OPEN
Citation: Transl Psychiatry (2016) 6, e811; doi:10.1038/tp.2016.64
http://www.nature.com/tp
Web End =www.nature.com/tp
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Cognitive and oculomotor performance in subjects with low and high schizotypy: implications for translational drug development studies
I Koychev1,2, D Joyce3, E Barkus4,5, U Ettinger6, A Schmechtig7, CT Dourish8, GR Dawson8, KJ Craig8 and JFW Deakin1
The development of drugs to improve cognition in patients with schizophrenia is a major unmet clinical need. A number of promising compounds failed in recent clinical trials, a pattern linked to poor translation between preclinical and clinical stages of drug development. Seeking proof of efcacy in early Phase 1 studies in surrogate patient populations (for example, high schizotypy individuals where subtle cognitive impairment is present) has been suggested as a strategy to reduce attrition in the later stages of drug development. However, there is little agreement regarding the pattern of distribution of schizotypal features in the general population, creating uncertainty regarding the optimal control group that should be included in prospective trials. We aimed to address this question by comparing the performance of groups derived from the general population with low, average and high schizotypy scores over a range of cognitive and oculomotor tasks. We found that tasks dependent on frontal inhibitory mechanisms (N-Back working memory and anti-saccade oculomotor tasks), as well as a smooth-pursuit oculomotor task were sensitive to differences in the schizotypy phenotype. In these tasks the cognitive performance of low schizotypes was signicantly different from high schizotypes with average schizotypes having an intermediate performance. These results indicate that for evaluating putative cognition enhancers for treating schizophrenia in early-drug development studies the maximum schizotypy effect would
be achieved using a design that compares low and high schizotypes.
Translational Psychiatry (2016) 6, e811; doi:10.1038/tp.2016.64; published online 17 May 2016
INTRODUCTIONCognitive impairment is a core symptom of schizophrenia that predicts functional outcome13 and treatment adherence.4 However, it is largely unaffected by currently available medication5 and the development of drugs to treat cognitive decits in schizophrenia is a recognised unmet need.6
Although a number of putative cognitive enhancers have been examined in trials none have been approved for treatment.7 Lack of clinical efcacy was the main reason for attrition revealing a critical gap in translating efcacy evidence from animal models to patients. One strategy to address this is to assess efcacy at Phase 1 of drug development and discontinue development if no efcacy is demonstrated thus saving time and resources from Phase 2 and 3 trials.8 Such experimental or translational medicine studies use biomarkers of the proposed core pathophysiology as proxies of efcacy rather than traditional clinical end-points to shorten the time needed to reach Go/No-Go decisions.9 This approach can be complemented by selecting either patients who are more likely to respond (mild symptomatology or medication-free) or subclinical healthy volunteers that lack the confounds of patient groups (for example, heterogeneous symptom and medication proles).
In the case of schizophrenia, early assessment of cognitive enhancers efcacy can be sought in surrogate patient populations
such as individuals with high levels of schizotypy.1012 Schizotypy is a term coined by Meehl13 under the inuence of Rado14 and is used to describe a latent personality structure associated with schizophrenia risk with the affected individuals referred to as schizotypes.13 The severity of these features range along a continuum that funnels into overt psychosis at its extreme.1517
Schizophrenia and schizotypy share a common genetic basis1820
and have a number of neurobiological similarities.21,22 These
encompass changes in grey23,24 and white25,26 matter morphology
but also neurobiological function,2730 including cognition.24,3134 Various lines of evidence point to a relative weakness of frontal inhibitory mechanisms being a key unifying feature of the schizophrenia spectrum nervous system, a phenomenon described in patients as far back as Bleuler.35 In schizotypy this manifests itself in impaired performance on inhibitory tasks (for example, negative priming (NP),36,37 lateral inhibition38 and the anti-saccade eye-movement task39,40) as well as working memory
(WM) tests33,4144 where inhibition of competing stimuli is crucial for successfully maintaining and manipulating information online. Fine motor control is another neurocognitive function dependent on frontal input that denes schizophrenia spectrum: schizotypal individuals,45,46 as well as patients47 have difculties
performing tasks such as smooth eye pursuit where the eyes are guided to follow a moving target. Despite these similarities high
1Department of Community-Based Psychiatry, Neuroscience and Psychiatry Unit, The University of Manchester, School of Community-Based Medicine, Manchester, UK;
2Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK; 3Cognition, Schizophrenia and Imaging Lab, Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, Denmark Hill, London; 4Department of Psychology, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, NSW, Australia; 5Department of Psychiatry, School of Community-Based Medicine, The University of Manchester, Manchester, UK; 6Department of Psychology, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany; 7Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK and 8P1vital, Wallingford, UK. Correspondence: Dr I Koychev, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Warneford Hospital, Warneford Lane, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK.
E-mail: mailto:[email protected]
Web End [email protected] Received 2 November 2015; revised 17 February 2016; accepted 19 February 2016
Cognitive and oculomotor performance across schizotypy I Koychev et al
2
schizotypes (HS) are generally spared psychotic episodes, repeated hospitalisations and chronic antipsychotic treatment.48
Importantly, for cognitive experiments intelligence quotient (IQ) and educational levels are within the normal population range.49
The combination of schizophrenia spectrum neurobiology and lack of confounds makes HS important surrogates for establishing early evidence of cognitive enhancer efcacy.10,11
Standardised questionnaires such as the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ),50 can be used to identify schizotypy psychometrically in the general population.51,52 While the SPQ
was designed to identify Schizotypal Personality Disorder (SPD) according to DSM-III-R, in practice high SPQ scorers tend to represent a broader schizotypy group with only ~ 55% of them attracting a diagnosis of SPD.50 A number of other scales are available5358 but the SPQ is preferred due to the well-established normative data.59,60 A methodological issue with psychometrically identied schizotypy, however, is that cognitive dysfunction may not be readily demonstrable as a meta-analysis in college students reported.61 This is an important pragmatic consideration as experimental medicine studies depend on reliable, well dened group differences in performance to merit inclusion in the drug development process.62,63 A potential route towards maximising the potential return on investment in an experimental medicine approach is to investigate whether a particular comparator group yields stronger effects in comparisons with schizotypes. To date large schizotypy studies have focussed on recruiting subjects with average schizotypy scores as comparators reasoning that individuals with low scores may exhibit atypical performance on cognitive tasks.61,62 This reects the existence of two alternative
theories describing the relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia. A quasi-dimensional framework proposes that ~ 10% of the general population have key neural changes (schizotaxia) that manifest in the development of schizophrenia or extreme schizotypal symptoms.17,64 According to this model, there is no difference in the neurocognitive performance of healthy volunteers with the exception of individuals with extreme schizotypal symptoms. In contrast a fully dimensional framework proposes that schizotypal personality traits and its associated neurocognitive changes exist on a continuum from healthy individuals to patients with schizophrenia.65,66 According to this model, average schizotypes (AS) have an intermediate cognitive performance relative to HS and low schizotypes (LS).
In the present study, we aimed to establish whether individuals with low or average schizotypy should be used as comparators in studies evaluating putative cognitive enhancers in HS. Thus, we compared the cognitive performance of low, average and HS on a range of cognitive measures (testing inhibitory function and motor control). Based on recent evidence supporting the fully dimensional framework of schizotypy,6769 we hypothesised that LS would be the comparator group that would yield the more robust statistical difference in comparison with HS.
MATERIALS AND METHODS Subjects, study criteria and design
The study included three groups of participants based on their schizotypy scores as measured by the SPQ (HS, AS and LS dened as scores of 441, 2136 and 9, respectively). SPQ was chosen as pilot studies had conrmed the sensitivity of SPQ to the factors of interest and enabled us to power the present study to detect signicant results.59 The HS and AS were recruited within a larger, multi-center study investigating the effects of risperidone, amisulpride and nicotine on cognition in schizotypy. LS were recruited specically for the purposes of the current analysis.
As part of the procedures of the larger study, AS and HS were recruited from three sites in the United Kingdom (Manchester, London and Cardiff) via an online version of the SPQ in its short70 and full version.50 The online questionnaire was advertised via university research volunteering e-mails, social networking sites and advertisements in local media. Participants were screened by telephone for signicant mental health and medical
history. Included participants were invited to a screening appointment at which consent was obtained and the full SPQ was completed again (regardless of whether they had completed the short or full SPQ online). Participants were assigned to the AS or HS groups on the basis of the screening visit full SPQ score only. Other inclusion criteria at the screening visit were age between 18 and 45 years, uency in the English language, no relevant medical history and body mass index in the range of 1830. Exclusion criteria were history or presence of mental health disorders (screened for using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview71),
daily consumption of more than ve cigarettes or eight standard caffeinated drinks, history of migraines, signicant visual or hearing impairment. Participants meeting the inclusion criteria were invited to a separate day of testing. On the day, following randomisation, a placebo or nicotine (7 mg) patch was applied to the participants' forearm. Three hours later, a capsule of either amisulpride (400 mg), risperidone (2 mg) or placebo was administered. Approximately 1.5 h later, the participants were tested using a battery of neuropsychological and eye-movement tasks. It consisted of (i) two WM tests (a verbal one and a non-verbal one) based on robust evidence for WM impairment in the schizophrenia spectrum;33,4144
(ii) a verbal uency (VF) task based on the observation that frontal cortex abnormalities are preferentially affected in the schizophrenia spectrum;27,28,36,37,72 (iii) an anti-saccade eye-movement task on the basis
of it tapping inhibitory mechanisms and evidence for abnormality in the schizophrenia spectrum;39,72,73 (iv) a smooth-pursuit eye-movement
(SPEM) task on the basis of consistent evidence for smooth-pursuit abnormalities in schizophrenia and schizotypy.4547 The current study included only participants that were treated with both a placebo patch and a placebo capsule (27 HS and 31 AS). Full details of the study from which these subgroups were selected are detailed in previous publications.62,63
A group of LS was recruited in Manchester only using the online database from the three-centre study described above. Participants attended a single appointment where they provided consent, completed the SPQ and were included if their score was 9 and if they met the same inclusion and exclusion criteria as described above. Included participants completed the same testing battery as the AS and HS groups, but received no treatment. About 35 participants were recruited of which 5 were excluded (4 due to SPQ49 and 1 participant for having a relevant medical history). Participant demographics are presented in Table 1.
Task descriptionsWM (N-Back) task. Series of letters (measuring 4x4 visual degrees) were presented centrally on a monitor. Each letter remained on the screen for 1 s and a blank screen of the same duration separated the stimuli. Participants were instructed to respond by pressing a key when they saw a letter identical to a preceding one with a varying number of letters between the two target letters. The two targets followed each other in the 1-Back condition; they were separated by one and two letters in the 2-Back and 3-Back conditions respectively. There were also blocks to control attention to the task where participants needed to respond when they saw a target letter (0-Back). Before starting the task, participants completed a practice run (one block of each condition). During the task, 3 blocks of each condition were completed (0-Back, 1-Back, 2-Back and 3-Back) with the 12 blocks presented pseudo-randomly. In each block 14 stimuli were presented pseudo-randomly, so that there were 3 target and 11 non-target images. Overall, for every WM load there was a maximum of 9 correct answers and 33 opportunities for errors of commission (that is, opportunities to respond to an incorrect stimulus). For the purposes of statistical analysis, we extracted (i) percentage of correct responses and (ii) number of errors of commission for the 1-Back, 2-Back and 3-Back conditions, yielding a total of six variables.
Spatial WM task. A number of treasure chests were presented on a monitor against a background and responses were recorded using a computer mouse. The number of coins and chests were equal in each trial with only one chest containing a coin at any one time. Participants searched for all available coins by clicking on the chests and were instructed not to choose targets they had already found coins. A practice run with three chests was completed, followed by three blocks of four, six and eight treasure chests, respectively (four task repetitions within each block). Between each repetition, the conguration of the boxes on the page was altered so that the likelihood of stereotypical searching strategies was reduced. The outcome variables for each difculty level (four, six and eight chests) were: (i) average number of within trial errors (selecting a chest previously searched within that trial) and (ii) average
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 8
Cognitive and oculomotor performance across schizotypy I Koychev et al
3
Table 1. Summary of the GLMs
Variable Schizotypy group contrast Estimate/coefcient s.e. P-value
Years of education High vs average 0.808 0.572 0.162
Low vs average 1.915 0.557 0.001 Low vs high 1.107 0.577 0.058 Smooth pursuit velocity gain (0.25 Hz) High vs average 0.021 0.032 0.78
Low vs average 0.096 0.031 0.005 Low vs high 0.075 0.032 0.053 Smooth pursuit velocity gain (0.50 Hz) High vs average 0.063 0.072 0.654
Low vs average 0.213 0.070 0.006 Low vs high 0.150 0.073 0.099 Anti-saccade mean percentage errors High vs average 13.676 6.266 0.074
Low vs average 7.169 6.080 0.466 Low vs high 20.844 6.366 0.003 3-Back mean number of commission errors High vs average 0.647 0.291 0.068
Low vs average 0.155 0.283 0.848 Low vs high 0.801 0.296 0.019
Abbreviation: GLM, generalised linear model. GLMs were constructed for each relevant variable from the canonical correlation analysis, with the three-level ordinal variable schizotypy group predicting the measured task performance variable. Contrast estimates for relative levels of schizotypy (rather than beta coefcients) and s.e. are shown. Signicant results are shown highlighted in bold for condence level Po0.05.
number of between trial errors (selecting a chest previously searched within that task repetition) or a total of six variables.
VF task. Letter and category VF were assessed during a succession of 1-min periods. In the letter VF task test, participants were asked to name as many words as they could beginning with the letters F, A and S (FAS condition). Names, places or numbers, as well as repetitions were categorised as errors. In the category swap VF test, participants were asked to come up with words from two categories (fruit and furniture) while alternating between them. Only words consistent with the respective categories were recorded as correct. The outcome variables for this task were (i) mean number of correct words across the FAS condition, (ii) number of correct transitions in the category swap condition. Total number of VF variables was 2.
Oculomotor tasksRecording: Eye movements were recorded at 1000 Hz sampling rate (EyeLink 1000, SR Research, Kanata, ON, Canada). Participants were seated 57 cm away from a 17-inch monitor with their head resting on a chinrest. The target was a 0.3 diameter black dot presented on a light grey background.
Anti-saccade task: A nine-point calibration and a practice trial were carried out before the beginning of the task. A trial started with the black dot in the central position of the screen (0) for a random duration of 10002000 ms. The target then jumped to one of four possible peripheral positions (7.25, 14.5) for 1000 ms. Each of the possible locations was used 15 times in a random order resulting in 60 trials in total. Participants were required to look at the target in the centre and then to direct their gaze at the exact mirror image position as fast and accurately as possible when the target jumped to either side.
SPEM task: In the SPEM task, the target moved horizontally across the screen (from 14.5 to +14.5) in a sinusoidal waveform at three different target velocities (0.25, 0.5 and 0.75 Hz). It started its movement from 14.5. For each of the target velocities, the target completed 10 full cycles. Participants were asked to keep their eyes on the target as closely as possible.
Analysis: Anti-saccade data were analysed using EyeLink DataViewer software (SR Research). Saccades were dened as having minimum amplitude of 1 and minimum latency of 100 ms. Three variables were extracted from the data: (i) saccade latency; (ii) directional error rate describing the percentage of error trials (that is, trials where the participants rst saccade was towards the target) divided by the total number of valid trials (that is, error trials plus correct trials, excluding eye blink trials); (iii) saccade amplitude.
Two dependent variables were derived for the SPEM analysis using LabView (National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA) for each of the three speeds (six variables in total). Mean eye velocity was divided by target velocity to yield time-weighted average velocity gains. The analysis included sections in the central 50% of the ramp excluding saccades and eye blink. Saccadic frequency (N per second) for each velocity was also measured across the entire pursuit task at each velocity.
Intelligence quotientIQ was determined using the National Adult Reading Test,74 a reading-based estimate of premorbid intelligence. Participants were asked to pronounce irregularly spelt words from a standardised written list. Individual IQ scores were calculated on the basis of the number of correctly pronounced words.
Statistical analysis demographic variablesThe three groups were compared in terms of their demographic variables (age; premorbid IQ (NART); years of education) and SPQ scores using oneway analyses of variance.
Statistical analysis test batteryResearch aiming to identify biomarkers of disorder by examining cognitive performance on a battery of tests yields a number of dependent variables, often as a function of the number of experimental conditions/stimuli manipulations.
In such exploratory studies, this yields a large number (6 30 = 180) of potential associations that very often exceeds the number of participants. Traditionally, this problem is addressed by choosing a candidate univariate-dependent variable and relevant predictors and a model is constructed using multiple-variable regression. This process is repeated for all dependent variables of interest, leading to the potential problem of accounting for Type II false discovery errors at the hypothesis testing stage.75,76
An alternative approach is to use data-driven multivariate methods such as principal component analysis to expose the latent structure in a single set of multivariate data, by nding a set of basis vectors onto which observations are projected. This yields a lower dimensionalmore parsimoniousrepresentation that explains the largest amount of variance in the data. While this may reduce the number of regression models required, it is agnostic to relationships between the predictors and measurements and there is no a priori guarantee that the reduced set of variables will be signicantly smaller than the original.
To address these potential issues, we used a two-stage approach. There were two sets of multivariate data, X and Y, representing sets of all observations of the P = 6 predictors (schizotypy group, study site, sex, years of education, predicted IQ score and age) and q = 23 candidate biomarkers, respectively. Rather than search for separate reduced-dimensional representations of X and Y independently (that is, using principal component analysis), we rst used canonical correlation analysis (CCA)77 combined with relevance network detection to extract relationships of interest.78 CCA
is a technique whereby two sets of multivariate data (in this context, representing multiple demographic and clinical predictors and multiple cognitive battery testing outcomes) can be simultaneously reduced in dimension, exposing any latent structure while maintaining the maximum correlation between the reduced-dimensionality representations. The standard linear regression model is a special case of this method.77 The
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 8
Cognitive and oculomotor performance across schizotypy I Koychev et al
4
Figure 1. (a) Relevance network extracted from complete data set (dotted linesnegative correlation between variables; solid linespositive correlation between variables; numbers indicate direction and strength of association); corresponding generalised linear models (GLMs) for ordinal schizotypy group predicting mean number of commission errors on 3-Back task; (b) mean percentage errors for anti-saccade task; (c) mean pursuit velocity gain ( per second) at target velocities of 0.25 and 0.5 Hz; (d) signicant multiple general linear hypothesis tests (Tukey contrasts) for low, average and high schizotypy are show with P-values for each GLM.
advantage of CCA is that dimensionality reduction of both X and Y is performed simultaneously while preserving maximal correlations between observations of X and Y projected onto these reduced representations. The CCA algorithm is applied iteratively, so that the rst applicationthe leading canonical variatesrepresents linear combinations of observations in Y best predicted by linear combinations of observations in X. Subsequent applications (revealing the trailing canonical variates) are less predictive and can be discarded because they explain less of the data; analogous to extracting principle components by only keeping the largest eigenvectors.79
The computed canonical variates of X and Y are labelled UL and VL, respectively, with L = [1, 2, 3, , min(p, q)]. We chose the rst two or three of these canonical variates and then computed another set of equiangular vectors between UL and VL such that ZL = UL+VL. Each observation in X and Y was then projected onto ZL (by inner products), which is equivalent to a correlation measure between each observation in X and Y with the vectors Z. Then a similarity scorerepresenting associationwas computed by summing over these projections.78 These scores were then visualised showing a network of variates in X and Y with the similarity scores representing association strength. By adjusting a threshold T = [0,1] over these scores, the visualised network is made simpler or more complex by only showing higher association (as T approaches unity) or by allowing more liberal thresholds (as T approaches zero), respectively. Those variables which appear in the relevance network are then candidates for further analysis in this instance using regression analyses. To illustrate the effect of the ordinal variables on the biomarker variables, contrasts were computed using Tukeys Honest Signicant Difference (HSD) test for multiple comparisons.
Ethical approvalThe study was approved by The University of Manchester Ethics committee (reference number 08176) and carried out in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki.
RESULTSDemographicsThe three groups did not differ signicantly in terms of age (F (2,85) = 1.454, P = 0.24), IQ (F(2,85) = 0.043, P = 0.96) and gender (LS group 13 f and 14 m; AS group 16 f and 15 m; HS group 15f and 15 m; 2(2) = 0.026, P = 0.98). The years of education model was statistically signicant (F(2,85) = 5.949, P = 0.004), which was due to the LS group having signicantly more years of education relative to the AS group but not the HS group (Table 1).
Cognitive tests analysisWe conducted the exploratory CCA on all six demographic variables. Of the measured biomarker variables, we removed those which were of no interest, that is, cognitive performance at the control (0-Back) condition of the N-Back task. To avoid difculties with matrix calculations and data imputation involved in CCA, we only included participants with complete data sets on all demographic and biomarker variables, leading to the inclusion of n = 83 participants to derive the relevance network (ve LS participants excluded). The nal sample size was LS 22, AS 31 and HS 30.
Using the rst L = [1,2,3] canonical variates and a liberal threshold T = 0.35, we were able to derive a relevance network, which revealed associations that might be usefully subjected to regression analyses to quantify association of a given demographic variable (inX) with its associated biomarker (in Y) shown in Figure 1a.
Of all the demographic variables (schizotypy group, study site,
sex, years of education, predicted IQ score and age), in X, schizotypy group was the only variable showing associations in the relevance network. Years of education had relatively weak
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 8
Cognitive and oculomotor performance across schizotypy I Koychev et al
5
associations, which did not survive the T threshold. For the candidate biomarkers, Y, the schizotypy group relevance network showed negative associations for velocity gain on the (i) 0.25 Hz and (ii) 0.50 Hz sinusoid smooth-pursuit task and positive associations between (iii) mean percentage errors on the oculomotor anti-saccade task and (iv) mean number of commission errors on the 3-Back WM task. The relevance network did not demonstrate signicant associations between schizotypy and the Spatial WM or VF variables.
To explore the quantitative relationship between the relevant variables, generalised linear models were constructed for schizotypy group (as an ordinal variable) and the 3-Back commission errors (Figure 1b), the mean percentage errors on the anti-saccade task (Figure 1c) and the ratio of velocity gain scores for the two SPEM variables (Figure 1d). To illustrate the effect of schizotypy group (low, average and high) on the biomarker variables, contrasts were computed using Tukeys HSD for multiple comparisons (Table 2). The results conrmed the observed main effect of schizotypy group and demonstrated that LS vs HS comparisons were more consistently statistically signicant relative to AS vs HS comparisons. With respect to the anti-saccade error rate and N-Back errors of commission, the LS vs HS comparison was statistically signicant while the AS vs HS comparison approached signicance. In the SPEM models, the LS group performed signicantly better than the AS group and the LS vs HS comparisons were signicant at trend level. In contrast there was no signicant difference between AS and HS groups in terms of SPEM performance.
DISCUSSIONThe objective of this study was to clarify the relationship between levels of schizotypy and neurocognition by comparing the cognitive performance of three groups of healthy volunteers dened by their SPQ scores. Participants were assessed on a range of neuropsychological and eye-movement tests that are abnormal across the schizophrenia spectrum to establish whether LS or AS is the optimal control group for comparison with HS in translational studies of cognitive enhancers.
Of the candidate biomarker variables in the ve tasks, those that measured inhibitory control and had sufcient difculty, were most sensitive to schizotypy. For example, in N-Back, only errors of commission (dependent on the ability to inhibit responses to irrelevant stimuli) at the most difcult condition (3-Back) showed HS performing worse than LS. In contrast, the groups did not differ in percentage of correctly identied N-Back stimuli or any spatial WM variables. Similarly, in the anti-saccade task (where inhibitory control is crucial) HS made signicantly more errors relative to LS and AS. In the SPEM task, only the 0.25 and 0.50 Hz conditions were sensitive to schizotypy levels, but importantly this effect was evident only in comparisons between LS and HS. In the 0.75 Hz condition, the groups did not signicantly differ suggesting that all participants found this condition difcult. The follow-up regression models conrmed that for each of the four variables, where the CCA model identied a schizotypy effect, the LS vs HS comparisons showed more robust and signicant effects relative to AS vs HS (Table 2).
These data indicate that differences in performance on cognitive and oculomotor tasks are largest between LS and HS groups. This is consistent with the fully dimensional model of schizotypy,65 which has recently been supported by convergent lines of evidence.80 For example, the latent structure of schizotypy personality features were found to t a dimensional distribution in a taxomeric analysis.69 In addition, psychotic-like experiences (hallucinations and delusional ideation) in the general population t a continuous distribution.67,68 These ndings are of particular
relevance to translational studies seeking evidence for cognitive enhancer efcacy, that is, enrichment of schizotypy studies is best
achieved by comparing LS and HS groups while using tasks that maximise the extent of cognitive difference between these groups. The potential utility of such enrichment is highlighted by our nding that on a number of measures HS were indistinguishable from the control groups. This argues that psychometrically dened schizotypes are a high functioning population, a conclusion consistent with the practice of schizotypy studies recruiting participants among university and college students. Also while some studies do not nd difference in cognition between HS and controls (either LS or AS) abnormal functional imaging response is demonstrable.21 This demonstrates the risks involved in using a single task to assess cognition in this
Table 2. Demographics and descriptives
Variable Low schizotypy
Average schizotypy
High schizotypy
Mean s.d. Mean s.d. Mean s.d.
DemographicsSPQ score 3.8 3.1 27.8 4 50.1 6.1 Age 25.3 5.0 23.8 4.6 25.4 5.8 Premorbid IQ (NART) 114.2 6.2 114.2 4.9 113.8 6.0 Years of education 16.3 2.2 15.5 1.7 17.4 2.6
N-Back percentage correct1-Back 99.6 0.8 99.5 0.7 97.8 0.8
2-Back 92.2 2.6 96.2 2.5 94.7 2.8
3-Back 86.7 2.3 83.3 2.3 78.1 2.5
N-Back errors of commission1-Back 0.1 0.1 0.1 0.1 0.3 0.1
2-Back 0.1 0.1 0.0 0.1 0.2 0.1
3-Back 0.1 0.2 0.7 0.2 1.4 0.2
Spatial working memory between search errorsLoad 1 0.1 0.1 0.1 0.1 0.0 0.1 Load 2 0.7 0.2 0.5 0.2 0.5 0.2 Load 3 1.0 0.4 2.0 0.4 2.0 0.4
Spatial working memory: within search errorsLoad 1 0.0 0.0 0.1 0.0 0.0 0.0 Load 2 0.2 0.1 0.1 0.1 0.3 0.1 Load 3 0.6 0.3 0.5 0.3 0.4 0.3
Verbal uency: mean number correct wordsFAS task 14.6 4.0 15.5 4.3 13.7 3.6 Category swap task 7.7 1.4 8.0 1.5 7.1 1.3
Verbal uency: mean number correct transitionsCategory swap task 6.8 0.3 7.7 0.3 7.4 0.3
Anti-saccadeErrors 28.2 17.1 36.2 24.2 49.5 26.3 Amplitude gain 119.4 23.5 122.2 28.0 116.1 37.1 Peak velocity 295.8 47.3 302.3 70.9 298.4 63.4
Smooth pursuit eye-movement task: velocity gain0.25 Hz velocity 97.1 4.7 89.9 10.2 91.24 8.70.50 Hz velocity 84.2 14.4 78.9 15.9 82.3 14.40.75 Hz velocity 67.4 20.7 61.1 23.2 67.3 20.1
Smooth pursuit eye-movement task: saccadic frequency0.25 Hz velocity 0.8 0.4 0.9 0.4 0.8 0.30.50 Hz velocity 1.4 0.4 1.5 0.4 1.6 0.50.75 Hz velocity 2.3 0.5 2.2 0.6 2.5 0.7
Abbreviations: FAS task (a verbal uency task - see Materials and methods); IQ, intelligence quotient; NART, National Adult Reading Test; SPQ, Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire.
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 8
Cognitive and oculomotor performance across schizotypy
I Koychev et al
6 high functioning group. A popular approach in schizophrenia6 and Alzheimers disease81 research, and adopted for this study, is to use a battery of tests.62,63,82 However, this strategy risks increasing
the false positive rate through multiple comparisons.75,76 We
sought to address this issue by performing a canonical analysis77
which identied the variables where a signicant effect of schizotypy was present thereby greatly reducing the number of post hoc comparisons. We propose that similar analyses should be used in the initial work-up of battery data sets.
The nding of preferential impairment in measures of inhibitory processes is consistent with evidence in schizotypy,22 while inhibitory control decits have been proposed as core features of schizophrenia.35 Several tests have been developed to probe inhibition directly (for example, NP and lateral inhibition tasks). In NP, participants are instructed to attend to previously inhibited distractors. Normal inhibitory function leads to increased reaction times to the previously ignored stimulus. In both patients with schizophrenia and schizotypes, this delay is diminished.36,37,83
Lateral inhibition is based on the observation that conditioning to a stimulus is slower if it has previously been presented without consequence. Similar to NP, the period of conditioning is shortened in schizophrenia patients84 and schizotypes.72,85,86 Successful inhibition is critical for complex cognitive functions such as WM, which has emerged as a potential endophenotype of schizophrenia.87 This is based on studies reporting subtle WM impairment in individuals with genetic88,89 and
phenomenological90,91 vulnerability to schizophrenia. The currently reported N-Back performance in schizotypes adds to a growing body of evidence showing WM impairment in schizotypy.33,41,44 Our data also highlight that not all WM tasks
are sensitive to schizotypy. Instead it appears that elements directly relevant to inhibition are more useful in this high functioning group.
Failure of the HS group to inhibit saccades in the anti-saccade task also replicates previous ndings.39,40,46,73,92 This abnormality
is again shared between HS and schizophrenia patients47 and was
recently shown to be dependent on a neural circuitry encompassing putamen, thalamus, cerebellum and visual cortex.72 We
also found that saccade gain during SPEM was lower in HS, a nding replicating previous studies.45,46 The ability to follow a
visual object moving at constant speed depends on a frontoparieto-occipital network, which projects an expected target trajectory to the motor cortex.93 The generated prediction is then continuously corrected with occipital cortex tracking data. Evidence regarding the neural basis of smooth-pursuit impairment in schizotypy comes from a fMRI study demonstrating lower BOLD occipital signal.94 This nding is compatible with reports of abnormal occipitally generated evoked responses in schizotypy32
and schizophrenia.9597 It is known that sensory cortex is under frontal cortex modulatory control98 and it may be that a primary dysfunction in these afferents underlies the observed occipital decit.99
LimitationsThere are several limitations of this exploratory analysis. Firstly, the
AS and HS groups were recruited as part of a larger placebo-controlled study which investigated the effects of acute administration of risperidone, amisulpride and nicotine on cognition. AS and HS participants were required to attend two appointments (treatment was given, and data on treatment effects on cognitive performance were obtained, on the second, full-day appointment), were placebo-treated and were recruited from three sites in the UK. In contrast, the LS group attended a single 3-h appointment in which the data were recorded, did not receive placebo medication and were recruited in Manchester only. We therefore cannot rule out the possibility that the superior performance of the LS group was due to these protocol
differences. It is feasible that fatigue or placebo effects could have led to reduced performance in AS relative to LS groups. However, the superior performance of the LS group was conned solely to measures where a difference between the placebo-treated AS and HS groups was already evident and for which compelling evidence for an abnormality in the schizophrenia spectrum already exists. The lack of schizotypy effect on other tasks argues against a generalised placebo or fatigue effects-driven cognitive superiority in the LS group. Secondly, AS group had a signicantly lower number of years of education compared with the LS group. We accounted for this signicant difference by including years of education as a predictor in the canonical testing and the results showed that this variable did not have a signicant independent effect on the biomarker variables. In addition, the groups did not differ in terms of projected IQ, a measure arguably more relevant to cognition research than years of education which tends to reect socio-economic status. Finally, we did not have information on the family history of schizophrenia among the participantsit is possible that the groups were different in this respect.
CONCLUSIONIn summary, this study explored the neurocognitive correlates of low, average and high schizotypy. The results revealed an impairment that primarily affected inhibitory processes in HS subjects. Importantly we found that the performance of AS subjects in these tests was intermediate between that of LS and HS subjects. These results argue in favour of the fully dimensional theory of schizotypy and strongly suggest that subjects with LS are the most appropriate control group for further experimental medicine schizotypy studies with putative cognition enhancing agents for the treatment of cognitive decits in schizophrenia.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
IK has been awarded a Manchester Strategic PhD Studentship, sponsored by the University of Manchester and P1vital Ltd. The PhD project of AS was partly funded by P1vital Ltd. UE is supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Et 31/2-1). JFWD has been supported by the Manchester Biomedical Research Centre. JFWD has carried out paid consultancy work and speaking engagements for Servier, Merck Sharpe and Dohme, Astrazeneca, Janssen, and Eli Lilly. The fees are paid to the University of Manchester to reimburse them for the time taken. JFWD, GRD and CTD own shares in P1vital Ltd. EB acknowledges the support of a Young Investigator Award from NARSAD. KJC worked for P1vital Ltd at the time that this research was conducted. He is currently an employee and shareholder in Covance Inc. DJ declares no relevant conicts of interest.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The Manchester and Cardiff sites received facility and staff support by the Manchester and Cardiff Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Facilities, respectively.
REFERENCES
1 Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, Mintz J. Neurocognitive decits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the "right stuff"? Schizophr Bull 2000; 26: 119136.
2 Hofer A, Baumgartner S, Bodner T, Edlinger M, Hummer M, Kemmler G et al. Patient outcomes in schizophrenia II: the impact of cognition. Eur Psychiatry 2005; 20: 395402.
3 Milev P, Ho BC, Arndt S, Andreasen NC. Predictive values of neurocognition and negative symptoms on functional outcome in schizophrenia: a longitudinal rst-episode study with 7-year follow-up. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162: 495506.
4 Burton SC. Strategies for improving adherence to second-generation anti-psychotics in patients with schizophrenia by increasing ease of use. J Psychiatr Pract 2005; 11: 369378.
5 Heinrichs RW. The primacy of cognition in schizophrenia. Am Psychol 2005; 60: 229242.
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 8
Cognitive and oculomotor performance across schizotypy I Koychev et al
7
6 Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Kern RS, Baade LE, Barch DM, Cohen JD et al. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165: 203213.
7 Hurko O. Future drug discovery and development. Mol Genet Metab 2010; 100: S92S96.
8 Hurko O. The uses of biomarkers in drug development. Ann NY Acad Sci 2009; 1180: 110.
9 Kuhlmann J, Wensing G. The applications of biomarkers in early clinical drug development to improve decision-making processes. Curr Clin Pharmacol 2006; 1: 185191.
10 Dourish CT, Dawson GR. Precompetitive consortium approach to validation of the next generation of biomarkers in schizophrenia. Biomarker Med 2014; 8: 58.11 Koychev I, Barkus E, Ettinger U, Killcross S, Roiser JP, Wilkinson L et al. Evaluation of state and trait biomarkers in healthy volunteers for the development of novel drug treatments in schizophrenia. J Psychopharmacol 2011; 25: 12071225.
12 Dawson GR, Goodwin G. Experimental medicine in psychiatry. J Psychopharmacol
2005; 19: 565566.
13 Meehl PE. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1962; 46: 935944.
14 Rado S. Dynamics and classication of disordered behavior. Am J Psychiatry 1953; 110: 406416.
15 Lenzenweger MF. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, and schizophrenia: Paul E. Meehl's blueprint for the experimental psychopathology and genetics of schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 2006; 115: 195200.
16 Tsuang MT, Stone WS, Tarbox SI, Faraone SV. An integration of schizophrenia with schizotypy: identication of schizotaxia and implications for research on treatment and prevention. Schizophr Res 2002; 54: 169175.
17 Meehl PE. Schizotaxia revisited. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989; 46: 935944.18 Calkins ME, Curtis CE, Grove WM, Iacono WG. Multiple dimensions of schizotypy in rst degree biological relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Bull 2004; 30: 317325.19 Tarbox SI, Pogue-Geile MF. A multivariate perspective on schizotypy and familial association with schizophrenia: a review. Clin Psychol Rev 2011; 31: 11691182.20 Yaralian PS, Raine A, Lencz T, Hooley JM, Bihrle SE, Mills S et al. Elevated levels of cognitive-perceptual decits in individuals with a family history of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr Res 2000; 46: 5763.21 Ettinger U, Meyhofer I, Steffens M, Wagner M, Koutsouleris N. Genetics, cognition, and neurobiology of schizotypal personality: a review of the overlap with schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry 2014; 5: 18.22 Ettinger U, Mohr C, Gooding DC, Cohen AS, Rapp A, Haenschel C et al. Cognition and brain function in schizotypy: a selective review. Schizophr Bull 2015; 41: S417S426.23 Kuhn S, Schubert F, Gallinat J. Higher prefrontal cortical thickness in high schizotypal personality trait. J Psychiatr Res 2012; 46: 960965.24 Raine A, Sheard C, Reynolds GP, Lencz T. Pre-frontal structural and functional decits associated with individual differences in schizotypal personality. Schizophr Res 1992; 7: 237247.25 Nelson MT, Seal ML, Phillips LJ, Merritt AH, Wilson R, Pantelis C. An investigation of the relationship between cortical connectivity and schizotypy in the general population. J Nerv Ment Dis 2011; 199: 348353.26 Volpe U, Federspiel A, Mucci A, Dierks T, Frank A, Wahlund LO et al. Cerebral connectivity and psychotic personality traits. A diffusion tensor imaging study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2008; 258: 292299.27 Corlett PR, Fletcher PC. The neurobiology of schizotypy: fronto-striatal prediction error signal correlates with delusion-like beliefs in healthy people. Neuropsychologia 2012; 50: 36123620.28 Corlett PR, Murray GK, Honey GD, Aitken MR, Shanks DR, Robbins TW et al. Disrupted prediction-error signal in psychosis: evidence for an associative account of delusions. Brain 2007; 130: 23872400.29 Kumari V, Antonova E, Geyer MA. Prepulse inhibition and "psychosis-proneness" in healthy individuals: an fMRI study. Eur Psychiatry 2008; 23: 274280.30 Kumari V, Gray JA, Geyer MA, ffytche D, Soni W, Mitterschiffthaler MT et al. Neural correlates of tactile prepulse inhibition: a functional MRI study in normal and schizophrenic subjects. Psychiatry Res 2003; 122: 99113.31 Cochrane M, Petch I, Pickering AD. Aspects of cognitive functioning in schizotypy and schizophrenia: evidence for a continuum model. Psychiatry Res 2012; 196: 230234.32 Koychev I, El-Deredy W, Haenschel C, Deakin JF. Visual information processing decits as biomarkers of vulnerability to schizophrenia: an event-related potential study in schizotypy. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48: 22052214.33 Park S, Holzman PS, Lenzenweger MF. Individual differences in spatial working memory in relation to schizotypy. J Abnorm Psychol 1995; 104: 355363.34 Keefe RS, Harvey PD. Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Handb Exp Pharmacol 2012; 213: 1137.
35 Bleuler E. Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias. International Universities Press: New York, NY, USA, 1950.
36 Park S, Lenzenweger MF, Pueschel J, Holzman PS. Attentional inhibition in schizophrenia and schizotypy: a spatial negative priming study. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 1996; 1: 125149.
37 Peters ER, Pickering AD, Hemsley DR. 'Cognitive inhibition' and positive symptomatology in schizotypy. Br J Clin Psychol 1994; 33: 3348.
38 Evans LH, Gray NS, Snowden RJ. A new continuous within-participants latent inhibition task: examining associations with schizotypy dimensions, smoking status and gender. Biol Psychol 2007; 74: 365373.
39 Ettinger U, Kumari V, Crawford TJ, Flak V, Sharma T, Davis RE et al. Saccadic eye movements, schizotypy, and the role of neuroticism. Biol Psychol 2005; 68: 6178.
40 Gooding DC. Anti-saccade task performance in questionnaire-identied schizo-types. Schizophr Res 1999; 35: 157166.
41 Gooding DC, Tallent KA. Spatial, object, and affective working memory in social anhedonia: an exploratory study. Schizophr Res 2003; 63: 247260.
42 Park S, McTigue K. Working memory and the syndromes of schizotypal personality. Schizophr Res 1997; 26: 213220.
43 Schmidt-Hansen M, Honey RC. Working memory and multidimensional schizotypy: dissociable inuences of the different dimensions. Cogn Neuropsychol 2009; 26: 655670.
44 Tallent KA, Gooding DC. Working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizotypic individuals: a replication and extension. Psychiatry Res 1999; 89: 161170.
45 Gooding DC, Miller MD, Kwapil TR. Smooth pursuit eye tracking and visual xation in psychosis-prone individuals. Psychiatry Res 2000; 93: 4154.
46 Holahan AL, O'Driscoll GA. Antisaccade and smooth pursuit performance in positive- and negative-symptom schizotypy. Schizophr Res 2005; 76: 4354.
47 Levy DL, Sereno AB, Gooding DC, O'Driscoll GA. Eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia: characterization and pathophysiology. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 2010; 4: 311347.
48 Raine A, Lencz T, Mednick SA. Schizotypal Personality. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1995.
49 Raine A. Schizotypal personality: neurodevelopmental and psychosocial trajectories. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2006; 2: 291326.
50 Raine A. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull 1991; 17: 555564.
51 Avramopoulos D, Stefanis NC, Hantoumi I, Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Stefanis CN. Higher scores of self reported schizotypy in healthy young males carrying the COMT high activity allele. Mol Psychiatry 2002; 7: 706711.
52 Lenzenweger MF, O'Driscoll GA. Smooth pursuit eye movement and schizotypy in the community. J Abnorm Psychol 2006; 115: 779786.
53 Mason O, Claridge G. The Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (O-LIFE): further description and extended norms. Schizophr Res 2006; 82: 203211.
54 Peters ER, Joseph SA, Garety PA. Measurement of delusional ideation in the normal population: introducing the PDI (Peters et al. Delusions Inventory). Schizophr Bull 1999; 25: 553576.
55 Rust J. The rust inventory of schizotypal cognitions (RISC). Schizophr Bull 1988; 14: 317322.
56 Stefanis NC, Hanssen M, Smirnis NK, Avramopoulos DA, Evdokimidis IK, Stefanis CN et al. Evidence that three dimensions of psychosis have a distribution in the general population. Psychol Med 2002; 32: 347358.
57 Eyseneck HJ. Eyseneck SBG Manual of the Eyseneck Personality Scales (EPS Adult). Hodder & Stoughton: London, UK, 1991.
58 Chapman LJ, Kwapil TRScales for the measurement of schizotypy. In: Raine A, Lencz T, Medinck SA(eds) Schizotypal Personality. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1995 pp 79106.
59 Koychev I, McMullen K, Lees J, Dadhiwala R, Grayson L, Perry C et al. A validation of cognitive biomarkers for the early identication of cognitive enhancing agents in schizotypy: a three-center double-blind placebo-controlled study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2012; 22: 469481.
60 Stefanis NC, Smyrnis N, Avramopoulos D, Evdokimidis I, Ntzoufras I, Stefanis CN. Factorial composition of self-rated schizotypal traits among young males undergoing military training. Schizophr Bull 2004; 30: 335350.
61 Chun CA, Minor KS, Cohen AS. Neurocognition in psychometrically dened college Schizotypy samples: we are not measuring the "right stuff". J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2013; 19: 324337.
62 Koychev I, El-Deredy W, Mukherjee T, Haenschel C, Deakin JF. Core dysfunction in schizophrenia: electrophysiology trait biomarkers. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2012; 126: 5971.
63 Schmechtig A, Lees J, Grayson L, Craig KJ, Dadhiwala R, Dawson GR et al. Effects of risperidone, amisulpride and nicotine on eye movement control and their modulation by schizotypy. Psychopharmacology 2013; 227: 331345.
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 8
Cognitive and oculomotor performance across schizotypy I Koychev et al
8
64 Matthysse S, Holzman PS, Lange K. The genetic transmission of schizophrenia: application of Mendelian latent structure analysis to eye tracking dysfunctions in schizophrenia and affective disorder. J Psychiatr Res 1986; 20: 5767.
65 Eyseneck HJ. Eyseneck SBG Psychoticism as a Dimension of Personality. Hodder&-Stoughton: London, UK, 1976.
66 Claridge G. Single indicator of risk for schizophrenia: probable fact or likely myth? Schizophr Bull 1994; 20: 151168.
67 Barrett TR, Etheridge JB. Verbal hallucinations in normals, I: people who hear "voices". Appl Cogn Psychol 1992; 6: 379387.
68 Lincoln TM. Relevant dimensions of delusions: continuing the continuum versus category debate. Schizophr Res 2007; 93: 211220.
69 Rawlings D, Williams B, Haslam N, Claridge G. Taxometric analysis supports a dimensional latent trait structure for schizotypy. Pers Individ Dif 2008; 44: 16401651.
70 Raine A, Benishay D. The SPQ-B: A brief screening instrument for Schizotypal personality disorder. J Pers Disord 1995; 9: 346355.
71 Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry 1998; 59: 2233, quiz 34-57.
72 Aichert DS, Williams SC, Moller HJ, Kumari V, Ettinger U. Functional neural correlates of psychometric schizotypy: an fMRI study of antisaccades. Psychophysiology 2012; 49: 345356.
73 O'Driscoll GA, Lenzenweger MF, Holzman PS. Antisaccades and smooth pursuit eye tracking and schizotypy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55: 837843.
74 Nelson H, OConnell A. National Adult Reading Test (NART). NFER-Nelson: Windsor, UK, 1991.
75 Benjamini Y, Hochberg T. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B 1995; 57: 289300.76 Benjamini Y, Hochberg T. On the adaptive control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing with independent statistics. J Educ Behav Stat 2000; 25: 6083.
77 Hotelling H. Relations between two sets of variates. Biometrika 1936; 28: 321377.78 Gonzalez I, Cao KA, Davis MJ, Dejean S. Visualising associations between paired 'omics' data sets. BioData Mining 2012; 5: 19.79 Hastie T, Tibshirani R, Freedman J. The Elements of Statistical Learning. Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2001.80 Nelson MT, Seal ML, Pantelis C, Phillips LJ. Evidence of a dimensional relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013; 37: 317327.81 Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 1999; 56: 303308.82 Schmechtig A, Lees J, Perkins A, Altavilla A, Craig KJ, Dawson GR et al. The effects of ketamine and risperidone on eye movement control in healthy volunteers. Transl Psychiatry 2013; 3: e334.83 Minas RK, Park S. Attentional window in schizophrenia and schizotypal personality: Insight from negative priming studies. Appl Prev Psychol 2007; 12: 140148.84 Kumari V, Ettinger ULatent inhibition in schizophrenia and schizotypy: a review of the empirical literature In: Lubow RE (ed.) Latent Inhibition. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2010 pp 419447.
85 Gray NS, Fernandez M, Williams J, Ruddle RA, Snowden RJ. Which schizotypal dimensions abolish latent inhibition? Br J Clin Psychol 2002; 41: 271284.
86 Gray NS, Snowden RJ, Peoples M, Hemsley DR, Gray JA. A demonstration of within-subjects latent inhibition in the human: limitations and advantages. Behav Brain Res 2003; 138: 18.
87 Park S, Gooding DC. Working memory impairment as an endophenotypic marker of a schizophrenia diathesis. Schizophr Res Cogn 2014; 1: 127136.
88 Glahn DC, Therman S, Manninen M, Huttunen M, Kaprio J, Lonnqvist J et al. Spatial working memory as an endophenotype for schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 53: 624626.
89 Pirkola T, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Glahn D, Kieseppa T, Haukka J, Kaprio J et al. Spatial working memory function in twins with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 58: 930936.
90 McClure MM, Barch DM, Flory JD, Harvey PD, Siever LJ. Context processing in schizotypal personality disorder: evidence of specicity of impairment to the schizophrenia spectrum. J Abnorm Psychol 2008; 117: 342354.
91 Roitman SE, Mitropoulou V, Keefe RS, Silverman JM, Serby M, Harvey PD et al. Visuospatial working memory in schizotypal personality disorder patients. Schizophr Res 2000; 41: 447455.
92 Gooding DC, Shea HB, Matts CW. Saccadic performance in questionnaire-identied schizotypes over time. Psychiatry Res 2005; 133: 173186.
93 Thier P, Ilg UJ. The neural basis of smooth-pursuit eye movements. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2005; 15: 645652.
94 Lencer R, Nagel M, Sprenger A, Heide W, Binkofski F. Reduced neuronal activity in the V5 complex underlies smooth-pursuit decit in schizophrenia: evidence from an fMRI study. NeuroImage 2005; 24: 12561259.
95 Lalor EC, Yeap S, Reilly RB, Pearlmutter BA, Foxe JJ. Dissecting the cellular contributions to early visual sensory processing decits in schizophrenia using the VESPA evoked response. Schizophr Res 2008; 98: 256264.
96 Yeap S, Kelly SP, Sehatpour P, Magno E, Javitt DC, Garavan H et al. Early visual sensory decits as endophenotypes for schizophrenia: high-density electrical mapping in clinically unaffected rst-degree relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63: 11801188.
97 Yeap S, Kelly SP, Thakore JH, Foxe JJ. Visual sensory processing decits in rst-episode patients with Schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2008; 102: 340343.
98 Barcelo F, Suwazono S, Knight RT. Prefrontal modulation of visual processing in humans. Nat Neurosci 2000; 3: 399403.
99 Koychev I, Deakin JF, Haenschel C, El-Deredy W. Abnormal neural oscillations in schizotypy during a visual working memory task: support for a decient top-down network? Neuropsychologia 2011; 49: 28662873.
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articles Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if the material is not included under the Creative Commons license, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to reproduce the material. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Web End =http://creativecommons.org/licenses/ http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Web End =by/4.0/
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 8
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Nature Publishing Group May 2016
Abstract
The development of drugs to improve cognition in patients with schizophrenia is a major unmet clinical need. A number of promising compounds failed in recent clinical trials, a pattern linked to poor translation between preclinical and clinical stages of drug development. Seeking proof of efficacy in early Phase 1 studies in surrogate patient populations (for example, high schizotypy individuals where subtle cognitive impairment is present) has been suggested as a strategy to reduce attrition in the later stages of drug development. However, there is little agreement regarding the pattern of distribution of schizotypal features in the general population, creating uncertainty regarding the optimal control group that should be included in prospective trials. We aimed to address this question by comparing the performance of groups derived from the general population with low, average and high schizotypy scores over a range of cognitive and oculomotor tasks. We found that tasks dependent on frontal inhibitory mechanisms (N-Back working memory and anti-saccade oculomotor tasks), as well as a smooth-pursuit oculomotor task were sensitive to differences in the schizotypy phenotype. In these tasks the cognitive performance of 'low schizotypes' was significantly different from 'high schizotypes' with 'average schizotypes' having an intermediate performance. These results indicate that for evaluating putative cognition enhancers for treating schizophrenia in early-drug development studies the maximum schizotypy effect would be achieved using a design that compares low and high schizotypes.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer