OPEN
Citation: Transl Psychiatry (2016) 6, e957; doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.130
Web End =10.1038/tp.2016.130
http://www.nature.com/tp
Web End =www.nature.com/tp
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Early changes in emotional processing as a marker of clinical response to SSRI treatment in depression
BR Godlewska1, M Browning2, R Norbury3, PJ Cowen1 and CJ Harmer2
Antidepressant treatment reduces behavioural and neural markers of negative emotional bias early in treatment and has been proposed as a mechanism of antidepressant drug action. Here, we provide a critical test of this hypothesis by assessing whether neural markers of early emotional processing changes predict later clinical response in depression. Thirty-ve unmedicated patients with major depression took the selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitor (SSRI), escitalopram (10 mg), over 6 weeks, and were classied as responders (22 patients) versus non-responders (13 patients), based on at least a 50% reduction in symptoms by the end of treatment. The neural response to fearful and happy emotional facial expressions was assessed before and after 7 days of treatment using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Changes in the neural response to these facial cues after 7 days of escitalopram were compared in patients as a function of later clinical response. A sample of healthy controls was also assessed. At baseline, depressed patients showed greater activation to fear versus happy faces than controls in the insula and dorsal anterior cingulate. Depressed patients who went on to respond to the SSRI had a greater reduction in neural activity to fearful versus happy facial expressions after just 7 days of escitalopram across a network of regions including the anterior cingulate, insula, amygdala and thalamus. Mediation analysis conrmed that the direct effect of neural change on symptom response was not mediated by initial changes in depressive symptoms. These results support the hypothesis that early changes in emotional processing with antidepressant treatment are the basis of later clinical improvement. As such, early correction of negative bias may be a key
mechanism of antidepressant drug action and a potentially useful predictor of therapeutic response.
Translational Psychiatry (2016) 6, e957; doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.130
Web End =10.1038/tp.2016.130 ; published online 22 November 2016
INTRODUCTIONMajor depression is associated with a range of negative biases in the processing of emotional information.1 For example, compared with healthy controls, depressed patients selectively recall more negative, self-related emotional information in memory tasks and demonstrate negative biases in the perception of social signals such as emotional facial expressions.2,3 Such biases have been associated with aberrant responses across a network of neural areas involved in emotional processing. Untreated patients with depression show enhanced reactivity to negative stimuli in networks involved in emotional salience and attention such as the amygdala, visual cortex, insula and thalamus as well as differences in response in areas thought to have a role in emotion monitoring, evaluation and regulation including the anterior cingulate (ACC) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.47 These negative biases appear to have a key role in the pathophysiology and maintenance of depressive states.1
We have proposed that the therapeutic effect of antidepressant drugs is mediated by early reversal of these negative emotional biases.8 For example, in healthy volunteers, 7 days' treatment with the selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitor (SSRI), citalopram, and the selective noradrenaline re-uptake inhibitor, reboxetine, diminished the recognition of negative emotional faces, increased recall of positive self-referential words and attenuated the amygdala response to fearful faces as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).911 Studies in depressed
patients have also shown attenuation of the neural response to sad and fearful faces after antidepressant treatment in limbic areas including the amygdala, insula and ACC.4,7,12 However, these
studies have typically been conducted after 68 weeks of therapy, by which time clinical response is well established. As such, it is difcult to assess whether changes in neural responsivity are a cause or effect of changes in depression symptoms.
To address this question, we recently assessed the effects of just 7 days' treatment with the SSRI, escitalopram (10 mg), in depressed patients using a double-blind, placebo-controlled study.13 Amygdala response to fearful versus happy faces was normalised following escitalopram relative to placebo treatment before any signicant clinical response. Such early changes in processing may have a critical role in the emergence of clinical therapeutic effects over time as the patient responds to the reduced impact of negative events, stressors and cues.8 Early changes in information processing were seen in both behavioural and fMRI studies, and may lead to a gradual change in clinical symptomatology through interaction with on-going environmental and social stimuli.
If this hypothesis of antidepressant action is correct, we would expect early changes in emotional processing to be predictive of later clinical response to SSRI therapy in depression. The current study tested this critical hypothesis using a functional neuroimaging emotional paradigm. We predicted that a greater reduction in the neural response to fearful versus happy facial expressions
1Psychopharmacology Research Unit, University Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK; 2Psychopharmacology and Emotion Research Laboratory, University Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK and 3Department of Psychology, Whitelands College, University of Roehampton, London, UK. Correspondence: Professor CJ Harmer, Psychopharmacology and Emotion Research Laboratory, University Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK.
E-mail: mailto:[email protected]
Web End [email protected] Received 24 December 2015; revised 19 May 2016; accepted 1 June 2016
An early marker of response to SSRI treatment BR Godlewska et al
2
Table 1. Clinical and demographic data for week 6 responders (50% decrease in HAM-D scores) versus non-responders (o50% decrease in HAM-D scores)
Responders Non-responders Responders versus non-responders
Healthy volunteers Patients versus healthy controls
Gender (F:M) 12:10 8:5 0.163, P = 0.686 17:12 0.014, P = 0.905 Age (mean (s.d.)) 30 (11.5, 2061) 30.1 (10, 2052) 0.000, P = 0.984 30 (9.6, 1958) 0.004, P = 0.949 Trait anxiety (mean (s.d., range)) 59.4 (8.7, 4374) 63.3 (10.3, 6174) 1.427, P = 0.421 29.6 (7.8, 2044) 203.9, Po0.000
HAM-D (mean (s.d., range)) 23.1 (4.9, 1432) 22.9 (4, 1429) 0.006, P = 0.939 0.4 (0.8, 03) 707.4, Po0.000 BDI (mean (s.d., range)) 31.6 (6.7, 1949) 32.4 (5.340) 0.147, P = 0.704 0.9 (1.5, 05) 691.5, Po0.000 Week 1 change in HAM-D (mean (s.d.)) 5 (6.8) 1.9 (3.4) 2.373, P = 0.133Week 6 reduction in trait anxiety 13.4 (12.4) 6.4 (10.8) 2.834, P = 0.102Length of current episode (in months) 5.9 (6.0) 8.8 (8.7) 1.369, P = 0.250Number of antidepressant-naive 8/22 6/13 0.326, P = 0.568
Abbreviations: ANOVA, analysis of variance; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; HAM-D, Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; STAI, Spielbergers State-Trait Anxiety inventory. Treatment resistance dened as a failure to respond to adequate courses (in terms of length and dose) of at least two antidepressants from different classes). Continuous variables are shown as mean (s.d.). There are no statistically signicant differences between responders and non-responders. Gender and number of antidepressant-naive were performed using 2-tests, all other comparisons suing ANOVA. Patients and controls differed in terms of clinical scores (BDI, HAM-D and STAI-T; all P = 0.000), although there were no differences in terms of gender and age.
across a network of previously identied brain areas, including the amygdala, insula and cingulate cortex, after 1 week of treatment would be associated with an increased response to treatment at week 6. We did not use placebo treatment because of ethical concerns about giving placebo for 6 weeks to depressed patients. In addition, our above-mentioned study13 showed that after 7 days of treatment with escitalopram the amygdala activity in depressed patients was similar to that in healthy controls, whereas placebo produced no such effect. Healthy controls were included to show baseline differences between the tested groups, and to explore whether there were differences between controls and responders and non-responders.
MATERIALS AND METHODS Participants
Thirty-ve participants (20 F:15M) with major depression completed the fMRI and treatment protocol (see Table 1). An additional four patients consented to take part, but dropped out before the end of the 6-week period of treatment (two after the rst session and two before the 6-week assessment). Thirty-one healthy control participants completed baseline scans; however, data from two participants were lost because of computer error, leaving a sample of 29 (17 F:12M). All participants were assessed for the presence of current and past psychiatric disorder with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV14. The depressed patients met the criteria for a primary diagnosis of major depressive disorder; the control participants had no current or lifetime diagnosis. Exclusion criteria for the study included psychosis or substance dependence as dened by DSM-IV, being at clinically signicant risk of suicidal behaviour, having contraindications to escitalopram treatment or being treated with psychotropic medication less than 3 weeks before the study (5 weeks for uoxetine). We also excluded patients with major somatic or neurological disorders, who were pregnant or breast-feeding, with any contraindications to MRI or concurrent medication, which could alter emotional processing. All participants were right-handed. The study was approved by the Oxford Research Ethics Committee, and all participants gave written informed consent.
Study design and drug treatmentThe sample of patients (n = 35) and healthy controls (n = 29) were assessed at baseline. Patients received 10 mg escitalopram each morning for a period of 6 weeks without dose adjustment. In the patient group, assessment of depressive severity and treatment response was made using the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D),15 Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)16 and Spielbergers State-Trait Anxiety inventory (STAI)17 at baseline, week 1 and week 6. The fMRI assessments were completed at the same time points. The current analysis focuses on the degree to which early changes (between baseline and week 1) in the function of emotional
processing systems was able to predict clinical response at week 6 in the depressed patients. After the 6-week duration of the study, all patients were offered treatment openly with escitalopram according to usual clinical practice (data not captured). Clinical response to the SSRI was dened as a reduction in HAM-D of 50% or more from baseline after 6 weeks of treatment.18 Healthy controls were assessed at baseline only with HAM-D, BDI and STAI, and had a single fMRI scan performed at the same time.
fMRI data acquisitionfMRI data were acquired on a 3 T Siemens TIM TRIO (Siemens, Erlangen,
Germany). Data were acquired with a voxel resolution of 3 3 3.5 mm, repetition time (TR)/ echo time (TE)/ ip angle = 2000 ms/28ms/89o. A total of 256 volumes were acquired in an experiment lasting 8.5 min. T1-
weighted structural images were acquired using a magnetisation-prepared rapid acquisition by gradient echo sequence with a voxel resolution1.0 1.0 1.0 mm on a 208 256 200 grid, TE/ inversion time/TR = 4.68 /900/2040 ms. To monitor cardiac and respiratory processes, subjects wore a pulse oximeter and respiratory bellows.
fMRI experimental taskDuring fMRI scanning, participants completed a well-validated gender discrimination task involving the rapid presentation of fearful and happy faces.12 This task, which involves passive processing of emotional information, under the form of fearful and happy faces, with minimal attentional effort, provides an incidental measure of emotional processing and is believed to be a better probe of limbic function.19 In this task, nine 30-s blocks of a baseline xation cross were interleaved with eight 30-s blocks of the emotional task (four blocks of fear and four blocks of happy). Each face was presented for 100 ms, and the subjects were asked to report the gender of the face via an MRI-compatible key pad.
fMRI preprocessing and statistical analysisfMRI data were preprocessed and analysed using FMRIB Software Library (FSL).20 Briey, motion correction was applied using a rigid body registration to the central volume; the brain matter was segmented from non-brain using a mesh deformation approach. Gaussian spatial smoothing was applied with a full-width half-maximum of 5 mm; high-pass temporal ltering was applied using a Gaussian-weighted running line lter, with a 3 dB cutoff of 120 s.
A general linear model was tted in pre-whitened data space. Two explanatory variables (plus their temporal derivatives) were modelled: fear faces and happy faces. All explanatory variables were convolved with a default haemodynamic response function (Gamma function, delay = 6 s, s.d. = 3 s), and ltered by the same high-pass lter as the data. The impact of physiological noise on the BOLD signal was reduced using the Physiological Noise Modelling tool of FSL. Pulse oximetry and respiratory bellows' data were processed by Physiological Noise Modelling to create
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 7
An early marker of response to SSRI treatment
BR Godlewska et al
3
33 nuisance regressors that were added to the rst-level fMRI model. The full model was simultaneously regressed against the BOLD data, giving the best-tting amplitudes for each explanatory variable while accounting for the physiological noise.
The task contrast of interest in this study was the relative activation of fearful versus happy faces. The degree to which the change in neural activity in this contrast, induced by 1 week of SSRI treatment, predicted participants clinical response on the HAM-D to medication over 6 weeks was tested using a three-level analysis. The rst level consisted of the fearful versus happy contrast maps, as described above, calculated for each depressed subject and each visit. Second-level, xed effects analyses calculated the degree to which these difference maps changed across the rst week of treatment for each depressed subject. Lastly, a third-level, between-subject, random effects analysis assessed whether this change in neural activity differed between depressed patients who went on to respond to the medication and those who did not. Baseline HAM-D score was included as a regressor in the third-level analysis to account for the potential inuence of initial depression severity effects on both early neural differences and overall clinical response (NB although baseline HAM-D score did not differ between the two groups, responders and nonresponders (Table 1) and equivalent results were obtained when the analysis was run without this covariate).
The results of all group-level analyses were corrected across the whole brain using cluster-based thresholding with a height threshold of Z42.3 and a (whole-brain-corrected) spatial extent threshold of P = 0.05. We also dened bilateral amygdala, bilateral insula and ACC cortex masks as a priori regions of interest based on their sensitivity to early effects of antidepressant drug treatments in previous studies.21 These masks were derived from the HarvardOxford Cortical and Subcortical anatomical atlases and were used in small volume correction analyses (clusters determined by Z42.3 and a (corrected) cluster signicance threshold of
P = 0.05) of the data. All reported analyses are corrected across the whole brain unless otherwise specied (labelled as small volume correction (SVC)). Lastly, the causal relationship between early change in fMRI signal, change in symptom score across the rst week of treatment and response status following 6 weeks of treatment was examined using mediation analyses. The mean change in activity within the functional clusters identied in the previous, predictive analyses was entered as independent variables in separate mediation analyses. In these analyses, patient response status was entered as the dependent variable and the change in symptom score across the rst week was entered as a mediating variable. These analyses therefore tested whether the change in fMRI signal directly predicted future response to the antidepressant, or whether the neural effect on treatment response was signicantly mediated by the initial change in symptomswhich would suggest that the fMRI signal acted simply as a marker of early symptom change. A bootstrapping procedure, as implemented by the PROCESS command for SPSS with 10 000 samples, was used to determine the condence intervals of the direct and indirect effects of the mediation models.
RESULTSClinical and demographic dataAfter 6 weeks escitalopram treatment, 22 out of 35 patients (62%)
were classied as responders. There were no differences between responders and non-responders in terms of gender, age, baseline depression severity, baseline trait anxiety or duration of current episode (See Table 1, all effect sizes o0.45 Cohens d). However, week 6 responders versus non-responders showed numerically greater improvements in HAM-D score after 7 days of treatment, although this did not reach statistical signicance (t = 1.8, degree of freedom = 32.3, P = 0.08, Cohens d effect size 0.57).
fMRI data. At baseline, across all participants, fearful compared with happy faces activated a network of areas previously implicated in threat-relevant face processing. This included bilateral amygdala, bilateral insula and left middle temporal gyrus (Supplementary Table 1). Depressed patients displayed signicantly greater pretreatment activation than controls to fearful versus happy faces in both the left insula (P = 0.02, family wise error (FWE)-corrected, SVC) and ACC cortex (P = 0.02, FWE-corrected, SVC; see Figure 1).
Early changes in fMRI response to fearful versus happy faces following 7 days escitalopram were predictive of later clinical response, controlling for baseline depression HAM-D severity. Week 6 responders showed a greater decrease in neural response to fearful versus happy faces following 7 days escitalopram in the left amygdala, insula, anterior and posterior cingulate, bilateral supramarginal gyri and bilateral thalamus (Po0.05, FWE-corrected; see Table 2 and Figure 2). These clusters anatomically overlapped with the left insula and ACC clusters identied in the baseline comparison between depressed and control participants described above. A formal conjunction analysis identied signicant clusters within both regions22 (ACC cluster: max Z-value = 3.1, corrected P-value = 0.05, coordinates = 45, 70, 53; insula cluster: max Z-value = 3.1, corrected P-value = 0.005, coordinates = 64, 61, 36).
A number of additional covariates were entered into the model to check the specicity of this effect. In this control analysis, early change in HAM-D after 1 week, baseline trait anxiety and week 6 change in trait anxiety were entered in as regressors of no interest. The results from this whole-brain analysis conrmed that early change in neural response to faces of fear versus happiness predicted clinical response even after accounting for these possible confounds (Supplementary Table 2).
It is possible that baseline differences in neural markers of emotional processing between responders and non-responders might also correlate with clinical outcome at 6 weeks, and such baseline effects could also contribute to the relationship between change in emotional processing over 7 days of treatment and later therapeutic response. However, we found no statistically signicant differences to fearful versus happy faces at baseline (before treatment) in responders versus non-responders at a whole brain level or within the regions of interest. Interestingly, analysis of the fMRI data after 1 week of treatment alone was sufcient to predict outcome at 6 weeks with the same network of regions being involved (insula, thalamus, amygdala svc, thalamus and cingulate, see Supplementary Table 3). These additional analyses suggest that it is the ability of escitalopram to modify emotional processing early in treatment rather than baseline reactivity, which is related to later individual clinical response.
Lastly, we employed mediation analyses to test whether the relationship between early change in fMRI measures and response to treatment at week 6 was mediated by the change in depressive symptoms across the rst weekthat is, whether the fMRI measures reected early symptom change rather than being directly related to later response. Separate mediation analyses were performed for each of the functional clusters identied in Table 2. In all of these analyses, the direct effect of the fMRI signal on response status remained signicant (all Po0.012) after controlling for the mediating effects of early symptom change. In none of the analyses was the indirect effect (via early symptom change) signicant (all bootstrapped condence intervals spanned 0). In other words, mediation analysis provided additional evidence that the observed change in fMRI signal was directly related to the response to antidepressants and was not merely a marker of initial symptom response.
DISCUSSIONThe principal nding of our study is that changes in neural processing of emotional information in the rst week of SSRI treatment in depressed patients are predictive of short-term (6 weeks) therapeutic response. This early change in neural activity preceded clinically signicant changes in depressive symptomatology and was not dependent on concurrent change in symptoms at the time of the second scan. The changes in neural activity occurred in regions which, at baseline, were differentially activated by fearful versus happy faces and which showed hyperactivity in depressed relative to control participants. As
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 7
An early marker of response to SSRI treatment BR Godlewska et al
4
Figure 1. Baseline differences in the processing of negative versus positive affective stimuli differ between depressed patients and non-depressed control subjects. (a) Results of SVC-corrected analyses in the anterior cingulate and left insula; (b) extracted signal change from the identied clusters. All analyses were thresholded at z = 2.3 and cluster-corrected with a FWE Po0.05. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; FWE, family wise error; SVC, small volume correction.
Table 2. Prediction of clinical response from an early change in neural response to fearful compared with happy facial expressions after 7 days escitalopram treatment
MNI Coordinates, peak Cluster size, voxels Z-value P-value
x y z
Left amygdala/left insula 40 22 8 2204 3.76 2.15e09
Anterior cingulate cortex 8 4 38 2175 3.96 2.67e07
Left supramarginal gyrus extending to left postcentral gyrus 40 28 32 876 3.66 0.0002 R supramarginal gyrus/right superior temporal gyrus 58 32 2 712 3.56 0.0009 Bilateral thalamus 12 22 10 601 3.18 0.003
Abbreviation: MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute. Wholebrain analysis at Z = 2.3 for responders versus non-responders.
such, early normalisation of the neural systems responsible for negative processing biases in depression may be an important mechanism of antidepressant action.
The results are consistent with the cognitive neuropsychological theory of antidepressant action, which proposes that conventional antidepressant medications act through an early remediation of the negative emotional biases that characterise the depressed state.8 This theory hypothesises that treatments for depression reduce the overwhelming inux of automatic negative cues early in treatment, before the patient being aware of any changes in subjective state or in the clinical symptoms of depression. This early change in emotional processing, however, is the mechanism that drives the later clinical response as the patient's day-to-day environment is re-experienced through this more positive emotional perspective.8
Neural networks involving the amygdala, insula, cingulate and thalamus have been implicated in the generation of negative emotional biases in depression,2,21 and our baseline comparison
between the healthy controls and depressed patients replicated the pattern of increased response to negative versus positive faces in parts of this circuit. Previous studies have also suggested that hyperactivity of this circuitry in response to negative versus positive emotional stimuli is attenuated following treatment with antidepressants.4,7,12,23,24 In a previous, placebo-controlled, paral
lel group study we found that that the amygdala hyperarousal exhibited by depressed patients in response to negative faces resolved after just 7 days of SSRI treatment and before signicant changes in the clinical state.13 The latter study is important in showing that the effect of 7 days SSRI treatment on neural responses in depressed patients cannot be attributed to the
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 7
An early marker of response to SSRI treatment
BR Godlewska et al
5
Figure 2. Early changes in the processing of negative versus positive affective stimuli predict week 6 treatment response to escitalopram across a network of areas involved in emotional processing. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; AMG, amygdala; INS, insula; STC, superior temporal cortex; THAL, thalamus. Whole-brain-corrected analysis at z = 2.3, responders versus non-responders.
nonspecic effects of drug administration, repeat fMRI testing or changes in clinical state. The current investigation now provides support for the proposal that these early changes in neural processing of emotional information produced by antidepressant treatment are indeed important as a mechanism of later clinical response.
The amygdala, insula, ACC and thalamus are part of an integrated neural system, which is important for monitoring and affective response to emotional stimuli.25 Thalamicamygdala connections have been proposed to be critical for the rapid monitoring and detection of salient stimuli for further processing.26 In depression, increased amygdala arousal has been suggested to create a bottom-up signal, outside of direct conscious awareness, that biases emotional stimulus processing across the higher cortical areas involved in a more complex processing; this results in maladaptive perceptions of the environment and social interactions.2 Indeed, a number of key studies in depression indicate that the hyperactivity of amygdala response exists even if the emotional valence of the stimulus is processed automatically by masking conscious perception.2 An
early reduction in amygdala response to negative versus positive stimuli with SSRI treatment may therefore not be perceived as an immediate change in the subjective state. Instead, such a change in implicit processing would be hypothesised to set the scene for more adaptive perceptions of environmental stimuli, which then inuence the conscious state over time.
Besides a decrease in amygdala responsiveness, the current study also highlights a role for early changes in the response of the cingulate and insula to emotional stimuli. These areas form key nodes in Maybergs limbiccortical model of depression27 and
have been identied in meta-analyses of functional neuroimaging studies as underpinning negative biases of emotional processing in depression.28,29 The current data set adds to this body of work, suggesting that emotion-specic modulation of response in this area following SSRI treatment may provide an early marker of therapeutic response.
It is of note that the difference in response to negative versus positive facial expressions largely seems to be driven by increased responses to happy cues rather than reduced responses to negative cues, although both effects may be present to some extent in areas like the amygdala. Similarly, differences between patients and controls at baseline in response to emotional cues seemed largely to be in response to the positive facial expressions. These observations are consistent with a growing focus on reduced positive processing in depression30 and also with behavioural evidence that increased perception of happy facial expressions rather than reduced processing of negative facial expressions is related to later clinical outcome with antidepressant drug treatment.31 It is difcult to draw denitive conclusions on a pattern extracted from this kind of fMRI analysis that reveals an overall interaction between positive versus negative cue processing; however, it suggests that a focus on the effects of boosting
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 7
An early marker of response to SSRI treatment
BR Godlewska et al
6
positive affective processing may be an important consideration in future research studies exploring early mechanisms of treatment response in depression.
In addition to the neural changes described above, 7 days treatment with escitalopram was associated with some reduction in HAM-D scores, and this was numerically greater in later clinical responders. This raises the possibility that the change in neural activity found in the second scan in subsequent treatment responders might be secondary to this early improvement,32
rather than the neural changes leading to the subsequent clinical response. However, when this initial change in HAM-D score was accounted for in our statistical model, the early changes in neural response were still predictive of the therapeutic outcome. A secondary mediation analysis further suggested that early changes in neural response to emotional information mediate later clinical change, independent from any inuence of early mood change. Therefore, early change in clinical symptoms does not seem to be driving the prediction provided by changes in emotional processing response.
Previous studies have identied differences in baseline (pre-treatment) neural response to emotional material as a predictor of likely clinical response. In particular, increased perfusion and response in the rostral cingulate have been related to increased likelihood of response to antidepressant treatment.33 A recent PET study further suggested that basal perfusion of the insula may serve as a specic biomarker for response to drug versus psychological treatment,34 with increased perfusion related to a higher likelihood of remission with SSRI therapy, and the converse being true for cognitive behavioural therapy. However, the current pattern of results suggests that early change in neural response following SSRI treatment may be more sensitive to clinical response than baseline measures in fMRI. This approach has the potential to test the specic effects of a particular treatment at a given dose and time in patients at an early stage of the intervention.
Our ndings have a number of clinical implications. First, they suggest that rapid change in emotional processing circuitry is related to clinical effectiveness of a particular treatment. As such, emotional processing models may usefully be applied in the drug-discovery programmes to facilitate decision-making about novel treatments in development. Critically, we have found early positive biases in emotional processing across antidepressant classes, including SSRIs, norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors and atypical antidepressants such as mirtazapine10,11,35 as well as
treatments with novel mechanisms of action such as the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist AZD6765.36 Second, as many patients do not respond to the rst-line treatment with a given agent and waiting time for response can compound the delay in effective treatment, an early marker that could be used to tailor treatment in an individual would have signicant advantages. It is also worth noting that relevant fMRI changes in emotional processing can be identied after single doses of antidepressant drugs in healthy volunteers.8 This raises the intriguing possibility that assessing the effect of single doses of medication in depressed patients might also have predictive value.
Future studies should, therefore, assess whether this early signature of antidepressant drug action can be used to predict response on an individual-patient level, using a priori dened neural and/or behavioural criteria and in a larger cohort of patients. It will also be necessary to examine whether these early changes in emotional processing predict placebo response, that is, function as a general marker of improvement rather than a specic drug-induced mechanism. Our study was limited by the absence of a placebo control condition to test this hypothesis, although in our earlier work we have found little inuence of placebo treatment on emotional processing.13
In summary, this study provides the key evidence that, following the initiation of antidepressant treatment, early changes in the activity of neural systems responsible for emotional processing mediate later clinical response. These observations may be relevant to the development of improved therapeutic approaches to depression as well as for the early detection of clinical response.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
CJH has received consultancy fees from P1vital, Lundbeck, Servier, Eli-Lilly. She is a company director of Oxford Psychologists Ltd and holds shares in P1vital. MB is employed on a part time basis by P1vital. PJC has been a member of a Lundbeck advisory board for the last 3 years. The remaining authors declare no conict of interest.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The study was funded by the MRC programme grant to PJC (G0701421).
REFERENCES
1 Beck AT. The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobio-logical correlates. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165: 969977.
2 Disner SG, Beevers CG, Haigh EA, Beck AT. Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nat Rev Neurosci 2011; 12: 467477.
3 Roiser JP, Sahakian BJ. Hot and cold cognition in depression. CNS Spectr 2013; 18: 139149.
4 Sheline YI, Barch DM, Donnelly JM, Ollinger JM, Snyder AZ, Mintun MA. Increased amygdala response to masked emotional faces in depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant treatment: an fMRI study. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 50: 651658.
5 Surguladze S, Brammer MJ, Keedwell P, Giampietro V, Young AW, Travis MJ et al. A differential pattern of neural response toward sad versus happy facial expressions in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57: 201209.
6 Suslow T, Konrad C, Kugel H, Rumstadt D, Zwitserlood P, Schning S. Automatic moodcongruent amygdala responses to masked facial expressions in major depression. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67: 155160.
7 Victor TA, Furey ML, Fromm SJ, Ohman A, Drevets WC. Relationship between amygdala responses to masked faces and mood state and treatment in major depressive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67: 11281138.
8 Harmer CJ, Goodwin GM, Cowen PJ. Why do antidepressants take so long to work? A cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant drug action. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 102108.
9 Harmer CJ, Shelley NC, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM. Increased positive versus negative affective perception and memory in healthy volunteers following selective serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibition. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161: 12561263.
10 Harmer CJ, Mackay CE, Reid CB, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM. Antidepressant drug treatment modies the neural processing of nonconscious threat cues. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59: 816820.
11 Norbury R, Mackay CE, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM, Harmer CJ. Shortterm anti-depressant treatment and facial processing. Functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Br J Psychiatry 2007; 190: 531532.
12 Fu CH, Williams SC, Cleare AJ, Brammer MJ, Walsh ND, Kim J et al. Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in major depression by antidepressant treatment: a prospective, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61: 877889.
13 Godlewska BR, Norbury R, Selvaraj S, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ. Shortterm SSRI treatment normalises amygdala hyperactivity in depressed patients. Psychol Med 2012; 42: 26092617.
14 Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M, First MB. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM 1 V (SCID). Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York, NY, USA, 1995.
15 Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurgery 1960; 23: 5662.16 Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1961; 4: 561571.17 Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Bibliography. 2nd edn, Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1989.18 Angst J, DeliniStula A, Stabl M, Stassen HH. Is a cutoff score a suitable measure of treatment outcome in shortterm trials in depression? A methodological meta analysis. Hum Psychopharmacol 1993; 8: 311317.19 Costafreda SG, Brammer MJ, David AS, Fu CH. Predictors of amygdala activation during the processing of emotional stimuli: a meta-analysis of 385 PET and fMRI studies. Brain Res Rev 2008; 58: 5770.
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 7
An early marker of response to SSRI treatment
BR Godlewska et al
7
20 Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens T, Woolrich M, Smith S. FSL. Neuroimage 2012; 62: 782790.
21 Price JL, Drevets WC. Neurocircuitry of mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010; 35: 192216.
22 Nichols T, Brett M, Andersson J, Wager T, Poline JB. Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage 2005; 25: 653660.
23 Delaveau P, Jabourian M, Lemogne C, Guionnet S, Bergouignan L, Fossati P. Brain effects of antidepressants in major depression: a metaanalysis ofemotional processing studies. J Affect Disord 2011; 130: 6674.
24 Davidson RJ, Irwin W, Anderle MJ, Kalin NH. The neural substrates of affective processing in depressed patients treated with venlafaxine. Am J Psychiatr 2003; 160: 6475.
25 Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, Lane R. Neurobiology of emotion perception II: implications for major psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 54: 515528.
26 Davis M, Whalen PJ. The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol Psychiatry. 2001; 6: 1334.
27 Mayberg HS. Limbiccortical dysregulation: a proposed model of depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1997; 9: 471481.
28 Diener C, Kuehner C, Brusniak W, Ubl B, Wessa M, Flor H. A metaanalysis of neurofunctional imaging studies of emotion and cognition in major depression. Neuroimage 2012; 61: 677685.
29 Hamilton JP, Etkin A, Furman DJ, Lemus MG, Johnson RF, Gotlib IH. Functional neuroimaging of major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis and new integration of base line activation and neural response data. Am J Psychiatry. 2012; 169: 69370.
30 Lewis G, Kounali D, Button KS, Duffy L, Wiles NJ, Munaf MR et al. Reduced Positive Bias in People with Depression: Evidence for Depressive Realism in a Prospective Longitudinal Cohort (in submission to Lancet Psychiatru).
31 Tranter R, Bell D, Gutting P, Harmer C, Healy D, Anderson IM. The effect of serotonergic and noradrenergic antidepressants on face emotion processing in depressed patients. J Affect Disord. 2009; 118: 8793.
32 Taylor MJ. Rapid onset of true antidepressant action. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2007; 9:
475479.
33 Pizzagalli DA. Frontocingulate dysfunction in depression: toward biomarkers of treatment response. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011; 36: 183206.
34 McGrath CL, Kelley ME, Holtzheimer PE, Dunlop BW, Craighead WE, Franco AR et al. Toward a neuroimaging treatment selection biomarker for major depressive disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70: 821829.
35 Rawlings NB, Norbury R, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ. A single dose of mirtazapine modulates neural responses to emotional faces in healthy people. Psycho-pharmacology (Berl) 2010; 212: 625634.
36 Harmer CJ, Rylands R, Parsons L, Dawson G, Deakin B, Goodwin et al. Comparative Antidepressant Mechanisms of Lanicemine And Ketamine 2: Effects on Emotion Processing (in submission).
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articles Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if the material is not included under the Creative Commons license, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to reproduce the material. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Web End =http://creativecommons.org/licenses/ http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Web End =by/4.0/
The Author(s) 2016
Supplementary Information accompanies the paper on the Translational Psychiatry website (http://www.nature.com/tp)
Translational Psychiatry (2016), 1 7
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Nature Publishing Group Nov 2016
Abstract
Antidepressant treatment reduces behavioural and neural markers of negative emotional bias early in treatment and has been proposed as a mechanism of antidepressant drug action. Here, we provide a critical test of this hypothesis by assessing whether neural markers of early emotional processing changes predict later clinical response in depression. Thirty-five unmedicated patients with major depression took the selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitor (SSRI), escitalopram (10 mg), over 6 weeks, and were classified as responders (22 patients) versus non-responders (13 patients), based on at least a 50% reduction in symptoms by the end of treatment. The neural response to fearful and happy emotional facial expressions was assessed before and after 7 days of treatment using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Changes in the neural response to these facial cues after 7 days of escitalopram were compared in patients as a function of later clinical response. A sample of healthy controls was also assessed. At baseline, depressed patients showed greater activation to fear versus happy faces than controls in the insula and dorsal anterior cingulate. Depressed patients who went on to respond to the SSRI had a greater reduction in neural activity to fearful versus happy facial expressions after just 7 days of escitalopram across a network of regions including the anterior cingulate, insula, amygdala and thalamus. Mediation analysis confirmed that the direct effect of neural change on symptom response was not mediated by initial changes in depressive symptoms. These results support the hypothesis that early changes in emotional processing with antidepressant treatment are the basis of later clinical improvement. As such, early correction of negative bias may be a key mechanism of antidepressant drug action and a potentially useful predictor of therapeutic response.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer