ARTICLE
Received 18 Sep 2015 | Accepted 12 Jan 2016 | Published 23 Feb 2016
DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698 OPEN
Fear of large carnivores causes a trophic cascade
Justin P. Suraci1,2,3, Michael Clinchy3, Lawrence M. Dill4, Devin Roberts1 & Liana Y. Zanette3
The fear large carnivores inspire, independent of their direct killing of prey, may itself cause cascading effects down food webs potentially critical for conserving ecosystem function, particularly by affecting large herbivores and mesocarnivores. However, the evidence of this has been repeatedly challenged because it remains experimentally untested. Here we show that experimentally manipulating fear itself in free-living mesocarnivore (raccoon) populations using month-long playbacks of large carnivore vocalizations caused just such cascading effects, reducing mesocarnivore foraging to the benet of the mesocarnivores prey, which in turn affected a competitor and prey of the mesocarnivores prey. We further report that by experimentally restoring the fear of large carnivores in our study system, where most large carnivores have been extirpated, we succeeded in reversing this mesocarnivores impacts. We suggest that our results reinforce the need to conserve large carnivores given the signicant ecosystem service the fear of them provides.
1 Department of Biology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada V8W 2Y2. 2 Raincoast Conservation Foundation, Sidney, British Columbia, Canada V8L 3Y3. 3 Department of Biology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada N6A 5B7. 4 Department of Biological Sciences, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada V5A 1S6. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to J.P.S.(email: mailto:[email protected]
Web End [email protected] ).
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10698 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 1
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698
Large carnivores are fearsome predators that pose real and perceived threats to human life1 and livelihoods15, which is why humans have attempted, and largely succeeded, at
extirpating them everywhere57. The loss of large carnivores is now being recognized as possibly humankinds most pervasive inuence on nature8, in part because the fear (perceived predation risk911) they inspire in other animals may constitute a signicant ecosystem service critical to conserving biodiversity and ecosystem function6,1214. Being at the top of the food chain, large carnivores can play a dual role in structuring ecosystems by affecting both large herbivores and mesocarnivores, causing both tri-trophic cascades (large carnivoreherbivoreplant) and mesopredator cascades (large carnivoremesopredatormesopredators prey), affecting diverse species at multiple lower trophic levels6,8,1214. By both killing and frightening their prey, large carnivores could have a dual impact on these dual cascades, for the straightforward reason that frightened prey (in this case large herbivores and mesocarnivores) eat less9,11,12,15. The mere presence of large carnivores may therefore give rise to a landscape of fear9, buffering lower trophic levels from overconsumption by large herbivores and mesocarnivores. Failing to consider fear risks substantially underestimating the role large carnivores play, since fear may be as or more important than direct killing in causing trophic cascades, according to current theory and experiments primarily on captive invertebrates1619. Given the potential for human-large carnivore conict, there have been justiable calls for direct experimental evidence that the fear large carnivores inspire can provide a signicant ecosystem service4,20. The absence of such direct evidence to date is due to the challenge of experimentally manipulating fear in free-living wildlife, it being only very recently experimentally demonstrated that fear itself is powerful enough to affect wildlife population dynamics21.
To test whether the fear of large carnivores can itself cause cascading effects on mesocarnivore foraging and multiple lower trophic levels, we conducted a spatially and temporally replicated eld experiment in which we manipulated fear using month-long playbacks of large carnivore vocalizations (Fig. 1; Supplementary Table 1). The experiment was conducted on wild, free-living raccoons (Procyon lotor) on several small coastal Gulf Islands (BC, Canada). The raccoon is a mesocarnivore subject to much research regarding mesopredator release14,22. Most of the large carnivores known to hunt (wolf (Canis lupus), cougar (Puma concolor)) or harass (black bear (Ursus americanus)) raccoons were extirpated from the Gulf Islands last century22,23, the sole remaining large carnivore being the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris), which harasses and kills raccoons here, and has been present for millennia, having always been kept by local aboriginal peoples24. In a previous mensurative experiment comparing Gulf Islands with and without raccoons22, we documented that raccoons impact multiple marine species, reducing the abundance of intertidal crabs and sh, and even subtidal red rock crabs (Cancer productus).
To experimentally test whether the fear of large carnivores could itself mediate the impacts of raccoons on marine biota, we broadcast large carnivore predator (dog) or non-predator (local pinnipeds; harbour seal (Phoca vitulina), Steller sea lion (Eumetopias jubatus)) vocalizations over two large sections of shoreline for 1 month, and then reversed the treatments for a second month, using a repeated measures design to spatially replicate our results, which we further spatially and temporally replicated by repeating the same manipulation on a different island the following year. We assayed the immediate reaction of raccoons to the large carnivore predator and non-predator vocalizations by directly observing their reactions to 10 s playbacks, and assessed their response to the month-long
playbacks using multiple video surveillance and time-lapse cameras to continuously lm both experimental sections of shoreline over both entire month-long playback periods
To test the cascading effects of our experiment on lower trophic levels, we utilized the same methods (intertidal quadrats and subtidal crab trapping) used in our previous mensurative experiment to evaluate effects on raccoon prey22, and we conducted sh trapping and a mark-recapture experiment additionally quantifying effects on species not directly eaten by raccoons, that are instead a competitor (staghorn sculpin (Leptocottus armatus)) and prey (periwinkle snail (Littorina scutulata)) of the prey (red rock crab) of raccoons25,26. Staghorn sculpins are subtidal sh that, like red rock crabs, enter the intertidal at high tide to feed on small invertebrates25, but unlike red rock crabs, evidently successfully escape being eaten by raccoons. Periwinkle snails are too small to be food for raccoons but are eaten by red rock crabs, which use a distinctive method of dispatching them, permitting the level of mortality to be quantied using standard mark-recapture procedures26.
Here we report signicant cascading effects of the fear of large carnivores across multiple trophic levels in an intertidal food web, which effectively reversed the impacts of mesocarnivore populations on marine biota by markedly suppressing mesocarnivore foraging. These results indicate that the fear large carnivores inspire in their prey can account for a major component of their role in structuring ecosystems, reinforcing the value of large carnivore conservation in ensuring the continuation of this critical ecosystem service.
ResultsCascading effects of the fear of large carnivores. Fear of large carnivores dramatically reduced mesocarnivore foraging (Fig. 2; Supplementary Tables 2 and 3). The immediate reaction of raccoons to the 10 s predator playbacks was to either abandon foraging entirely by leaving the intertidal (Fig. 2a) or reduce foraging (Fig. 2b) and increase vigilance. Critically, these same responses persisted throughout the month-long playbacks. Large
Fear of large carnivore
Mesocarnivore foraging
Intertidal prey Subtidal prey
Crab prey
+
Crab competitor
Figure 1 | Fear of large carnivores caused a trophic cascade. Diagram illustrating how broadcasting playbacks of large carnivore vocalizations affected multiple lower trophic levels. Green and red arrows represent positive and negative effects, respectively, on foraging, abundance or survival. Solid arrows connect predator and prey; dashed arrows connect species affected, but not directly eaten, by another.
2 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10698 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698 ARTICLE
Immediate reaction Long-term response
a b
c d
92
36
32
64
Probability of remaining
in the intertidal (% of trials)
following 10 s playback
Time (s) spent foraging in 60 s
following 10 s playback
Time (s) spent in the intertidal
per occurrence on camera
during month-long playback
69
18
16
Proportion of time spent foraging
per occurrence on camera
during month-long playback
27
24
48
46
32
23
9
8
16
0
0
0
0
Non-predator PredatorPlayback
Non-predator PredatorPlayback
Non-predator PredatorPlayback
Non-predator PredatorPlayback
Figure 2 | Fear of large carnivores reduced mesocarnivore foraging. (a) Probability of remaining in the intertidal (% of trials; log-linear test, w21 11.96,
Po0.001; n 45 (predator) and 27 (non-predator)), and (b) time spent foraging (out of 60 s; ANOVA, F1,33 15.85, Po0.001; n 22 and 17) immediately
following 10 s predator and non-predator playbacks. (c) Time spent in the intertidal (per occurrence on camera; Linear Mixed Effects Model (LMM), Likelihood Ratio (LR) w21 9.66, P 0.002; n 51 and 43), and (d) proportion of time spent foraging (per occurrence on camera; LMM, LR w21 11.86,
P 0.001; n 62 and 52) during month-long predator and non-predator playbacks. Values are meanss.e.m.
a b c
d
168
40
104
12
Intertidal crabs m2
Intertidal polychaetes m2
9
84
Intertidal fish m2
20
52
Subtidal red rock crabs per trap
0
126
30
78
6
42
10
26
3
0
0
0
Non-predator Predator Non-predator Predator Non-predator Predator Non-predator Predator
Playback
Playback
Playback
Playback
Figure 3 | Fear of large carnivores beneted the mesocarnivores prey. Abundance of (a) intertidal crabs (Quasi-Poisson Generalized Linear Model (GLM), F1,36 12.11, P 0.001; n 20 (predator) and 20 (non-predator)), (b) intertidal sh (Poisson GLM, Likelihood Ratio w21 5.15, P 0.023; n 20
and 20), (c) intertidal polychaete worms (Quasi-Poisson GLM, F1,36 4.54, P 0.039; n 20 and 20) and (d) subtidal red rock crabs (Poisson Generalized
Linear Mixed Effects Model; Walds w21 10.83, P 0.001; n 20 and 20) following month-long predator and non-predator playbacks. Values are
meanss.e.m.
carnivore playbacks caused raccoons to spend less time in the intertidal (Fig. 2c), and less time feeding when they were present (Fig. 2d), with the cumulative consequence that they spent 66% less time foraging over the course of the month. This dramatic reduction in mesocarnivore foraging in turn dramatically benetted the mesocarnivores prey (Fig. 3; Supplementary Tables 4 and 5). Following the month-long large carnivore playbacks, there were 97% more intertidal crabs (Fig. 3a), 81% more intertidal sh (Fig. 3b), 59% more polychaete worms (Fig. 3c) and 61% more subtidal red rock crabs (Fig. 3d; Supplementary Fig. 1) compared with after the non-predator playbacks. Finally, fear of large carnivores clearly had cascading effects on multiple lower trophic levels (Fig. 1), as the reduction in raccoon foraging (Fig. 2c,d) that produced the increased relative abundance of red rock crabs (Fig. 3d) was associated with a decreased abundance of the red rock crabs competitor (staghorn sculpin; Fig. 4a) and led to lower survival of the red rock crabs prey (periwinkle snail; Fig. 4b; Supplementary Table 6).
DiscussionOur results unambiguously experimentally demonstrate that the fear of large carnivores can itself cause a trophic cascade. Manipulating fear itselfby hanging a speaker from a treecaused cascading effects on predation and competition at multiple trophic levels in the ocean (Fig. 1). Moreover, there were
likely many more effects than we measured, potentially cascading down to the level of primary producers, since red rock crabs are themselves signicant predators26 and periwinkle snails are signicant grazers27 (Supplementary Discussion).
Our results additionally demonstrate that failing to consider the fear large carnivores inspire risks signicantly underestimating their role, given the striking magnitude of the effects (Figs 3 and 4), and that these effects were comparable in magnitude to those documented in our previous mensurative experiment comparing islands with and without raccoons. The presence of raccoons on an island reduces the respective abundance of intertidal crabs and sh, and subtidal red rock crabs, by 90%, 94% and 38% (ref. 22), impacts all reversed by the 97%, 81% and 61% relative increase in each (Fig. 3) resulting from the reduction in raccoon foraging caused by the fear of large carnivores (Fig. 2). That the fear of large carnivores could itself be powerful enough to reverse these impacts corresponds with the fact that the human extirpation of most large carnivores from the Gulf Islands means raccoons here no longer have much to fear, and act accordingly, as they forage night and day (being more normally nocturnal) far from cover (deep into the intertidal)22, and rarely look up from eating, spending o1 s vigilant per minute (Methods). Our experiment reversed this now unrestrained foraging by restoring the fear of large carnivores to a system from which it has largely been lost, revealing the signicance of the ecosystem service the presence of the now extirpated large carnivores
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10698 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 3
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698
a b
4
100
Change in staghorn sculpins
per trap over 1 month
Periwinkle snail survival (%)
per tide cycle
2
90
0
80
2
70
4
60
Non-predator PredatorPlayback
Non-predator PredatorPlayback
Figure 4 | Fear of large carnivores affected a competitor and prey of the mesocarnivores prey. (a) Change in abundance of staghorn sculpins over one month (Linear Mixed Effects Model, Likelihood Ratio w21 21.17, Po0.001; n 20 (predator) and 20 (non-predator)) and (b) survival of
periwinkle snails per tide cycle (Binomial Generalized Linear Mixed Effects Model, Walds w21 9.51, P 0.002; n 10 and 8) during month-long
predator and non-predator playbacks. Values are meanss.e.m.
(wolves, cougars and black bears) provided, solely through the fear they inspired.
Similar restorative effects of the fear of large carnivores have been attributed to the reestablishment of a landscape of fear accompanying the reintroduction or recolonization of large carnivores6,9,13,28,29; and broad-scale impacts accompanying the loss of large carnivores that cannot be explained by the reduction in direct killing alone have likewise been attributed to the associated loss of the fear of large carnivores6,1214,30. The evidence to date that the fear of large carnivores can play a central role in structuring ecosystems comes largely from natural experiments6,13,20, and compelling alternative explanations often exist for the patterns observed20. Our results in no way refute these alternatives, but our being able to cause a trophic cascade by directly manipulating fear does conclusively demonstrate such a thing is possible, and so corroborates that the fear of large carnivores can play the role attributed to it.
Experimentally manipulating the fear of large carnivores demonstrably affected mesocarnivore behaviour (Fig. 2), which in turn evidently caused a fear cascade affecting the behaviour of at least some of the mesocarnivores prey. Red rock crab abundance increased signicantly over the course of the month-long large carnivore playbacks (Supplementary Fig. 1; Supplementary Discussion), which could only be due to a behavioural change in habitat use, as reproduction in this species requires at least a year31. Red rock crabs occur in large subtidal populations and move into the intertidal to forage, where they are killed and eaten by raccoons, which leave the remains, and thus chemical cues, of dead crabs in the water22. Surviving red rock crabs may modify their habitat use in response to these chemical cues, as has been demonstrated experimentally in other crab species32. The fear cascade evident from the increased abundance of red rock crabs may therefore have resulted from straightforward mechanisms: increasing the fearfulness of raccoons reduced their foraging (Fig. 2), which presumably led to reduced fearfulness in red rock crabs by decreasing chemical cues in the water, in turn leading to the red rock crabs increased use of the intertidal (Supplementary Fig. 1).
The loss of large carnivores from habitats across the globe has been linked to far-reaching ecosystem-level consequences including biodiversity loss and changes in habitat structurecaused by outbreaks of large herbivores and mesocarnivores6,8,9,12,14,22,2830. Our results suggest that
restoration of these ecosystems will require more than just addressing the overabundance of these middle trophic level species, for example, through hunting or removal programs. Such numerical suppression may only affect a subset of hyper-abundant large herbivore and mesocarnivore populations, while the remaining individuals are free to engage in unrestricted foraging. Effective ecological restoration may depend on reestablishing the fear of large carnivores in these ecosystems, which has the potential to affect entire populations of their prey (rather than just those individuals subject to direct killing or removal)17,33, suppressing prey foraging behaviour and thereby mitigating the impacts of overconsumption on lower trophic level species. The potential ecosystem-level benet of the mere presence of large carnivores, and the landscape of fear they produce, should therefore be a central consideration in making informed management decisions regarding large carnivore populations.
Our experimental results support the contention that, when it comes to conserving biodiversity and maintaining healthy ecosystems, fear has its uses6,1214. By inspiring fear, the very existence of large carnivores on the landscape, in and of itself, can provide a critical ecosystem service human actions cannot fully replace, making it essential to maintain or restore large carnivores for conservation purposes on this basis alone6,12,14,30. Ensuring the continuation of this critical ecosystem service the fear of large carnivores provides requires attenuating our own fear of them, which can be accomplished by promoting tolerance and coexistence with large carnivores as an accompaniment to other programs to reduce human-large carnivore conicts16,3435.
Methods
Study area. Vegetation in the Gulf Islands falls within the Coastal Douglas Fir (Psuedostuga menziesii) biogeoclimatic zone, and the region experiences a mild Mediterranean climate36. This work was conducted on four Gulf Islands. Coal Island (140 ha; 48 410 0300 N, 123 220 3200 W) is a single-owner private island consisting of B78% forest. Portland Island (225 ha; 48 430 3300 N, 123 220 2000 W) and Wallace Island (87 ha; 48 560 3400 N, 123 330 0400 W) are both fully forested parkland, being entirely within the Gulf Islands National Park Reserve (Portland), or mostly BC Provincial Park (Wallace, 83% of land area). Penelakut Island(954 ha; 48 570 3000 N, 123 380 3400 W) is the traditional territory of the Penelakut
First Nation and home to B350 people, all residing in a small village on the north end of the island. The majority of Penelakut Island (B86%) is forested. Domestic dogs were present on all Gulf Islands on which this study was conducted, either as the pets of permanent residents (Coal and Penelakut Islands) or accompanying
4 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10698 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698 ARTICLE
park visitors (Portland and Wallace Islands), though study sites were chosen well away from areas of high human and dog use to minimize interference.
Motivation and objectives. We experimentally manipulated the fear of large carnivores over 2 years and at multiple sites in the Gulf Islands, achieving both temporal and spatial replication of our results. In 2013, we tested the immediate reaction of raccoons to 10 s playbacks of large carnivore vocalizations on Coal, Portland and Wallace Islands. We then used month-long playbacks of large carnivore vocalizations to test for long-term behavioural responses by raccoons and cascading effects on marine biota, on Coal Island in 2013 and on Penelakut Island in 2014. The objectives in these 2 years were similar but complementary. In 2013, we focused on testing whether the fear of large carnivores was sufcient to mitigate the impacts of raccoons on intertidal and shallow subtidal prey. In 2014, we sought(1) to replicate the results from the Coal Island experiment concerning raccoon prey abundance, while expanding the focus of the study, (2) to test whetherthe effects of fear on raccoon behaviour observed in 10 s playback experiments persisted throughout our month-long treatments and (3) to test for further cascading effects of fear among intertidal and shallow subtidal species not directly eaten by raccoons. All work was conducted in compliance with the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care, and was approved by the Animal Care and Use Committees of the University of Victoria and the University of Western Ontario.
Preparing the playbacks. We manipulated the fear of large carnivores using playbacks of domestic dog (large carnivore predator) and local pinniped (harbour seal and Steller sea lion; non-predator) vocalizations. Pinniped vocalizations provide an excellent control for dogs; the two call types are qualitatively similar, and Gulf Islands raccoons are certain to be as familiar with pinniped as with dog vocalizations. Most importantly, pinnipeds represent no threat to raccoons, and analyses of red rock crab abundance data veried that there was no difference between pinniped playbacks and silence with respect to raccoon impacts on marine prey (Supplementary Discussion). Sound les were acquired from online audio and video databases, and library archives. In testing the immediate reaction of raccoons to large carnivore vocalizations, we used multiple 10 s exemplars of predator and non-predator vocalizations (10 dog and 5 pinniped), and matched the temporal properties (duration, attack and number of staccato elements) of these two groups of playbacks by visually inspecting the spectrograms and waveforms of all exemplars37. We ensured that there were no differences in overall frequency characteristics between the two groups using t-tests to compare each of four frequency characteristics (peak: t1,13 0.36, P 0.735; minimum: t1,13 1.46,
P 0.180; maximum: t1,13 0.63, P 0.551; range: t1,13 0.62, P 0.556; n 10
(predator) and 5 (non-predator) for all tests). We broadcast 10 s calls at a mean (s.d.) volume of 78.0 (2.1) dB at 1 m, with no difference in volume between predator and non-predator treatments (t1,13 1.3, P 0.234; n 10 and 5). All
playbacks were broadcast using identical speakers (Nexxtech Mini Cube 2.0) and mp3 players (Coby Electronics MP301).
To test the long-term response of both raccoons and the nearshore marine community to the fear of large carnivores, we again used playbacks of dog (predator) and pinniped (non-predator) vocalizations. We composed playlists using multiple exemplars of both call types (n 11 predator and 9 non-predator
exemplars) ranging in duration from 8 to 79 s, with no difference in duration between the two treatments (predator (means.d.): 34.120.6 s; non-predator:26.818.3 s; t1,18 0.84, P 0.412). These two sets of playbacks were again
matched for temporal properties using visual inspection of spectrograms and waveforms, and we used t-tests to conrm that there were no differences in overall frequency characteristics between predator and non-predator playlists (peak frequency: t1,18 0.03, P 0.973; minimum: t1,18 1.44, P 0.180;
maximum: t1,18 0.76, P 0.459; range: t1,18 0.86, P 0.400; n 11 (predator)
and 9 (non-predator) for all tests). All calls were broadcast at a mean (s.d.) volume of 86.1 (2.9) dB at 1 m, with no difference in volume between predator and non-predator treatments (t1,18 0.13, P 0.895; n 11 and 9). All
playbacks were broadcast using identical speakers (Nexxtech Mini Cube 2.0) and mp3 players (The Source HeadRush 2GB mp3 player).
Raccoon immediate reaction to large carnivore vocalizations. All 10 s playback trials were conducted by two researchers (J.P.S. and D.R.) between 15 May and 16 September 2013. We located diurnally active raccoons foraging in the intertidal and broadcast a randomly selected predator or non-predator playback from a concealed location. The raccoons behaviour was video recorded immediately before and immediately following the 10 s playback using a handheld digital video camera with a 70 optical zoom (Sony DCR-SX45 Handycam). Immediately following each
trial, the distance between the speaker and the focal animals location at the time of the playback was measured using a rangender (Bushnell Sport 450). Calls were broadcast at an average (s.d.) distance of 35 (16) m, and the distance between the speaker and the focal animal did not differ between predator and non-predator treatments (one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA); F1,70 0.96, P 0.33; n 45
(predator) and 27 (non-predator)). Habitat variables that could potentially impact the raccoons ability to hear the playbackincluding wind speed, rainfall and wave actionwere measured for each trial, and showed no difference between treatments (Wilcoxon rank-sum test; 0.184P40.87 for all variables; n 45 and 27 for
all tests). As the reaction of conspecics to our playback treatments could conceivably have inuenced the focal animals behaviour, we also quantied the number of conspecics within 50 m of the focal animal at the time of the playback. In general, raccoons were 450 m from any conspecic during playback trials (median (range) conspecics within 50 m 0 (0, 5)), and there was no difference
in the number of conspecics present between the two treatments (Wilcoxon rank-sum test, W 687, P 0.27, n 45 and 27). To minimize the likelihood of
repeated sampling of raccoons, playback locations on a given island were separated by a median linear distance of 266 m, comparable to the spacing of sampling points (273 m) commonly utilized in raccoon live-trapping studies38. The minimum distance between trials of the same treatment did not differ between predator and non-predator treatments (ANOVA; P 0.17; n 45 (predator) and 27
(non-predator)). Each playback type was only played once at a given location.
A single researcher (D.R.), who was blind to the playback treatment, scored all video recordings of 10 s playback trials, and estimated three behavioural measures. First, we scored whether or not the focal raccoon ed the intertidal (that is, moved from the exposed shoreline into the surrounding forest) within the 3 min immediately following the playback. For those individuals that did not ee,we calculated the amount of time (s) devoted to foraging and to vigilance (see Supplementary Fig. 2 for behaviour scoring methodology) in the 60 s immediately before and immediately following the playback. Gulf Islands raccoons spent o1 s of every minute vigilant in the 60 s before the playback (0.90.3 s (means.e.m.), range 014), and instead spent the great majority of their time foraging. We
calculated the change in foraging as the time spent foraging following the playback minus time spent foraging before the playback. An identical calculation was made for the change in vigilance. We report that raccoons signicantly decreased foraging in response to the predator playback, relative to the non-predator playback. We found a correspondingly strong increase in vigilance in the 60 s following the predator playback (vigilance increased by 27.11.7 (s.e.m.) s, relative to pre-playback, n 22 raccoons), as compared with the non-predator playback
(vigilance increased by 9.71.7 s, n 17; Supplementary Table 2).
Raccoon long-term response to the fear of large carnivores. The month-long playback manipulations were conducted on Coal Island in 2013 (10 May to 13 July) and Penelakut Island in 2014 (28 June to 25 August). On each island, we chose two shoreline sites separated by sufcient distance (1.2 km on Coal and 2.7 km on Penelakut) that sounds were not detectable between sites. We used a repeated measures design, presenting both predator and non-predator treatments at all sites and alternating the order of treatment presentation between sites on each island, allowing us to control for the effects of site and seasonality on raccoon behaviour and intertidal community data. On a given island, each site received either the predator or non-predator treatment for 28 days (treatment period 1), followed by the opposite treatment for a subsequent 28 days (treatment period 2), witho1 week separating the two treatment periods. Starting treatment (predator or non-predator) was randomly assigned. Throughout each treatment period, we monitored raccoon behaviour (Penelakut), the abundance of intertidal prey (Coal) and subtidal prey (Coal and Penelakut), and the survival and abundance of marine species not directly eaten by raccoons (Penelakut).
At all sites, ve identical sets of speakers and mp3 players housed in weatherproof boxes were deployed at regular intervals along sections of shoreline, attached to trees just above the high water line. Speakers played multiple exemplars of large carnivore predator (dog) or non-predator control (pinniped) vocalizations at regular intervals throughout each 28-day treatment period. On Coal Island in 2013, the speaker systems were deployed at 25-m intervals across 100 m of shoreline, and broadcast either predator or non-predator playbacks 24 h per day (raccoons on Coal were active both day and night; J.P.S., pers. obs.). Each speaker played a randomized playlist of calls interspersed by periods of silence, broadcasting calls 40% of the time and remaining silent for 60% of the time.
To reduce the likelihood of raccoon habituation to the playbacks, speakers were intermittently turned off completely for 14 days at a time during each 28-day treatment period21 such that speakers were active for a total of 19 days during treatment period 1 and 18 days during treatment period 2.
On Penelakut Island in 2014, the ve speaker systems were deployed at 50-m intervals across 200 m of shoreline and broadcast calls only at night (1900 to 0900 hours), as this is the period when raccoons on Penelakut are most active (J.P.S., personal observation). Speakers remained on for the full 28 days of each treatment period and played randomized playlists of either dog or pinniped calls 20% of the time, remaining silent for 80% of the time. In addition, two motion-sensitive speaker systems were deployed at each site, one at each end of the 200-m treatment area. These custom-built speaker systems used identical components to those described above, but were modied to incorporate a motion sensor, which activated a 10 s playback (using the same sets of calls described above) when triggered by a raccoon passing within B5 m of the sensor. These speaker systems were always active, but remained silent unless triggered. There is no evidence that the minor methodological differences in fear manipulation between the 2 years of the experiment had any effect on our results. Indeed, the effect size on subtidal red rock crab abundance was identical in both years (Supplementary Discussion).
We measured the long-term effects of the fear of large carnivores on several aspects of raccoon foraging on Penelakut Island in 2014, using a network of cameras deployed at each treatment site. Two colour/infrared video surveillance cameras (Speco Technologies HT7915DNV Bullet Cameras), recording to custom-
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10698 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 5
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698
built digital video recording systems21, were deployed within each treatment site and spaced 100 m apart. We estimated the duration of time raccoons spent in the intertidal for all independent raccoon occurrences on camera as the time (s) from an individual raccoon rst entering a cameras eld of view to when it exited the eld of view. Two time-lapse cameras (Moultrie Game Spy M-990i trail cameras set to time-lapse mode) spaced 100 m apart were deployed within each treatment site and programmed to record one photograph every 30 s. For each time-lapse image in which a raccoons entire body was visible, we scored the raccoons behaviour as either foraging or vigilant (Supplementary Fig. 2). For each individual raccoon, we determined the total number of photos in which it appeared, and calculated the proportion of those photos scored as foraging. A single individual (J.P.S.) scored all images of raccoons taken from time-lapse cameras. To determine whether these scores were repeatable, ve independent observers, blind to the fear manipulation treatment, re-scored a total of 184 time-lapse photos (21% of all photos). Agreement between the original scores (used in the analyses presented here) and those made by the ve observers was 80%.
For both camera types (video and time lapse), we only used data from raccoons recorded between 1900 and 0900 hours, when playback systems on Penelakut Island were active. Unless multiple raccoons occurring on a single camera could be classied with certainty as unique individuals, raccoon occurrences on camera were only treated as independent if they were separated by 430 min (refs 39,40). All raccoon behaviour variables were calculated per camera per night of the treatment period40,41; camera night is therefore the unit of replication in all behavioural analyses (see below). Camera placement remained constant throughout the duration of the experiment, and we therefore analysed all behavioural data using mixed effects models with Camera ID included as a random effect to account for variation due to camera placement.
Measuring cascading effects of fear. In 2013, we used standard quadrat sampling methods to test whether our month-long playback manipulations affected the abundance of raccoon intertidal prey. On the basis of previous work22 and direct observations of raccoon foraging, our a priori prediction was that our fear manipulations would affect the abundances of small (o5 cm carapace width)
intertidal crabs (shore crabs (Hemigrapsus oregonensis, H. nudus), black-clawed crabs (Lophopanopeus bellus), porcelain crabs (Petrolisthes spp.) and juvenile northern kelp crabs (Pugettia producta)), intertidal sh (pricklebacks (family Stichaeidae) and northern clingsh (Gobiesox maeandricus)), and polychaete worms (families Terebellidae, Orbiniidae, Nereidae and Glyceridae). Following methods described in Suraci et al.22, we quantied species abundance in ten0.25 0.25-m quadrats at each treatment site on Coal Island at the end of each
28-day treatment period.
To test whether our playback manipulations affected red rock crab abundance, we trapped crabs across 200-m sections of shoreline centred at each treatment site by setting ve collapsible mesh crab traps per site just below the low intertidal zone, spaced 50 m apart and left in place for 24 h (ref. 22). On Coal Island in 2013, crab traps were set at the end of each month-long treatment period. Sampling effort was intensied on Penelakut Island in 2014 such that crab trapping was conducted once before the application of any playback treatments (on 11 June 2014) to establish a pretreatment baseline crab abundance, and then at the end of each week during both month-long treatment periods (that is, four times per treatment period). In both years, trap locations remained constant across all trapping sessions at a given site.
The effects of the fear of large carniovres may extend beyond those species directly subject to raccoon predation to affect the competitors and prey of the raccoons prey. Red rock crabs are major intertidal predators, and may compete for resources with other intertidal predators of similar body size, including staghorn sculpins, which are not subject to raccoon predation (Supplementary Discussion). We tested whether our fear manipulation treatments affected staghorn sculpin abundance by setting conical sh traps (minnow traps) across both treatment sites on Penelakut Island in 2014. Five traps, spaced 50 m apart and baited with B100 g of frozen herring, were set in the mid intertidal zone at each site and left in place for 24 h. Traps were deployed three times per 28-day treatment period, once immediately before the start of each treatment period and again at the mid-point and end of each treatment period. The same trap locations were used for all sh trap deployments at both sites.
Red rock crabs are known to affect the abundance of several species of gastropod prey, including periwinkle snails (Supplementary Discussion). We hypothesized that, during predator treatments, reduced raccoon predation on red rock crabs would result in increased red rock crab predation on periwinkle snails relative to non-predator treatments. To test this, we performed four replicate short-term snail mark-recapture experiments nested within our fear manipulation experiment on Penelakut Island in 2014, comparing the proportion of marked snails killed by red rock crabs during predator and non-predator treatments. Periwinkle snails were collected from high intertidal beds of Fucus algae at each site and transported back to the laboratory where they were marked with a small dab of acrylic paint on the apex of the shell, and held overnight in seawater tanks. Following Rochette and Dill42, snails were released the next day at each of four release points (spaced 30 to 50 m apart) within the 200-m treatment area at each site. All release points were located at the same tide level (1.0 m above mean lower low water) on areas of at rock away from large boulders or crevices42. Twenty snails were released within a 5-cm radius of each release point during afternoon rising tides when release points were submerged under at least 1.5 m of water. The
following morning at low tide, immediately following exposure of the release points, two researchers searched a 4-m radius around each point, recovering marked live snails and the marked apices of crushed snail shells. Red rock crab predation on snails produces a characteristic shell crushing pattern26, allowing one to reliably diagnose snail mortality due to red rock crab predation, and counting only shell apices rather than all crushed shell fragments ensures that each crushed snail is only counted once42. We estimated the proportion of marked snails surviving red rock crab predation over one tide cycle as the number of live snails recovered divided by the total number of live snails and crushed apices recovered at each release point (the fate of snails not recovered could not be reliably ascribed to red rock crab predation). To minimize potential bias due to low recovery rates, we only used data from trials for which at least 50% of the 20 released snails were recovered, alive or dead (n 18 trials). For these trials, the average recovery rate
was 71% (range 5095%), and did not differ between predator and non-predator
treatments (t1,16 0.04, P 0.97). This snail mark-recapture study was
replicated four times on Penelakut Island in 2014, at the mid-point and end of each month-long treatment period, using the same four release points at each site throughout both treatment periods.
Statistical analyses. All model assumptions were checked using statistical tests for normality and homogeneity of variance, and the t of all models was visually inspected using residual versus tted value plots and quantilequantile plots43. Where appropriate, means and s.e.s were calculated on normalized data, and back-transformed to the original scale of the data for presentation in gures.
All (Generalized) Linear Mixed Effects Models (LMM) were t using the lme4 package in R44.
We used a log-linear analysis to test whether playback treatment affected the proportion of trials in which the focal raccoon ed the intertidal following 10 s playbacks, using a model that included terms for treatment, island and a treatment island interaction. Data on both the change in foraging and the change
in vigilance exhibited by raccoons that did not ee the intertidal following 10 s playbacks were BoxCox transformed and analysed using separate two-way ANOVA models including the main effects of treatment and island, and a treatment island interaction (Supplementary Table 2).
We estimated the duration of time spent in the intertidal during month-long playback treatments for all independent raccoon occurrences on video surveillance cameras, and then used the median duration per camera night in our analysis. Median duration data were natural log-transformed and analysed using a LMM (Supplementary Table 3). We estimated the proportion of time that raccoons spent foraging when present in the intertidal from time-lapse camera data, as described above, and then calculated the average of these proportion foraging values for all individuals on a given camera night, weighted by the total number of photos of each individual. Raccoon occurrences on camera that produced fewer photos were thereby devalued relative to occurrences with many photos and thus more information. Nightly weighted mean proportions of time spent foraging were then analysed using LMM (Supplementary Table 3). The signicance of model terms was tested using Likelihood Ratio Tests43. In both behavioural analyses, we tested for main effects of treatment and study site as well as a treatment site interaction.
We also tested for a main effect of time since the start of the treatment period (night, measured in days: 128) and an interaction between treatment and night. This allowed us to determine whether raccoon behavioural responses to the treatments changed over the course of the treatment period (for example,due to habituation) and whether any such changes differed between predator and non-predator treatments. Finally, we tested for a three-way treatment site night
interaction. We found no evidence for raccoon habituation to predator playbacks across the month-long treatment periods in either behavioural measure; the effect of night and the treatment night interaction were nonsignicant in both analyses
(Supplementary Table 3).
Quadrat data on the abundances of intertidal prey were analysed using Generalized Linear Models with a Poisson distribution. All models were checked for overdispersion43, and those showing evidence of overdispersion (intertidal crabs and polychaete worms) were ret using the Quasi-Poisson distribution. Treatment, site and their interaction were included as xed effects in all Generalized Linear Models (Supplementary Table 4). Following Zuur et al.43, the signicance of all main effects and interactions was tested using Likelihood Ratio Tests for models t with the Poisson distribution (intertidal sh (Fig. 3b)), and F-tests for models t with the Quasi-Poisson distribution (intertidal crabs (Fig. 3a) and polychaete worms (Fig. 3c)).
We analysed the effect of the fear manipulation treatments on shallow subtidal red rock crab abundance across the 2 years of the experiment, using as our response variable the number of red rock crabs caught per trap at the end of treatment periods 1 and 2 in both 2013 and 2014. These data were analysed using a Generalized Linear Mixed Effects Model (GLMM) with a Poisson distribution and checked for overdispersion (ratio of null to residual deviance 1.17). We tested for
main effects of treatment and year, and their interaction. Within a given year, sites and trap locations remained constant across treatment periods, so site and trap location were included in the analysis as nested random effects (trap location nested within site). The signicance of main effects and interactions in the GLMM was tested using type II Walds w2-test45 (Supplementary Table 5).
We calculated the change in staghorn sculpin abundance across each treatment period by subtracting the number of sculpins caught in each trap at the mid-point
6 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10698 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698 ARTICLE
and end of each treatment period from the number caught in the same trap immediately before that treatment period. These data were analysed using LMM, including trap location (constant across sampling events) as a random effect. We tested for the main effects of treatment and site, and a treatment site interaction
(Supplementary Table 6).
The effect of our playback treatments on the proportion of marked periwinkle snails escaping crab predation was determined by scoring all surviving snails as 1 and all crushed snails as 0 in each trial of the mark-recapture experiment, and analysing these data using GLMM with a binomial error distribution. There was no evidence for overdispersion in these data (ratio of null to residual deviance 1.10).
Snail release point (constant across all trials at a given site) was included as a random effect. We again tested for the main effects of treatment and site, and a treatment site interaction (Supplementary Table 6).
References
1. Packer, C., Ikanda, D., Kissui, B. & Kushnir, H. Conservation biology: lion attacks on humans in Tanzania. Nature 436, 927 (2005).
2. Dickman, A. J., Macdonald, E. A. & Macdonald, D. W. A review of nancial instruments to pay for predator conservation and encourage human-carnivore coexistence. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 13937 (2011).
3. Treves, A. & Bruskotter, J. Tolerance for predatory wildlife. Science 344, 476 (2014).
4. Woodroffe, R. & Redpath, S. M. When the hunter becomes the hunted. Science 348, 1312 (2015).
5. Chapron, G. et al. Recovery of large carnivores in Europes modern human-dominated landscapes. Science 346, 1517 (2014).
6. Ripple, W. J. et al. Status and ecological effects of the worlds largest carnivores. Science 343, 151 (2014).
7. Packer, C., Swanson, A., Ikanda, D. & Kushnir, H. Fear of darkness, the full moon and the nocturnal ecology of African lions. PLoS ONE 6, e22285 (2011).
8. Estes, J. A. et al. Trophic downgrading of planet earth. Science 333, 301 (2011).9. Laundr, J. W., Hernndez, L. & Altendorf, K. B. Wolves, elk, and bison: reestablishing the landscape of fear in Yellowstone National Park, U.S.A. Can.J. Zool. 79, 1401 (2001).10. Brown, J. S., Laundr, J. W. & Gurung, M. The ecology of fear: optimal foraging, game theory, and trophic interactions. J. Mammal. 80, 385 (1999).
11. Brown, J. S. & Kotler, B. P. Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Ecol. Lett. 7, 999 (2004).
12. Terborgh, J. & Estes, J. A. Trophic Cascades: Predators, Prey, and the Changing Dynamics of Nature (Island Press, 2010).
13. Creel, S. & Christianson, D. Relationships between direct predation and risk effects. Trends Ecol. Evol. 23, 194 (2008).
14. Prugh, L. R. et al. The rise of the mesopredator. BioScience 59, 779 (2009).15. Lima, S. L. & Dill, L. M. Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can. J. Zool. 68, 619 (1990).
16. Schmitz, O. J., Beckerman, A. P. & OBrien, K. M. Behaviorally mediated trophic cascades: effects of predation risk on food web interactions. Ecology 78, 1388 (1997).
17. Schmitz, O. J., Krivan, V. & Ovadia, O. Trophic cascades: the primacy of trait-mediated indirect interactions. Ecol. Lett. 7, 153 (2004).
18. Preisser, E. L., Bolnick, D. I. & Benard, M. F. Scared to death? The effects of intimidation and consumption in predator-prey interactions. Ecology 86, 501 (2005).
19. Railsback, S. F. & Harvey, B. C. Trait-mediated trophic interactions: is foraging theory keeping up? Trends Ecol. Evol. 28, 119 (2013).
20. Dobson, A. P. Yellowstone wolves and the forces that structure natural systems. PLoS Biol. 12, e1002025 (2014).
21. Zanette, L. Y., White, A. F., Allen, M. C. & Clinchy, M. Perceived predation risk reduces the number of offspring songbirds produce per year. Science 334, 1398 (2011).
22. Suraci, J. P., Clinchy, M., Zanette, L. Y., Currie, C. M. A. & Dill, L. M. Mammalian mesopredators on islands directly impact both terrestrial and marine communities. Oecologia 176, 1087 (2014).
23. Golumbia, T. A history of species introductions in Gwaii Haanas and Gulf Islands National Park Reserves in British Columbia, Canada: Implications for management. Trans. W. Sect. Wildl. Soc. 42, 20 (2006).
24. Hanson, D. K. Subsistence during the late prehistoric occupation of Pender Canal, British Columbia. Can. J. Archaeol. 19, 29 (1995).
25. Tasto, R. N. in The Marine Resources of Anaheim Bay (eds Lane, E. D. & Hill, C. W.) (Fisheries Bulletin, 1975).
26. Behrens Yamada, S. & Boulding, E. G. The role of highly mobile crab predators in the intertidal zonation of their gastropod prey. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 204, 59 (1996).
27. Mrowicki, R. J. & OConnor, N. E. Wave action modies the effects of consumer diversity and warming on algal assemblages. Ecology 96, 1020 (2015).
28. Fortin, D. et al. Wolves inuence elk movements: Behavior shapes a trophic cascade in Yellowstone National Park. Ecology 86, 1320 (2005).
29. Berger, K. M., Gese, E. M. & Berger, J. Indirect effects and traditional trophic cascades: a test involving wolves, coyotes, and pronghorn. Ecology 89, 818 (2008).
30. Ritchie, E. G. et al. Ecosystem restoration with teeth: what role for predators? Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 265 (2012).
31. Orensanz, J. M. & Gallucci, V. F. Comparative study of postlarval life-history schedules in four sympatric species of Cancer (Decapoda: Brachyura: Cancridae). J. Crustacean Biol. 8, 187220 (1988).
32. Hill, J. M. & Weissburg, M. J. Crabs interpret the threat of predator body size and biomass via cue concentration and diet. Anim. Behav. 92, 117123 (2014).
33. Werner, E. E. & Peacor, S. D. A review of trait-mediated indirect interactions in ecological communities. Ecology 84, 10831100 (2003).
34. Packer, C. et al. Conserving large carnivores: dollars and fence. Ecol. Lett. 16, 635 (2013).
35. Woodroffe, R., Hedges, S. & Durant, S. M. To fence or not to fence. Science 344,
46 (2014).
36. Jewell, K. J., Arcese, P. & Gergel, S. E. Robust predictions of species distribution: Spatial habitat models for a brood parasite. Biol. Conserv. 140, 259 (2007).
37. Blumstein, D. T., Cooley, L., Winternitz, J. & Daniel, J. C. Do yellow-bellied marmots respond to predator vocalizations? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62, 457 (2008).
38. Gehrt, S. D. & Prange, S. Interference competition between coyotes and raccoons: a test of the mesopredator release hypothesis. Behav. Ecol. 18, 204 (2007).
39. OBrien, T. G. in Camera Traps in Animal Ecology (eds OConnell, A. F., Nichols, J. D. & Karanth, K. U.) 7196 (Springer, 2011).
40. Brook, L. A., Johnson, C. N. & Ritchie, E. G. Effects of predator control on behaviour of an apex predator and indirect consequences for mesopredator suppression. J. Appl. Ecol. 49, 1278 (2012).
41. Bowkett, A. E., Rovero, F. & Marshall, A. R. The use of camera-trap data to model habitat use by antelope species in the Udzungwa Mountain forests, Tanzania. Afr. J. Ecol. 46, 479 (2008).
42. Rochette, R. & Dill, L. M. Mortality, behavior and the effects of predators on the intertidal distribution of littorinid gastropods. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 253, 165 (2000).
43. Zuur, A. R., Ieno, E. N., Walker, N. J., Saveliev, A. A. & Smith, G. M. Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R 123135 (Springer, 2009).
44. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2014) Available at http://www.R-project.org/
Web End =http://www. http://www.R-project.org/
Web End =R-project.org/ .
45. Bolker, B. M. et al. Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24, 127 (2009).
Acknowledgements
We thank J. Sibbald and N. Gray for assistance; Parks Canada, B.C. Parks, Betty Shields and the Penelakut First Nation for access; and D. Blumstein, J. Estes, C. Krebs,J. Laundr, C. Packer, E. Preisser and E. Ritchie for help improving the manuscript.G. Holoboff provided original illustrations. This research was funded by the Animal Behaviour Society, the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada and the Canadian Foundation for Innovation.
Author contributions
All authors originally conceived of the study, and J.P.S. and D.R. conducted the eldwork, with the assistance of M.C. and L.Y.Z. J.P.S. and M.C. performed the data analysis and drafted the manuscript. All authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript.
Additional information
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =http://www.nature.com/ http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =naturecommunications
Competing nancial interests: The authors declare no competing nancial interests.
Reprints and permission information is available online at http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =http://npg.nature.com/ http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =reprintsandpermissions/
How to cite this article: Suraci, J. P. et al. Fear of large carnivores causes a trophic cascade. Nat. Commun. 7:10698 doi: 10.1038/ncomms10698 (2016).
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articles Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if the material is not included under the Creative Commons license, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to reproduce the material. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Web End =http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:10698 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10698 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 7
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Nature Publishing Group Feb 2016
Abstract
The fear large carnivores inspire, independent of their direct killing of prey, may itself cause cascading effects down food webs potentially critical for conserving ecosystem function, particularly by affecting large herbivores and mesocarnivores. However, the evidence of this has been repeatedly challenged because it remains experimentally untested. Here we show that experimentally manipulating fear itself in free-living mesocarnivore (raccoon) populations using month-long playbacks of large carnivore vocalizations caused just such cascading effects, reducing mesocarnivore foraging to the benefit of the mesocarnivore's prey, which in turn affected a competitor and prey of the mesocarnivore's prey. We further report that by experimentally restoring the fear of large carnivores in our study system, where most large carnivores have been extirpated, we succeeded in reversing this mesocarnivore's impacts. We suggest that our results reinforce the need to conserve large carnivores given the significant "ecosystem service" the fear of them provides.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer