ARTICLE
Received 1 Dec 2015 | Accepted 28 Dec 2016 | Published 8 Feb 2017
DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 OPEN
Amygdala-hippocampal dynamics during salient information processing
Jie Zheng1,*, Kristopher L. Anderson2,3,*, Stephanie L. Leal4, Avgusta Shestyuk2, Gultekin Gulsen1,Lilit Mnatsakanyan5, Sumeet Vadera6, Frank P.K. Hsu6, Michael A. Yassa4,5, Robert T. Knight2,3 & Jack J. Lin5
Recognizing motivationally salient information is critical to guiding behaviour. The amygdala and hippocampus are thought to support this operation, but the circuit-level mechanism of this interaction is unclear. We used direct recordings in the amygdala and hippocampus from human epilepsy patients to examine oscillatory activity during processing of fearful faces compared with neutral landscapes. We report high gamma (70180 Hz) activation for fearful faces with earlier stimulus evoked onset in the amygdala compared with the hippocampus. Attending to fearful faces compared with neutral landscape stimuli enhances low-frequency coupling between the amygdala and the hippocampus. The interaction between the amygdala and hippocampus is largely unidirectional, with theta/alpha oscillations in the amygdala modulating hippocampal gamma activity. Granger prediction, phase slope index and phase lag analysis corroborate this directional coupling. These results demonstrate that processing emotionally salient events in humans engages an amygdala-hippocampal network, with the amygdala inuencing hippocampal dynamics during fear processing.
1 Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of California, Irvine, California 92697, USA. 2 Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720, USA. 3 Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720, USA. 4 Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, University of California, Irvine, California 92697, USA. 5 Comprehensive Epilepsy Program, Department of Neurology, University of California, Irvine, California 92868, USA. 6 Department of Neurological Surgery, University of California, Irvine, California 92868, USA. * These authors contributed equally to this work. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to J.J.L. (email: mailto:[email protected]
Web End [email protected] ).
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14413 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 1
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413
Swift detection of social, emotional or threatening stimuli is critical for adaptive tness in humans. When we interact with each other, emotionally salient stimuli, such as fearful
facial expressions, provide ecologically relevant signals that focus our attention towards perceptually relevant information. Thus, recognizing motivationally salient information constitutes an important social and biologically meaningful incentive and plays a key role in guiding our interpersonal behaviour1.
Successful detection of and response to motivationally important stimuli have been shown to rely on activity within two brain structuresthe amygdala and hippocampus. In particular, the amygdala is critical for prioritizing salient information such as emotion2, valence3 and motivation4. The hippocampus is thought to be important for contextual modulation of fear5, emotion judgment6 and emotional memory7all operations that are critical for remembering motivationally salient stimuli.
It is commonly assumed that the amygdala exerts directional inuence onto the hippocampus during processing of salient information8. This network model of salience processing is primarily based on rodent data. For example, the amygdala receives direct subcortical inputs thought to facilitate rapid detection of salient information9, consistent with a proposed role of the amygdala in early cognitive engagement that may inuence subsequent hippocampal mnemonic processing. Several studies also indicate that manipulating amygdala function alters hippocampal processing of salient information1012. Evidence for this directional inuence in humans has only been indirectly inferred from behaviour13 and neuroimaging14 studies showing that memory enhancement for emotionally arousing stimuli is positively associated with markers of endogenous norepinephrine release from the basolateral amygdala (BLA)15. However, there is no direct electrophysiological evidence for amygdala-hippocampal connectivity in humans and thus their directional relationship is unknown.
We addressed this question by presenting salient (dynamic fearful faces) and neutral (landscapes) stimuli to patients with medication resistant epilepsy in whom stereotactic electrodes had been implanted in the amygdala and hippocampus for pre-surgical evaluation. First, we hypothesized that high gamma (HG; 70180 Hz) band activity (a spatially precise measure of neuronal spiking16) will occur earlier in the amygdala than in the hippocampus, consistent with a directional relationship. We next examined the electrophysiological evidence for connectivity between the amygdala and hippocampus. Low-frequency oscillations (theta 47 Hz and alpha
812 Hz) are ubiquitous in the human hippocampus17 and amygdala18; fear conditioning studies in rodents suggest that they provide a temporal window for inter-regional communication19. Therefore, we hypothesized that low-frequency oscillations mediate functional connectivity between the amygdala and hippocampus by coupling spiking activity in the hippocampus (as indexed by the HG signal) to low-frequency oscillations in the amygdala. Finally, consistent with the model of detection/ prioritization by the amygdala and post-detection processing by the hippocampus1012, we hypothesized that the synchronous activity in these two regions would be biased such that it is more likely that the amygdala exerts directional inuence on the hippocampus rather than the reverse.
In this study, we show that the amygdala and hippocampus are both engaged in the early stages of salience processing with increased intraregional HG activity and enhanced inter-regional low-frequency synchrony when attending to aversive compared with neutral stimuli. The coupling between these two regions is predominantly unidirectional, with low-frequency oscillations in the amygdala entraining hippocampal HG activity. Overall, these results provide evidence for a directional inuence
from the amygdala to the hippocampus during processing of motivationally salient stimuli.
ResultsExperiment design and electrode localization. We recorded oscillatory activity in local eld potentials (LFPs) from nine human participants with intracranial depth electrodes implanted into the amygdala and the hippocampus. Electro-oculogram (EOG) electrodes and an eye tracker were used for one subject to evaluate the potential inuence of saccadic muscle movements on neural signals. We examined neuronal responses while individuals watched aversive movie clips containing blocks of dynamic fearful faces and neutral movie clips of landscapes (Fig. 1a). We employed dynamic fearful faces as a form of aversive stimuli, rather than static facial expressions, to provide participants with temporal cues that mimic real-life social exchanges20. The localization of depth electrodes was determined based on co-registered pre- and post-implantation magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), as well as registration to a high-resolution anatomical atlas, labelled with medial temporal lobe regions of interest. Localization of each electrode was performed in a semi-automated manner, guided by the anatomical atlas and visually checked by an experienced rater (S.L.L.). In all subjects, there were two to three depth electrodes located in the BLA and one to three electrodes located in the hippocampus (dentate gyrus (DG)/CA3 or CA1, Fig. 1b and Supplementary Fig. 1). A three-dimensional rendering of the amygdala and hippocampus showed that for all subjects, the electrodes were located in the basal aspects of the amygdala and the anterior hippocampus (Fig. 1c).
Local power and event-related potentials. Neuronal networks typically demonstrate activity in several oscillatory bands that cover both low- and high-frequency spectra with distinct roles in neuronal communication21. Whereas high gamma band activity is a spatially precise measure of local neuronal population spiking16, temporal synchronization of low-frequency phase is thought to mediate inter-regional communication22. Therefore, we rst determined the spectral specicity of low- and high-frequency oscillations in LFP. The power spectral density (PSD) plots revealed that each subject had a specic frequency peak in the theta/alpha and high gamma frequency ranges (Fig. 1d). These peaks are thought to reect coherent oscillatory processes23. We then band passed the raw LFP signal to extract separate frequency components. These analyses showed that the low frequency of the amygdala and high gamma band power envelope of the hippocampus tended to co-occur in time during the aversive condition (Supplementary Fig. 2). Additional analyses demonstrated that event-related potentials (ERP; Supplementary Fig. 3) and ocular muscle activity (Supplementary Figs 4 and 5a,b) did not contaminate the neural signals used in subsequent analyses.
Amygdala and hippocampus high gamma tracks salient stimuli. We explored the temporal prole of the oscillatory response to fearful faces versus landscapes. Electrodes localized in the amygdala (BLA) and hippocampus (DG/CA3 CA1) with
high-resolution MRIs as shown in Fig. 1c were included in the analysis. We then focused on the temporally resolved changes in HG amplitude and examined the coordinated timing of amygdala and hippocampus neuronal responses during the processing of aversive compared with neutral stimuli (Fig. 1e). The onset time was dened as the earliest time point at which two conditions showed a signicant difference in HG amplitudes; the peak time was dened as the latency of the maximum magnitude of differences in HG amplitudes between the conditions. The
2 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14413 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 ARTICLE
a
10 s
~24 s
b
9 x 8 x
Sync channel
L R
Baseline
(black screen)
Neutral condition
(landscape scenes)
Aversive condition
(fearful face)
c
L
CA1
Left
DG/CA3 Sub PrC LEC MEC BLA CORT CeA
R
Right
e
Aversive condition
d
50
40
30
20
10
10
20
30
Power (dB)
S1
Neutral conditionDifference between two conditions
Significant difference between two conditions
Amygdala
S2
S3
1 Amygdala
S4
S5
0
S6
High gamma activity (z-score over baseline)
S7
S8
10
S9
30
30
250
0
50
40
30
20
10
0
S1
Hippocampus
S2
1 Hippocampus
S3
S4
S5
S6
S7
10
20
30
S8
10 S9
0
0.5 0.5 1 1.5 Time (s) Clip duration
30
30
250
5 10 100 250
0
Frequency in log scale (Hz)
Frequency in linear scale (Hz)
Figure 1 | Task, electrode locations, power spectral density and high gamma activity. (a) Participants watched silent movie clips consisting of alternating blocks of neutral (landscapes) and aversive stimuli (fearful faces). (b) Example MRI and template for a single subject. Electrodes were localized in each participant using co-registered pre-implantation and post-implantation structural T1-weighted MRI scans. A high-resolution template of the hippocampal subelds and amygdala nuclei was aligned to each participants pre-implantation scan to visualize electrode locations in subject-specic anatomical space. Regions of interest (ROIs) in the medial temporal lobe included the DG/CA3, CA1, subiculum (Sub), perirhinal cortex (PrC), lateral and medial entorhinal cortex (LEC, MEC), parahippocampal cortex (PhC), BLA, CeA and the CORT. Each electrode location was determined by selecting the centre of the electrode (indicated by cross-hairs) and determining which ROI best encompassed the centre of the electrode. (c) Electrode localization of all subjects, rendered onto a three-dimensional amygdala and hippocampus model based on the high-resolution template. Red dots indicate electrodes in the amygdala; blue dots denote electrodes in the hippocampus. (d) Power spectral density (PSD) in log scale for the amygdala (upper panel) and hippocampus (lower panel). Peaks within theta (47 Hz)/alpha (812 Hz) and high gamma range (70180 Hz) were consistently observed in all subjects. The black arrow denotes the power peak in the high gamma range, which is also shown for each individual subject on a linear scale (30250 Hz) in the small plots.(e) High gamma amplitude (70180 Hz), averaged across participants ( s.e.m. shown as shading around the mean trace) and locked to stimulus onset, is shown for electrodes located in the amygdala and hippocampus (DG/CA3 CA1). Dotted lines represent signicant differences between the aversive and
neutral condition. (permutation test, see methods). L, left; R, right.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14413 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 3
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413
S1 S2 S3 S4
S5
BLA
WM
BLABLA*BLA *BLA
Sub DG/CA3 *DG/CA3
CA1
BLA *BLA
BLA
*BLA
WM BLA
BLA *BLA
Sub *CA1
WM WM
BLA
Phc Sub *DG/CA3
CA1
WM *BLA
BLA
DG/CA3
*CA1 CA1/WM
WM
*BLA
BLA CA1 *CA1
WM
BLA BLA *BLA
Sub *DG/CA3
CA1 CA1
S6 S7 S8 S9
BLA
Sub DG/CA3 *DG/CA3
CA1
BLA *BLA
MEC
Sub Sub *CA1
WM P>0.05 P>0.01
0.18
Low frequency phase locking value
(aversive condition - neutral condition)
0.14
BLA/CORT
DG/CA3 DG/CA3
*CA1
WM
0.10
0.06
0.02
0.02
Figure 2 | Differences in amygdala-hippocampal low-frequency phase coupling. Low-frequency phase coupling differences (aversivelandscapes) for pairs of electrodes targeting the amygdala (red dots) and hippocampal subelds (blue dots), depicted with hive plots. Differences in the PLV between aversive and neutral conditions are presented in colour, with warmer colours indicating a greater magnitude of the contrast. Signicance levels derived from permutation testing are indicated by the thickness of lines connecting each electrode pair. Asterisks represent electrode pairs with the most signicant PLV that were used for directional coupling analyses in Figs 3 and 4.
average HG amplitude across trials was higher for the aversive relative to the neutral condition after 12318 ms (means.e.m.) in the amygdala and after 24122 ms in the hippocampus post-stimulus onset (onset time, t-test, Po0.05). Similarly,
HG activity peaked earlier in the amygdala compared with the hippocampus (amygdala 49331 ms versus hippocampus
64142 ms; peak time, t-test, Po0.05). These ndings indicate that the amygdala and hippocampus are both engaged in the early stages of salience processing, with amygdala activation preceding hippocampal activation.
Amygdala-hippocampal low-frequency coupling. Given the distinct HG temporal proles of the amygdala and hippocampus, we then investigated whether these two regions interact through low-frequency phase coupling. The inter-regional coordination was examined with phase locking values (PLV) as a metric of effective connectivity between electrodes targeting the amygdala (red) and hippocampus (blue, Fig. 2). The three most medial electrodes targeting the amygdala and four most medial electrodes targeting the hippocampus were included in the phase coupling analysis. Note that this selection criterion was agnostic with regard to the experimental conditions (that is, aversive versus neutral) and the magnitude of phase coupling. Because of individual differences in the frequency of the event-related low-frequency band, we rst selected a specic band for each subject centred on the low-frequency peak in the PSD (amygdala
6.020.49 Hz; hippocampus 6.510.16 Hz, means.e.m.,
Fig. 1d) with a bandwidth of 4 Hz (ref. 24). We characterized the timing relationships of the low-frequency phase among all electrode pairs for each subject and calculated the PLV within and between the aversive and neutral conditions. Within each condition, amygdala-hippocampal PLVs were signicantly increased compared with the null distribution (all Po0.05, permutation test, Supplementary Fig. 6). We next tested PLV differences across conditions and found enhanced PLV between the low-frequency phases of BLA and the low-frequency phases of DG/CA3 as well as CA1 hippocampal subelds while viewing aversive compared with neutral movie clips (all Po0.05, permutation test, Fig. 2).
To validate the role of low-frequency phase coupling in coordinating amygdala-hippocampal network communication
during processing of motivationally salient information, we examined the PLV spectra between the most signicant phase coupling electrode pairs (denoted by asterisks in Fig. 2). Across all subjects, the PLV increased when viewing aversive fearful faces compared with neutral stimuli (main effect: F (1,67) 8.88, P 0.004) and peaked in the low-frequency
band for BLA-CA1 and BLA-CA3 compared with BLA- parahippocampal and BLA- subiculum electrode pairs (Supplementary Fig. 7a). Further, the magnitude of low-frequency PLV varied among hippocampal sub-regions (F (3,67) 2.88, P 0.042) with
the highest PLV observed for the BLA-CA1 electrode pairs. The phase coupling between BLA and CA1/CA3 were signicantly greater than the other hippocampal sub-regions (t-test, Po0.05;
Supplementary Fig. 7b). These ndings indicate that the amygdala and CA1/CA3 regions of the hippocampus exhibit strong low-frequency synchrony during processing of aversive stimuli.
Directional amygdala-hippocampus phase-amplitude coupling. On the basis of the strong low-frequency phase coupling between the amygdala and hippocampus across all subjects, we then examined the directional inuence of these responses with phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) across the two structures. To accomplish this, we again restricted our analysis to the most signicant low-frequency phase coupling electrode pairs from the PLV analysis. Given that the electrodes with signicant local PAC are more likely to engage in inter-regional coherence25,26, we rst found that PAC within both the amygdala and the hippocampus was increased when viewing aversive stimuli compared with neutral stimuli (Supplementary Fig. 8). We then examined the directional PAC between the amygdala and hippocampus in the aversive condition compared with the neutral condition (Fig. 3a). We found that the HG amplitude in the hippocampus was phase locked to the amygdala low-frequency rhythms (all Po0.01, permutation test, Fig. 3a). The stronger coupling between the amygdala low-frequency phase and the hippocampus HG power was also observed in the theta trough-locked averaging of timefrequency plot in the aversive condition (Po0.05, Supplementary Fig. 9a), while this modulation was nearly absent when examining the reverse direction (for example, HG amplitude in the amygdala phase
4 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14413 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 ARTICLE
a
S1
S2 S3 S4 S5 S6 S7 S8
S9
250
100
Hippocampus Amygdala
Frequency for amlitude (Hz)
50
2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20
Frequency for phase of amygdala (Hz)
Frequency for phase of hippocampus (Hz)
z-score
6 5 4 3 2 1 0
2 5 10 20
2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20
7
250
100 50
2
0
5 10 20 2
2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20 2 5 10 20
b
S2 S3 S4 S5 S6 S7 S8 S9
S1
4 + + + + + + +
z-PAC
2200 0 200 200 0 200 200 0 200 200 0 200 200 0 200 200Time lag (ms)
0 200 200 0
PAC using amygdala theta and hippocampus gamma
PAC using hippocampus theta and amygdala gamma
200 200 0 200 200 0 200
Figure 3 | Amygdala-hippocampus directional PAC and phase lag analysis. (a) PAC comodulogram for differences between the aversive and the neutral conditions is shown, with warmer colours denoting higher z-scores. The high gamma amplitude in the hippocampus was phase-locked to the phase of amygdala low-frequency (theta and alpha) rhythms (all Po0.01, permutation test). In contrast, the reciprocal directional PAC modulation (for example, high gamma amplitude in the amygdala phase-locked to phase of hippocampal low-frequency activity) was nearly absent. (b) Phase lag analysis. The
PAC modulation index between low-frequency phase and high gamma amplitude was estimated across time lags for the aversive condition. The red line represents the z-PAC between subject-specic amygdala low-frequency phase and hippocampal high gamma amplitude (70180 Hz). The blue line denotes the z-PAC between hippocampus low-frequency phase and amygdala high gamma amplitude. Cross ( ) denotes subjects who showed the amygdala
low-frequency activity leading hippocampal gamma.
locked to the phase of hippocampal low-frequency activity. Fig. 3a and Supplementary Fig. 9b).
We also examined whether the PAC varied as a function of the time lag between low-frequency and HG signals. We posited that an amygdala to hippocampus directionality would result in a conduction delay22, which would translate to a relative phase shift between low-frequency and HG oscillations (Supplementary Fig. 12). Specically, an earlier phase of amygdala low-frequency oscillations entraining hippocampal HG would result in the strongest PAC. We found that in the aversive condition, PAC between low frequency of the amygdala and HG of the hippocampus peaked around zero time lag ( 13.152.92 ms
versus hippocampus to amygdala directionality 13.5222.83 ms), with 7 out of 9 subjects demonstrating that the amygdala low-frequency was leading the hippocampal HG (Fig. 3b, denoted by near the red line, Pearsons w2-test 6.73, P 0.035).
In contrast, PAC between amygdala HG and hippocampus low-frequency was lower and failed to demonstrate a consistent peak at any time lag.
Since spectrally broad transients such as evoked activity can produce spurious PAC results27, we examined the spectral specicity of the modulated HG activity from the PAC results. To identify the rhythmic low-frequency uctuation of the higher-frequency power time series, we determined the centre frequency
of the HG signal from the PAC and ltered the raw signal within the HG band to extract the analytic amplitude of the signal (that is, envelope), which was then subjected to the PSD analysis. All subjects showed individual narrow-band low-frequency (412 Hz) peaks in the gamma envelope, thus supporting oscillatory properties of a separate low-frequency modulating signal in the HG band (Supplementary Fig. 10). To examine potential inuence of eye movements on the PAC results, we ran an independent component analysis (ICA) on the EOG combined with white matter referenced amygdala and hippocampal activity in one participant (subject 9)28. Components composed of EOG activity (three components, based on mixing weights, Supplementary Fig. 4f) were removed from the raw intracranial data and all analyses were re-run with this cleaned data. The observed PAC effect remained signicant after ICA correction (Supplementary Fig. 11). Further, there was no signicant PAC between EOG channels and the amygdala as well as the hippocampus (Supplementary Fig. 5c,d). These ndings indicate that eye movement contamination did not contribute to the original results.
Granger causality and phase slope index. To further examine the directionality of the amygdala-hippocampal circuit, we
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14413 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 5
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413
a
0.03
0.02
0.01
0.03
0.02
0.01
0
Aversive condition Neutral condition99.9% treshold
Granger index (a.u.)
Phase slope index (a.u.)
Aversive condition Neutral condition
Aversive condition Neutral condition
99.9% treshold
0 0 10 20 30
10 20 30
0 Frequency (Hz)
b
2
1
0
1
2
2
1
0
1
2
0 0.5
Stimuli onset
1
Modulating is leading modulated signal (P<0.01)
1.5
0 0.5 1 1.5
Time (s)
Figure 4 | Granger causality analyses and PSI. (a) Granger causality analyses demonstrated consistently stronger inuence for the amygdala-tohippocampus direction (top left) than for the hippocampus-to-amygdala direction (top right) when contrasting the aversive to the neutral condition. The red and blue solid lines represent the real data from aversive and neutral conditionss.e.m., respectively. Dotted lines denote 99% condence intervals for the null distribution. (b) PSI between the aversive and the neutral conditions calculated point-by-point across time using the low-frequency signal from the modulating channel (coloured in red) and high gamma signal from the modulated channel (coloured in blue). Dotted lines above the graph denote signicant differences between the two signals (all Po0.01, permutation test), showing that low-frequency activity from amygdala precedes hippocampus gamma for the majority of the stimuli duration (bottom left). The PSI did not show a directional inuence from the hippocampus to amygdala (bottom right).
utilized two complementary measures that rely on frequency and phase respectively: spectral Granger causality and phase slope index (PSI)29,30. The Granger causality measure quanties the strength of directional inuences between LFPs in the frequency domain by testing whether the LFP from one structure (for example, hippocampus) can be better predicted by incorporating information from the LFP from the other structure (for example, amygdala) and vice versa. PSI quanties phase difference as a function of frequency, with a positive phase slope indicating that the signal from the rst structure is leading the signal from the second structure. In low-frequency bands, we found signicant Granger causal inuence from the amygdala to hippocampus but not in the reciprocal direction (all Po0.01, permutation test, Fig. 4a and Supplementary
Fig. 13a) only for the aversive condition. Signicant differences between the aversive compared with the neutral condition were evident in eight out of nine subjects (Supplementary Fig. 14). The PSI analysis showed positive phase slopes from the amygdala to the hippocampus for the aversive compared with the neutral condition that remained signicant between 0.58 and1.16 s after stimulus onset (Fig. 4b and Supplementary Fig. 13b). In the reverse direction, the PSI did not show a positive value and there were no differences between the two conditions. These ndings provide converging evidence of the amygdala to hippocampus directionality during processing of motivationally salient information.
DiscussionHere, we demonstrate that processing of motivationally salient stimuli depends on coordinated neural oscillations between the amygdala and hippocampus, two critical nodes in the emotion processing circuit. Prior works have suggested that the amygdala rapidly detects salient stimuli, whereas the hippocampus engages in contextual and mnemonic processing7,31; however, the nature of their interaction and timing remains unclear. The current ndings show that the interaction between the two structures is mediated by low-frequency phase coherence and that this relationship is directional, with amygdala low-frequency oscillations entraining hippocampal gamma. Our results are robust across individual subjects and provide a mechanism by which the amygdala inuences hippocampal activity during recognition and emotional memory of salient information. We were able to disentangle the temporal dynamics of the interaction between the amygdala and hippocampus during processing of aversive stimuli by demonstrating an earlier post-stimulus onset of HG activity in the amygdala (as early as 120 ms post-stimulus) relative to the hippocampus (B240 ms post-stimulus). Overall, these ndings showed that salient stimuli are processed with distinct temporal windows in the amygdala and hippocampus. The early amygdala activity observed in this study may reect the fast automatic detection of motivational salience of information, while the later hippocampal onset may indicate formation or reactivation of emotional memory7,31.
6 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14413 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 ARTICLE
Previous research in affective processing using intracranial electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings in humans has focused on either the amygdala or hippocampus, but the oscillatory mechanism mediating communication across the two structures has remained unclear. Using retrograde tracing techniques in monkeys hippocampal formation, Amaral and Cowan demonstrated that labelled neurons were found predominately in the anterior amygdaloid area, basolateral nucleus and periamygdaloid cortex32. The perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices have connections with the basolateral and accessory nuclei33. Although the primate tracing study could not provide anatomical specicity at the level of hippocampal subelds, rodent studies have shown that the ventral CA1 region and, to a lesser extent, CA3 receive the most robust amygdalar inputs34. Our ndings are consistent with these known anatomical connections.
We further demonstrate that amygdala-hippocampal interactions were predominantly mediated through low-frequency coherence. Specically, the PLV between the amygdala and hippocampus was enhanced when participants viewed fearful faces compared with landscapes. Given the long temporal window afforded by low-frequency rhythms, phasephase coupling between different brain regions has been extensively studied in animal emotional research to understand regulation of inter-regional communication. In rodent models, the degree of fearful memory retrieval35,36 and its long-term consolidation37 are directly related to the degree of theta synchrony across the amygdala and hippocampus, implicating synchronous low-frequency oscillations as the neural correlate of fearful memory in the medial temporal network. Our study utilizing human intracranial EEG recording and precise medial temporal lobe subeld segmentation provides high-resolution temporo-spatial evidence for a similar oscillatory amygdala-hippocampal network in human salience processing and converges with the extant animal literature.
The directional inuence of the amygdala onto hippocampus in anxiety and fearful emotional responses is increasingly recognized in animal models. Optogenetic manipulation of this circuit has demonstrated that selective activation of BLA terminals within the ventral hippocampus increases anxiety-related behaviour and reduces social interactions10. During contextual fear learning, enhancement of GABA neuro-transmission in the BLA promotes hippocampal dendritic spine remodelling and fear retention, while blockade of GABA sites in BLA ameliorates these structural and behavioural effects12. In response to predatory threats, hippocampal place cells showed unstable ring patterns and increased theta power, an effect that was prevented by lesioning of the amygdala11. Thus, the modulatory effects of the amygdala on the hippocampus for processing of fearful stimuli are supported by animal studies demonstrating downstream alterations in neurotransmission, structure and electrophysiology. Our results provide conrmation of this directional modulation in humans and provide evidence that this modulation is controlled by neural oscillations. Specically, amygdala low-frequency activity modulates hippocampal HG via amygdala-hippocampal theta/ alpha synchrony during detection of motivationally salient information, providing a mechanism for the computation of local and long-range communication. These ndings have implications for understanding decits in the processing of salient environmental events in neuropsychiatric disorders with altered oscillatory control38.
There are several limitations in the current study. First, the two stimulus sets (fearful faces and landscapes) are inherently different in their properties. Thus, although we performed several analyses to show that evoked broadband activity did not
inuence the obtained results, potential contributions of evoked response could not be completely excluded. Second, the stimuli lacked neutral faces to contrast fearful faces and thus the differences between fearful faces and landscapes could be attributed to fearful content, emotion, facial expression, faces or other aversive stimuli. While our stimuli do not specically address processing of fearful faces, contrasting fearful face movie clips with landscapes allows us to probe oscillatory mechanisms underlying processing of motivationally salient information. In accord with the recent literature highlighting a broader function of the amygdala, including processing of valence, emotion and value, we believe that our ndings would generalize to other stimuli with social or survival signicance39.
Methods
Participants. Data were obtained from nine patients (four female, ve male, age 2458, Supplementary Table 1) who had stereotactically implanted intracranial depth electrodes (Integra or Ad-Tech, 5 mm inter-electrode spacing) placed at the University of California, Irvine, Medical Center to localize the seizure onset zone for possible surgical resection. The institutional review boards of University of California at Berkeley and at Irvine approved the research, and written informed consent was obtained from each subject before testing. Electrode placement was exclusively guided by clinical needs, and patient selection was solely based on MRI conrmed depth electrode placement in the amygdala and hippocampus. Recordings were conducted from four patients (subjects 14) with depth electrodes localized ipsilateral and ve patients (subjects 59) contralateral to or outside of seizure onset zone. There were no seizures recorded in any of the epochs, and any epochs with interictal epileptiform activity were removed from analysis. Comparable high gamma, PLV and PAC results were observed in all nine subjects, and there were no differences in the magnitude of effects between recordings from electrodes ipsilateral and contralateral to the seizure focus.
Depth electrode localization. MRI scans. Electrodes were localized in each participant using co-registered pre-implantation and post-implantation structural T1-weighted MRI scans. The pre-implantation scans were all 1 mm isotropic. The post-implantation scans were either 1 mm isotropic (subjects 1, 5, 6, 8 and 9) or 0.75 0.75 7 mm (subjects 24 and 7). For each participant, the
post-implantation scan was registered to the pre-implantation scan usinga six-parameter rigid body transformation (three rotations and three translations in xz directions), implemented in Advanced Normalization Tools(ANTs http://stnava.github.io/ANTs/
Web End =http://stnava.github.io/ANTs/ )40.
Anatomical masking. For determining exact electrode locations, we useda high-resolution anatomical template (0.55 mm) developed in our laboratory with manual tracings of hippocampal subelds and amygdala nuclei. This template has been used in past studies4143. Regions of interest (ROIs) in the medial temporal lobe included the DG/CA3, CA1, subiculum, entorhinal cortex, perirhinal cortex, parahippocampal cortex, BLA, central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA), and the cortical nuclei of the amygdala (CORT)44,45. Segmentations for hippocampal subelds followed our previously published protocols46. Briey, the segmentation included DG/CA3, CA1 and subiculum, and procedures followed closely the atlas of Duvernoy44, in which the subelds are dened along the anterior-posterior axis of the hippocampus. Amygdala segmentation procedures were based onEntis et al.45, but were modied to dene three regions: BLA, CeA and CORT. To label these sub-regions of the amygdala, three key points were identied: (1) the medial tip of the alveus (up to the optic tract); (2) the most lateral point of the entorhinal sulcus; and (3) bottom of the circular sulcus. These three points are easily observable and provide a reliable landmarking system for segmenting the amygdala sub-regions. After identifying these points, lines were drawn to connect the three points to each other, creating four quadrants (basomedial and basolateral were combined to form the basolateral complex).
Labelling individual participant scans. The labelled high-resolution anatomical template (resampled to 1 mm isotropic) was aligned to each individuals pre-implantation scan using ANTs Symmetric Normalization40, such that the labels could be applied to each participants MRI. This allowed for visualization and accurate identication of electrode locations using anatomical labels in each participants space. Each electrode location was determined by selecting the centre of the electrode artefact and identifying the region of interest that encompassed the centre. Cases where electrodes were on the border between ROIs or between grey matter and white matter were noted as such.
Behavioural task. Participants watched silent movie clips on a laptop computer placed on the service tray at a comfortable distance in front of them. The total length of the task was about 7 min, consisting of alternating 9 blocks of landscapes and eight blocks of fearful faces (B24 s/block, see Fig. 1a). During the experiment, participants viewed 70 landscapes and 71 fearful face clips (clip duration:
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14413 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 7
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413
2.81.3 s; means.d.). Within each block, movie clips were shown continuously without breaks. This paradigm was used in previous fMRI studies and demonstrated reliable amygdala activation47,48.
Data collection and preprocessing. Intracranial EEG data were acquired using a Nihon Kohden recording system (256 channel amplier, model JE120A), analogue-ltered above 0.01 Hz and digitally sampled at 5,000 Hz. All the data were analysed in MATLAB combined with open source toolboxes and custom scripts. After acquisition, neuronal recordings were band-pass ltered from 0.1 to 350 Hz using a zero phase delay nite impulse response (FIR) lter with Hamming window and then down-sampled to 2,000 Hz for subsequent analyses. A regression method49 was used to lter 60 Hz noise and its harmonics. All hippocampal and amygdala electrodes were re-referenced to the nearest electrode located in white matter. A neurologist with subspecialty training in epilepsy (J.J.L.) manually inspected all EEG signals to identify and remove all data with interictal epileptiform discharges as well as excessive noise, including broadband electromagnetic noise from hospital equipment. To avoid potentially biasingthe results, the neurologist was blinded to the location and behavioural task(for example, aversive versus neutral condition) associated with EEG signals and the artefact rejection was performed before additional data processing.
Electro-oculogram and perisaccadic high gamma activity. To investigate the possibility of eye-movement-related contamination of our results, one subject (S9) was recorded along with a free head xation eye tracker (SensoMotoric Instruments, Inc. RED 250 mobile) and EOG recordings. The eye tracker, connected to the laptop with Universal Serial Bus (USB), was placed at the inner edge of the presentation laptop. Before the task, the eye tracker was calibrated using the iView RED-m software. Timing between the eye tracker recording and the video clips was synchronized through an E-Prime script. After averaging the velocity of the eyes in the X and Y dimensions from the eye tracker data across clips, two-sample t-tests were performed for each data point to test for signicant differences in patterns of eye movements between conditions that could affect the results. In addition to the eye movement analysis, we estimated the time course of high gamma activity (HG) in the vertical and horizontal EOG. Again, two-sample t-tests were performed for each data point to test for signicant differences between conditions. As a third test to investigate the contribution of ocular activity to our ndings, we used ICA (Bell-Sejnowski ICA algorithm)28, implemented in the EEGLAB toolbox for Matlab on the EOG combined with white matter re-referenced amygdala and hippocampal activity to compute the weights of contributions from each channel of data to each independent component. Any components composed of mostly EOG activity were removed from the data by zeroing those components and re-projecting the remaining components into the channel space. All analyses were then re-run with this corrected data.
Saccades were detected through a velocity-based algorithm6. The velocity of eye movement was estimated as the rst derivative of the Euclidean distance between eye positions at successive sample points. A threshold was set at the 99th percentile of velocities and saccades were marked at the time points of the peak velocities that surpassed this threshold, with a minimum spacing of 200 milliseconds between saccade events. High gamma (70180 Hz) activity in hippocampal and amygdala contacts was then epoched based on these events. After baseline correction( 200 ms to 0 ms), two-sample t-tests were performed for each data point to
determine signicant differences between conditions. To further investigate the possibility that PAC analyses could have been inuenced by EOG activity, we calculated the PAC using the gamma-range component of the EOG signal and low-frequency phase from the hippocampus and amygdala (the same analysis as described in the Methods Phase-amplitude coupling section). We observed no signicant PAC pattern between the amygdala or hippocampus and the EOG HG signal, which indicates that there was no appreciable inuence from potential eye movement artifacts on the PAC analysis.
Power spectral density (PSD) and subject-specic low-frequency band.
PSD was estimated using Welchs method50 (pwelch.m in Signal Processing Toolbox from MATLAB) wherein the PSD was estimated for each subject separately using 1 s time windows with a 50% overlap. The slope of the power spectrum was estimated using the linear regression approach in a semi-log space, where the power P at each discrete frequency f was estimated from the frequency itself using the following formula:
Pf f 0b e 1
where f 0 is a two column matrix composed of the discrete frequency bands of interest and a column of ones; b is the regression coefcient (the slope of the model), and e is the error term. In intracranial power spectra, b is typically negative, which is important given that task-related increases in neural activity result in a broadband upward shift within the high gamma range51. To characterize spectral dependence between the amygdala and hippocampus in a narrow band, we selected the subject-specic low-frequency band based on the PSD plots. Specically, the value of exponent w in the power-law relation P 1f was obtained
by tting a straight line to the experimentally measured PSD. Then the distances between the PSD and the tting curve were calculated at each frequency point. The
subject-specic band was dened as the farthest points from the tting curve within the low-frequency range with a bandwidth of 4 Hz.
Frequency decomposition. Data from the amygdala and hippocampus were rst ltered into subject-specic low-frequency bands (Amygdala:xAmyy 6.00.49 Hz; hippocampus: xHippy 6.50.16 Hz; means.e.m.) and
high gamma (xg, 70180 Hz) using a two-way, zero-phase lag, least-squaresFIR lter to prevent phase distortion (eeglt.m function in EEGLAB toolbox52).
The length of the lter in points was determined by the specic frequency, cycle number (usually 45 cycles) and sampling rate. The centre frequency of each frequency bin is spaced apart by 0.1 of the lower-frequency band and the bandwidth varied by multiplying the fractional bandwidth index 0.3 with the centre frequency. For example, for the frequency bin centred at 10 Hz, the next frequency bin is 10 0.1 10 11 Hz and the bandwidth is 10 0.3 2 6 Hz. Since
we choose adaptive rather than xed bandwidths, the bandwidths within low-frequency range were sufciently narrow to dene a meaningful phase while the bandwidths within higher-frequency range were broad enough to t the sidebands caused by the assumed modulating lower-frequency band27. We then applied the Hilbert transform (hilbert.m function in Signal Processing Toolbox from MATLAB) to estimate the amplitude (ax[n]) and phase (yx[n]) for both bands, which yields a complex time series:
Sx n ax n
eif n 2
where ax[n] represents the instantaneous analytic amplitude and fx[n] is the instantaneous phase.
Event-related potentials. The ERPs were calculated individually for both the amygdala and hippocampus separately. The LFP signals were rst low-pass ltered at 30 Hz using the same lter parameters as described in Methods (Frequency decomposition section). Then the ltered signals were segmented into epochs, and the ERPs were calculated by averaging across these epochs within each condition. After baseline correction by subtracting the mean baseline( 500 ms to 0 ms) value from all data points, two-sample t-tests were performed
for each data point to determine signicant differences between conditions.
High gamma activity. To investigate the time course of HG activity within the amygdala and hippocampus, we extracted the high-frequency banded signal using the frequency decomposition and averaged the results within each region. Specically, the HG amplitude data were divided into epochs from 0.5 to 1.5 s
from the start of each movie clip. Data within each trial were normalized by converting each data point into a z-score relative to the entire trial time-series.
The computed z-scores were then baseline corrected by subtracting an average of pre-stimulus baseline data points (a 500 ms time period taken from the beginning of the session, when the blank screen was presented, see Fig. 1a). This allows us to eliminate the positive skew typically observed for HG amplitude values53. We then statistically compared the event-related HG amplitude changes between the landscape and the fearful face conditions using a cluster-based permutation test, computing statistics at the cluster level and correcting for multiple comparisons54. First, all the trials from the two experimental conditions were randomly shufed, and the means of each condition were subtracted 1,000 times to create a null distribution of differences between conditions. T-scores were then computed for each null difference time series by comparing it to the entire distribution of null difference time series at every time point. Then, all t-scores corresponding to uncorrected P values of 0.05 or less were formed into clusters with any neighbouring such t-scores. The sum of the t-scores in each cluster is the mass of that cluster and the most extreme cluster mass in each of the 1,000 sets of tests was recorded and used to estimate the distribution of the null hypothesis. Finally, clusters were obtained from the true data, and the percentage of null cluster masses greater than each true data cluster mass was taken as the corrected P value for that cluster. Onsets and offsets of the HG activity for each condition were computed by taking the rst and last time sample that passed a signicance level of Po0.05. Time-to-peak was dened for fearful face and landscape conditions as the latency of the maximum amplitude in the HG range across all trials within each condition during the range of time where a signicant difference between the two conditions was found.
Phase locking value (PLV). To quantify the inter-electrode low-frequency phase coupling, phase differences were calculated for each electrode pair (i, j) using the phase time series fx[n] obtained from equation (2). The frequency range covers the low-frequency peaks from both the amygdala and hippocampus (identied in PSD) with a bandwidth of 4 Hz. The phase difference between the two time courses fyij[n] indexes the coherence between each electrode pair and is expressed asthe PLV index (equation (3)). PLVs range between 0 and 1, with values approaching 1 if the phase differences between the electrodes vary little across time.
PLV
1 N
3
The ltered data were segmented into one-second windows and then separated according to task condition before computing a PLV for each electrode pair and
XNn1ef n
8 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14413 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 ARTICLE
condition. Relative increases (or decreases) in PLV in response to fearful faces versus landscape scenes were assessed by subtracting the two PLVs for each electrode pair. To test the signicance of differences between two conditions and, at the same time, maximally eliminate the inuence of ERPs, we did a two-step approach as described below. We rst permuted trials and computed the99th percentile threshold within each condition. Only the electrode pairs whose PLVs rose above the threshold for both conditions were selected for calculating the PLV difference. Then the statistical signicance for each electrode pair was estimated using a cluster-based permutation test, in which a null distribution was created by randomly assigning trials (i.e., one-second windows) into two conditions, computing the relative PLV differences between conditions, and repeating this procedure 1,000 times. The observed data were then compared with this null distribution to estimate a P value. The results were depicted as hive plots to visualize the magnitude of the PLV and the associated P values among pairs of electrodes55.
To test the regional specicity of this phase coupling effect, an analysis of variance with pairwise comparisons was conducted to test differences between hippocampal sub-regions using the R statistic toolbox. First, theShapiroWilk test (function Shapiro.test) was conducted to test whether the data were approximately normally distributed. Second, the homogeneity of variances was tested using Bartletts test (function Bartlett.test). Then the pairwise comparison was computed using the function pairwise t-test. In addition, to further address the continuous consistency of the phase relationship between the amygdala and hippocampus, the PLV spectra were created as a function of frequency (130 Hz) for all BLA and hippocampal electrode pairs averaged across all subjects for each condition and the difference was calculated by subtracting neutral from aversive condition and z-score normalized. A fth-order spline interpolation was implemented (spapi.m in Curve tting toolbox from MATLAB) to smooth the curve of PLV difference spectra.
Phase-amplitude coupling. On the basis of the previous phase coupling analysis, we analysed the PAC between BLA and hippocampus electrode pairs that exhibited the most signicant PLV in each patient. To extract the directionality information from these electrode pairs, the relationship between lower-frequency (130 Hz; delta, theta, alpha, beta) phase from electrode i and higher-frequency (30250 Hz; gamma) amplitude from electrode j was examined individually for each condition by calculating the circular linear correlation between the instantaneous phase of low-frequency and the instantaneous phase of high frequency56.
rf
r2
ca
1
A; F v
b
2 ; v
b
2 5
PnNn1 zif ; n z jf ; n is the cross-spectrum between two time series, n is the centre frequency of
the targeted frequency range, and Im denotes the imaginary part. We use b as the bandwidth for which the phase slope is calculated and choose it to be eight times of the frequency resolution df29. For example, when calculating directionality fora phase modulation centred at 8 Hz rhythm with a 1 Hz frequency resolution,a phase slope estimated between 4 and 12 Hz is reasonable (b 4 Hz). To
understand interaction and directionality between the signals involved in cross frequency coupling, we segmented the phase of the modulating signal and the power envelope of the modulated signal into N epochs and used them as inputs to calculate the PSI for both aversive and neutral conditions. As the interactions between modulating and modulated signals require certain amount of time, and if the speed at which these waves travel is similar, the sign of the PSI informs about which signal is temporally leading the other one. In other words, when the phase differences between the sender and recipient signal increase with the corresponding frequencies, a positive slope of the phase spectrum is expected. By performing the PSI analysis with a sliding window of 100 ms, spaced at 25 ms (75% overlap), we were able to track the switching of the directionality between two signals at each time point. To assess the statistical signicance of PSI, we applied the analogous non-parametric approach as describe above by randomly shufing the trials within a condition. Then the PSI null distribution was created at each time point, and the 99.5th percentile threshold was dened for each condition separately.
Granger causality. To further investigate interactions between brain regions, we computed spectral Granger causality. Granger causality represents how much introducing past measurements from a rst time series can decrease the variance of the prediction error for a second time series at the current time point. Spectral Granger causality extends this analysis to the frequency domain59,60. The time-domain data was rst low-pass ltered at 85 Hz and then down-sampled to 250 Hz before tting to an autoregressive model and computing spectral Granger causality. The model order m was determined by the Akaike information criterion61 (using the Multivariate Granger Causality (MGVC) Matlab Toolbox), which is a tradeoff between sufcient spectral resolution and over-parameterization. Model orders were estimated for each patient and variedfrom 7 to 13. The Granger index was computed using the rst 1,500 ms from each clip as trial realizations. To further address whether this directionality was task specic, the null hypothesis distributions of spectral Granger causality for each condition were created by randomly swapping clip segments between channels. The spectral Granger causality was considered signicant if they exceededthe 99% condence interval of this null hypothesis distribution. To further test
s
4
r2 sa
2rcarsarcs
1 r
where Ci;j f
S f
p is the complex coherence, Si;j f
1N
S f S f
2 cs
Where rca c(cosfyi[n], agj[n]), rsa c(sinfyi[n], agj[n]) and rcs c(sinfyj[n],
cosfgj[n]) with c(x, y) equal to the Pearson correlation between x and y, f[n] equals to the instantaneous phase from the modulating signal, and a[n] equals to the instantaneous analytic amplitude from the modulated signal. To compare the signicance of the difference between correlation coefcients r1 and r2, we applied
Fishers z-transform to normalize correlation coefcients such that zr 12 log 1r1 r
, and calculated the difference Drz z(r1) z(r2) (ref. 57). The
permutation test described for the previous analyses was also used here to create a null distribution of PAC differences between two conditions for each electrode pair. Then the z-PAC was dened as the z-score of the real difference in PAC between the two conditions based on the distribution of phase-amplitude coupling values obtained from these null distributions with the positive value indicating PAC increase in the aversive condition versus neutral condition.
Phase-amplitude coupling with time lag. As the communication between the modulating and modulated signal builds, the carrier oscillation is likely to appear rst in the modulating signal. In other words, immediately preceding the synchronization between two signals, the driver signals should be predictive of the receiver signal. To probe the direction of functional coupling and to further explore how the neuronal dynamics in low-frequency and gamma bands interact, we examined the PAC magnitude as a function of the time lag between modulating and modulated signals. A diagram of the data processing steps is shown in Supplementary Fig. 12. The cross-regional PAC was calculated using the same algorithm as outlined in the PAC analysis section but with different time lags(in 10 ms intervals for the total duration range of 200 ms) by shifting one signal relative to the other one.
Power spectral density (PSD) of the high gamma envelope. The goal of this step was to identify rhythmic uctuations present in the higher-frequency power time series. We tested the assumption that if the power time series within the higher-frequency band are synchronized with a lower-frequency oscillation, the power time series will not be constant over time but instead will uctuate at the specic lower oscillation frequency58. For example, if a signal banded at 80 Hz is synchronized with a 6 Hz oscillation, the power time series of the 80 Hz might itself oscillate at 6 Hz. To test this hypothesis, we rst selected the subject-specic high-frequency band centred at the frequency with the strongest PAC and bandwidth of 40 Hz. Then, the PSDs of the selected high-frequency bands were
calculated using the same methods as described in Methods (Power spectral
density and subject-specic low-frequency band section).
Theta-trough locked averaging of timefrequency plot. A theta-trough locked timefrequency averaging plot for a single subject was created to demonstrate the oscillatory features of the underlying signal5. To create the tracing in the lower panels of Supplementary Fig. 9, the raw signal from the amygdala/hippocampus was rst ltered within subject-specic low-frequency band (5.89.8 Hz for this subject) using the same FIR lter parameters as described in the Methods (Frequency decomposition section). By applying the Hilbert transform, the phase at each time point was extracted from the ltered data within ( p, p], where
p radians corresponded to a theta trough and 0 radians corresponded to a theta peak (cosine phase). Then, the signal was segmented into 1 s epochs centred at the theta troughs, which were identied as the local minima of the phase less than( p 0.01) and the theta-trough locked ERP were generated by averaging across
all epochs. For Supplementary Fig. 9 upper panel, a set of normalized instantaneous power time series from the hippocampus/amygdala was constructed, with centre frequencies ranging from 1 to 250 Hz. To facilitate comparisons between different frequency bands, the band-pass ltered signals within the specic frequency range were rst normalized by subtracting the temporal mean and dividing by the temporal s.d. The normalized instantaneous power time series were generated by applying the Hilbert transform and taking the square of the extracted amplitude time series. These power series were then segmented into the epochs centred on the corresponding theta trough (Supplementary Fig. 9 lower panel), P values for each time point in each frequency band were calculated using the non-parametric permutation test described in Methods (High gamma activity section).
Phase slope index (PSI). As an index of dominant unidirectional interaction30, PSI indicates the direction of coupling between two systems. Given a pre-specied bandwidth parameter, it reects the change of phase difference between neighbouring frequency bins, weighted with the magnitude of the coherence. On the basis of the assumption that independent sources do not contribute to the imaginary part of the cross-spectrum, the PSI is dened as
fci;j Im
Xf 2FC i:j f Ci;j f df
0
@
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14413 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 9
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413
whether the Granger causality is signicantly different between conditions, the null difference time series were computed by subtracting the Granger time series from the aversive condition and the ones from the neutral condition, which were calculated by randomly ipping the segments between conditions. Then foreach frequency point, there were two Granger values as the upper or lower range of the 99% condence interval, where 98% of the null Granger difference time series were either less or greater than these values. It shows a greater Granger for the aversive condition compared with the neutral one if the real difference line is above the upper condence interval while the neutral condition has a stronger Granger if the real difference line is below the lower condence interval.
Data availability. The data that support the ndings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.
References
1. Adolphs, R. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat. Rev. 4, 165178 (2003).
2. Janak, P. H. & Tye, K. M. From circuits to behaviour in the amygdala. Nature 517, 284292 (2015).
3. Jin, J., Zelano, C., Gottfried, J. A. & Mohanty, A. Human amygdala represents the complete spectrum of subjective valence. J. Neurosci. 35, 1514515156 (2015).
4. Jennings, J. H. et al. Distinct extended amygdala circuits for divergent motivational states. Nature 496, 224228 (2013).
5. Ji, J. & Maren, S. Hippocampal involvement in contextual modulation of fear extinction. Hippocampus 17, 749758 (2007).
6. Wang, S. et al. Neurons in the human amygdala selective for perceived emotion. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, E3110E3119 (2014).
7. Holland, A. C. & Kensinger, E. A. Emotion and autobiographical memory. Phys. Life Rev. 7, 88131 (2010).
8. McGaugh, J. L. The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 128 (2004).
9. LeDoux, J. E. The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life (Simon & Schuster, 1996).
10. Allsop, S. A., Vander Weele, C. M., Wichmann, R. & Tye, K. M. Optogenetic insights on the relationship between anxiety-related behaviors and social decits. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 241 (2014).
11. Kim, E. J. et al. Alterations of hippocampal place cells in foraging rats facing a predatory threat. Curr. Biol. 25, 13621367 (2015).
12. Giachero, M., Calfa, G. D. & Molina, V. A. Hippocampal dendritic spines remodeling and fear memory are modulated by GABAergic signaling within the basolateral amygdala complex. Hippocampus 25, 545555 (2015).
13. Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., Petry, M. C. & Lang, P. J. Remembering pictures: pleasure and arousal in memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn Mem. Cogn. 18, 379390 (1992).
14. Kensinger, E. A. & Schacter, D. L. Remembering the specic visual details of presented objects: neuroimaging evidence for effects of emotion. Neuropsychologia 45, 29512962 (2007).
15. Segal, S. K. & Cahill, L. Endogenous noradrenergic activation and memory for emotional material in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 12631271 (2009).
16. Buzsaki, G., Anastassiou, C. A. & Koch, C. The origin of extracellular elds and currents--EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes. Nat. Rev. 13, 407420 (2012).17. Buzsaki, G. & Moser, E. I. Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 130138 (2013).
18. Pape, H. C. & Driesang, R. B. Ionic mechanisms of intrinsic oscillations in neurons of the basolateral amygdaloid complex. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 217226 (1998).
19. Likhtik, E. & Gordon, J. A. Circuits in sync: decoding theta communication in fear and safety. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 235236 (2014).
20. LaBar, K. S., Crupain, M. J., Voyvodic, J. T. & McCarthy, G. Dynamic perception of facial affect and identity in the human brain. Cereb. Cortex 13, 10231033 (2003).
21. Buzsaki, G. & Draguhn, A. Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science
304, 19261929 (2004).
22. Bastos, A. M., Vezoli, J. & Fries, P. Communication through coherence with inter-areal delays. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 31, 173180 (2015).
23. Liu, J. & Newsome, W. T. Local eld potential in cortical area MT: stimulus tuning and behavioral correlations. J. Neurosci. 26, 77797790 (2006).
24. Mussel, P., Ulrich, N., Allen, J. J., Osinsky, R. & Hewig, J. Patterns of theta oscillation reect the neural basis of individual differences in epistemic motivation. Sci. Rep. 6, 29245 (2016).
25. von Nicolai, C. et al. Corticostriatal coordination through coherent phase-amplitude coupling. J. Neurosci. 34, 59385948 (2014).
26. Sweeney-Reed, C. M. et al. Thalamic theta phase alignment predicts human memory formation and anterior thalamic cross-frequency coupling. eLife 4 (2015).
27. Aru, J. et al. Untangling cross-frequency coupling in neuroscience. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 31, 5161 (2015).
28. Bell, A. J. & Sejnowski, T. J. An information-maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural Comput. 7, 11291159 (1995).
29. Jiang, H., Bahramisharif, A., van Gerven, M. A. & Jensen, O. Measuring directionality between neuronal oscillations of different frequencies. Neuroimage 118, 359367 (2015).
30. Nolte, G. et al. Robustly estimating the ow direction of information in complex physical systems. Phys. Rev. Lett. 100, 234101 (2008).
31. Phelps, E. A. Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 198202 (2004).
32. Amaral, D. G. & Cowan, W. M. Subcortical afferents to the hippocampal formation in the monkey. J. Comparat. Neurol. 189, 573591 (1980).
33. Stefanacci, L., Suzuki, W. A. & Amaral, D. G. Organization of connections between the amygdaloid complex and the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices in macaque monkeys. J. Comparat. Neurol. 375, 552582 (1996).
34. McDonald, A. J. & Mott, D. D. Functional neuroanatomy of amygdalohippocampal interconnections and their role in learning and memory. J. Neurosci. Res 95, 797820 (2017).
35. Seidenbecher, T., Laxmi, T. R., Stork, O. & Pape, H. C. Amygdalar and hippocampal theta rhythm synchronization during fear memory retrieval. Science 301, 846850 (2003).
36. Popa, D., Duvarci, S., Popescu, A. T., Lena, C. & Pare, D. Coherent amygdalocortical theta promotes fear memory consolidation during paradoxical sleep. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 65166519 (2010).
37. Pape, H. C., Narayanan, R. T., Smid, J., Stork, O. & Seidenbecher, T. Theta activity in neurons and networks of the amygdala related to long-term fear memory. Hippocampus 15, 874880 (2005).
38. Voytek, B. & Knight, R. T. Dynamic network communication as a unifying neural basis for cognition, development, aging, and disease. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 10891097 (2015).
39. Morrison, S. E. & Salzman, C. D. Re-valuing the amygdala. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 221230 (2010).
40. Avants, B. B. et al. A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. Neuroimage 54, 20332044 (2011).
41. Leal, S. L., Tighe, S. K. & Yassa, M. A. Asymmetric effects of emotion on mnemonic interference. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 111, 4148 (2014).
42. Reagh, Z. M., Watabe, J., Ly, M., Murray, E. & Yassa, M. A. Dissociated signals in human dentate gyrus and CA3 predict different facets of recognition memory. J. Neurosci. 34, 1330113313 (2014).
43. Reagh, Z. M. & Yassa, M. A. Object and spatial mnemonic interference differentially engage lateral and medial entorhinal cortex in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, E4264E4273 (2014).
44. Duvernoy, H. M. The Human Hippocampus: Functional Anatomy, Vascularization, and Serial Sections with MRI 3rd edn (Springer, 2005).
45. Entis, J. J., Doerga, P., Barrett, L. F. & Dickerson, B. C. A reliable protocol for the manual segmentation of the human amygdala and its subregions using ultra-high resolution MRI. Neuroimage 60, 12261235 (2012).
46. Yassa, M. A. & Stark, C. E. A quantitative evaluation of cross-participant registration techniques for MRI studies of the medial temporal lobe. Neuroimage 44, 319327 (2009).
47. Schacher, M. et al. Amygdala fMRI lateralizes temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 66, 8187 (2006).
48. Riley, J. D., Fling, B. W., Cramer, S. C. & Lin, J. J. Altered organization of face-processing networks in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 56, 762771 (2015).
49. Mitra, P. & Bokil, H. Observed Brain Dynamics. Chapter 7.3.4 (Oxford University Press, 2007).
50. Welch, P. D. The use of Fast Fourier Transform for the estimation of power spectra: a method based on time averaging over short, modied periodograms. IEEE Trans. Audio Electroacoust. 15, 7073 (1967).
51. Miller, K. J., Sorensen, L. B., Ojemann, J. G. & den Nijs, M. Power-law scaling in the brain surface electric potential. PLoS Comput. Biol. 5, e1000609 (2009).
52. Delorme, A. & Makeig, S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 921 (2004).
53. Grandchamp, R. & Delorme, A. Single-trial normalization for event-related spectral decomposition reduces sensitivity to noisy trials. Front. Psychol. 2, 236 (2011).
54. Maris, E. & Oostenveld, R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177190 (2007).
55. Krzywinski, M., Birol, I., Jones, S. J. & Marra, M. A. Hive plots--rational approach to visualizing networks. Brief. Bioinform. 13, 627644 (2012).
56. Berens, P. CircStat: a MATLAB Toolbox for circular statistics. J. Stat. Softw. 31, 121 (2009).
10 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14413 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 ARTICLE
57. Voytek, B. et al. Shifts in gamma phase-amplitude coupling frequency from theta to alpha over posterior cortex during visual tasks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4, 191 (2010).
58. Cohen, M. X. Assessing transient cross-frequency coupling in EEG data.J. Neurosci. Methods 168, 494499 (2008).59. Ding, M., Bressler, S. L., Yang, W. & Liang, H. Short-window spectral analysis of cortical event-related potentials by adaptive multivariate autoregressive modeling: data preprocessing, model validation, and variability assessment. Biol. Cybern. 83, 3545 (2000).
60. Ding, M., Chen, Y. & Bressler, S. L. in Handbook of Time Series Analysisx (eds Schelter, B, Winderhalder, M. & Timmer, J.) 437460 (Wiley-VCH, 2006).
61. Akaike, H. A new look at the statistical model identication. IEEE Trans. Autom. Control 19, 716723 (1974).
Acknowledgements
We thank Prof Hennric Jokeit and colleagues at the Swiss Epilepsy Center, Hirslanden Hospital, Zurich, Switzerland, for providing the video stimuli. We thank Dr Mehdi Abouzari and Rebecca Stevenson for assistance in manuscript preparation andDr Randolph Frederik Helfrich for input with analysis methods and helpful comments on the manuscript. This study was supported by NINDS 2R37NS021135, NIDCDR01 007293, and the Nielsen Corporation to R.T.K. and UCI School of Medicine Bridge Fund and NINDS K23 NS060993 to J.J.L.
Author contributions
J.Z. and K.L.A. collected, analysed and interpreted the data; S.L.L. performed electrode localization; A.S. and G.G. assisted with the analysis; S.V., L.M., F.P.K.H. managed
patients and surgeries; R.T.K., M.A.Y., J.J.L. conceived the study, interpreted the data and wrote the manuscript with input from all authors.
Additional information
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =http://www.nature.com/ http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =naturecommunications
Competing nancial interests: The authors declare no competing nancial interests.
Reprints and permission information is available online at http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =http://npg.nature.com/ http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =reprintsandpermissions/
How to cite this article: Zheng, J. et al. Amygdala-hippocampal dynamics during salient information processing. Nat. Commun. 8, 14413 doi: 10.1038/ncomms14413 (2017).
Publishers note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional afliations.
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articles Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if the material is not included under the Creative Commons license, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to reproduce the material. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Web End =http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
r The Author(s) 2017
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14413 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14413 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 11
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Nature Publishing Group Feb 2017
Abstract
Recognizing motivationally salient information is critical to guiding behaviour. The amygdala and hippocampus are thought to support this operation, but the circuit-level mechanism of this interaction is unclear. We used direct recordings in the amygdala and hippocampus from human epilepsy patients to examine oscillatory activity during processing of fearful faces compared with neutral landscapes. We report high gamma (70-180 Hz) activation for fearful faces with earlier stimulus evoked onset in the amygdala compared with the hippocampus. Attending to fearful faces compared with neutral landscape stimuli enhances low-frequency coupling between the amygdala and the hippocampus. The interaction between the amygdala and hippocampus is largely unidirectional, with theta/alpha oscillations in the amygdala modulating hippocampal gamma activity. Granger prediction, phase slope index and phase lag analysis corroborate this directional coupling. These results demonstrate that processing emotionally salient events in humans engages an amygdala-hippocampal network, with the amygdala influencing hippocampal dynamics during fear processing.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer