man: a selective role for endogenous opioids," Brain Research, vol. 251 no. 1, pp. 83-92, DOI: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)91276-8, 1982.
[28] G. M. Smith, E. Lowenstein, J. H. Hubbard, H. K. Beecher, "Experimental pain produced by the submaximum effort tourniquet technique: further evidence of validity," Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, vol. 163 no. 2, pp. 468-474, 1968.
[29] J.-S. Han, "Acupuncture and endorphins," Neuroscience Letters, vol. 361 no. 1–3, pp. 258-261, DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.12.019, 2004.
[30] A. Hankey, "Studies of advanced stages of meditation in the Tibetan Buddhist and Vedic traditions. I: a comparison of general changes," Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 3 no. 4, pp. 513-521, DOI: 10.1093/ecam/nel040, 2006.
[31] W. Hart, The Art of Living: Vipassana-Meditation as Taught by S.N. Goenka, 1987.
[32] M. Amanzio, F. Benedetti, "Neuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: expectation- activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystems," Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 19 no. 1, pp. 484-494, 1999.
[33] R. B. Bausell, L. Lao, S. Bergman, W.-L. Lee, B. M. Berman, "Is acupuncture analgesia an expectancy effect? Preliminary evidence based on participants' perceived assignments in two placebo-controlled trials," Evaluation and the Health Professions, vol. 28 no. 1,DOI: 10.1177/0163278704273081, 2005.
[34] D. Le Bars, J. C. Willer, "Pain modulation triggered by high-intensity stimulation: implication for acupuncture analgesia?," International Congress Series, vol. 1236, pp. 11-29, 2002.
[35] J. S. Han, L. Terenius, "Neurochemical basis of acupuncture analgesia," Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology, vol. 22, pp. 193-220, 1982.
[36] J. S. Han, G. X. Xie, X. Z. Ding, S. G. Fan, "High and low frequency electro-acupuncture analgesia are mediated by different opioid peptides," Pain, vol. 18, 1984.
[37] Z.-L. Guo, A. R. Moazzami, S. Tjen-A-Looi, J. C. Longhurst, "Responses of opioid and serotonin containing medullary raphe neurons to electroacupuncture," Brain Research, vol. 1229, pp. 125-136, DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.020, 2008.
[38] D. Mayer, "Biological mechanisms of acupuncture," The Biological Basis for Mind Body Interactions, vol. 122, pp. 457-477, 2002.
[39] Z.-Q. Zhao, "Neural mechanism underlying acupuncture analgesia," Progress in Neurobiology, vol. 85 no. 4, pp. 355-375, DOI: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.05.004, 2008.
[40] F. Qu, J. Zhou, "Electro-acupuncture in relieving labor pain," Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 4 no. 1, pp. 125-130, DOI: 10.1093/ecam/nel053, 2007.
[41] S.-X. Ma, "Neurobiology of acupuncture: toward CAM," Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 1 no. 1, pp. 41-47, 2004.
[42] V. Napadow, R. Dhond, K. Park, J. Kim, N. Makris, K. K. Kwong, R. E. Harris, P. L. Purdon, N. Kettner, K. K. S. Hui, "Time-variant fMRI activity in the brainstem and higher structures in response to acupuncture," NeuroImage, vol. 47 no. 1, pp. 289-301, DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.060, 2009.
[43] P. M. Lang, J. Stoer, G. M. Schober, J. F. Audette, D. Irnich, "Bilateral acupuncture analgesia observed by quantitative sensory testing in healthy volunteers," Anesthesia and Analgesia, vol. 110 no. 5, pp. 1448-1456, DOI: 10.1213/ANE.0b013e3181d3e7ef, 2010.
[44] I. Lund, J. Näslund, T. Lundeberg, "Minimal acupuncture is not a valid placebo control in randomised controlled trials of acupuncture: a physiologist's perspective," Chinese Medicine, vol. 4, article 1,DOI: 10.1186/1749-8546-4-1, 2009.
[45] P. M. Wayne, R. Hammerschlag, H. M. Langevin, V. Napadow, J. J. Park, R. N. Schnyer, "Resolving paradoxes in acupuncture research: a roundtable discussion," Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 15 no. 9, pp. 1039-1044, DOI: 10.1089/acm.2009.0466, 2009.
[46] K. Itoh, H. Kitakoji, "Acupuncture for chronic pain in Japan: a review," Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 4 no. 4, pp. 431-438, DOI: 10.1093/ecam/nel092, 2007.
[47] T. Lundeberg, I. Lund, A. Sing, J. Näslund, "Is placebo acupuncture what it is intended to be?," ,DOI: 10.1093/ecam/nep049, .
[48] A. Hróbjartsson, P. C. Gøtzsche, "Placebo interventions for all clinical conditions," Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, vol. no. 3, 2004.
[49] P. Barlas, A. S. Lowe, D. M. Walsh, G. D. Baxter, J. M. Allen, "Effect of acupuncture upon experimentally induced ischemic pain: a sham-controlled single-blind study," Clinical Journal of Pain, vol. 16 no. 3, pp. 255-264, DOI: 10.1097/00002508-200009000-00012, 2000.
[50] F. Musial, T. Gabriel, N. Amthor, "Is Electroacupuncture a placebo in experimental tourniquet pain?," European Journal of Integrative Medicine, vol. 1 no. 1, pp. 16-17, 2008.
[51] P. Petrovic, E. Kalso, K. M. Petersson, M. Ingvar, "Placebo and opioid analgesia—imaging a shared neuronal network," Science, vol. 295 no. 5560, pp. 1737-1740, DOI: 10.1126/science.1067176, 2002.
[52] I. Tracey, A. Ploghaus, J. S. Gati, S. Clare, S. Smith, R. S. Menon, P. M. Matthews, "Imaging attentional modulation of pain in the periaqueductal gray in humans," Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 22 no. 7, pp. 2748-2752, 2002.
[53] T. D. Wager, J. K. Rilling, E. E. Smith, A. Sokolik, K. L. Casey, R. J. Davidson, S. M. Kosslyn, R. M. Rose, J. D. Cohen, "Placebo-induced changes in fMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain," Science, vol. 303 no. 5661, pp. 1162-1167, DOI: 10.1126/science.1093065, 2004.
[54] J. H. O. Turunen, P. T. Mäntyselkä, E. A. Kumpusalo, R. S. Ahonen, "How do people ease their pain? A population-based study," Journal of Pain, vol. 5 no. 9, pp. 498-504, DOI: 10.1016/j.jpain.2004.08.003, 2004.
[55] F. Eippert, J. Finsterbusch, U. Bingel, C. Büchel, "Direct evidence for spinal cord involvement in placebo analgesia," Science, vol. 326 no. 5951,DOI: 10.1126/science.1180142, 2009.
[56] H. L. Fields, I. Basbaum, M. M. Heinricher, "Central nervous system mechanisms of pain modulation," Wall and Melzack’s Textbook of Pain, pp. 125-142, 2006.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright © 2011 Kyung-Eun Choi et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Abstract
Acupuncture and meditation are promising treatment options for clinical pain. However, studies investigating the effects of these methods on experimental pain conditions are equivocal. Here, the effects of electroacupuncture (EA) and meditation on the submaximum effort tourniquet technique (SETT), a well-established, opiate-sensitive pain paradigm in experimental placebo research were studied. Ten experienced meditators (6 male subjects) and 13 nonmeditators (6 male subjects) were subjected to SETT (250 mmHG) on one baseline (SETT only) and two treatment days (additional EA contralaterally to the SETT, either at the leg on ST36 and LV3 or at the arm on LI4 and LI10 in randomized order). Numeric Rating Scale (NRS) ratings (scale 0–10) were recorded every 3 min. During baseline, meditation induced significantly greater pain tolerance in meditators when compared with the control group. Both the EA conditions significantly increased pain tolerance and reduced pain ratings in controls. Furthermore, EA diminished the group difference in pain sensitivity, indicating that meditators had no additional benefit from acupuncture. The data suggest that EA as a presumable bottom-up process may be as effective as meditation in controlling experimental SETT pain. However, no combined effect of both the techniques could be observed.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
1 Chair of Complementary and Integrative Medicine, University of Duisburg-Essen, 45276 Essen, Germany
2 Department of Integrative and Complementary Medicine, Institute for Social Medicine, Epidemiology and Health Economics, Immanuel Hospital Berlin, Charité University Medical Center Berlin, 14109 Berlin, Germany