Motor planning of goal-directed action is tuned by the emotional valence of the stimulus: a kinematic study
OPEN
R
A
P
P. O. Esteves, L. A. S. Oliveira, A. A. Nogueira-Campos, G. Saunier, T. Pozzo,J. M. Oliveira, E. C. Rodrigues, E.Volchan & C. D.Vargas
The basic underpinnings of homeostatic behavior include interacting with positive items and avoiding negative ones. As the planning aspects of goal-directed actions can be inferred from their movement features, we investigated the kinematics of interacting with emotion-laden stimuli. Participants were instructed to grasp emotion-laden stimuli and bring them toward their bodies while the kinematicsof their wrist movement was measured. The results showed that the time to peak velocity increasedfor bringing pleasant stimuli towards the body compared to unpleasant and neutral ones, suggesting higher easiness in undertaking the task with pleasant stimuli. Furthermore, bringing unpleasant stimuli towards the body increased movement time in comparison with both pleasant and neutral ones while the time to peak velocity for unpleasant stimuli was the same as for that of neutral stimuli. There was no change in the trajectory length among emotional categories. We conclude that during the reach-to- kinematic features of a goal-directed action are tuned by the emotional valence of the stimuli.
The basic underpinnings of homeostatic behavior include motor interactions with emotion-laden objects. Compelling evidence shows that in humans emotion-laden contexts aect motor output16. Employing readiness potential, an electrophysiological marker of motor preparation, our group demonstrated that bringing unpleasant stimuli towards the body results in a higher cost compared to pleasant stimuli, indicating that motor planning encompasses an estimate of the action value (costs and gains)4. In such a context, preparing to interact with pleasant stimuli would recruit pre-set approach-like motor repertoires4. Conversely, when preparing to interact with unpleasant stimuli, the discrepancy between the required action and the objects aversiveness would result in a broader mobilization of neural resources4,5.
The kinematic parameters of a given action have long been postulated to reect the content of the motor plan713. Kinematic invariants of actions can be captured from dierent individuals performing the same task such as, for instance, picking up a glass from a table. Indeed, the kinematic features of the upper-limb during goal-directed actions have been proven to reect the participants intentions (grasping or pointing at something; grabbing to throw, liing or tting something)13. The same holds for the intrinsic characteristics of the object (geometry, texture, weight, size, and shape) with which the agent interacts14. Besides, social intentions and their motoric components translate into specic kinematic features1518.
Since the kinematic features of goal-directed actions reect motor planning10 and are modulated by intentions1318, the kinematics of actions directed towards unpleasant stimuli could reect action costs, whereas those directed towards pleasant stimuli could reveal facilitation. Such a result would be in agreement with the idea that
Laboratrio de Neurobiologia II, Instituto de Biofsica Carlos Chagas Filho, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brasil. Programa de Ps-graduao em Cincias da Reabilitao Centro Universitrio Augusto Motta, Rio de Janeiro, Brasil. Departamento de Fisiologia, Instituto de Cincias Biolgicas, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Juiz de Fora, Brasil. Laboratrio de Cognio Motora, Instituto de Cincias Biolgicas, Universidade Federal do Par, Belm, Brasil. Universitaire, UFR STAPS, Dijon, France. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to C.D.V. (email: [email protected])
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS
1
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Movement Time (s)
Mean Standard Error Mean Standard Error PHASE A PHASE B
Unpleasant 0.553 0.019 0.597 0.020
Neutral 0.540 0.019 0.569 0.020 Pleasant 0.548 0.018 0.589 0.020 Statistics Values [F(2,40)=6,27, p=0.004]
Peak Velocity (cm/s)
Unpleasant 107.530 5.439 150.715 8.104
Neutral 110.273 5.944 155.657 8.606 Pleasant 109.203 5.621 151.104 8.248 Statistics Values [F(2, 40)=4,082, p=0.024]
Time to Peak Velocity (%)
Unpleasant 53.465 1.669 53.015 1.412
Neutral 54.733 1.676 52.694 1.314 Pleasant 53.361 1.587 53.838 1.540 Statistics Values [F(2,40)=6,29, p=0.004]
Movement Trajectory Length (cm)
Unpleasant 34.163 1.337 53.203 1.655
Neutral 34.195 1.328 52.995 1.677 Pleasant 34.356 1.253 53.035 1.670 Statistics Values [F(2,40)=1,283, p=0.288]
Table 1. Mean and Standard Error for each parameter for reach-to-grasp (phase A) and bring-to-the-body (phase B).
motor plans encompass the costs and gains (value)4,19 of a given action. To address this issue we employed a paradigm consisting of an analysis of real movements of grasping emotion-laden stimuli and bringing them toward the body. During reach-to-grasp (phase A), when the participant interacted with the stimulus by the rst time, our conjecture was that the stimuli aective load should be made evident in the measured kinematic parameters. Crucially, we hypothesized that action facilitation would be especially manifested during the bring-to-the-body period (phase B), in congruence with the nal goal of the action.
Results
The aective rating of emotion-laden stimuli collected from twenty participants tested by means of repeated measures Anova with the emotional category as the factor revealed a main eect of valence [F (2, 38)=226,09, p< 0.0001)] (Fig.1A). The same statistical approach also yielded a main eect of arousal [F (2, 38)=25,87, p< 0.0001)] (Fig.1B). Post hoc analysis of the valence dimension showed higher scores for pleasant (7.09 0.16, Mean and SE) compared to neutral (4.8 0.11) and unpleasant (2.67 0.17) stimuli. Furthermore, participants rated the unpleasant stimuli with lower scores compared to the neutral ones. In the arousal dimension participants gave similar scores for unpleasant (3.65 0.37) and pleasant (4.33 0.34) stimuli and both were higher than the neutral (1.300.10) ones.
Kinematics Parameters. The eect of emotion over a goal-directed action was veried by means of a three-way repeated measures Anova with the phases (reach-to-grasp and bring-to-the-body), valence (unpleasant, neutral and pleasant) and blocks (1, 2 and 3) as independent factors for each kinematics parameter.
Movement Time. A significant interaction between phase (reach-to-grasp and bring-to-the-body) and valence (unpleasant, neutral and pleasant) [F (2, 40)=6,27, p= 0.004] was found in relation to the length of time of the movement. The Movement Time was longest for reach-to-grasp unpleasant and pleasant stimuli in comparison to neutral stimuli. Additionally, the Movement Time was longer to bring-to-the-body unpleasant stimuli compared to the pleasant and neutral stimuli, and longer for bringing pleasant stimuli toward the body than neutral stimuli (Fig.2A). A main eect for phase [F (1, 20) = 5,33, p= 0.032] and for valence [F (2, 40) = 30,17, p< 0.001] was also revealed. Post-hoc comparisons showed that the reach-to-grasp phase was shorter than the bring-to-the-body phase and unpleasant stimuli lasted more than pleasant and neutral ones. Furthermore, the duration was longer for pleasant than neutral stimuli. No other eects were observed for this parameter (See Table1).
Peak Velocity. Repeated measures Anova performed on Peak Velocity showed signicant interaction between phase and valence [F(2, 40)=4,0822, p= 0.024]. Participants had lower Peak Velocity values to reach-to-grasp (Phase A) unpleasant stimuli than pleasant and neutral stimuli. Moreover, during the bring-to-the-body phase (Phase B), Peak Velocity was lower for pleasant and unpleasant stimuli than neutral stimuli. A main eect of phase F(1, 20)=49,622, p< 0.001] and of valence F(2, 40)=10,744, p< 0.001] was also revealed. Post-hoc comparison showed that Peak Velocity was lower during Phase A when compared to Phase B. Additionally, Peak Velocity was lower for pleasant and unpleasant than neutral stimuli. No other eects were observed for this parameter (See Table1 and Fig.2B).
Valence
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS
2
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Figure 1. Aective Rating. Valence (A) and Arousal (B). Means and Standard Error, n=20. Unpleasant, Neutral and Pleasant Stimuli. *p<0.01.
Figure 2. Kinematics of reach-to-grasp (phase A) and Bring-to-the-body (phase B). (A) Movement Time; (B) Peak Velocity; (C) Time to Peak Velocity; (D) Movement Trajectory Length. *p<0.05.
Time to Peak Velocity. A signicant interaction between phase and valence was found for the Time to Peak Velocity parameter [F(2, 40) = 6,29, p = 0.004]. Post-hoc comparisons showed that the deceleration phase was longer when reach-to-grasp pleasant and unpleasant stimuli when compared to neutral ones. Interestingly, during the bring-to-the-body phase, deceleration was shorter for pleasant stimuli when compared to unpleasant and neutral stimuli (Figs2C and 3). No other eects were observed for this parameter (See Table1).
Movement Trajectory Length. No signicant interaction between phase and valence was found for the Movement Trajectory Length parameter [F (2, 40) = 1,28, p = 0.288] (Fig.2D and Table1). A main eect was found for Phase [F (1, 20)=77,23, p= 0.0001]. The reach-to-grasp length was shorter than the bring-to-the-body one. No other eects were observed for this parameter (See Table1).
Discussion
In the present study we aimed to evaluate whether the kinematic features of a goal-directed action are aected by the emotional valence of the stimuli. We employed a real interaction paradigm in which volunteers were asked to physically interact with stimuli classied as unpleasant, neutral and pleasant. The kinematic analysis revealed that the Time to Peak Velocity, Movement Time and Peak Velocity parameters were modulated by the valence of the stimuli, whereas there was no emotional category eect in the trajectory length of the movement. The valence eects reected in Time to Peak Velocity, Movement Time and Peak Velocity can thus not be attributed to dierences in trajectory length.
Previous work showed that kinematics is affected by the emotional context induced by valence-laden pictures20,21. In the present study, the source of emotion is inherent to the goal of action. To the best of our
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS
3
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Figure 3. Tangential Velocity Prole. Mean of three blocks for 21 participants in phase B (bring-to-the-body) for each emotional category. Unpleasant is in black, neutral in grey, and pleasant in white.
knowledge, we show for the rst time that the temporal features of motion kinematics are tuned by the emotional valence of the stimulus with which one is about to interact, thus strengthening the premise that emotion aects motor planning.
The participants spent more time in reach-to-grasp and bring-to-the-body pleasant and unpleasant stimuli compared to neutral stimuli. Moreover, participants attained lower Peak Velocity values to reach-to-grasp unpleasant stimuli than pleasant and neutral ones. Interestingly, during the bring close to the body phase, Movement Time was even longer for unpleasant stimuli when compared with pleasant and neutral ones. The Peak Velocity parameter followed a similar trend during this phase, being lower for pleasant and unpleasant stimuli compared to neutral ones. In the same vein, it has been shown that viewing unpleasant pictures promotes the slowing of reaction times1,3, modulates force production2 and reduces body sway22,23. Thus, our interpretation is that Movement Time and Peak Velocity parameters might reect a global aective load eect, being specially sensitive to the stimuli unpleasantness.
Analysis of the Time to Peak Velocity showed a longer deceleration time (shorter Time to Peak Velocity) in the reach-to-grasp phase (phase A) in the valence-laden conditions compared to the neutral condition. At rst glance, this result could be interpreted as a facilitation eect for the neutral condition. Indeed, a longer deceleration time could indicate a higher demand to perform the task13,28. When the participants have to rst interact with an emotion-laden stimulus, a high recruitment of aective resources is expected to occur24,25. Furthermore, emotional stimuli catch more attention1 and promote a more careful evaluation to dene their aversiveness or pleasantness26, hence the apparent facilitation eect (shorter deceleration time) for the neutral stimuli. Crucially, for this parameter the deceleration time was shorter when pleasant as compared to unpleasant and neutral stimuli were brought close to the body. During this phase, when the participant achieves the goal of the motor plan, he has already identied the stimuli valence. If a longer deceleration time indicates higher demand to perform the task13,27, shorter deceleration time could correspond to higher easiness in achieving the purpose of the task. Thus, the pleasantness of the stimulus would in fact facilitate action implementation.
In accordance with this hypothesis, the velocity of saccade movements has been shown to be greater when performed towards human faces19, thus valence-laden, in contrast to an image of a neutral object. Also, higher Peak Velocity and lower Time to Peak Velocity values were described when participants were asked to reach-to-grasp towards another person compared to a single-agent condition27. Likewise, lower readiness potential amplitudes have been found to precede the grasping of pleasant stimuli compared to unpleasant and neutral ones4. The authors proposed that the pleasantness of the stimuli recruited preset approach-like circuits in the brain, making the action less costly. In another line of evidence, lower readiness potentials preceded grooming actions performed in a pleasant social bonding context6. Finally, lower corticospinal excitability was found during reach-to-grasp pleasant stimuli compared to unpleasant and neutral ones5. The authors argued that the pleasant stimuli triggered an urge to move that required greater suppression, reecting enhanced control preceding actions towards those stimuli. Applied to the present results, we propose that the implied intention1318 embedded
in bring-to-the-body a pleasant stimulus matched with preset approach-like motor repertoires yield a shorter deceleration time in the pleasant condition compared to the unpleasant and the neutral conditions.
The lack of dierence in time to peak velocity found between unpleasant and neutral stimuli when they were brought close to the body may be explained as follows: preset withdrawal programs triggered by object aversiveness would compete with the implementation of an imposed bring close to the body action, resulting in the mobilization of more neural resources and thus in higher cost4,5. This incongruence did not translate however into any modulation over action kinematics, suggesting that bringing an unpleasant stimulus towards the body was implemented as if it was a neutral stimulus. In other words, our conjecture is that, to comply with the experimenters instructions and interact with the unpleasant stimulus, the participants might implicitly reduce its unpleasantness as if it was a neutral stimulus (se also28,29 for an attenuation eect of unpleasant stimuli induced by their reappraisal).
In conclusion, the kinematics of reach-to-grasp and bring-to-the-body movements is aected by the emotional valence of the stimulus to which one interact. Shorter deceleration time indicates higher easiness in bringing pleasant stimuli close to the body whereas the lack of any dierences with the neutral and unpleasant stimuli might result
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS
4
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Figure 4. Experimental setup. The reach-to-grasp (A) and bring-to-the-body (B) phases of the action are indicated, as well as the initial distance between the hand and the stimulus. The white rectangle indicates the position of the mobile sensor on the participants wrist.
from having to comply with the demand of performing an otherwise unwanted action. Kinematics thus seems to embody the motor intentions that relate to the value (costs and gains) of the stimuli with which one is interacting.
Methods
Participants. Twenty-ve right-handed male students aged 2136 years old (27.71 4.12) participated in this study. A single gender sample was chosen because emotionally laden stimuli categorization is gender specic30,31. Participants reported having no neurological or neuropsychiatric disease. Written informed consent was provided by each participant. All experimental protocols were approved by the local Ethics Committee (CEP n092376/2013 - 5257 Hospital Universitrio Clementino Fraga Filho/UFRJ). The methods were carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines of the Hospital. Handedness was assessed with the Edinburgh handedness inventory32.
Stimuli selection. A set of 60 emotional-laden objects was placed inside identical transparent cylinders to facilitate uniform grip and was balanced in weight. A total of 39 emotional-laden stimuli (thirteen unpleasant, neutral and pleasant) were selected using a Self-Assessment Scale (SAM)33 from a previous study4. The objects inside the cylinders were also evaluated based on their dimensions in a behavioral test in which participants judged the type of the grasp they would employ to interact with each stimuli4. The type of the grasp (precision grasp or whole grasp) was balanced among the three emotional categories.
Accommodated inside each transparent cylinder, the unpleasant stimuli were a chicken gizzard, a cake with hair, articial vomit, a preserved cockroach, articial excrement, preserved rotten food, a bluebottle on a biscuit, a preserved dead rat, a rotten artichoke, a preserved chicken foot, an articial spider, an articial snake and a preserved sh eye; the neutral ones were adhesive tape, a pencil sharpener, a crumpled paper ball, silver paper clips, binder clips, a sponge, a glue stick, a piece of plastic bag, an alkaline battery, cotton balls, pieces of colored wire, spun wool and a strip of staples; and the pleasant stimuli were a chocolate candy, chocolate tablet, money, a wrapped condom, mobile phone, some soccer cards, two toys cars, marbles, a gold trophy, a ball, a television remote control, an MP3 player and wrist watch.
Procedure. The experiment was conducted in a sound-attenuated room under ambient light. The participant was asked to sit in a comfortable chair facing a table on which the stimuli (identical transparent cylinders containing the emotion laden object) were presented, one at a time, by an experimenter seated behind a black curtain in front of each participant. The stimulus was presented on a mobile wooden tray with a holder for the stimulus, xed 30cm from the hand of the participant (Fig.4). The experimenter withdrew the tray to change the stimulus aer each trial. Each stimulus was presented once in a randomized block of 39 trials. The experiment comprised a total of four blocks. The rst one was a training block to familiarize participants with the experimental task. Approximately 3 minutes of rest between blocks were given to the participant. The total duration of the experiment was about y minutes.
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS
5
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Kinematic data collection was performed in tandem with a readiness potential experiment4. Participants performed the task with their le hand, as stronger readiness potential negativity was reported for movements with the non-dominant le hand4. At the beginning of each trial, the participants were instructed to rest their le hand on the table on a load cell and focus on the point where the stimulus would appear. Upon the stimulus presentation they were instructed to wait for a few seconds and, whenever they felt ready, grasp the cylinder (containing the emotional-laden object) with their le hand and bring it close to their chest. Aer that, they returned the cylinder to the tray and repositioned their hand on the load cell in the initial position (Fig.4). A training session ensured that participants waited approximately 3 s to initiate the task. This was accomplished by giving verbal feedback during the training without explicit information about the desired interval.
Each participant was also instructed to keep his le elbow in contact with the table and to avoid making any other movement during the experiment. The right arm, not involved in the task, rested on a pillow during the experimental session. To motivate participants to pay attention to the stimuli, they were asked to carefully observe each stimulus in order to be able to identify, aer the experiment, which stimuli they had seen during the experimental session. At the end of the experiment all participants except one evaluated each stimulus in both dimensions of emotion (valence and arousal)4 using the Self-Assessment Scale (SAM)33.
Kinematic recording. An electromagnetic tracking device, the Polhemus Fastrak (SPACE FASTRAK, Colchester, VT, USA) was used to record the le wrist position in three-dimensional coordinates. A stationary transmitter was xed 30 cm in front of the participant and established the global coordinate system. A mobile sensor was xed with an adhesive tape on the dorsal aspect of the le wrist at the intersection point between the styloid process of the ulna and the middle nger. Data acquisition was synchronized with the stimulus presentation. Real-time 3-D position of the le wrist was tracked at 100Hz during the execution of the task, permitting o-line calculation of wrist displacement over time and the velocity prole.
Data analysis. Data analysis was made offline using Matlab soware (Mathworks, USA). A 3-D reconstruction of the wrist trajectory was performed and its tangential velocity prole was calculated and ltered with a h-order low pass lter at 10Hz. The action was divided in two phases: (A) reach-to-grasp and (B) bring-to-the-body. A Matlab script was used to determine the onset of the reach-to-grasp phase, calculated as ve percent of the rst peak of velocity14. The beginning of phase B was determined as the lowest velocity prole value between phases A and B. The end of the bring-to-the-body phase (phase B) was dened as the smallest value aer its peak. The tangential velocity prole of each separated phase was time-normalized by a linear interpolation of 200 points. A second Matlab script was designed to detect discrepant shapes of the velocity prole. The two phases of all trials were plotted together so that discrepant shapes from the participants median value for each condition and each phase could be marked and then excluded through visual inspection blind to the valence condition. If one outlier phase was beyond the average the whole trial was discarded from the analysis. Less than 10% of trials were excluded using these criteria. Four participants were excluded from the kinematic analysis due to technical problems in data collection.
The following parameters were calculated for the remaining 21 participants: movement time (MT), peak velocity (PV), time to peak velocity (TPV) and movement trajectory length (MTL) for reach-to-grasp and bring-to-the-body phases. Movement time was dened as the time interval between the onset and oset of each phase. Movement trajectory length was determined as the distance traveled for each participant in each phase. Peak Velocity corresponds to the maximal velocity attained for each participant in each phase. Time to peak velocity represents the timing of motion, i.e.: the proportion between the duration of the acceleration and the deceleration times12, that is, the ratio between acceleration time (AT) and movement time (MT). This index indicates how long the acceleration time of a movement lasted with respect to the total duration of the movement. A ratio greater than 0.5 indicates that the deceleration time is shorter than acceleration time. This parameter indicates the motor system strategy to perform the action, since the deceleration time increases with task demand13.
Statistical Analyses. A Three-way repeated measures Anova was performed using Statistica 7 soware, with phases (reach-to-grasp and bring-to-the-body), valence (unpleasant, neutral and pleasant) and blocks (1, 2 and 3) as independent variables for each kinematics parameter. Post hoc analysis was assessed using Duncans test whenever a signicant eect was found, i.e. both on main eects and on interactions. One-way repeated measures Anova was run for the valence and arousal ratings separately having the emotional category (unpleasant, neutral and pleasant) as within factor. Duncans post-hoc analysis was employed on any signicant eect that emerged from the ANOVA. The level of signicance was set to 0.05.
We introduced block as a factor in the ANOVA to guarantee that there would not be any learning or habituation eect with respect to the emotional valence of the stimuli34.
References
1. Erthal, F. S. et al. Load-dependent modulation of aective picture processing. Cogn Aect Behav Neurosci 5(4), 38895 (2005).2. Coombes, S. A., Cauraugh, J. H. & Janelle, C. M. Emotion and movement: activation of defensive circuitry alters the magnitude of a sustained muscle contraction. Neurosci. Lett. 396, 192196 (2006).
3. Pereira, M. G. Sustained and transient modulation of performance induced by emotional picture viewing. Emotion. 6(4), 62234 (2006).
4. de Oliveira, L. A. S. et al. Preparing to Grasp Emotionally Laden Stimuli. PLoS ONE. 7(9), e45235 (2012).5. Nogueira-campos, A. A., Oliveira, L. A. S., Esteves, P. O., Rodrigues, E. C. & Vargas, C. D. Preparing to grasp an emotional-laden object: a TMS study. PLoS ONE. 9(4), e94824 (2014).
6. Campagnoli, R. R. et al. Preparing to caress: a neural signature of social bonding. Front.in Psyc. 6(16), doi: 10.3389 (2015).7. Bernstein, N. The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Oxford, Pergamon Press, London (1967).8. Soechting, J. F. & Lacquaniti, F. Invariant characteristics of a pointing movement in man. J. Neurosci. 1, 71020 (1981).9. Jeannerod, M. In Attention and Performance IX (ed Long, J. & Baddeley, A.) 153168 (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, 1981).
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS
6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
10. Paulignan, Y., Frak, V. G., Toni, I. & Jeannerod, M. Inuence of object position and size on human prehension movements. Exp. Brain Res. 114,226234 (1997).
11. Desmurget, M., Prablanc, C., Jordan, M. & Jeannerod, M. Are Reaching movements Planned to be Straight and Invariant in the Extrinsic Space? Kinematic Comparison Between Compliant and Unconstrained Motions. Quart. Jour. Exp. Psyc. 52A(4), 9811020 (1999).
12. Papaxanthis, C., Pozzo, T., Vinter, A. & Grishin, A. The representation of gravitational force during drawing movements of the arm. Exp Brain Res. 120, 233242 (1998).
13. Marteniuk, R. G., Mackenzie, C. L., Jeannerod, M., Athenes, S. & Dugas, C. Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. Canad. J. of Psyc. 41(3), 365378 (1987).
14. Sartori, L., Straulino, E. & Castiello, U. How Objects Are Grasped: The Interplay between Aordances and End-Goals. PLoS ONE. 6(9), e25203 (2011b).
15. Becchio C., Sartori L., Bulgheroni M. & Castiello U. (2008a). The case of Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde: a kinematic study on social intention. Conscious. Cogn. 17 557564
16. Sartori, L., Becchio, C. & Castiello, U. (2011a). Cues to intention: the role of movement information. Cognition. 119, 242252 (2011a).
17. Becchio, C., Manera, V., Sartori, L., Cavallo, A. & Castiello, U. Grasping intentions: from thought experiments to empirical evidence. Front. In Hum. Neurosci. 6(117), doi: 10.3389 (2012).
18. Ansuini, C., Cavallo, A., Bertone, C. & Becchio, C. The visible face of intention: why kinematics matters. Front. in Psyc. 5(815), 16 (2014).
19. Xu-Wilson, M., Zee, D. S. & Shadmehr, R. The intrinsic value of visual information aects saccade velocities. Exp. Brain. Res. 196, 475481 (2009).
20. Ambron, E., Rumiati, R. I. & Foroni, F. Do emotions or gender drive our actions? A study of motor distractibility. Cogn. Neurosc. 7(14), 1609, doi: 10.1080/17588928.2015.1085373. (2016).
21. Ferri, F. et al. When Action Meets Emotions: How Facial Displays of Emotion Inuence Goal-Related Behavior. PLoS ONE. 5(10), e13126, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013126 (2010).
22. Azevedo, T. M. et al. A freezing-like posture to pictures of mutilation. Psyc. 42(3), 25560 (2005).23. Facchinetti, L. D., Imbiriba, L. A., Azevedo, T. M., Vargas, C. D. & Volchan, E. Postural modulation induced by pictures depicting prosocial or dangerous contexts. Neurosci. Lett. 13, 410(1), 526 (2006).
24. Bonnet, M., Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J. & Requin, J. Modulation of spinal reexes: Arousal, pleasure, action. Psychophysiology. 32,
367372 (1995).
25. Hajcak, G. et al. Emotion facilitates action: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study of motor cortex excitability during picture viewing. Psychophysiology. 44, 9197 (2007).
26. Bradley, M. M. et al. Activation of the visual cortex in motivated attention. Behavioral Neuroscience. 117, 369380 (2003).27. Becchio, C., Sartori, L., Bulgheroni, M. & Castiello, U. The case of Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde: a kinematic study on social intention. Conscious Cogn. 17(3), 55764 (2008)
28. Oliveira, L. A. et al. Autonomic reactions to mutilation pictures: positive aect facilitates safety signal processing. Psychophysiology. Jul, 46(4), 8703 (2009).
29. Mocaiber, I. et al. Antecedent descriptions change brain reactivity to emotional stimuli: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of an extrinsic and incidental reappraisal strategy. Neuroscience. Oct 13, 193, 2418 (2011).
30. Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Sabatinelli, D. & Lang, P. J. Emotion and motivation II: Sex dierences in picture processing. Emotion. 1, 300319 (2001).
31. Sabatinelli, D., Flaisch, T., Bradley, M. M., Fitzsimmons, J. R. & Lang, P. J. Aective picture perception: gender dierences in visual cortex? Neuroreport. 15, 1109 (2004).
32. Oldeld, R. C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsyc. 9(1), 97113 (1971).33. Lang, P. J., Ohman, A. & Vaitl, D. The International Affective Picture System Photographic slides. The Centre for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, (1988a).
34. Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J. & Cuthbert, B. N. Emotion, novelty, and the startle reex: habituation in humans. Behav Neurosci. 107(6), 97080 (1993).
Acknowledgements
This work was funded by Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Cientfico e tecnolgico CNPq (Grant numbers 306817/2014-4 and 480108/2012-9), Fundao de Amparo a Pesquisa do Estado do Rio de Janeiro FAPERJ (Grants E-26/110.526/2012 and E26/010.002902/2014) and Financiadora de Estudos e Projetos FINEP (PROINFRA HOSPITALAR grant 18.569-8). This work has been produced as part of the activities of FAPESP Research, Dissemination and Innovation Center for Neuromathematics-NeuroMat (Grant 2013/07699-0, FAPESP). This work was also supported by CAPES-COFECUB (Project no. 819-14).
Author Contributions
Conception and design of experiments: L.A.S.O., E.C.R., E.V. and C.D.V. Performance of experiments: L.A.S.O. Data analyses: P.O.E., L.A.S.O. and A.A.N.-C. Contributions to methods/analysis tools: G.S. and J.M.O. Preparation and production of tables and the gures: P.O.E. and J.M.O. Writing the paper: All Authors. Reviewing the manuscript: All Authors.
Additional Information
Competing nancial interests: The authors declare no competing nancial interests.
How to cite this article: Esteves, P. O. et al. Motor planning of goal-directed action is tuned by the emotional valence of the stimulus: a kinematic study. Sci. Rep. 6, 28780; doi: 10.1038/srep28780 (2016).
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articles Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if the material is not included under the Creative Commons license, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to reproduce the material. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS
7
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Nature Publishing Group Jul 2016
Abstract
The basic underpinnings of homeostatic behavior include interacting with positive items and avoiding negative ones. As the planning aspects of goal-directed actions can be inferred from their movement features, we investigated the kinematics of interacting with emotion-laden stimuli. Participants were instructed to grasp emotion-laden stimuli and bring them toward their bodies while the kinematics of their wrist movement was measured. The results showed that the time to peak velocity increased for bringing pleasant stimuli towards the body compared to unpleasant and neutral ones, suggesting higher easiness in undertaking the task with pleasant stimuli. Furthermore, bringing unpleasant stimuli towards the body increased movement time in comparison with both pleasant and neutral ones while the time to peak velocity for unpleasant stimuli was the same as for that of neutral stimuli. There was no change in the trajectory length among emotional categories. We conclude that during the "reach-to-grasp" and "bring-to-the-body" movements, the valence of the stimuli affects the temporal but not the spatial kinematic features of motion. To the best of our knowledge, we show for the first time that the kinematic features of a goal-directed action are tuned by the emotional valence of the stimuli.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer