ARTICLE
Received 15 Oct 2015 | Accepted 29 Mar 2016 | Published 10 May 2016
Xiaoming Wang1,*, Alexandra L. Bey2,*, Brittany M. Katz3, Alexandra Badea4, Namsoo Kim5, Lisa K. David5, Lara J. Duffney1,2, Sunil Kumar3, Stephen D. Mague5, Samuel W. Hulbert2, Nisha Dutta6, Volodya Hayrapetyan5, Chunxiu Yu5, Erin Gaidis5, Shengli Zhao2, Jin-Dong Ding7, Qiong Xu1,8, Leeyup Chung1, Ramona M. Rodriguiz3, Fan Wang2, Richard J. Weinberg9, William C. Wetsel2,3,6,10, Kafui Dzirasa3,10, Henry Yin5,10& Yong-hui Jiang1,2,10,11
Human neuroimaging studies suggest that aberrant neural connectivity underlies behavioural decits in autism spectrum disorders (ASDs), but the molecular and neural circuit mechanisms underlying ASDs remain elusive. Here, we describe a complete knockout mouse model of the autism-associated Shank3 gene, with a deletion of exons 422 (De422). Both mGluR5-Homer scaffolds and mGluR5-mediated signalling are selectively altered in striatal neurons. These changes are associated with perturbed function at striatal synapses, abnormal brain morphology, aberrant structural connectivity and ASD-like behaviour. In vivo recording reveals that the cortico-striatal-thalamic circuit is tonically hyperactive in mutants, but becomes hypoactive during social behaviour. Manipulation of mGluR5 activity attenuates excessive grooming and instrumental learning differentially, and rescues impaired striatal synaptic plasticity in De422 / mice. These ndings show that deciency of Shank3 can impair mGluR5-Homer scaffolding, resulting in cortico-striatal circuit abnormalities that underlie decits in learning and ASD-like behaviours. These data suggest causal links between genetic, molecular, and circuit mechanisms underlying the pathophysiology of ASDs.
1 Department of Pediatrics, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA. 2 Department of Neurobiology, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA. 3 Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA. 4 Department of Radiology, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA. 5 Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA.
6 Department of Cell Biology, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA. 7 Department of Ophthalmology, Duke University, Durham,North Carolina 27710, USA. 8 Department of Child Health Care, The Childrens Hospital of Fudan University, 399 Wanyuan Road, Shanghai 201102, China.
9 Department of Cell Biology and Physiology, The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, North Carolina 27599, USA. 10 Duke Institute for Brain Sciences, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA. 11 University Program in Genetics and Genomics, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA. * These authors contributed equally to this work. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to Y.-h.J. (email: mailto:[email protected]
Web End [email protected] ).
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 1
DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 OPEN
Altered mGluR5-Homer scaffolds and corticostriatal connectivity in a Shank3 complete knockout model of autism
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459
Despite signicant progress in identifying genetic defects in autism spectrum disorders (ASDs), the molecular and neural circuit mechanisms that underlie the
behavioural impairments remain poorly dened. Genetic studies have consistently identied mutations in genes implicated in synaptic development and function1,2, although no clear consensus has emerged regarding the specic synapse types or brain regions whose dysfunction underlies ASDs. Recent studies suggest that the pathophysiology of ASDs involves not only aberrant synaptic connections but also defective development of neural networks and abnormal neural synchronization3,4. Neuroimaging investigations indicate that ASDs are associated with perturbed neural connectivity5; however, its exact nature remains uncertain6,7. Early studies identied reduced functional connectivity8, whereas recent reports implicate hyper-connectivity in multiple brain regions9,10. Further limiting their interpretability, these studies were conducted primarily in high-functioning ASD patients for whom etiologies are mostly unknown.
Mouse models can provide unique insights into the basic biological mechanisms underlying ASDs, but the development of these models is challenging because the biological basis for the majority of ASDs remains unknown11. Moreover, most animal models lack strong construct validity supported by human genetic studies12. SHANK3(PROSAP2)-related mutations represent a unique opportunity to address this challenge13. Genetic defects of SHANK3 are one of the best replicated ndings in autism genetics1416. Point mutations typically lead to limited disruption of isoform-specic expression of SHANK3, due to multiple intragenic promoters and alternatively spliced coding exons within the gene17. However, the vast majority of SHANK3 mutations found in ASDs are deletions of the entire gene. Most patients carrying deletions of the entire SHANK3 gene in 22q13.3 deletion or Phelan-McDermid syndrome (PMS) have the diagnosis of ASDs16,18,19.
Eleven lines of Shank3 isoform-specic mutant mice, with deletions of different exons or point mutations [De47, De49 (two lines), De9, De11, De1316, De21 (two lines) e21InsG3680 (two lines) and e21R1117X], have been reported2028. These Shank3 mice show variable molecular, synaptic and behavioural phenotypes, likely because different sets of Shank3 isoforms were disrupted in each line. While these data support heterogeneity in the phenotypes of Shank3 mutations, most of them lack construct validity because the same exonic deletions have not been reported in humans.
Accordingly, we generated Shank3 complete knockout mice, by deleting the protein-coding exons 422 (De422). Here we present results from molecular, ultrastructural and electrophysiological analyses, high-resolution magnetic resonance histology (MRH), diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) (structural connectomics), in vivo multi-circuit mapping (functional connectomics), behavioural testing and pharmacological analyses of the De422 mice. Together, these data support the signicance of these mice as a particularly suitable model for SHANK3-related human neurodevelopmental and neuropsychiatric disorders.
ResultsGeneration of Shank3 complete knockout mice. We and others have reported several isoform-specic Shank3 mutant mice (Supplementary Fig. 1a). To disrupt all murine Shank3 isoforms, we adopted a two-step gene targeting and a Cre/loxP strategy to ox exons 422 (e422oxed); these exons span 58 kb and include the coding sequences for all Shank3 protein isoforms (Fig. 1a and Supplementary Fig. 1be). E422oxed mice were bred with CMV-Cre mice to generate mice with deletions of
exons 422 (De422) (Fig. 1b). Loss of all known Shank3 mRNA and protein isoforms in De422 / mice was conrmed (Fig. 1c,d and Supplementary Fig. 1f).
After backcrossing to the C57BL/6J strain for more than eight generations, 10 cohorts of De422 mice were used for behavioural testing (Supplementary Table 1). De422 mice were viable, without any gross developmental defects, although ear opening and paw position developmental milestones in De422 / pups on postnatal day 4 (P4) were delayed (Supplementary Table 2).
The body weights of De422 / mice were similar to the other genotypes regardless of sex (Supplementary Fig. 1g), and we did not observe spontaneous seizures in these mice. No genotype differences were found in preference for snifng a social stimulus (Supplementary Table 2). Both genotypes demonstrated habituation-dishabituation to olfactory stimuli; however, responses were less robust for De422 / mice (Supplementary Fig. 1h).
De422 / mice display core behavioural features of ASDs. More than half of De422 / mice (16/30) developed skin lesions by 5 months of age (range: 47 months) across three cohorts; no such lesions were seen in De422 / (0/30) or
De422 / (0/29) mice. Lesions were observed around the eyes, on the ears, back of the head, and under the chin of mice on both
C57BL/6J and mixed C57BL/6J-129R1 backgrounds (Fig. 1e). These lesions appeared to be caused by excessive self-grooming (Supplementary Movie 1). To examine this propensity in De422 / mice that had not developed lesions, we evaluated grooming in the home cage before and after misting with water.
Although all mice increased grooming after spraying, the De422 / mice engaged in signicantly higher rates of grooming, relative to De422 / and De422 / animals (Fig. 1f).
To examine another aspect of repetitive behaviour, mice were tested on the hole-board. While De422 / and De422 /
mice engaged in fewer nose-pokes than De422 / mice, these differences were not signicant (Supplementary Table 2).
However, De422 / mice engaged signicantly fewer of the 16 holes (Fig. 1g) than the other genotypes; they also had a greater tendency to re-investigate the same hole (Fig. 1h). Collectively, we nd that De422 / mice display more repetitive behaviours and they sample their environment in a more restricted pattern than De422 / and De422 / mice.
To evaluate social preferences in neonates, we examined De422 juveniles (P15) in a nest-homing test. In a one-way test, the latency to approach a sample of the home nest did not differ among the genotypes (Supplementary Table 2). However, when given a choice between the home nest and an unfamiliar nest sample from a developmentally matched litters nest, the De422 / mice failed to show a preference for home nests, unlike De422 / and De422 / mice (Fig. 1i). In a further examination of social behaviour, we monitored the formation of dominance hierarchies over 6 days where three unfamiliar adults of the same genotype were placed into each cage. Since no sex differences were found (Supplementary Table 2), the data were collapsed across sex. From the number and direction of agonistic behaviours observed within each triad, a dominance ranking was calculated for each mouse. All De422 / mice established a hierarchy rank by day 1 and this remained stable through day 6. In contrast, 75% of hierarchies of De422 / mice were unstable; either dominance was not maintained across days, or no dominant animal could be identied in the triad (w2(1, N 30) 19.3, Po0.001).
Mice were next evaluated for sociability. Similar levels of preference for the novel social stimuli in the social afliation and social preference tests were displayed by the three genotypes
2 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 ARTICLE
a
Shank3 e4-9
Shank3 e4-22
7 8 13 14 15
Shank3
1 2 3 4 5 6 9 10 11 12 16 17 18 19 20 21 22
loxP1 loxP2 loxP3
Promoter 1
(Shank3a)
Promoter 2 (Shank3b)
Promoter 3
(Shank3c)
Promoter 4 (Shank3d)
Promoter 5 (Shank3e)
Promoter 6 (Shank3f)
b c
DNA ladder
Shank3a
CMV-Cre
Shank3b
Shank3c
Shank3d
Shank3e
Actin
1 10 22
2 3 21
11
Shank3 e4-22
+/+ / +/+ / /
+/+ /
+/+ /
+/+ /
+/+
1,000 bp
500 bp
200 bp
loxP1-2 loxP3
loxP1-3
Shank3 e4-9
Shank3 e4-22
d
e
f Spray test
e4-9 /
e4-22 /
e4-22 /
#
+/+
+/+
12
*
250 kD
150 kD
100 kD
10
+
Grooming rate
(s min1 )
8
*
Shank3
+/+
6
+
4
+/
/
Actin
2
C terminal Ab N terminal Ab
0 Pre-spray Post-spray
g
Hole-board exploration
h Hole-board exploration
0
i Juvenile nest preference test
+/+ +/ /
Home
120
* +
50
Stranger
90
% of holes explored
Probability of returning to a hole
100
40
80
#
100 # *
Latency to nest (s)
70
80
30
60
60
50
20
40
40
30
20
10
20
10
0
0
+/+ +/ /
+/+ +/ /
j k
Social dyadic Social dyadic
250
*
40
Interaction time (s)
200
+
*
Self-grooming (s)
30
+
150
Bi-directional
Non-reciprocated
20
100
50
10
0
0
+/+ +/ /
+/+ +/ /
Figure 1 | Generation of Shank3 complete knockout mice and their ASD-like behaviours. (a) Schematic design for Shank3 complete knockout mice using a Cre-loxP strategy. Alternatively spliced exons are in red and promoters are indicated by arrows. loxP sites are green triangles. (b) e422oxed mice were
crossed with CMV-Cre mice to generate deletion of De49 or De422, respectively. (c) All mRNA isoforms of Shank3 were deleted in De422 / ( / ) mice relative to De422/ ( / ) mice, as shown by RTPCR. (d) Western blot shows that all Shank3 protein bands are absent in /
brain, using Shank3 C- and N-terminal antibodies. The experiments were repeated three times. (e) Skin lesions were observed in 450% of / mice,
but not in / or De422 / ( / ) mice (w2(2,N
89) 38.4, Po0.001). (f) / mice spent signicantly more time in self-grooming (RMANOVA:
genotype F(2,46) 5.68, Po0.01), relative to / and / mice (pso0.01), n 1518/genotype. (g,h) Hole-board exploration. (g) On the hole-board, / mice explored fewer holes (pso0.05) than the other genotypes (F (2,21)
5.64, Po0.02) (h) with the / mice showing a trend for increased
probability of re-investigating holes (F(2,21) 2.99, Po0.08); n 78/genotype. (i) / pups (P15) failed to demonstrate a preference for their home
over a strangers nest, whereas / and / littermates preferred and rapidly entered (pso0.001) their home nest (RMANOVA: nest-
choice genotype F
(2,25)
4.38, Po0.03); n 810/genotype. (j,k) Responses in the social dyadic test. (j) No genotype differences were detected in
bidirectional contact in the social dyadic test. However, the duration of non-reciprocated interaction was prolonged (pso0.001) in / mice
(F(2,37) 11.30, Po0.001); n 1015/genotype. (k) During the social dyadic test, / mice engaged in more self-grooming (F
(2,37)
4.99, Po0.02)
than the other genotypes (pso0.02); n 1015/genotype. For all panels, *Po0.05 from / ; Po0.05 from / ; #Po0.05, within genotype for
post hoc comparisons. All data are expressed as meanss.e.m. RTPCR, PCR with reverse transcription.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 3
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459
(Supplementary Fig. 2a); no genotype differences were discerned in the duration or numbers of contacts with the non-social and social stimuli across all test phases (Supplementary Table 2). To evaluate social behaviour in a novel environment more critically, we conducted social dyadic tests. In pairings of De422 males with age-matched C3H males, the time spent in and the numbers of bidirectional interactions did not differ among the genotypes; the mice only displayed mild social investigations characterized by approach behaviours and snifng of the head, face, anogenital and shoulder areas of the partner mouse (Fig. 1j and Supplementary Table 2). However, De422 / mice engaged in longer and greater numbers of non-reciprocated behaviours (where the Shank3 mouse initiated the social interaction, but the C3H partner did not reciprocate and disengaged its partner by ignoring, leaving, or turning away from the target mouse) than the other genotypes (Fig. 1j and Supplementary Table 2). Moreover, De422 / animals engaged in more self-grooming during the social test than did De422 / or De422 / mice (Fig. 1k). Thus the De422 / mice have normal levels of social interest, but they persist in unsuccessful efforts to engage the social partner, and they engage in non-social behaviours that include repetitive self-grooming behaviours during social encounters.
Ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) were assessed in both pups and adults, as communication impairments represent a major feature of SHANK3-related disorders and ASDs. The P4 De422 / pups emitted signicantly fewer USVs that were of shorter duration than De422 / and De422 / pups (Fig. 2ac). Although the frequencies and amplitudes of calls from De422 / mice were lower than the other two genotypes (Supplementary Fig. 2b,c), all USVs were within a range appropriate for pup distress calls29. Upon exposure to oestrous females, De422 / adult males emitted fewer calls, and their calls were signicantly shorter in duration and reduced in amplitude relative to the other genotypes (Fig. 2df and Supplementary Table 2). By comparison, no genotype differences were observed in the peak frequency of adult USVs (Supplementary Table 2).
De422 / comorbidities phenocopy SHANK3 patients. Motor performance in De422 / mice was decient on both the accelerating and steady-speed rotarod tasks (Fig. 2g), despite normal grip strength (Supplementary Table 2). In their home cages, we observed that De422 / mice were hypoactive.
This response was conrmed in the open eld, where they showed reduced locomotion (Fig. 2h) and a tendency towards decreased rearing (Supplementary Table 2). De422 / mice also displayed reduced locomotion and spent less time in the centre of the open eld compared with the other genotypes (Supplementary Fig. 2d and Supplementary Table 2). In the light dark emergence test, De422 / mice delayed their entry into the lighted chamber (Supplementary Fig. 2e) and made fewer transitions between chambers than the other genotypes (Supplementary Fig. 2f). Collectively, these results suggest that De422 / mice present with anxiety-like behaviours.
While habituating mice to arenas for the novel object recognition memory test, many De422 / mice tried to escape from the test chamber. Since we did not observe escape responses in their home cages, we hypothesized that novelty was inducing this behaviour. When mice were placed individually into clean home cages, no escape behaviour was observed (Fig. 2i). However, when the mice were transferred to a novel larger arena, one-half of the De422 / mice escaped; no De422 / mice engaged in this response. Similarly, during testing in the Morris water maze, we found that De422 / mice leapt from the hidden
platform and away from the experimenter on more trials than the other genotypes (Fig. 2j). This behaviour may be similar to the enhanced reactivity to novel environments reported in SHANK3-related ASD patients15,18,19.
Cognitive performance was examined using several different paradigms. Pre-attentive function was unchanged when analysed by prepulse inhibition, but startle reactivity was reduced in De4
22 / compared with the De422 / mice (Supplementary Table 2). Hippocampal function was evaluated with the Morris water maze. De422 / mice showed normal acquisition performance (Supplementary Fig. 2g), but displayed slower reversal learning when the hidden platform was moved from the northeast to the southwest quadrant (Supplementary Fig. 2h). During probe trials, De422 / mice were impaired in locating the target quadrant, compared with the other genotypes (Supplementary Fig. 2ik). In the visible platform task, swim time did not differ among genotypes (Supplementary Table 2), indicating that sensory and motor function and motivation were intact. In fear conditioning, no signicant genotype differences were noted for cued conditioning, although De422 / mice showed a small enhancement in freezing during contextual testing (Supplementary Table 2). Since Shank3 is enriched in the striatum (Supplementary Fig. 6a), De422 mice were examined in a striatal-dependent instrumental learning task30. Each bar-press was reinforced with a food pellet across seven sessions. The De422 / mice were unable to learn this task, showing a dramatic decit after the second training session (Fig. 2k). In summary, we nd that fear learning is intact in De422 /
mice, whereas hippocampal spatial memory is mildly perturbed, and striatal learning is severely impaired.
Altered functional connectivity in a frontostriatal circuit. Alterations in brain oscillatory function and global network connectivity have been proposed to mediate ASDs7,31. To test whether Shank3 disruption was sufcient to alter oscillatory activity across limbic networks, De422 / and De422 /
mice were implanted with microwire arrays into the prelimbic cortex (PRL_CX), cingulate cortex (CG_CX), medial dorsal nucleus of thalamus (THAL), ventral hippocampus (V_HIP), and nucleus accumbens (NAC). The NAC was targeted for in vivo analysis instead of the dorsolateral striatum (DLS) because the NAC is the projection target of the CG_CX, and this latter brain region is heavily involved in social behaviours32. Following surgical recovery, local eld potentials (LFP) and single-unit activities were recorded before and after exposure to a novel mouse as a test for social responsiveness (Fig. 3a).
Signicant changes in oscillatory power were observed across many brain areas upon introduction of a social stimulus to both genotypes. Lower NAC oscillatory power in the 711 Hz band was observed in De422 / mice, both before and after exposure to the social stimulus (Fig. 3b and Supplementary Table 3). Exposure to the novel mouse increased coherence (reecting increased functional connectivity) throughout the limbic network in De4
22/ controls (Fig. 3c and Supplementary Table 4), especially in the 711 Hz band, representing a network previously proposed to mediate behavioural decits in ASDs31. In contrast, De422 /
mice displayed diminished functional activation (that is, a smaller task-induced increase in coherence) in the 711 Hz network across the cingulate cortico-striatal-thalamic axis (Fig. 3d,e). Other elements of the 711 Hz network (the hippocampal-striatal and cingulate cortico-thalamic circuits) may also have been affected, though these differences did not reach statistical signicance (Supplementary Table 3). Likely contributing to these changes, NAC-dependent functional connectivity before the introduction of the novel mouse tended to be higher in De422 / than in
4 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 ARTICLE
a
b
c
P4 USV P4 USV
Adult USV
P4 USV
Adult USV
+
100
100
Call duration (ms)
+/+
+/
/
*
*
80
+
+
80
Number of calls
60
60
40
40
20
20
0
0
+/+ +/ /
+/+ +/ /
+/+ +/ /
d
e
f
Adult USV
*
+/+
+/
/
*
600
+
60
Call duration (ms)
500
50
Number of calls
400
40
300
30
200
20
100
10
0
0
+/+ +/ /
g
h
Rotarod Open field
Rotarod
350
Accelerating (440 r.p.m.)
+/+ +/ /
3,000
Distance traveled (cm)
*
350
Steady speed (16 r.p.m.)
+
300
300
2,500
Latency to fall (s)
Latency to fall (s)
Escaping Escaping
250
250
2,000
200
200
1,500
150
150
1,000
100
100
50
50
500
0
0 1 2 3 4
+/+ +/ /
0 1 2 3 4Trial number
Trial number
i j k
Instrumental conditioning
Sessions
Jump No jump
Lever presses per min
20
Jump No jump
800
MWM trials with jumping
&
700
&
Number of mice
15
10
5
4
3
2
1
0
+/
600
500
400
300
5
200
100
0
0
*
* *
*
+/+ +/+
+/+
/ +/ 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
+/+
/ /
/
Clean cage
Novel chamber
Figure 2 | De422 / mice display additional ASDs-related and comorbid behaviours. (af) Abnormal USV communication. P4 De422 / ( / )
pups emitted signicantly fewer USVs (a) (F(2,32) 7.29, Po0.003) and of shorter duration (b) (F
(2,29)
4.59, Po0.02), than the other genotypes (pso0.01). (c) Representative spectrographs of P4 USVs; n 813/genotype. Upon exposure to females, adult / males emitted fewer USVs (d) (F
(2,38)
6.15, Po0.01)
that were of signicantly shorter duration (e) (F(2,34) 7.12, Po0.01) than the other genotypes (pso0.02). (f) Representative spectrographs of adult USVs;n 1018/genotype. (g,h) Impaired motor performance. (g) On the accelerating rotarod (left) / mice had shorter latencies to fall (RMANOVA: genotype
effect F(2,27) 5.33, Po0.02) than the other genotypes (pso0.01). On the steady-speed rotarod (right), / mice also fell sooner (RMANOVA: genotype
effect F(2,27) 15.86, Po0.001) than the other genotypes (pso0.005); n 912/genotype. (h) Hypoactivity in the open eld. / mice had reduced
locomotion (F(2,23) 6.04, Po0.01) relative to the other genotypes (pso0.001); n 610/genotype. (i,j) Escape behaviours in different environments. (i) No
mice escaped from new home cages. However, six out of 13 / mice escaped from a novel environment (w2(1,
N 24) 6.77, Po0.01); n 1113/genotype.
(j) In the MWM, / mice escaped from the hidden platform on signicantly more trials than the other genotypes (w2(2,
N 1,680) 314.01, Po0.001);
n 1113/genotype. (k) Impaired instrumental learning. / mice had difculty learning to press a lever for food reward (RMANOVA: genotype effect
F(2,102) 4.7, Po0.05, genotype session F(12,
102)
3.0, Po0.001) relative to the other genotypes (pso0.001); n 48/genotype. For all panels, *Po0.05
from / , Po0.05 from / for post hoc comparisons; &Po0.05 for w2 analyses. All data are expressed as meanss.e.m. MWM, Morris water maze.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 5
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459
a
b
27 Hz
711 Hz
+/+
##
Test-related power
(dB)
/ *
1 20.5 1.5
0.5 0.5
1 0 1.5 0.5
2 1
0 1
2 21.5 1.5
0.5 0.5 0
0
## ## ##
5 min 5 min
C3H
FI-C3H
CG_CX
NAC
THAL
PRL_CX
V_HIP
CG_CX
NAC
THAL
PRL_CX
V_HIP
1530 Hz 3055 Hz
FI-Empty
### ## ##
CG_CX NAC
THAL PRL_CX V_HIP
1 s C3H introduced
Test-related power
(dB)
## ##
1 1
1
## ## ##
1 mv
0.5
0.5
CG_CX
NAC
THAL
PRL_CX
V_HIP
CG_CX
NAC
THAL
PRL_CX
V_HIP
c
+/+ /
d
27 Hz 711 Hz 1530 Hz 3055 Hz
Empty Empty
Test-related connectivity Test-related connectivity
CG_CX 0.7 35% 35%
0.7
V_HIP
NAC
V_HIP
NAC
V_HIP
NAC
V_HIP
NAC
PRL_CX
THAL
PRL_CX
THAL
PRL_CX
THAL
PRL_CX
THAL
CG_CX
CG_CX
CG_CX
e
f
+/+ Empty
/ Empty
+/+ C3H
/ C3H
NAC CG_CX NAC THAL
+/+ Empty
711 Hz 711 Hz
*
##
+/+ C3H
/ C3H
## NAC
CG_CX
THAL
## ##
10
14
Firing rate (s1 )
16
Functional connectivity
0.90.80.70.60.50.40.30.20.1 0
Functional connectivity
0.90.80.70.60.50.40.30.20.1 0
**
/ Empty
**
0
0
0
##
##
N =109/158
N =91/76
N = 21/33
## ##
##
g h
Empty 55
C3H
+/+ Empty / Empty
11
+/+ C3H / C3H
+/+ C3H
+/+
/
6
0
6
50
/ C3H
CG_CX
CG_CX
CG_CX
Amplitude frequency (Hz)
50 NAC
NAC
NAC
25 Hz CFPC
25 Hz CFPC
NAC CG_CX THAL
0 5
0 5
THALCG_CXNAC
5
0 5
Temporal offset (ms)
0
50
50
0
50
50
0
THAL
THAL
*
THAL
NAC
THAL
CG_CX
10 1 10 1 10
0
0 1530 Hz
0 3055 Hz
27 Hz
711 Hz
1530 Hz
Phase frequency (Hz)
Figure 3 | Aberrant functional neural connectivity in De422 mice. (a) Schematic representation of the forced social interaction test (top). A 4 s trace of LFP activity in De422 mice, recorded before and following introduction of the C3H social stimulus mouse (arrow at bottom). (b) Test-related changes in oscillatory power in De422 / ( / ) and De422/ ( / ) littermates. (Mixed model ANOVA genotype testcondition interaction (see
Supplementary Table 3)). *Po 0.05, / versus / mice, #Po0.05, comparisons within genotype. (c,d) Diagrams showing functional connectivity
across the recorded brain areas before and after introduction of the social stimulus mouse in / (c) and / (d) mice. The thickness of the
connecting lines corresponds to the coherence between each pair of brain areas in the specied frequency band. The image on the right shows the change in coherence observed across the network following introduction of the social stimulus mouse. Only coherence measures that demonstrated signicant testcondition interactions are shown. (e) Signicant genotype by testcondition interaction identied in the social-related network using mixed model ANOVA. **Po0.05, for signicant genotype effect, / versus / mice (FDR-corrected); ##Po 0.01, within genotype comparison (FDR-corrected)
(see Supplementary Table 3). (f) Effects of genotype and testconditionsin unit ring rates (mixed model ANOVA, genotype testcondition interaction
(see Supplementary Table 3)). *Po 0.05, for signicant genotype effect, / versus / mice; ##Po0.05, for testcondition effects within genotype.
(g) LFP phase-amplitude coupling (CFPC) in / and / mice. LFP-b (1530 Hz) and -g (3055 Hz) oscillatory activities were modulated by the
phase of locally recorded 25 Hz oscillations. An example of CFPC across a single NAC channel is shown during the forced interaction test (left). Effects of genotype and testconditions in CFPC (right) (mixed model ANOVA, genotype testcondition interaction (see Supplementary Table 3)). ##Po0.05, for
testcondition effects within genotype. (h) Directional network communication during social interaction. A temporal delay between thalamic to striatal oscillatory activity was diminished in the / animals. *Po0.05, / versus / (FDR-corrected rank sum test). The thickness of a line in the panel
to the right denotes the degree of functional connectivity. For all panels, n 1114/genotype. All data are expressed as meanss.e.m.
6 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 ARTICLE
De422/ mice. We conclude that Shank3 deciency results in hyperactivation of this social neural network under basal conditions, whereas the gain induced by the social stimulus is diminished in De422 / mice.
Aside from abnormal network activity, De422 / mice also displayed lower unit ring rates in NAC both before and after introduction of the social stimulus (Fig. 3f). No genotype differences were observed in cross-frequency phase coupling or phase lock of unit responses (two measures of local connectivity) within the cortical, striatal and thalamic microcircuits (Fig. 3g and Supplementary Fig. 3). We quantied directional oscillatory interactions during the social stimulus presentation, as a measure of information transfer across the cortico-striatal-thalamic circuit. In both De422 / and De422 / mice, oscillatory synchrony was directed from thalamus to cortex and striatum in the 711 Hz frequency band; however, the temporal delay between thalamic to striatal oscillatory activity was diminished in the De422 / animals (Fig. 3h).
NAC ring decit is due to local Shank3 disruption. The NAC ring decit observed in the De422 / mice could result from developmental inuences of Shank3 disruption on NAC function, changes in the function of NAC inputs from other brain regions (for example, hippocampus), or a direct change in the ring properties of NAC independent of development. To examine directly these possibilities, we used e422oxed mice (Fig. 1a). We bilaterally infected the NAC of these e422oxed mice with an AAV10-Cre virus to selectively knockdown Shank3 in the NAC; an AAV10-GFP virus served as the control (Supplementary Fig. 4a). Four weeks after injections, these mice were surgically implanted with recording electrodes in NAC and unit activity was recorded during the baseline period of the social stimulus probe test. The reduced Shank3 expression, locations of viral infection (Supplementary Fig. 4b) and NAC electrode placement (Supplementary Fig. 4c) were veried post mortem. We found that the mice treated with AAV10-Cre exhibited signicantly lower ring rates in the NAC than AAV10-GFP controls (Supplementary Fig. 4d). Thus, these results provide direct evidence that selective deciency of Shank3 in the NAC is sufcient to reduce the ring rate of medium spiny neurons (MSNs), recapitulating the phenotype observed in the De422 / mutants.
Altered brain morphometry and structural connectivity. To probe anatomic changes that might underlie the observed impairments in behaviour and functional connectivity, we imaged xed brains of seven adult De422 / and seven C57BL/6J controls, using high-eld MRH and a high-resolution (43 mm) DTI protocol33,34. Quantitative analysis revealed slightly larger brains in De422 / mice (472.519.7 mm3; Ms.e.m.) than in controls (459.715.7 mm3); however, this did not reach statistical signicance. After normalizing regional to total brain volume, multiple grey matter structures were found to differ between De422 / and control mice (Fig. 4a,b and
Supplementary Table 5). The basal ganglia (globus pallidus,17.8%; substantia nigra, 9.6%; and caudate putamen, 5.9%) and thalamus (ventral thalamic nuclei, 8.7%; rest of the thalamus,7.0%) were enlarged, while the olfactory areas ( 18.5%),
hippocampus ( 4.1%), and amygdala ( 7.7%) were smaller
in the De422 / mice. With the exception of the fornix, all the other ve white matter tracts had reduced volumes relative to controls (Fig. 4c,d).
To gain insight into white matter integrity, we performed DTI, comparing fractional anisotropy (FA) contrast between white matter tracts and the cortex in both genotypes. Qualitative
evaluation of FA images showed reduced intensity in white matter relative to grey matter in De422 / mice, compared
with controls (Supplementary Fig. 5a). Signicant reductions were identied for the spinal trigeminal tract, cingulum, and the cerebral peduncle (Fig. 4e and Supplementary Fig. 5b). We further examined how changes in FA contrast (normalized to cortex) correlated across various bre tracts throughout the brain (Supplementary Fig. 5c). Signicant correlations were found for white matter tracts in De422 / mice (48 versus 32 correlational pairs in controls).
Voxel-based morphometry conrmed the regional results and revealed additional areas of volume loss in the De422 / mice.
The effect size is shown as a texture map overlaid on the average template for the control group (Fig. 4f). Corrected P values using all brain voxels exceeded the 5% false discovery rate threshold (FDR). Areas exhibiting signicant FA reductions (after FDR correction) included the genu of the corpus callosum and cingulum, the anterior commissure, mbria, optic tract, areas around the medial lemniscus and nigrostriatal bundles, as well as the cerebral peduncle, cerebellum and brainstem. We detected a unilateral change in the internal capsule, implying an asymmetric change in white matter resulting from Shank3 deciency.
Dysfunctional striatal synapses. Our functional and structural connectivity results suggest that striatal circuitry may be dysfunctional in De422 / mice. Accordingly, synaptic function was examined in acute striatal slices. We used whole-cell patch-clamp recordings to assess excitatory synaptic transmission in the DLS, which receives strong projections from sensorimotor cortical regions and has been implicated in habit formation and compulsive behaviour35. First, we characterized the intrinsic excitability of MSNs by measuring spike frequency in response to depolarizing steps of current injection. De422 / MSNs showed enhanced excitability, compared with De422 / and De422 / neurons (Fig. 5a), while no genotype difference was observed for resting membrane potential (Fig. 5b).
De422 / mice displayed a marked reduction in the frequency of spontaneous excitatory postsynaptic currents (sEPSCs) (Fig. 5c,d), consistent with decreased spine density and number of glutamatergic synapses on MSNs in these mice. In contrast, the mean sEPSC amplitude was not affected by Shank3 deciency (Fig. 5e,f). We used high-frequency stimulation (HFS) -induced long-term depression (LTD) to probe synaptic plasticity. LTD was signicantly reduced in De422 / mice, compared with the De422 / and De422 / mice (Fig. 5g). These results indicate that multiple aspects of synaptic function are impaired in De422 / striatal neurons.
Changes in synaptic organization in De422 / mice. These abnormalities in synaptic function in De422 / striatum led us to investigate possible underlying morphological changes in synapses. Golgi staining revealed that spine density in the striatum of De422 / mice was reduced, but not in the hippocampus (Fig. 5h,i). Electron microscopic examination showed that the postsynaptic density (PSD) was signicantly shorter and thinner in striatal synapses of De422 / compared with De422 / mice (Fig. 5jl). There was a similar trend for smaller PSD structures in hippocampal and cortical synapses (data not shown for cortical synapses).
To analyze the molecular changes that may contribute to dysfunctional synapses, we examined synaptic proteins in PSD fractions from De422 striata and hippocampi (Supplementary
Fig. 6b). Pan-SAPAP and SAPAP3 levels were decreased only in striatum, while GluN2A was reduced only in hippocampus (Supplementary Fig. 6b). The most prominent changes in
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 7
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459
a
P < 0.05 P < 0.1
b
% Difference Cohen d
Relative volume (% of control)
16
20
5
14
Control
P < 0.05 P < 0.1
15
Volume (% of brain)
12
3
8
Effect size (no s.d.)
/
10
10
5
1
0
6
5
1
4
10
3
2
15
0
20
5
Iped
APT
SN
GP
DpMe
VT
SC
THAL
CPu
Olf
BS
VS
IC
Hyp
Amy
HIP
Iped
APT
SN
GP
DpMe
VT
SC
THAL
CPu
Olf
BS
VS
IC
Hyp
Amy
HIP
c d e
0.6
20
4
300
P < 0.05 P < 0.1
Control /
0.5
Volume (% of brain)
% Difference Cohen d
Relative volume
(% of control)
Effect size (no s.d.)
difference (% of cortex)
0.4
Control /
2
250
0
Relative FA contrast
10
200
0.3
0
150
0.2
10
2
100
0.1
50
0.0
20
4
0
fx st ot ac fi cp
fx
st
ot
ac
fi
cp
sp5
cg
cp
st
fi
ac
ic
cc
mcp
fx
ll
f
DBM (P < 0.05) DBM (P < 0.05)
FA (q < 0.05) FA (q < 0.05)
Grey matter White matter
cc
cg
ac
ml/ns
A29
HcDg
IC
fi
CPu
cc
ot
Amy
Iped: interpeduncular nucleus APT: anterior pretectal nucleiSN: substantia nigraGP: globus pallidusDpMe: deep mesencephalic nuclei VT: ventral thalamic nucleiSC: superior colliculusTHAL: rest of the thalamusCPu: caudate putamenBS: rest of the brainstemOlf: olfactory areasVS: ventriclesIC: inferior colliculusHyp: hypothalamusAmy: amygdalaHIP: hippocampus
fx: fornixst: stria terminalisot: optic tractac: anterior commissurefi: fimbriacp: cerebral pedunclesp5: spinal trigeminal nervecg: cingulumic: internal capsulecc: corpus callosummcp: middle cerebellar pedunclell: lateral lemniscusml: medial lemniscusns: nigrostriatal bundleA29: Brodmann area 29HcDG: hippocampal dentate gyrus Cblm: cerebellum
cp Cblm
Figure 4 | Abnormalities in brain structure in De422 / mice. (a,b) Volume changes in grey matter of De422 mice. (a) Signicant volume differences in several brain regions (normalized to total brain volume) were observed in De422 / ( / ) mice, relative to controls. t-test, n 7 mice/genotype.
(b) These differences are also apparent when the data are expressed as relative volume (shown as percentage of control) or Cohens d changes, where positive values represent enlargement and negative values represent reduction in / mice. (c,d) Volume changes were observed in white matter tracts.
t-test, n 7 mice/genotype. (e) Changes in diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) parameters, computed relative to the cortex fractional anisotropy (FA),
100*(ROI FA-cortex FA)/cortex FA. Reduced FA values were observed in white matter tracts in / mice relative to controls, accompanied by a 20%
average reduction in FA values. t-test, n 6 mice/genotype. (f, left) Statistical parametric maps based on Jacobians and FA pin-point localized changes
(arrows) in volume and microstructural organization in / mice. While the patterns are distinct, the anterior commissure (ac) and corpus callosum (cc)
emerge as common biomarkers that change in both volume and FA. (right) Full names for abbreviations in (af). All data are expressed as meanss.e.m. ROI, regions of interest.
De422 / mice were for the Homer family proteins (Fig. 6ac). Homer1b/c was markedly decreased (18% of
/ ) in striatum and mildly reduced in hippocampus (77%
of / ) relative to De422 / mice. There was no signicant
change for Homer1a in either brain region. In the De422 / mice, Homer2 was signicantly decreased in both brain areas (48% of / in striatum; and 78% of / in hippocampus).
Homer3 (the least abundant isoform) was undetectable in striatum, but was reduced in the De422 / hippocampus (20% of / ). Unexpectedly, we found that mGluR5 was
substantially increased in striatum (165% of / ), but not in
De422 / hippocampus. These results indicate region-specic alterations in PSD proteins in De422 / brains, especially in the striatum.
8 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 ARTICLE
a
b
+/+ +/+
/
*
+/
/
0
40
+/ /
Spike frequency (Hz)
*
40
*
20
0
20
60
80
100
20
15
10
5
0
+/+ +/
/
0 100 300 400
200 Current (pA)
c d e f
*
2.0
1.5
1.0
0.5
0.0
1.0
1.0
0.5
0.0
0 10 20 30 40 50
Cumulative probability
0.5
0.0
100
50
0 0 10 20Time (min)
+/+
sEPSC frequency (Hz)
Cumulative probability
sEPSC amplitude (pA)
+/+
/
0 10
100 Hz HFS
+/+
/
20 Inter-event interval (s)
30
+/+
/
sEPSC amplitude (pA)
RMP (mV)
g
h i
% Baseline EPSC amplitude
1.2
ST HIP
1
*
Spines per m
+/+
0.8
+/+
0.6
+/+
/
+/
/
0.4
/
0.2
30 40
0 ST HIP
j ST HIP
+/+
/
k
l
*
350 +/+
/
PSD length (nm)
25
+/+ /
300
PSD thickness (nm)
20
250
*
200
15
150
10
100
5
50
0 ST HIP ST HIP
0 ST HIP
Dark Light
Figure 5 | Electrophysiological characterization of striatal medium spiny neurons in De422 mice. (a) Spike frequency in response to current injection. De422 / ( / ) neurons showed enhanced excitability with 200400 pA injected current (*Po0.05) compared with De422/ ( / )
and De422 / ( / ) neurons (F(3,
267)
242.8, Po0.0001); n 2832 neurons/genotype. (b) No genotype differences in RMPs were observed;
n 2832/genotype. (c,d) The frequency of sEPSCs was signicantly reduced in / neurons; *Po0.001, t-test, n 1314/genotype. (e,f) The sEPSC
amplitude was not altered in / neurons; n 1314/genotype. (g) HFS-induced LTD was impaired in / neurons; (planned comparison, t-test,
Po0.05 for mice / , Po0.01 for / , and p40.05 for / mice; n 812/genotype). (h) Sample images of Golgi-impregnated neurons in
striatum (ST) and CA1 area of hippocampus (HIP) from / and / mice. Scale bar: 5 mm. (i) A decrease in spine density was found in striatum of / mice. *Po0.001, t-test, n 97 branches from 50 cells of three mice/genotype. (jl) Altered PSD ultrastructure in / mice. (j) Representative
images for EM from striatum and CA1 hippocampus. Scale bar, 0.2 mm. (k) The PSD length was signicantly decreased in striatum. (l) The PSD is thinner in striatum, especially its cytoplasmic light portion. *Po0.005, t-test. n 79 synapses from 4 / mice and n 139 synapses from 6 / mice for
striatum; n 120 synapses from 6 / mice and n 148 synapses from 6 / mice for hippocampus. All data are expressed as meanss.e.m.
RMP, resting membrane potential.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 9
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459
Altered cellular distributions of Homer1b/c and mGluR5. We next examined the possible mechanism by which synaptic Homer1b/c was reduced. The mRNA levels for Homer1a and Homer1b/c in De422 / striatum and hippocampus were similar to / controls (Fig. 6d). Although total Homer1b/c
protein was slightly decreased in De422 / striatum (Fig. 6e,f), this is unlikely to explain the drastic reduction of Homer1b/c in the PSD. Sub-fractionation studies revealed a marked increase of Homer1b/c in the cytosolic fraction from De422 / striatum (379% of / ), while that in the hippocampus was similar to
controls (Fig. 6g,h). In contrast, striatal cytosolic mGluR5 was
decreased (70% of / ), with no genotype differences in the
hippocampus (Fig. 6i). Homer1b/c was also examined in De49 mice. The decreased expression of Homer1b/c in the PSD and the cytosolic accumulation of this protein in De49 / striatum were less prominent than in De422 / mice (Supplementary
Fig. 6c,d,f,h).
The increase in cytosolic Homer1b/c in De422 / striatum was conrmed by Homer1b/c immunostaining of striatal neurons (Fig. 6j). Similar changes were observed in the NAC but not in the hippocampus or neocortex (Supplementary Fig. 6e,g). Conversely, the co-localization of Homer1b/c with the presynaptic marker Bassoon was signicantly decreased in De422 / striatum, consistent with decreased Homer1b/c in the PSD (Fig. 6k,l).
Homer 1b/c immunostaining completely overlapped with the neuronal marker NeuN (Supplementary Fig. 7g), but only partially overlapped in dopamine D1 receptor (D1R) labelled neurons (dened by D1-td-Tomato) (Supplementary Fig. 7h). Since the striatum is composed primarily of MSNs containing either D1Rs or D2Rs, our ndings suggest that mGluR5-Homer scaffolds are likely altered in both D1R- and D2R-positive MSNs.
Altered mGluR5/ Homer1 organization in striatal synapses. To corroborate the increased mGluR5 in striatal PSDs, we immune-stained for mGluR5 and PSD-95 in striatal slices. Co-localization studies revealed their increased association in De422 / mice (Fig. 7a,b), supporting the biochemical evidence for elevated mGluR5 in synapses. Using an antibody recognizing the extracellular domain of mGluR5, we found that that surface mGluR5s were unchanged in cortical-striatal neuronal co-cultures (Supplementary Fig. 7ad). However, the ratio of surface to intracellular mGluR5 was reduced in De422 / MSN dendrites but not in soma (Fig. 7ce), suggesting a selectively increased intracellular pool of mGluR5.
The aforementioned mislocalization of Homer1b/c and mGluR5 likely causes disruption of the mGluR5-Homer scaffold, and this is supported by the reduced interaction of Homer1b/c and mGluR5 in De422 / striatum (Fig. 7f). To further test if mGluR5-mediated signalling was altered, we examined the phosphorylation state of several kinases in the mGluR5-mediated pathway from De422 / striatal and hippocampal slices.
(Fig. 7g). Under the baseline condition, levels of p-ERK1/2 and p-S6K were signicantly elevated in De422 / striatum, but not in hippocampus (Fig. 7h,i). Activation of mGluR5 with the selective group I metabotropic agonist (S)-3,5-dihydroxyphenylglycine (DHPG) enhanced levels of p-ERK1/2 and p-S6K in the striatum in both genotypes. However, the DHPG-induced increase (fold change) of ERK1/2 and S6K phosphorylation was less apparent in De422 / striatum (Fig. 7k,l), suggesting a weaker ligand-dependent response for mGluR5. By contrast, there was no genotype difference in p-mTOR level in De422 striatum and hippocampus before or after DHPG treatment (Fig. 7j,m). Since mGluR5 signalling regulates protein synthesis in hippocampus36, we also evaluated protein synthesis in De422 / striatum. No genotype difference was observed
(Supplementary Fig. 7e,f), though cell type-specic changes cannot be excluded. These results suggest that mGluR5 function may be enhanced at baseline, but the ligand-dependent response of mGluR5 is attenuated in the De422 / striatum.
mGluR5 modulators correct impaired behaviour and LTD. The reduced association of mGluR5 with Homer1b/c suggests that mGluR5-related function may be compromised, whereas the increased mGluR5 in the PSD raises the possibility that mGluR5-mediated functions may be augmented in the De422 / striatum. To determine whether these changes may mediate some of the abnormal behaviours in De422 / mice, we performed behavioural analysis in mice treated with a mGluR5 antagonist (MPEP) or a positive allosteric modulator of mGluR5 (CDPPB). The hypoactivity in the open eld in the De422 / mice was normalized with MPEP; this compound also suppressed the increased self-grooming (Fig. 8a,c). In contrast, CDPPB failed to alter the hypoactivity, while it augmented self-grooming in De422 / mice (Fig. 8b,d). The inverse effects of MPEP and CDPPB on grooming behaviour support the hypothesis that enhanced mGluR5 function contributes to the elevated self-grooming in De422 / mice.
We had found instrumental learning to be profoundly impaired in De422 / mice (Fig. 2k). Since there is evidence for reduced mGluR5 function in De422 / mice and CDPPB has been reported to rescue impaired learning in Shank2 mutant mice37, we reasoned that positive allosteric modulation of mGluR5 might restore the impaired instrumental learning. Indeed, we found that CDPPB treatment partially rescued instrumental learning in De422 / mice (Fig. 8eh).
We also tested the effects of CDPPB on synaptic function and neuronal excitability in striatal slices. CDPPB rescued the impaired LTD (Fig. 8i) but it did not change the probability of presynaptic release (PPR) in De422 / mice (Fig. 8j).
In addition, CDPPB did not change the frequency or amplitude of sEPSCs, and it failed to affect the resting membrane potential (Supplementary Fig. 8ac). CDPPB also reduced the hyper-excitability in MSNs of De422 / mice. However, a similar reduction in excitability was observed in De422 / neurons, suggesting that this rescue effect may not be specic (Fig. 8k).
Since Homer1b/c is an important mediator in the mGluR5 signalling pathway, we next examined whether there was any relationship between synaptic Homer1b/c and lever pressing in De422 / mice. After 7 days of lever-press training, we found that those mutants with increased numbers of lever presses had signicantly higher levels of Homer1b/c in their striatal PSDs than those with lower lever-press rates (Fig. 8l). We also observed that 7 days of treatment with CDPPB resulted in a slight increase of synaptic Homer1b/c in De422 / mice (Fig. 8m), consistent with the partial rescue of lever pressing by this compound. These data suggest that the disruption of mGluR5-Homer scaffolds, due to the reduction of Homer1b/c, may contribute to the impaired instrumental learning and striatal plasticity in the De422 / mice.
DiscussionOur complete Shank3 knockout mouse provides a new genetic model for studying mechanisms underlying ASDs and PMS. Since the most common genetic defect found in human ASDs is deletion of the entire SHANK3 gene15,18,19, the De422 mouse has better construct validity for SHANK3-related ASDs than previously reported lines of Shank3 mutant mice14,2025,27,28 (Supplementary Fig. 1a). These mice present with behavioural phenotypes that resemble features of SHANK3-related ASDs and PMS15,16,18,19 (Supplementary Table 6), such as increased
10 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 ARTICLE
a
ST HIP
b
c
Striatum
*
Hippocampus
2 +/+ +/ /
*
2.5 +/+ +/ /
Homer 1a
2
1.5
Homer 1b/c
-tubulin III
Protein level
1.5
1
Protein level
+
Homer 2
Homer 3
1
*
* *
**
0.5
* *
*
+
0.5
mGluR5
+/+ +/ / +/+ +/ /
0
0
Homer1a
Homer1b/c
Homer2
Homer3
mGluR5
Homer1a
Homer1b/c
Homer2
Homer3
mGluR5
d
e
f
1
1.2
*
ST HIP
1
Homer/Rpl13a
0.8
+/+
/
0.8
Homer1b/c
Actin
+/+ / +/+ /
Homer1b/c
0.6
0.6
0.4
0.4
0.2
0.2
0
0
Homer1a Homer1b/c
ST HIP
+/+
/
g h i
ST HIP
5
*
1.4
1.2
Cytosolic Homer1b/c
*
Cytosolic mGluR5
4
+/+
/
Homer 1b/c
-tubulin III
+/+ / +/+ /
1
3
0.8
0.6
+/+
/
mGluR5
2
0.4
1
0.2
0
0
ST HIP
ST HIP
j
Merge
Homer1b/c Bassoon DAPI
k
/+/+
0.4
*
0.3
Striatum
Pearsons coefficient
0.2
0.1
0
+/+ /
l
/+/+
Pearsons coefficient
0.4
Hippocampus
0.3
0.2
0.1
0 +/+ /
Figure 6 | Abnormal Homer and mGluR5 distribution in De422 / striatum. (a) Altered mGluR5-Homer scaffolds in the PSD fractions from striatum (ST) and hippocampus (HIP) of De422 / ( / ) mice. Note a marked decrease of Homer1b/c and increased mGluR5 in / striatum.
(b,c) Quantication of Homer and mGluR5 protein in striatum (b) and hippocampus (c). *Po0.05, / versus / ; Po0.05, / versus / ;
one-way ANOVA with Tukeys post hoc test; n 4/genotype for Homer1b/c, Homer2 and Homer3; n 9 for Homer1a; n 10 for mGluR5. (d) Homer1a and
Homer 1b/c mRNA expression was similar in / and / mice. n 3/genotype. (e,f) Total Homer1b/c protein was mildly reduced in / ST
(90% of / ); *Po0.05, t-test, n 7/genotype. (g) Altered mGluR5 and Homer1b/c in the cytosolic fractions from ST and HIP of / mice.
(h,i) Quantication of (g) showing increased Homer1b/c and decreased mGluR5 in the cytosol of the striatum of / mice; *Po0.01, t-test, n 46/
genotype. (jl) Immunostaining shows that Homer 1b/c accumulates in the somata of striatal neurons of / brain slices (j). Co-localization analysis
using Pearsons coefcient revealed a decreased correlation between Homer1b/c and Bassoon in / striatum (k), but not in hippocampus (l). *Po0.01,
t-test, n 56 /genotype). Scale bar, 10 mm. Experiments for western blots were repeated at least three times. All data are expressed as meanss.e.m.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 11
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459
a
c
mGluR5/PSD95/DAPI
mGluR5 PSD95
/+/+
+/+ /
Surface
Intracellular
Surface
Intracellular
Dendrite Soma
b d e f
*
*
IP
1.2
*
2
1
Pearson's coefficent
mGluR5 (S/I) dendrite
1
mGluR5 (S/I) soma
Homer/mGluR5 ratio
0.3
1.5
0.8
+/+ +/+ +/+
/ /
0.8
mGluR5
Homer1b/c
Input IgG
mGluR5
0.2
0.6
0.6
1
0.4
0.4
0.1
0.5
0.2
0.2
0
0
+/+ /
0 +/+ /
0 +/+ / +/+ /
g h
k
4
ST HIP e4-22
3
4 * *
*
+/+ +/+
/ /
+
3
DHPG
+
+
+
Basal p-ERK1/2
+/+ /
+/+ /
+/+ /
2
DHPG-induced increase of p-ERK1/2
2
1
1
p-ERK1/2
ERK
0
0 ST
HIP
ST HIP
p-S6K
S6K
p-mTOR
mTOR
i
l
8
4
Basal p-S6K
DHPG-induced
increase of p-S6K
*
6
3
4
2
2
1
0
0 ST HIP ST HIP
j
m
DHPG-induced increase of p-mTOR
1.5
1.5
Basal p-mTOR
1
1
0.5
0.5
0
0 ST HIP ST HIP
Figure 7 | Abnormal accumulation of mGluR5 in PSD and altered mGluR5-Homer1 scaffolds in De422 / striatum. (a) Immunostaining for mGluR5 (red) and the postsynaptic marker PSD-95 (green) in / and / striata. Scale bar, 10 mm. (b) Co-localization analysis revealed a higher correlation
between mGluR5 and PSD-95 in the striatum of / mice; *Po0.05, t-test, n 24 slices from 3 mice/genotype. (c, left) Immunostaining of surface
mGluR5 (red), intracellular mGluR5 (blue), and DARPP-32 (green) in dissociated striatal MSNs. (right) Sample images of surface and intracellular mGluR5 immunostaining in dendrites and somata. (d,e) The ratio of surface/intracellular mGluR5 is decreased in dendrites (d), but is not different in somata of MSNs between genotypes; *Po0.01, t-test, n 14 cells from three mice/genotype. (f) Co-immunoprecipitation of mGluR5 and Homer1b/c revealed a
decreased association in / striatum. *Po0.01, t-test, n 4 mice/genotype (g) Immunoblots show that basal levels of p-ERK1/2 and p-S6K are
increased in / striatal slices. (hj) The basal levels of p-ERK1/2 (h) and p-S6K (i), but not p-mTOR (j) were signicantly increased in the striatum (ST)
of / mice. *Po0.05, two-tailed t-test. (km), DHPG had a signicant smaller effect on p-ERK1/2 (k) and p-S6K (l) levels in / ST, but the effect on
p-mTOR levels (m) was similar between genotypes. The net increase of phosphorylation for each kinase was calculated by normalizing DHPG-induced phosphorylation to corresponding basal phosphorylation. *Po0.05, two tailed t-test. For ERK, n 21 for each genotype in ST, n 11 for each genotype in
hippocampus (HIP). For S6K, n 16 for each genotype in ST, n 8 for each genotype in HIP. For mTOR, n 19 for each genotype in ST, n 8 for each
genotype in HIP. Experiments for western blots were repeated at least three times. All data expressed as meanss.e.m.
repetitive behaviours, impaired USV communication and aberrant social behaviours, as well as common comorbidities associated with SHANK3 deciency. The De422 / mice displayed only mild behavioural impairments, in contrast to the severe phenotype in SHANK3 haploinsufcient
patients15,16,18,19. The basis for this discrepancy is unknown, but a similar pattern has been seen in other mouse models of human haploinsufciency23,3739.
The De422 / mice displayed a spectrum of changes in PSD proteins, synaptic functions and behaviours distinct from
12 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 ARTICLE
a
b
c
d
250
#
3,000
#
3,000
NS
150
*
NS
#
Grooming duration (s)
Grooming duration (s)
2,500
*
2,500
200
*
NS
*
Total distance
2,000
2,000
100
1,500
Total distance
150
1,500
100
+/+
/
1,000
1,000
50
500
500
50
0 Vehicle
MPEP
0 Vehicle CDPPB
0 Vehicle
MPEP
0 Vehicle
CDPPB
e
40
f
g
h
+/+ Vehicle +/+ CDPPB / Vehicle / CDPPB
Lever presses per min
Vehicle CDPPB
30
Vehicle
CDPPB
16
16
30
14
+/+ Vehicle +/+ CDPPB / Vehicle / CDPPB
Head-in per min
Average lever press
14
Average head-in
12
20
12
*
10
20
10
8
8
6
*
10
6
10
4
4
2
2
0 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
0 +/+ /
0 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
0 +/+ /
i
/ aCSF (n = 8)
Min
j k
+/+ aCSF +/+ CDPPB / aCSF / CDPPB
* *
+/+ aCSF
/ CDPPB
+/+ CDPPB
Current (pA)
/ aCSF
% Baseline EPSC
60
2.0
#
Frequency (Hz)
100
+/+ aCSF (n = 8)+/+ CDPPB (10 M, n = 7)
Min
100
/ CDPPB (10 M, n = 8)
1.5
40
#
PPR
1.0
*
50
50
20
#
0.5
*
0.0
0 100
200
300
400
00
10
20
30
40
00
10
20
30
40
Baseline
After HFS
Baseline
After HFS
Baseline
After HFS
Baseline
After HFS
l
m
Vehicle
CDPPB
/
Learner
0.5
Learner
Non-learner
/
Vehicle
0.3
*
0.4
CDPPB
0.25
*
learner
Homer 1b/c
(normalized to +/+)
Homer 1b/c
(normalized to +/+)
Non-
0.3
0.2
0.15
0.2
Homer 1b/c
-tubulin III
+/+
Homer 1b/c
-tubulin III
+/+
0.1
0.1
0.05
0
0
Figure 8 | Behavioural phenotypes in De422 mice are rescued by mGluR5 modulators. (a,b) Reduced activity in open eld in De422 / ( / ) mice
was rescued by the mGluR5 antagonist MPEP (a) but not by the mGluR5 positive allosteric modulator CDPPB (b). (c,d) Increased self-grooming in the /
mice was attenuated by MPEP (c) and was exacerbated by CDPPB (d). *Po 0.05, / versus / ; #Po0.05, within genotype for treatment. For (a,c),
vehicle: / , n 10, / , n 12; MPEP: / , n 10, / , n 12. For b,d, vehicle: / , n 8, / , n 12; CDPPB: / , n 9, / , n 11.
(eh) CDPPB enhanced lever pressing (e,f) and head-entries into the magazine (g,h) across the 7 days in / mice. *Po 0.01, planned comparison: two-
tailed t-test. vehicle: / , n 8, / , n 12; CDPPB: / , n 9, / , n 11. (i) CDPPB does not alter LTD in MSNs of / mice (left). CDPPB
rescues LTD in MSNs of / mice (right). (j) HFS increased probability of PPR in / mice but not in / mice. *Po0.05, paired t-test, n 7, /
CDPPB, n 8 for the other groups. (k) CDPPB decreased spike frequency in response to current injection in the MSNs of / and / mice. *Po0.05, / aCSF versus CDPPB; #Po0.05, / aCSF versus CDPPB. Bonferroni post hoc comparison, n 12 each genotype. (l) Homer 1b/c protein levels in the
striatum are associated with lever press performance. (left) Sample images for western blot. (right) Homer quantication. / mice with more than one
press/min at last session are dened as learners and / mice with o0.1 press/min are dened as non-learners. *Po0.05, two-tailed t-test. n 5 for
learners, n 7 for non-learners. (m) CDPPB increased Homer 1b/c levels in striatum in / mice. (left) Sample images for western blot. (right) Homer 1b/c
quantication. *Po0.05, two-tailed t-test. n 9 for vehicle, n 8 for CDPPB. All data are expressed as meanss.e.m.
previously described lines of Shank3 mice23,24,26,27,36,37. For example, the isoform-specic De49 mice22 were more impaired in some social tests and in spatial learning and memory, whereas
alterations in anxiety/escape behaviours, grooming and striatal Homer1 protein levels were more apparent in De422 / mice.
The impaired striatal LTD, increased neuronal excitability and
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 13
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459
impaired striatal-dependent learning in the De422 / mice have not been reported in other lines of Shank3 mutants.
Behavioural proles among different lines of the mutant mice are also distinct. Although excessive grooming has been observed in other lines, skin lesions were reported only in De11, De1316, and De422, but not in De47, De49, De9 and De21 (refs 2026,40), suggesting that the severity of over-grooming is Shank3 isoform-specic.
The high levels of Shank3 expression in striatum may explain the prominent striatal defects found in De422 / mice21.
Changes in Homer1b/c and mGluR5 proteins were prominent in striatum, but not in neocortex and hippocampus. Furthermore, De422 / striatum had reduced spine density and attenuated
PSD structures that are consistent with the deciency of Shank3 and reduced Homer1 scaffolding proteins in this brain region. The decreased sEPSC frequency in De422 / MSNs, along with their reduced spine densities, suggests a loss of excitatory synapses. While the striatum receives glutamatergic inputs from both cortex and thalamus41, it is unclear whether both corticostriatal and thalamostriatal synapses are affected by the Shank3 deletion. We interpret the signicantly increased excitability of striatal De422 / MSNs as a homoeostatic compensation for reduced glutamatergic transmission, allowing spikes to be generated with less synaptic input. This increased excitability is consistent with our in vivo recordings, which show enhanced baseline functional connectivity between the cortex and striatum, yet reduced coupling between these areas during social interaction.
The De422 / mice also showed impaired striatal LTD, a form of synaptic plasticity associated with protein synthesis42.
While this impairment might reect reduced overall excitatory transmission, it may also be attributed to impaired mGluR5 signalling due to the disrupted mGluR5-Homer1 scaffolds, previously shown to be critical for striatal LTD43. This hypothesis is supported by the fact that impaired LTD in De422 / mice was completely rescued by CDPPB, a mGluR5 positive allosteric modulator. While CDPPB did not signicantly alter the probability of PPR, it elevated levels of Homer1b/c in the De422 / PSD, implying that disrupted mGluR5-Homer scaffolds might be a major contributor to the impaired LTD.
This decit in LTD could be linked to the observed decits in instrumental learning and other behavioural impairments, but not to the increased self-grooming in De422 / mice.
Importantly, striatal-dependent instrumental learning was nearly abolished. Since instrumental learning involves acquiring new responses based on reward feedback44,45, our ndings suggest a role for Shank3 in reward-related processes in general, including social rewards, potentially relevant to the pathophysiology of ASDs.
Abnormal brain connectivity has been proposed to underlie human autistic behaviours3,4,6, but the evidence is inconsistent due to both limitations in experimental design and heterogeneity of ASD presentation in humans. Mutant mice provide an experimentally tractable model to dissect the role of Shank3 in ASD-like behaviours. The overall brain volume and ventricular size were increased, consistent with the increased head circumference reported in a subset of SHANK3 patients19. The abnormal neural circuit function and behaviours seen in our De422 mice are reminiscent of the alterations in grey matter content and the integrity of white matter reported in SHANK3 patients. For example, enlargements in the basal ganglia found in De422 / mice correspond to reported enlargement in the neostriatum of human patients46. The De422 / mice have reduced cingulate cortices and enlarged deep mesencephalic nuclei, which receive inputs from the striatum and have projections to the ventral thalamus47. The changes in these
structures may underlie the abnormal functional connectivity within the cortico-stiratial-thalamic circuit revealed from our in vivo recordings.
Multi-site in vivo recordings allowed us to document hyperactivity in the cortico-striatal-thalamic axis in De422 / mice. These recordings bear similarities to those in human ASD patients9,10. The altered pattern of oscillations in the 711 Hz frequency band in De422 / mice corresponds to the abnormalities at 811 Hz reported in human ASDs subjects31.
As with our mice, hyperactivity in the anterior cingulatestriatal-thalamic circuits has been observed also in human subjects with ASDs7. A study of functional connectivity focusing on SHANK3-decient patients may yield important new insights.
The deciency of Shank3 in De422 mice caused a redistribution of Homer and mGluR5 in striatal neurons. The drastic changes for Homer1b/c (decrease in the PSD but increase in the soma) are consistent with a putative role of Shank3 in the synaptic trafcking of Homer1b/c, as previously suggested by in vitro studies4850. The increased mGluR5 but disrupted mGluR5-Homer scaffolds in the PSD (presumably secondary to the mislocalized Homer1b/c) suggest a bidirectional alteration in mGluR5-mediated functions at synapses. The reduced association of Homer1 and mGluR5 could result in the hypofunction of mGluR5-mediated signalling, whereas the increased PSD-localized mGluR5 could be associated with enhanced mGluR5 function. This hypothesis of bidirectional change is supported by the elevation of basal p-ERK and p-S6K levels with a correspondingly blunted DHPG response. Because of differences in signalling between the striatum and hippocampus, it appears that mGluR5-mediated signalling could be synapse-and cell type-specic. Future experiments will investigate these possibilities. The bidirectional changes in mGluR5 function may contribute to the differential striatal-related behavioural impairments observed. That excessive grooming behaviour is moderated with the mGluR5 antagonist MPEP, but augmented with the positive allosteric modulator CDPPB, suggests that enhanced synaptic mGluR5 function underlies the excessive grooming seen in the De422 / mice.
Conversely, partial rescue of lever-press by CDPPB raises the possibility that decreased mGluR5 function may contribute to the impaired instrumental learning.
Altered mGluR5 signalling has been documented in a mouse model of fragile X syndrome (FXS)5154; recent work suggests that decreased association of mGluR5 with Homer1b/c contributes to the pathogenesis of this syndrome55,56. While our results are reminiscent of the disrupted mGluR5-Homer1 scaffolds in FXS mice, there are also important differences. First, the mGluR5-Homer scaffolds in De422 / mice are disrupted by reduced synaptic Homer1b/c, while there is no evidence that Homer1 protein is reduced in FXS mice56; moreover, Homer1a-mediated function is enhanced in FXS mice55. Second, altered striatal mGluR5 function in De422 / mice may be bidirectional, with no apparent evidence of altered protein synthesis, whereas in the FXS mouse both mGluR5 signalling and protein synthesis at hippocampal synapses are enhanced52,53. Third, the De422 / mice show reduced striatal
LTD, while FXS mice have enhanced hippocampal LTD54. These differences suggest that mGluR5 dysfunction may lead to distinct phenotypes in different brain regions and/or different genetic backgrounds. Phenotypic diversity has also been observed in other mouse models bearing mGluR5 dysfunction such as mouse models for TSC57,58 and human 16p11.2 deletion59. Nevertheless, the mGluR5 dysfunction shared in these models suggests that the mGluR5-Homer scaffold may represent a convergent molecular pathway underlying ASDs pathophysiology.
14 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 ARTICLE
In summary, the present work suggests that many of the observed abnormalities in De422 / may be explained by disrupted mGluR5-Homer scaffolds and altered mGluR5 signalling in the striatum (Fig. 9). The deciency of Shank3 protein leads to redistribution of Homer1b/c and mGluR5 protein at the synapse and soma. Our results suggest this could be a pathophysiological mechanism for ASDs in humans. While our ndings indicate that these processes could be linked to distinct behavioural phenotypes and synaptic dysfunction, further studies will be required to establish exact mechanistic links among these molecular changes, behavioural impairments and neural circuitry dysfunction in De422 / mice.
Immunocytochemistry and confocal microscopy. Eight-week-old mice were transcardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde in 1 PBS (pH 7.4). The brains
were dissected and post-xed in the same xative overnight. The brains were then cryoprotected by submerging them in 30% sucrose for 48 h. After embedding into optimal cutting temperature compound (O.C.T.) on dry ice, brainswere sectioned on a cryostat at 40 mm thickness. Slices with brain regions of interest were used for further staining with corresponding antibodies. See details in Supplementary Methods.
Corticostriatal Co-cultured neurons and mGluR5 surface immunostaining.
Corticostriatal co-cultures were prepared from P1 littermate De422 / and
De422 / mice according to a method described previously63 (see details in
Supplementary Methods).
Antibodies. See details in Supplementary Methods
Brain slices preparation and DHPG treatment. Hippocampal and striatal slices were prepared from 4-week-old mice, anaesthetized with isourane. Brains were dissected rapidly and cut coronally at 400 mm in dissection buffer containing the following (in mM): 75 sucrose; 87 NaCl; 2.5 KCl; 1.25 NaH2PO4; 26 NaHCO3; 10 glucose; 7 MgCl2; and 0.5 CaCl2. Slices were allowed to recover at 32 C for 1 h in a submersion chamber containing oxygenated articial cerebrospinal uid (aCSF) consisting of the following (in mM): 124 NaCl; 3 KCl; 1.25 NaH2PO4; 26 NaHCO3;
20 glucose; 1 MgCl2; and 2 CaCl2. For DHPG treatment, slices were transferred into six-well plates with or without 100 mM DHPG for 5 min. After treatment, slices were transferred into a 1.5 ml tube and frozen on dry ice for further western blot analysis. Protein synthesis in the striatal slices was evaluated by measuring puromycin incorporation into newly synthesized proteins using a puromycin antibody as described52.
Golgi staining. Golgi staining was performed using 8-week-old mice as described22. See details in Supplementary Methods.
Electron microscopy. Electron microscopy was performed using 8-week-old mice as described22. See details in Supplementary Methods.
Mouse cohorts for the behavioural tests. Mice were tested in 10 cohorts of mixed sex De422 / , De422 / and De422 / littermates beginning at 2 mos of age (except for neonatal studies as described below) (Supplementary
Table 1). Many of the methods described below (some behavioural anaylses are described in Supplementary Methods) have been reported previously by our group, allowing us to determine appropriate numbers of mice per cohort to assess signicant differences22,64. Since some mice developed lesions across testing, they were euthanized according to Duke IACUC policy and thus were excluded from subsequent tests. All experimenters handling animals or scoring behavioural data were blinded to genotype throughout the studies. Mice in cohorts 16, 9,10 were housed on a 14:10 h light:dark cycle with most testing occurring during the light cycle. Mice in cohorts 7 and 8 were housed on a 12:12 light:dark cycle. Mice were housed 45 per cage unless otherwise specied.
Spray test. Individual animals were acclimated to clean home cages for 5 min before lming (MediaRecorder2, Noldus Information Technologies, Leesburg, VA, USA). Mice were lmed for 5 min and then lightly misted with tap water from a spray bottle. Filming continued for an additional 10 min. Grooming behaviour was scored using TopScan software (CleverSys, Reston, VA, USA) and veried by a blinded observer. Because of the differing length of pre- and post-spray responses, the data were reported as seconds spent in self-grooming per min observation.
Hole-board test. Mice were placed individually into a 42 42 30 cm open eld
and allowed free exploration of a hole-board apparatus (42 42 3 cm) (AccuScan
Instruments, Columbus, OH, USA) as described22. The hole-board was made of white plexiglass with 16 equally spaced holes (3 cm in diameter) arranged in four rows. Animals were lmed with a digital video camera and hand-scored by a blinded observer for the numbers of nose-pokes and the location of each nose-poke.
Juvenile nest choice. This test was conducted on postnatal day 15, when pups had opened their eyes and were capable of thermoregulation and goal-directed locomotor behaviour65. Testing occurred in two phases. For the rst phase, 2 cm2 nesting material was removed from the home-cage nest and placed at one end of a clean and empty mouse cage. Testing began when a single pup was placed at the end of the cage opposite to the nesting material, with the head of the pup facing away from the nest. The latency of the pup to turn and enter the home nest was scored. If the pup did not nd the home nest after 60 s, the latency was scored as 60 s. At the end of the test, pups were quickly removed and placed into a clean 8 oz. holding cup. The testing cage was cleaned of any urine or boli, and an equal amount of stranger nesting materials was placed in the test cage opposite that of the
Complete loss of Shank3
Synapse
Soma
Homer1b/c
mGluR5
Homer mGluR5
mGluR5 function
Instrumental learning
Self-grooming Thalamus
Enhanced connectivity
Neocortex
Enhanced connectivity
Basal ganglia
Other behavioural impairments
Figure 9 | A proposed model of the impairments in Shank3 completely decient mice. In De422 / ( / ) mice, the deciency in Shank3
resulted in an abnormal redistribution of Homer1b/c and mGluR5 in synapses and soma of MSNs. Abnormal Homer1b/c trafcking in striatum leads to a reduction at synapses and accumulation in the soma. The decreased mGluR5-Homer association due to the reduced Homer1b/c at synapses perturbs mGluR5-mediated functions, and this contributes to the impaired instrumental learning and striatal synaptic plasticity. By comparison, the increased mGluR5 at the synapses may lead to enhanced mGluR5 signalling which contributes to the augmented self-grooming, and possibly the augmented functional connectivity in the cortical-striatalthalamic axis. The behavioural phenotypes found in the Shank3 completely decient mice may reect a complex interaction among changes at molecular, synaptic and neural circuit levels.
Methods
Generation of Shank3 mice with deletion of exons 422 (De422). The targeting constructs were prepared using a previously described recombineering method60.The 129/SvEv BAC clone (bMQ457K21) covering the Shank3 gene was rst identied in silico using the Ensembl mouse genome browser (http://www.ensembl. org
Web End =www.ensembl. http://www.ensembl. org
Web End =org ) and the clone was obtained from Geneservice Ltd, UK61. Shank3De4 22
mice were generated by a two-step targeting strategy using the Cre-loxP system. The rst (50) construct inserted the loxP1 and loxP2 sites anking exons 49 with a neomycin cassette. The second (30) construct inserted the loxP3 at a 30-site5 kb-downstream of exon 22 with a puromycin cassette (Fig. 1a and Supplementary Fig. 1b). See details in Supplementary Methods. All experiments were conducted with protocols approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Duke University.
Preparation of crude PSD and cytosolic fractions. Isolation of crude PSDs was performed using a previously described protocol62 with some modications(see details in Supplementary Methods).
Quantitative immunoblot and co-immunoprecipitation analysis. See details in Supplementary Methods. Images for immunoblot have been cropped for presentation. Full-size images are presented in Supplementary Fig. 9.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 15
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459
home-cage material. For the second test, the pup was returned to the testing cage and placed between the two nesting samples and facing the side of the cage. Behaviour was scored for 60 s and the latency to make the rst choice (familiar home-cage nest or stranger nest) was recorded. All tests were video-recorded and later scored using the Noldus Observer software (Noldus).
Social dyadic test. Male mice were housed individually for 414 days before testing22,66. All testing was performed under red-light illumination (o5 lux) 26 h after onset of the dark cycle. Plexiglass test chambers (48 26 20 cm) were
cleaned between each test with LabSan 256CPQ solution (Sanitation Strategies LLC, Williamston, MI) and relled with 1/8 cob bedding (Andersen Inc., Maumee, OH, USA). Shank3 males were paired to non-familiar C3H/HeJ males (Jackson Labs, Stock No. 000659) of the same age and approximate weight. Each Shank3 mouse and its C3H partner were placed at opposite ends of test chamber and were separated by a solid partition. After 5 min, the barrier was removed and the animals were allowed to interact freely. All tests were lmed and the videos were scored by observers blinded to the genotype of the animals using the Observer XT 9 software (Noldus Information Technologies, Leesburg, VA). The ethogram for behavioural scoring consisted of 24 behaviours67 and were collapsed into two categories of behaviour: the total time spent in bidirectional social interaction, consisting of one mouse engaging the other and the other animal reciprocating the social behaviour; and total time spent in non-reciprocated interaction, wherein the Shank3 mouses responses were not reciprocated by the C3H partner. All animals were scored also for self-grooming. Only one animal engaged in ghting necessitating early termination of the experiment; this animal was excluded from analysis.
Pup ultrasonic vocalizations. The USVs of neonates were examined during brief maternal separation on postnatal day 4. USVs from individually-isolated pups were recorded using an externally polarized condenser microphone with a frequency range of 10200 kHz that was attached 1520 cm above the oor of an isolation chamber. The microphone was connected to the Avisoft-UltrasoundGate recording software (Avisoft Bioacoustics, Berlin, Germany) and the pup-emitted calls were recorded to WAV sound les using parameters optimized for mice22. Pups were individually placed in the sound-proof chambers and calls were recorded for 60 s. WAV les for each pup were converted to spectrograms and analysed with automated whistle tracking parameters by the Avisoft SASLab Pro software (Avisoft Bioacoustics).
Adult ultrasonic vocalizations. The De422 males were given 1014 days of reproductive experience with a female partner before recordings vocalizations. USVs were examined by exposing adult male De422 mice to a novel, 812 week-old C57BL/6J female (Jackson Labs). Recordings were performed as described for pups except the microphone was suspended 25 cm above the centre of the test chamber oor. Test mice were acclimated to the chambers for 1015 min. Next, a female was introduced and USVs were recorded for 5 min. Ultrasonic calls were recorded as described for pups. To avoid possible white-noise artifacts, frequencies outside the 25120 kHz range were truncated and not included in the analyses. To avoid other ultrasonic artifacts, sounds o2 ms in duration were also excluded from the analyses68. Mice which did not call were excluded from subsequent analyses of call duration, peak amplitude and peak frequency.
Open-eld activity. Spontaneous activity in the open eld was conducted over 1 h in an automated Omnitech Digiscan apparatus (AccuScan Instruments, Columbus, OH, USA). Accuscan software scored the total distance travelled, vertical activity (beam-breaks), and time spent in the centre zone.
Lightdark emergence test. Mice were placed into the darkened side (B2 lux) of a two-chambered apparatus (Med-Associates, St Albans, VT) and given 5 min to freely explore the darkened and lighted (B750 lux) chambers. Infrared diodes within the test chamber tracked the location and activity of the animal throughout testing. The scored behaviours comprised the latency to enter into the lighted chamber, and the numbers of crossings between the lighted and darkened chambers.
Reactivity in a novel environment. A cohort of D422 / mice engaged in frequent escape behaviours from the chambers during testing for novel object recognition memory. To determine whether they had enhanced reactivity to a novel environment, testing was conducted in three phases over 10 min each: observation in the home cage, observation in a new cage, and observation in the novel object recognition memory chamber (41 18 30 cm, opaque plexiglass). A
blinded observer recorded escape behaviours.
Rotarod performance. Rotarod (Med-Associates) performance was tested as described69. On day 1 the rod accelerated from 4 to 40 r.p.m. over 5 min, and mice were given four successive 5 min trials with an inter-trial interval of 30 min. Trials were terminated when the mouse fell from the rod or at 300 s. On day 2 the rod was
maintained at a steady speed of 16 r.p.m., and four trials were conducted in the same manner as on day 1.
Continuous reinforcement training. Mice were placed on a food deprivation schedule to reduce their body weights to B85% of normal. Water was available at all times in the home cages. Training took place in Med-Associates operant chambers housed within a light-resistant and sound-attenuating chamber.
The chamber was equipped with a food magazine that delivered 14 mg dustless precision food pellets (Bio-Serv, Flemington, NJ, USA) and with a retractable lever located on the left side of the magazine. A computer with the Med-PC-IV program was used to control the equipment and record behaviour. Lever-press training consisted of 7 days of continuous reinforcement (each press earned one food pellet). Each session began with illumination of the house light and insertion of the lever into the chamber, and terminated with extinguishing the house light and retraction of the lever after 60 min or after 100 earned pellets (whichever came rst). For CDPPB treatment, the pellet dispenser was replaced with a liquid dipper which delivered a drop of sweetened, condensed milk for each lever press.
Drug treatments. The mGluR5 antagonist MPEP (M5345) and the mGluR5 positive allosteric modulator CDPPB (SML0235) were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St Louis, MO, USA). MPEP was dissolved in 0.9% normal saline; CDPPB was dissolved in 0.5% methylcellulose. MPEP (20 mg kg 1) or CDPPB (10 mg kg 1) were administered by intraperitoneal injection 30 min before behavioural testing.
Magnetic resonance histology. Brains of De422 / mice (N 7) and control
C57BL/6J (N 7, aged 6074 days) were actively stained, using a transcardiac
perfusion protocol; after ushing out the blood with PBS, the tissue was xed using a 10% formalin solution with 5 mM gadoteridol (ProHance, Bracco Diagnostics Inc.). Specimens were stored in formalin overnight then rehydrated in a PBS
ProHance (1:200) solution until imaging. Brain specimens were imaged within the cranium to avoid distortions and damage, using high eld (9.4T) MRH, with a DTI protocol consisting of 1 baseline (b 0) and diffusion in six directions (non
collinear diffusion gradient vector directions [1, 1, 0], [1, 0, 1], [0, 1, 1], [ 1, 1, 0],
[1, 0, 1] and [0, 1, 1]), using b values of 1,495 s mm 2, with TE-11.82 ms,
TR 100 ms, 512 256 256 matrix, 22 11 11 mm eld of view, diffusion
pulse amplitude 1,000 ms m 1, separation 5.76 ms, width 1.3 ms, at isotropic resolution of 43 mm. Images were processed to construct tensors and DTI parametric maps using the Diffusion Toolkit (http://trackvis.org/dtk/
Web End =http://trackvis.org/dtk/). Image segmentation and analysis were described in Supplementary Methods.
Whole-cell patch-clamp recording from brain slices. Acute striatal brain slices were prepared from adult mice (25 mos) using a Vibratome (see detail in Supplementary Methods). Recordings were made from MSNs in the DLS, identied visually with the aid of differential interference contrast-enhanced visual guidance. For voltage-clamp recordings, cells were clamped at 70 mV throughout the
experiments. Synaptic currents were recorded with an Axopatch 1D amplier (Axon Instruments), ltered at 5 kHz, and digitized at 10 kHz. EPSC amplitudes were examined using peak detection software in pCLAMP10 (Molecular devices). Slices were maintained at a temperature between 28 and 30 C and were stable within 1 C during the experiment.
For evoked EPSCs, test stimuli were delivered via a Master-8 stimulator (A.M.P.I., Jerusalem, Israel) at a frequency of 0.05 Hz through a bipolar twisted tungsten wire placed into the striatum. The duration and intensity of the stimulation was adjusted so that the size of the evoked EPSCs was 200500 pA. We analysed only recordings with series resistance o20 MO. The series resistance was not compensated, and if it changed more than 20% during the recording, the cell was not included in the analysis.
For current injection experiments, the recordings were performed in current clamp mode. Each step was 100 pA with a duration of 1,000 ms, and the number of spikes for each depolarizing step was counted. EPSC amplitudes were examined using peak detection software in pCLAMP10 (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, CA, USA). The HFS protocol used to induce LTD was paired with depolarization of the postsynaptic cell to 0 mV and it consisted of four 1 s, 100 Hz trains delivered every 10 s (ref. 42). For the effects of CDPPB on sEPSCs and LTD, 10 mm (nal concentration) of CDPPB was added in aCSF external solution for whole-cell patch clamp recording.
Multi-circuit in vivo recording. Three- to four- mos old De422 / mice and their De422 / littermate controls were anaesthetized with ketamine(100 mg kg 1) and xylazine (10 mg kg 1), placed in a stereotaxic device, and metal ground screws were secured to the skull above the cerebellum and at the anterior cranium. A total of 32 tungsten microwires were arranged in bundle arrays of 412 wires (each wire separated by at least 250 mm), and implanted as follows (listed as AP, ML, DV; all coordinates are measured in mm from bregma, except DV which is from dura).: V_HIP: 3.4, 3, 3.25; THAL: 1.5, 0.35, 2.88; NAC:
1.25, 1.15, 3.9; PRL_CX: 1.7, 0.25, 1.25; CG_CX: 1.98, 0.25, 0.75.
16 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 ARTICLE
Neurophysiological experiments were initiated following a 3-week recovery and were conducted during the light cycle. Full details of the procedures for electrode construction and surgical implantation have been previously described70. Neuronal activity was sampled at 30 kHz, high-pass ltered at 250 Hz, sorted online, and stored using the Cerebus acquisition system (Blackrock Microsystems Inc., Salt Lake City, UT, USA). Neuronal data were referenced online against a wire within the same brain area that did not exhibit a signal-to-noise ratio greater than 3:1. At the end of the recording, cells were sorted again using an ofine sorting algorithm (Plexon Inc., TX) to conrm the quality of the isolated cells. LFPs were low-pass ltered at 250 Hz and stored at 1,000 Hz. A second 0.5 Hz high-pass lter was applied before neurophysiological analysis. All neurophysiological recordings were referenced to a ground wire connected to both ground screws. Wires tested from the two screws were iso-electric, demonstrating that ground loops were not introduced by this design.
LFP oscillatory power and cross-area coherence. See details in Supplementary
Methods.
LFP phase analysis. See details in Supplementary Methods.
Forced social interaction test. Test mice were habituated under a wire cage (Galaxy Utility Cup, http://www.kitchen-plus.com
Web End =www.kitchen-plus.com ) for 30 min in their home-cage. The wire cage was then transferred to a plexiglass chamber (dimensions: 40 cm 23
cm 23 cm). Following an additional 5 min habituation period, neurophysiolo
gical activity was recorded for 10 min. An unfamiliar C3H/HeJ mouse (Jackson Labs) was introduced into the arena 5 min after the beginning of the 10 min neurophysiological recording.
General statistical analyses. The data were analysed with SPSS 21 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) or Microsoft Excel and expressed as meanss.e.m. Simple comparisons between Shank3De422 / and Shank3De422 / mice without regards to sex were conducted with independent t-tests. In cases where comparisons among the 3 genotypes were made or were related to test conditions, analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests were used. When comparisons between genotypes were made for within-subject measurements across different phases of the same test (for example, test days, locations within a test arena, or different intensities of stimuli), the data were analysed with repeated measures ANOVA (RMANOVA). When comparisons between genotypes of different groups (that is, sex, age or treatment condition) were made, two- or three-way ANOVA were used. For one-way ANOVAs, Fishers LSD method was used for the post hoc comparisons; for RMANOVAs and multi-factor ANOVAs, a Bonferroni-correction for multiple comparisons was applied. Statistical signicance was dened as Po0.05.
Statistical analyses of in vivo data is described in Supplementary Methods. Detailed statistics for all the results can be seen in Supplementary Data 1.
References
1. De Rubeis, S. et al. Synaptic, transcriptional and chromatin genes disrupted in autism. Nature 515, 209215 (2014).
2. Pinto, D. et al. Convergence of genes and cellular pathways dysregulated in autism spectrum disorders. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 94, 677694 (2014).
3. Kana, R. K., Libero, L. E. & Moore, M. S. Disrupted cortical connectivity theory as an explanatory model for autism spectrum disorders. Phys. Life Rev. 8, 410437 (2011).
4. Geschwind, D. H. & Levitt, P. Autism spectrum disorders: developmental disconnection syndromes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 103111 (2007).5. Minshew, N. J. & Keller, T. A. The nature of brain dysfunction in autism: functional brain imaging studies. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 23, 124130 (2010).
6. Uddin, L. Q., Supekar, K. & Menon, V. Reconceptualizing functional brain connectivity in autism from a developmental perspective. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 458 (2013).
7. Di Martino, A. et al. The autism brain imaging data exchange: towards a large-scale evaluation of the intrinsic brain architecture in autism. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 659667 (2014).
8. Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A. & Minshew, N. J. Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain 127, 18111821 (2004).
9. Supekar, K. et al. Brain hyperconnectivity in children with autism and its links to social decits. Cell Rep. 5, 738747 (2013).
10. Keown, C. L. et al. Local functional overconnectivity in posterior brain regions is associated with symptom severity in autism spectrum disorders. Cell Rep. 5, 567572 (2013).
11. Jeremy Willsey, A. & State, M. W. Autism spectrum disorders: from genes to neurobiology. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 30C, 9299 (2015).
12. Bey, A. L. & Jiang, Y. H. Overview of mouse models of autism spectrum disorders. Curr. Protoc. Pharmacol. 66, 5 66 6165 66 26 (2014).
13. Jiang, Y. H. & Ehlers, M. D. Modeling autism by SHANK gene mutations in mice. Neuron 78, 827 (2013).
14. Durand, C. M. et al. Mutations in the gene encoding the synaptic scaffolding protein SHANK3 are associated with autism spectrum disorders. Nat. Genet. 39, 2527 (2007).
15. Leblond, C. S. et al. Meta-analysis of SHANK mutations in autism spectrum disorders: a gradient of severity in cognitive impairments. PLoS Genet. 10, e1004580 (2014).
16. Moessner, R. et al. Contribution of SHANK3 mutations to autism spectrum disorder. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 81, 12891297 (2007).
17. Wang, X., Xu, Q., Bey, A. L., Lee, Y. & Jiang, Y. H. Transcriptional and functional complexity of Shank3 provides a molecular framework to understand the phenotypic heterogeneity of SHANK3 causing autism and Shank3 mutant mice. Mol. Autism 5, 30 (2014).
18. Soorya, L. et al. Prospective investigation of autism and genotype-phenotype correlations in 22q13 deletion syndrome and SHANK3 deciency. Mol. Autism 4, 18 (2013).
19. Sarasua, S. M. et al. Clinical and genomic evaluation of 201 patients with Phelan-McDermid syndrome. Hum. Genet. 133, 847859 (2014).
20. Bozdagi, O. et al. Haploinsufciency of the autism-associated Shank3 gene leads to decits in synaptic function, social interaction, and social communication. Mol. Autism 1, 15 (2010).
21. Peca, J. et al. Shank3 mutant mice display autistic-like behaviours and striatal dysfunction. Nature 472, 437442 (2011).
22. Wang, X. et al. Synaptic dysfunction and abnormal behaviors in mice lacking major isoforms of Shank3. Hum. Mol. Genet. 20, 30933108 (2011).
23. Schmeisser, M. J. et al. Autistic-like behaviours and hyperactivity in mice lacking ProSAP1/Shank2. Nature 486, 256260 (2012).
24. Kouser, M. et al. Loss of predominant Shank3 isoforms results in hippocampus-dependent impairments in behavior and synaptic transmission. J. Neurosci. 33, 1844818468 (2013).
25. Lee, J. et al. Shank3-mutant mice lacking exon 9 show altered excitation/ inhibition balance, enhanced rearing, and spatial memory decit. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 9, 94 (2015).
26. Duffney, L. J. et al. Autism-like decits in Shank3-decient mice are rescued by targeting actin regulators. Cell Rep. 11, 14001413 (2015).
27. Zhou, Y. et al. Mice with Shank3 mutations associated with ASD and schizophrenia display both shared and distinct defects. Neuron 89, 147162 (2016).
28. Speed, H. E. et al. Autism-associated insertion mutation (InsG) of Shank3 exon 21 causes impaired synaptic transmission and behavioural decits. J. Neurosci. 35, 96489665 (2015).
29. Bell, R. W., Nitschke, W. & Zachman, T. A. Ultra-sounds in three inbred strains of young mice. Behav. Biol. 7, 805814 (1972).
30. Yin, H. H. et al. Dynamic reorganization of striatal circuits during the acquisition and consolidation of a skill. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 333341 (2009).
31. Khan, S. et al. Local and long-range functional connectivity is reduced in concert in autism spectrum disorders. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 31073112 (2013).
32. Insel, T. R. & Fernald, R. D. How the brain processes social information: searching for the social brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 697722 (2004).
33. Allan Johnson, G., Badea, A. & Jiang, Y. Quantitative neuromorphometry using magnetic resonance histology. Toxicol. Pathol. 39, 8591 (2011).
34. Jiang, Y. & Johnson, G. A. Microscopic diffusion tensor imaging of the mouse brain. Neuroimage 50, 465471 (2010).
35. Yin, H. H. & Knowlton, B. J. Contributions of striatal subregions to place and response learning. Learn. Mem. 11, 459463 (2004).
36. Darnell, J. C. & Klann, E. The translation of translational control by FMRP: therapeutic targets for FXS. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 15301536 (2013).
37. Won, H. et al. Autistic-like social behaviour in Shank2-mutant mice improved by restoring NMDA receptor function. Nature 486, 261265 (2012).
38. Jerome, L. A. & Papaioannou, V. E. DiGeorge syndrome phenotype in mice mutant for the T-box gene, Tbx1. Nat. Genet. 27, 286291 (2001).
39. Penagarikano, O. et al. Absence of CNTNAP2 leads to epilepsy, neuronal migration abnormalities, and core autism-related decits. Cell 147, 235246 (2011).
40. Yang, M. et al. Reduced excitatory neurotransmission and mild autism-relevant phenotypes in adolescent Shank3 null mutant mice. J. Neurosci. 32, 65256541 (2012).
41. Doig, N. M., Moss, J. & Bolam, J. P. Cortical and thalamic innervationof direct and indirect pathway medium-sized spiny neurons in mouse striatum.J. Neurosci. 30, 1461014618 (2010).42. Yin, H. H., Davis, M. I., Ronesi, J. A. & Lovinger, D. M. The role ofprotein synthesis in striatal long-term depression. J. Neurosci. 26, 1181111820 (2006).
43. Sung, K. W., Choi, S. & Lovinger, D. M. Activation of group I mGluRs is necessary for induction of long-term depression at striatal synapses.J. Neurophysiol. 86, 24052412 (2001).44. Balleine, B. W., Delgado, M. R. & Hikosaka, O. The role of the dorsal striatum in reward and decision-making. J. Neurosci. 27, 81618165 (2007).
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 17
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459
45. Yin, H. H., Ostlund, S. B. & Balleine, B. W. Reward-guided learning beyond dopamine in the nucleus accumbens: the integrative functions of cortico-basal ganglia networks. Eur. J. Neurosci. 28, 14371448 (2008).
46. Langen, M., Durston, S., Kas, M. J., van Engeland, H. & Staal, W. G. The neurobiology of repetitive behavior:...and men. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 356365 (2011).
47. Rodriguez, M., Abdala, P., Barroso-Chinea, P. & Gonzalez-Hernandez, T. The deep mesencephalic nucleus as an output center of basal ganglia: morphological and electrophysiological similarities with the substantia nigra. J. Comp. Neurol. 438, 1231 (2001).
48. Sala, C., Roussignol, G., Meldolesi, J. & Fagni, L. Key role of the postsynaptic density scaffold proteins Shank and Homer in the functional architecture of Ca2 homeostasis at dendritic spines in hippocampal neurons. J. Neurosci. 25,
45874592 (2005).49. Hayashi, M. K. et al. The postsynaptic density proteins Homer and Shank form a polymeric network structure. Cell 137, 159171 (2009).
50. Tu, J. C. et al. Coupling of mGluR/Homer and PSD-95 complexes by the Shank family of postsynaptic density proteins. Neuron 23, 583592 (1999).
51. Ronesi, J. A. & Huber, K. M. Homer interactions are necessary for metabotropic glutamate receptor-induced long-term depression and translational activation.J. Neurosci. 28, 543547 (2008).52. Bhattacharya, A. et al. Genetic removal of p70 S6 kinase 1 corrects molecular, synaptic, and behavioral phenotypes in fragile X syndrome mice. Neuron 76, 325337 (2012).
53. Michalon, A. et al. Chronic pharmacological mGlu5 inhibition corrects fragile X in adult mice. Neuron 74, 4956 (2012).
54. Huber, K. M., Gallagher, S. M., Warren, S. T. & Bear, M. F. Altered synaptic plasticity in a mouse model of fragile X mental retardation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 77467750 (2002).
55. Ronesi, J. A. et al. Disrupted Homer scaffolds mediate abnormal mGluR5 function in a mouse model of fragile X syndrome. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 431440 S431 (2012).
56. Giuffrida, R. et al. A reduced number of metabotropic glutamate subtype 5 receptors are associated with constitutive homer proteins in a mouse model of fragile X syndrome. J. Neurosci. 25, 89088916 (2005).
57. Chevere-Torres, I. et al. Metabotropic glutamate receptor-dependent long-term depression is impaired due to elevated ERK signaling in the DeltaRG mouse model of tuberous sclerosis complex. Neurobiol. Dis. 45, 11011110 (2012).
58. Auerbach, B. D. & Bear, M. F. Loss of the fragile X mental retardation protein decouples metabotropic glutamate receptor dependent priming of long-term potentiation from protein synthesis. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 10471051 (2010).
59. Tian, D. et al. Contribution of mGluR5 to pathophysiology in a mouse model of human chromosome 16p11.2 microdeletion. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 182184 (2015).
60. Liu, P., Jenkins, N. A. & Copeland, N. G. A highly efcient recombineering-based method for generating conditional knockout mutations. Genome Res. 13, 476484 (2003).
61. Adams, D. J. et al. A genome-wide, end-sequenced 129Sv BAC library resource for targeting vector construction. Genomics 86, 753758 (2005).
62. Carlin, R. K., Grab, D. J., Cohen, R. S. & Siekevitz, P. Isolation and characterization of postsynaptic densities from various brain regions: enrichment of different types of postsynaptic densities. J. Cell Biol. 86, 831845 (1980).
63. Chen, M. et al. Sapap3 deletion anomalously activates short-term endocannabinoid-mediated synaptic plasticity. J. Neurosci. 31, 95639573 (2011).
64. Rodriguiz, R. M., Colvin, J. S. & Wetsel, W. C. Neurophenotyping genetically modied mice for social behavior. Methods Mol. Biol. 768, 343363 (2011).
65. Roubertoux, P. L. et al. Pre-weaning sensorial and motor development in mice transpolygenic for the critical region of trisomy 21. Behav. Genet. 36, 377386 (2006).
66. Rodriguiz, R. M., Chu, R., Caron, M. G. & Wetsel, W. C. Aberrant responses in social interaction of dopamine transporter knockout mice. Behav. Brain Res. 148, 185198 (2004).
67. Nehrenberg, D. L. et al. An anxiety-like phenotype in mice selectively bred for aggression. Behav. Brain Res. 201, 179191 (2009).
68. Holy, T. E. & Guo, Z. Ultrasonic songs of male mice. PLoS Biol. 3, e386 (2005).
69. Ribar, T. J. et al. Cerebellar defects in Ca2 /calmodulin kinase IV-decient
mice. J. Neurosci. 20, RC107 (2000).70. Dzirasa, K., Fuentes, R., Kumar, S., Potes, J. M. & Nicolelis, M. A. Chronic in vivo multi-circuit neurophysiological recordings in mice. J. Neurosci. Methods 195, 3646 (2011).
Acknowledgements
We thank Ute Hochgeschwender, Yi Qi, Gary Cofer, Yoonji Lee, Susan Burette and Xinyu Cao for technical support. X.W. was supported by a fellowship from the Phelan-McDermid Syndrome Foundation. R.J.W. was supported by NIH grant NS039444. A.L.B. was supported by the Ruth K. Broad and Autism Science Foundations and Duke MSTP T32 GM007171. Some of the behavioural experiments were conducted with equipment and software purchases with a grant from the North Carolina Biotechnology Center. Y.-h.J. was supported by NIH Grants MH098114, HD077197, MH104316 as well as awards from Autism Speaks, the Ruth K. Broad Foundation and Roche. K.D. is also supported by NIH grant MH104316. H.Y. was supported by NIH Grant AA021074. A.B. was supported by NIH K01 AG041211-01A1 and 2P41 EB015897-25. Y.-h.J., K.D. and W.C.W. were supported by an incubator award from the Duke Institute for Brain Science.
Author contributions
X.W. and Y.-h.J. conceived this project. X.W., Q.X., S.Z., F.W. and L.J.D. did the molecular experiments. B.M.K., S.K., S.D.M., L.K.D., S.D.M. and K.D. performed neurophysiology experiments and analysed the acquired data. V.H., N.K., C.Y., L.C. and H.Y. conducted slice electrophysiology. A.B. conducted MRH experiments and data analysis. J.-D.D. and R.J.W. performed EM studies. K.D. and H.Y. contributed equally. A.L.B., X.W., S.W.H., N.D., C.Y., B.M.K., E.G., R.M.R. and W.C.W. designed and conducted the behavioural studies; A.L.B. and R.M.R. did the statistical analyses. X.W., A.L.B., R.J.W., H.Y., A.B., K.D., W.C.W. and Y.-h. J. wrote the manuscript.
Additional information
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =http://www.nature.com/ http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =naturecommunications
Competing nancial interests: The authors declare no competing nancial interests.
Reprints and permission information is available online at http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =http://npg.nature.com/ http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =reprintsandpermissions/
How to cite this article: Wang, X. et al. Altered mGluR5-Homer scaffolds and corticostriatal connectivity in a Shank3 complete knockout model of autism. Nat. Commun. 7:11459 doi: 10.1038/ncomms11459 (2016).
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articles Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if the material is not included under the Creative Commons license, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to reproduce the material. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Web End =http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
18 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 7:11459 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms11459 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Nature Publishing Group May 2016
Abstract
Human neuroimaging studies suggest that aberrant neural connectivity underlies behavioural deficits in autism spectrum disorders (ASDs), but the molecular and neural circuit mechanisms underlying ASDs remain elusive. Here, we describe a complete knockout mouse model of the autism-associated Shank3 gene, with a deletion of exons 4-22 (Δe4-22). Both mGluR5-Homer scaffolds and mGluR5-mediated signalling are selectively altered in striatal neurons. These changes are associated with perturbed function at striatal synapses, abnormal brain morphology, aberrant structural connectivity and ASD-like behaviour. In vivo recording reveals that the cortico-striatal-thalamic circuit is tonically hyperactive in mutants, but becomes hypoactive during social behaviour. Manipulation of mGluR5 activity attenuates excessive grooming and instrumental learning differentially, and rescues impaired striatal synaptic plasticity in Δe4-22-/- mice. These findings show that deficiency of Shank3 can impair mGluR5-Homer scaffolding, resulting in cortico-striatal circuit abnormalities that underlie deficits in learning and ASD-like behaviours. These data suggest causal links between genetic, molecular, and circuit mechanisms underlying the pathophysiology of ASDs.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer