ARTICLE
Received 14 Dec 2015 | Accepted 2 Dec 2016 | Published 21 Feb 2017
DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 OPEN
Blunted ventral striatal responses to anticipated rewards foreshadow problematic drug use in novelty-seeking adolescents
Christian Bchel1,2, Jan Peters1, Tobias Banaschewski3, Arun L.W Bokde4, Uli Bromberg1, Patricia J. Conrod5,6, Herta Flor7, Dimitri Papadopoulos8, Hugh Garavan9,10, Penny Gowland11, Andreas Heinz12, Henrik Walter12, Bernd Ittermann13, Karl Mann14, Jean-Luc Martinot15,16, Marie-Laure Paillre-Martinot15,16, Frauke Nees7, Tomas Paus17,18,19,20, Zdenka Pausova21, Luise Poustka3, Marcella Rietschel7, Trevor W. Robbins22, Michael N. Smolka23,
Juergen Gallinat12, Gunter Schumann5,24, Brian Knutson2, & the IMAGEN consortiumw
Novelty-seeking tendencies in adolescents may promote innovation as well as problematic impulsive behaviour, including drug abuse. Previous research has not claried whether neural hyper- or hypo-responsiveness to anticipated rewards promotes vulnerability in these individuals. Here we use a longitudinal design to track 144 novelty-seeking adolescents at age 14 and 16 to determine whether neural activity in response to anticipated rewards predicts problematic drug use. We nd that diminished BOLD activity in mesolimbic (ventral striatal and midbrain) and prefrontal cortical (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) regions during reward anticipation at age 14 predicts problematic drug use at age 16. Lower psychometric conscientiousness and steeper discounting of future rewards at age 14 also predicts problematic drug use at age 16, but the neural responses independently predict more variance than psychometric measures. Together, these ndings suggest that diminished neural responses to anticipated rewards in novelty-seeking adolescents may increase vulnerability to future problematic drug use.
1 Department of Systems Neuroscience, Universitatsklinikum Hamburg Eppendorf, 20246 Hamburg, Germany. 2 Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California 94305, USA. 3 Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychiatry, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, 68159 Mannheim, Germany. 4 Institute of Neuroscience and Discipline of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin 2, Ireland. 5 Institute of Psychiatry, Kings College London, London SE5 8AF, UK. 6 Department of Psychiatry, Universit de Montreal, CHU Ste Justine Hospital, Montral, Qubec, Canada H3C 3J7. 7 Department of Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, 68159 Mannheim, Germany. 8 Commissariat lEnergie Atomique et aux Energies Alternatives, 14 CEA, DSV, I2BM, Neurospin bat 145, 91191 Gif-sur-Yvette, France. 9 Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin 2, Ireland. 10 Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington, Vermont 05401, USA. 11 School of Physics and Astronomy, University of Nottingham, Nottinghamshire NG7 2RD, UK. 12 Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charit Mitte, CharitUniversitatsmedizin Berlin, 10117 Berlin, Germany. 13 Physikalisch-Technische Bundesanstalt (PTB), 10587 Berlin, Germany. 14 Department of Addictive Behaviour and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, 68159 Mannheim, Germany. 15 Institut National de la Sant et de la Recherche Mdicale, INSERM Unit 1000 Imaging & Psychiatry, University Paris-Sud, 91400 Orsay, France. 16 Maison de Solenn, APHP Hpital Cochin, University Paris Descartes, 75006 Paris, France.
17 McGill University and Genome Quebec Innovation Centre, Montral, Qubec, Canada H3A 1A4. 18 Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5S 3E6. 19 School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, Nottinghamshire NG7 2RD, UK. 20 Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 2B4. 21 The Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario Canada, M5G 1X8.
22 Behavioural and Clinical Neurosciences Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK. 23 Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, and Neuroimaging Center, Technische Universitat Dresden, 01307 Dresden, Germany. 24 MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, London SE5 8AF, UK. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to B.K.(email: mailto:[email protected]
Web End [email protected] )
wA full list of consortium members appears at the end of the paper.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14140 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 1
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140
Individual differences in novelty seeking are associated with impulsive choice (or a preference for smaller but sooner over larger but later rewards)13. Specically, novelty-seeking traits
in adolescents4 can foreshadow later problematic behaviours including excessive drug use2,5,6. Novelty seeking, in general, and impulsive choice, in particular, may recruit distinct neural systems79 that include a motivational circuit comprising mesolimbic dopamine projections from the ventral tegmental area of the midbrain to the ventral striatum (VS)10 as well as a countervailing cognitive control circuit comprising prefrontal cortical (PFC) regions. The balance of activity in these circuits may shift over development, consistent with evidence for earlier development of the motivational circuit than the cognitive control circuit in humans1114. Since dopaminergic modulation of these circuits can inuence both motivation10 and cognitive control15, delays in the development of these circuits and their relative activity could increase impulsive choice, including drug use2.
Theoretical accounts differ, however, with respect to exactly how activity in these motivational and control circuits can inuence impulsive choice in adolescents16. On the one hand, impulsive choice in adolescents has been attributed to diminished motivation, such that drug abuse may reect attempts to compensate for motivational decits17,18. Support for this account has come from neuroimaging studies, suggesting that adolescents show diminished responses during anticipation of monetary rewards relative to adults1921, which are more pronounced in adolescents with contemporaneous drug use22. On the other hand, impulsive behaviour in adolescents has also been attributed to excessive motivation7,23, which could magnify the impact of received rewards and fuel subsequent impulsive choice8,24. Support for this countervailing view comes from neuroimaging studies, indicating that adolescents show enhanced responses to monetarily rewarding outcomes relative to adults7,23,25. More recent integrations of these ndings can resolve these apparent discrepancies by clarifying that adolescents show both diminished responses during reward anticipation, as well as increased responses to reward outcomes, relative to adults26,27.
For novelty-seeking adolescents, impulsive choices may confer benets as well as costs28. Although novelty-seeking adolescents have been labelled as reckless, stupid, irrational, callous, lazy or even violent29, novelty seeking could confer either proximal or distal advantages. For instance, novelty seeking encourages emigration away from relatives (which minimizes inbreeding)30, and can facilitate discovery and exploration of new opportunities and behaviours that might prove useful later in life. Novelty seeking may also increase self-esteem when valued by peers, since peer inuence increases over adolescence31. Finally, novelty seeking can elevate reproductive success in competitive environments in other species30 as well as humans, since others might perceive willingness to pursue novel options as a marker of ability32. For instance, in business, novelty seeking has been associated with creativity, entrepreneurial initiative and commercial success33.
Thus, while novelty-seeking behaviour can both harm and help adolescents, it is currently unclear how or when novelty-seeking traits promote pathology versus promise. In this research, we used a longitudinal design to identify which neural and behavioural factors predispose novelty-seeking adolescents to harmful outcomes specically related to problematic drug use (PDU). This design could reveal whether functional or structural neural markers at age 14 preceded PDU at age 16. We targeted motivational circuitry using a variant of a well-established neuroimaging task that reliably indexes individual differences in neural activity during reward anticipation (that is, the Monetary Incentive Delay Task)34,35. Previous research has associated
mesolimbic activity during reward anticipation with dopamine release36 as well as craving for drugs of abuse37. The current longitudinal design allowed us to test whether novelty-seeking adolescents with decreased neural responses during reward anticipation would be more likely to develop PDU (dened as the intake of increased amounts of licit and/or illicit drugs) over 2 years later. This design also afforded a direct comparison of neural versus psychometric predictors of PDU. Based on previous ndings implicating blunted neural responses during reward anticipation in adolescents with contemporaneous PDU22, we hypothesized that decreased neural responses during reward anticipation might predict eventual PDU in novelty-seeking adolescents, and further, that these neural markers might augment predictions afforded by more conventional psychometric measures.
Consistent with these hypotheses, we nd that novelty-seeking adolescents who go on to develop PDU initially show reduced neural activity during reward anticipation (specically, in the midbrain, VS and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex). These differences in neural activity cannot be accounted for by volumetric changes, and augment (or even exceed) predictions afforded by more conventional psychometric trait measures (specically, temporal discounting and low conscientiousness). In the future, neural markers of susceptibility to PDU may help researchers and clinicians to better target problematic symptoms and vulnerable individuals for intervention.
ResultsSample characteristics. Although the critical predictions focused on novelty-seeking adolescents, we rst sought to verify that novelty seeking was associated with PDU. In the target sample of subjects with high novelty-seeking scores (highest 25th percentile: n 283), 72 qualied as having PDU (25.4% PDU). Among
adolescents in the middle 2575% of novelty seekers (n 552),
102 qualied as having PDU (18.5% PDU), whereas in the lowest
25th percentile of novelty seekers (n 255), only 18 qualied as
having PDU (7.1% PDU). Incidence percentages thus supported the assumption that novelty-seeking traits appear relevant, but not sufcient, to confer vulnerability to PDU.
Behavioural and psychometric data. Comparisons also veried that the PDU group showed signicantly greater drug-taking scores than the control group at age 16 (n 72 controls
9.834.66, n 72 PDU group 20.245.43, F(1,142) 152.19,
Po10 10, analysis of variance (ANOVA)), even though these differences were not evident at age 14 (n 72 controls 6.333.04,
n 72 PDU group 6.934.97, F(1,142) 0.76, P 0.39, ANOVA;
group (PDU versus control) by time point (age 14 versus age 16) interaction F(1,284) 81.33, Po2 10 16, ANOVA). The PDU
and control groups did not signicantly differ with respect to pubertal status, age, gender, intelligence, novelty-seeking score, risk taking (CGT) or overall hit rate and reaction times in the Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) task (with the exception of the no gain condition; see Table 1) at age 14. The PDU group did, however, show steeper discounting of future rewards (log(discount rate); n 72 controls: 4.551.38, n 72 PDU: 3.991.53,
F(1,142) 5.23, P 0.024; ANOVA) and scored lower in
conscientiousness (n 72 controls: 25.446.20, n 72
PDU: 23.136.13, F(1,142) 5.09, P 0.026, ANOVA; see
Table 1) at age 14.
Functional neuroimaging data. A rst conrmatory voxel-wise analysis contrasted whole-brain activity during large versus small gain anticipation across both groups to verify main effects of reward anticipation at age 14. Across groups, large versus small gain
2 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14140 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 ARTICLE
Table 1 | Subject group characteristics and comparisons.
Control (n 72) Problematic drug use (n 72) Test P value
DemographicsAge (days) 14.48 (0.40) 14.38 (0.45) F(1,142) 1.82 0.18
Center site 15 21 5 6 8 8 9 9 11 10 10 12 12 8 X2(6) 8.95 0.18
PDS score 2.08 (0.28) 2.07 (0.26) F(1,142) 0.10 0.76
Gender (F/M) 45 27 41 31 X2(1) 0.46 0.50
Handedness (L/R) 9 63 6 66 X2(1) 0.67 0.41
Intelligence* 167.71 (20.09) 171.29 (18.67) F(1,142) 1.23 0.27
Socioeconomic status composite 7.21 (3.37) 7.15 (3.18) F(1,142) 0.01 0.92
ESPAD composite age 14 6.33 (3.04) 6.93 (4.97) F(1,142) 0.76 0.39
ESPAD composite age 16 9.83 (4.66) 20.24 (5.43) F(1,142) 152.19 0.000**
Behavioural measuresCGT risk taking 0.56 (0.14) 0.54 (0.14) F(1,142) 0.79 0.38
MCQ discounting (log) 4.55 (1.38) 3.99 (1.53) F(1,142) 5.23 0.024*
MID large gain RT 244.87 (26.55) 253.89 (31.70) F(1,142) 3.43 0.07
MID small gain RT 258.53 (35.72) 263.28 (36.33) F(1,142) 0.62 0.43
MID no gain RT 284.18 (44.64) 300.95 (55.75) F(1,142) 3.97 0.048*
MID large gain prob. 0.70 (0.09) 0.69 (0.09) F(1,142) 0.80 0.37
MID small gain prob. 0.70 (0.10) 0.68 (0.10) F(1,142) 1.26 0.26
MID no gain prob. 0.61 (0.11) 0.56 (0.12) F(1,142) 5.96 0.016*
Personality and psychopathologyTCI novelty seeking 127.93 (7.04) 127.42 (6.11) F(1,142) 0.22 0.64
NEO neuroticism 21.75 (7.98) 22.24 (8.48) F(1,142) 0.13 0.72
NEO extraversion 33.22 (4.97) 32.15 (5.41) F(1,142) 1.53 0.22
NEO openness 26.67 (5.49) 26.01 (5.82) F(1,142) 0.48 0.49
NEO agreeableness 28.21 (4.61) 26.99 (5.83) F(1,142) 1.95 0.16
NEO conscientiousness 25.44 (6.20) 23.13 (6.13) F(1,142) 5.09 0.026*
DAWBA emotional 65 2 5 63 7 2 X2(2) 4.09 0.13
DAWBA behavioural 55 13 4 54 13 5 X2(2) 0.12 0.94
DAWBA hyperactive 63 9 64 8 X2(1) 0.07 0.80
DAWBA any 47 16 9 47 18 7 X2(2) 0.37 0.83
CGT, Cambridge Gambling Task; DAWBA, Development and Well-Being Assessment; ESPAD, European School Survey Project on Alcohol and other Drugs; MCQ, Monetary-Choice Questionnaire;
MID, Monetary Incentive Delay Task; NEO, Neuroticism-Extraversion-Openness Five-Factor Inventory; PDS, Pubertal Development Scale; RT, reaction time; TCI, Temperament and Character Inventory --
Revised.
Intelligence reects the sum of these categories of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV): Similarities, Vocabulary, Block design, Matrix reasoning, Digit span forward.
*Signicant at uncorrected threshold of Po0.05 (n 144, t-test).
**Signicant at uncorrected threshold of Po0.01 (n 144, t-test).
y = 5 z = 3
R
10
8
6
4
2
0
L
T
Figure 1 | fMRI activity during anticipation of large versus small gains for control and PDU subjects combined (n 144). Overlaid on a mean
structural magnetic resonance scan showing a coronal (left) and an axial (right) section, activation display threshold is Po0.05 (whole brain corrected, t-test).
Table 2 | fMRI activity for high versus low gain anticipation contrast (PDU4Control).
Volume of interest (VOI) Right/Left T (126) P (uncorrected) Ventral striatum R 2.66 0.004*
L 1.75 0.042w
Midbrain R 1.32 0.095
L 2.69 0.004*
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex R 2.48 0.007*
L 2.10 0.019w
*Signicant at corrected threshold of Pr0.0083 (n 144, t-test corrected for multiple VOIs).
wSignicant at uncorrected threshold of Pr0.05 (n 144, t-test).
anticipation elicited expected increases in activity in mesolimbic regions including the VS (n 144; peak x, y, z: 11, 5, 5 mm,
Z 7.8, P 1.8E 13, corrected, t-test; 11, 5, 5 mm, Z 7.3,
P 8.8E 12, corrected) and midbrain (peak x, y, z: 6, 25, 12 mm, Z 5.3, P 1.7E 6, corrected; 8, 24, 9 mm,
Z 4.8, P 2.1E 5, corrected t-test; Fig. 1).
The second targeted analysis contrasted activity in six predened volumes of interest (see Methods) during large versus small gain anticipation in the PDU versus control groups at age
14 (Table 2; Figs 2 and 3; Supplementary Fig. 1). This analysis revealed signicant group differences in activity in the right VS (n 144; t(126) 2.66, P 0.004, uncorrected/P 0.027,
corrected, t-test), the left midbrain (n 144; t(126) 2.69,
P 0.004, uncorrected/P 0.024, corrected, t-test) and right
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (n 144; t(126) 2.48,
P 0.007, uncorrected/P 0.044, corrected, t-test). Although this
targeted analysis focused on high novelty-seeking adolescents, based on their documented vulnerability to future PDU, we further examined ventral striatal activity in subjects who scored in
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14140 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 3
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140
VS L VS R
3.5
3.5
2.5
1.5 1
0.5 0
0.5
3
3
*
2.5
1.5 1
0.5 0
0.5
z = 9
R
Bold signal (a.u.)
4 3 2 1 0
Bold signal (a.u.)
2
2
T
Controls PDU
Group
Controls PDU
Group
Midbrain L
Midbrain R
Bold signal (a.u.)
3.5
2.5
1.5 1
0.5 0
0.5
3.5
2.5
1.5 1
0.5 0
0.5
3
3
Bold signal (a.u.)
2
*
2
Controls PDU
Group
Controls PDU
Group
Figure 2 | Subcortical brain activity in anticipation of large versus small gains for control subjects (n 72) versus problematic drug users (n 72).
The PDU group showed decreased activation in bilateral ventral striatum (left, right) and midbrain (bottom). Overlaid on a mean structural magnetic resonance scan, activation display threshold is Po0.005 (uncorrected, t-test). Highlighted areas indicate volumes of interest in the ventral striatum (VS foci: 14, 8, 8) and midbrain (VTA foci: 9, 15, 15). Error bars s.e.m. *Signicant at threshold of Po0.0083 uncorrected or Po0.05 corrected
(n 144, t-test).
PFC L
3.5
2.5
1.5
1
0.5
0
0.5 Controls PDUGroup
z = 26 R
PFC R
Bold signal (a.u.)
3.5
2.5
1.5
1
0.5
0
0.5 Controls PDU 4
3
2
1
0 T
3
3
Bold signal (a.u.)
2
2
*
z = 36
Group
Figure 3 | Cortical brain activity in anticipation of large versus small gains for control subjects (n 72) versus problematic drug users (n 72). The
PDU group showed decreased activation in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (for VOI-based statistics, see Table 2). Overlaid on a mean structural magnetic resonance scan, activation display threshold is Po0.005 (uncorrected, t-test). Highlighted areas indicate volumes of interest in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC foci: 35, 36, 32). Error bars s.e.m. *Signicant at threshold of Po0.0083 uncorrected or Po0.05 corrected (n 144, t-test).
the middle quartiles and lower quartile on novelty-seeking traits. Decreased ventral striatal activity in the PDU group was only evident, however, in high novelty-seeking adolescents (Supplementary Figs 2 and 3).
A third exploratory voxel-wise analysis contrasted whole-brain activity during large versus small gain anticipation in the PDU versus control groups at age 14. Consistent with targeted
ndings, this analysis revealed group differences in activity in foci located in the bilateral VS (n 144; peak x, y, z: 15, 3, 9 mm,
Z 3.2, P 7.9E 4, uncorrected, t-test; peak x, y, z: 18, 0, 6 mm, Z 2.9; P 0.002, uncorrected, t-test), left midbrain
(peak x, y, z: 8, 21, 9 mm, Z 3.1, P 9.0E 4,
uncorrected, t-test), and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (peak x, y, z: 37, 5, 25 mm, Z 4.2, P 1.7E 5, uncorrected;
4 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14140 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 ARTICLE
x, y, z: 18, 21, 36 mm, Z 4.1, P 2.2E-5, uncorrected, t-test;
Figs 2 and 3).
Structural neuroimaging data. Reduced neural activity that precedes PDU could result from abnormal neural function, abnormal structure or both38. Volume of interest analysis of
voxel-based morphometry indices of grey matter density in the same six volumes used for functional comparisons revealed signicant group differences after correcting for multiple comparisons (Table 3; Fig. 4a; Supplementary Fig. 4). Specically, at age 14, high novelty seekers who eventually developed PDU showed increased grey matter density in the left VS (n 144; t(126) 2.51, P 0.007, uncorrected/P 0.040,
corrected, t-test), the left midbrain (n 144; t(126) 2.95,
P 0.002, uncorrected/P 0.011, corrected, t-test) and bilateral
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (right: n 144; t(126) 3.62,
P 0.001, uncorrected/P 0.001 corrected; left: t(126) 3.76,
P 0.001, uncorrected/P 0.001, corrected, t-test). The
dorsolateral prefrontal region that showed structural differences showed some overlap with regions that showed group differences in functional activity (Fig. 4b).
Behavioural and neural prediction of PDU. To compare the ability of psychological and neural variables of interest to predict the development of PDU, we further implemented a series of logistic regression models using statistically relevant psycho-metric and neural variables acquired at age 14 to predict PDU at age 16. Model comparison revealed that a model combining neural (activation in VS and dlPFC) with psychological variables
Table 3 | Structural differences in grey matter density indexed by voxel-based morphometry (PDU4control).
Volume of interest (VOI) Right/Left T (126) P (uncorrected) Ventral striatum R 2.25 0.013w
L 2.51 0.007*
Midbrain R 0.06 41
L 2.95 0.002*
Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex R 3.62 0.001* L 3.75 0.001*
*Signicant at corrected threshold of Po0.0083 (n 144, t-test corrected for multiple VOIs).
wSignicant at uncorrected threshold of Po0.05 (n 144, t-test).
a
PFC L
z = 27 R
PFC R
0.5
0.48
0.46
0.5
0.48
0.46
Grey matter density (a.u.)
0.44
0.42
0.4
Grey matter density (a.u.)
0.44
0.42
0.4 Controls PDU Group
* *
Controls PDU 3
2.5 2
1.5 1
0.5 0
Group
z = 33
T
b
z = 33
fMRI
R
VBM
Figure 4 | Cortical differences in grey matter volume for control subjects (n 72) versus prospective problematic drug users (PDU) (n 72).
(a) Increased grey matter density was observed for the PDU group in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. (b) The location of increased grey matter density (green) lies adjacent to reduced activation in the Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) task for the prospective problematic drug users (red). Overlaid on a mean structural magnetic resonance scale, volumetric display threshold is Po0.005 (uncorrected, t-test). Highlighted areas indicate volumes of interest in the prefrontal cortex. Error bars s.e.m. *Signicant at threshold of Po0.0083 uncorrected or Po0.05 corrected (n=144, t-test).
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14140 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 5
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140
Table 4 | Logistic regression models of psychological and neural features predicting problematic drug use in novelty-seeking adolescents two years later.
Psychological Neural Combined Delay discounting 0.28 (0.12)*2.30
0.29 (0.13)**2.23
Conscientiousness 0.07 (0.03)* 2.27
0.07 (0.03)*
2.12
R ventral striatum 0.11 (0.05)* 2.09
0.12 (0.05)*
2.16
R dorsolateral prefrontal cortex 0.30 (0.09)** 3.30
0.30 (0.10)**
3.07
Pseudo R2 (ML) 0.07 0.15 0.20 AIC 195 183 176 Classication % 58/55 67/65 74/66
AIC, Akaike Information criterion; ML, maximum likelihood.
Statistics are standardized coefcients, followed by standard errors of the mean in parentheses, and Z-scores (n 144, signicance: *Po0.05; **Po0.01, t-test).
Classication was determined using 10-fold cross-validation over ts of a linear support vector machine, with 50% classication representing chance (train/test rates).
(temporal discounting and Neuroticism-Extraversion-Openness Five-Factor Inventory conscientiousness) at age 14 best-predicted PDU at age 16 (n 144, Akaike Information criterion
(AIC) 176, pseudo R2 0.20, logistic regression). Interestingly,
the next most-predictive model included only neural variables (n 144, AIC 183, pseudo R2 0.15, logistic regression),
followed by the model that included only psychological variables (n 144, AIC 195, pseudo R2 0.07, logistic regression). These
ndings suggest that neural and psychological variables may account for independent variance in predicting PDU. Performing a formal classication using a linear support vector machine with threefold cross-validation implied that model predictions should generalize to other samples, and that classication accuracy was similar for the combined (neural and psychological) model (66% out of sample) and the model containing only neural variables (65% out of sample), but lower for the model containing only psychological variables (55% out of sample; Table 4).
DiscussionTo identify factors that confer vulnerability to PDU, we longitudinally characterized and tracked a large sample of novelty-seeking adolescents. We then compared individuals at age 14 who subsequently developed PDU at age 16 with those who did not. Importantly, these groups were carefully matched at age 14 on a range of relevant variables, including drug use. Individuals who later transitioned to PDU showed decreased right ventral striatal, left midbrain and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity during anticipation of large versus small gains at age 14. Consistent with greater impulsivity, these vulnerable individuals also showed steeper discounting of future rewards and lower conscientiousness scores at age 14. Notably, comparison of neural and psychological measures revealed that the neural markers predicted PDU as well as or better than the psychological variables.
Consistent with the primary prediction, novelty-seeking subjects with less ventral striatal activity during reward anticipation at age 14 were more likely to develop PDU at age 16. Reduced ventral striatal activity during gain anticipation has previously been observed in cross-sectional studies of substance abuse39 and other addictive behaviours40, and coheres with non-human primate research, suggesting that repeated drug intake can reduce activity in the VS41. Cross-sectional research, however, cannot clarify whether diminished neural responses to gain anticipation precede or result from substance abuse. Some relevant evidence, however, comes from longitudinal studies of
animals. For instance, one study implied that reduced dopamine D2 receptor availability in the striatum (assessed with positron emission tomography or PET) foreshadowed increased drug intake in primates41. Another study of rodents bred for impulsivity also indicated that reduced D2/D3 receptor availability in the striatum preceded increased drug intake42. This animal research highlights a critical role for longitudinal designs in clarifying causal pathways to substance abuse in humans. Consistent with animal results, the current ndings demonstrate in a longitudinal sample of novelty-seeking adolescents that reduced activation of the VS and the midbrain at age 14 precedes PDU at age 16, thus implying that reduced recruitment of the mesolimbic circuit not only results from, but also can precede and predict PDU.
Theorists have linked reduced activity in the mesolimbic circuit to blunted motivation for reward17,18,30. Since organisms typically seek to increase states associated with positive outcomes10, individuals with blunted neural responses during reward anticipation may require the promise of stronger rewards (for example, drugs of abuse) to elicit comparable levels of motivation. The reduced ventral striatal activation at age 14 observed in novelty-seeking adolescents at risk for PDU does not necessarily negate ndings, suggesting that drug use may also reciprocally decrease ventral striatal activity41. Instead, in combination with previously noted cross-sectional observations of reduced mesolimbic activity associated with addictive behaviour39,40, the present longitudinal ndings raise the possibility of a vicious cycle in which novelty-seeking individuals with less responsive mesolimbic circuits seek increased exposure to drugs of abuse, which can further blunt mesolimbic responsiveness, and so maintain addiction2.
In neuroimaging tasks that elicit reward anticipation (for example, the Monetary Incentive Delay Task), researchers have reported that subjects show increased ventral striatal activity during the anticipation of large gains in comparison with small gains, no gains and even comparable losses34,43. As in a previous cross-sectional study of adolescent smokers22, at-risk adolescents showed reduced ventral striatal activity during gain anticipation, but not in response to gain outcomes. Electrophysiological recordings in primates suggest that the ring of midbrain dopaminergic neurons increases proportional to anticipated gain magnitude44, but that ring in response to gain outcomes instead reects the inverse likelihood of previously anticipated gain (that is, the surprisingness of the gain outcome)45. More recently, optogenetic functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) research on rats indicated that phasic optogenetic stimulation of midbrain dopamine neurons increases fMRI
6 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14140 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 ARTICLE
activity in the striatum46. Thus, the present ndings are consistent with an account in which ventral striatal activity during reward anticipation reects phasic increases in dopamine ring and consequent release in the VS. More support for this account comes from a human study that combined the MID task with fMRI as well as [11C]raclopride PET to demonstrate that individuals who showed more fMRI activity during gain anticipation also showed more PET evidence of dopamine release to gain cues in the VS. Correspondence across imaging modalities was not evident, however, in striatal responses to gain outcomes36. In the present study, midbrain activity correlated robustly with ventral striatal activity during reward anticipation, and blunting of this anticipatory response predicted subsequent PDU at age 16 (Supplementary Table 1).
Although neuroimaging tasks less reliably elicit dorsolateral PFC activity than ventral striatal activity during reward anticipation, dlPFC regions also showed reduced activity during reward anticipation in novelty-seeking adolescents who went on to develop PDU. Theorists have posited that impulsive adolescent choice may stem from imbalances in the activity of rapidly developing mesolimbic motivational circuits versus more slowly maturing prefrontal control circuits8,9. dlPFC activity has specically been associated with planning, behavioural control and goal implementation47,48. More extensive longitudinal assessments might clarify whether reduced prefrontal functional activity reects a developmental delay or a lasting decit in adolescents at risk for PDU. Since ventral striatal regions connect to the prefrontal cortex through thalamic relays, which then reciprocally modulate the striatum10, additional research might also clarify whether reduced ventral striatal activity precedes reduced prefrontal activity or the opposite. Since PDUrs showed higher (rather than lower) grey matter density in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex regions, the observed decreases in functional activity could not be attributed to decreased structural grey matter integrity (for example, as in the case of partial voluming). Observed increases in dorsolateral prefrontal grey matter density are consistent, however, with the notion of a structural developmental delay in prospective problematic drug users at age 14, since developmental studies suggest that PFC thickness continually decreases over adolescence11,12, possibly as a result of synaptic pruning. Therefore, relatively greater prefrontal grey matter density might reect maturational delays in novelty-seeking adolescents that presage PDU.
Psychometric and behavioural measures have historically offered powerful tools for assessing individual differences in consideration of future rewards and long-term goals. For instance, measures of temporal discounting index a preference for smaller sooner rewards over larger later rewards. Low temporal discounting powerfully predicts future educational and economic success49,50, whereas high temporal discounting has instead been associated with addictive behaviour22,5153. Consistent with this cross-sectional evidence, the present longitudinal ndings indicate that high temporal discounting at age 14 was associated with PDU at age 16 in high novelty-seeking adolescents. Measures of conscientiousness index the tendency to follow socially prescribed norms for impulse control54, whereas low conscientiousness has been associated with a wide range of addictive behaviours (including tobacco, alcohol and drug use)55. The present ndings additionally indicate that low conscientiousness at age 14 was associated with PDU at age 16 in high novelty-seeking adolescents. While both high temporal discounting and low conscientiousness have been linked to compromised PFC function54,5658, direct model comparisons indicated that reduced ventral striatal and dorsolateral PFC activity during reward anticipation might uniquely contribute to predictions of PDU in novelty-seeking
adolescentsabove and beyond contributions from these relevant psychometric measures.
Despite strengths of the study design in combining validated neuroimaging probes with substantial matched longitudinal samples59, the design also has some limitations. For instance, cutoff criteria for PDU necessarily depend upon specic substances under consideration. In contrast to alcohol and cigarette consumption, in which a score compatible with daily use represented the cutoff, the threshold for other illicit drugs was instead dened based on lifetime use. For this longitudinal sample, low thresholds were adopted (particularly for illicit drugs such as crack, cocaine and narcotics), relative to other studies of early use60. Only a few adolescents qualied for PDU at age 16 with respect to use of illicit drugs (Supplementary Figs 5 and 6), whereas most instead qualied based on the use of licit drugs (for example, alcohol, cigarette or cannabis). The validity of the adopted criteria was supported, however, by the fact that the criteria predicted future PDU. Specically, the percentage of adolescents qualifying for PDU at age 16 was highest for the top quarter of novelty seekers (25.4% PDU), lower for the middle two quarters of novelty seekers (18.5% PDU) and lowest for the bottom quarter of novelty seekers (7.1% PDU). Based on previous research, we adopted a binary threshold criterion for PDU instead of a continuous outcome measure. This classication skirted correlational assumptions that the total amount of substance use maps linearly onto vulnerability. Such a correlational design might assume, for instance, that an individual who uses cigarettes, cannabis and alcohol should show a threefold difference in brain activity in predicted neural targets (for example, the VS) relative to an individual who uses only cannabis. These linear assumptions stand in contrast to the notion that substance abuse may reect the expression of an addictive syndrome61. Consistent with such a categorical distinction, only in the high novelty-seeking group did blunted ventral striatal activity clearly foreshadow later PDU (Supplementary Fig. 2). Further, while stressors and related negative arousal may also potentiate impulsive behaviours including substance abuse in adolescents62,63, the neuroimaging task employed in this study elicited gain but not loss anticipation, and so was primarily optimized to probe neural responses during anticipation of reward. Future research using neuroimaging probes that elicit anticipation of punishment might better probe links between negative arousal and adolescent vulnerability to substance abuse.
In conclusion, these longitudinal ndings in novelty-seeking adolescents demonstrate that diminished mesolimbic reward motivation along with impaired prefrontal control may confer risk for future PDU. Importantly, these ndings suggest that high novelty seeking alone does not necessarily lead to PDU, and that neuroimaging measures may augment psychometric measures in identifying vulnerability. Rather than limiting developmental possibilities29, these ndings may help clinicians to visualize modiable markers that can eventually be therapeutically targeted to prevent vulnerability or even to promote ourishing as novelty seekers transition from adolescence to adulthood64.
Methods
Subjects. Data for this study came from the IMAGEN project65, and were collected at multiple sites across Europe. At age 14, a large cohort of adolescents completed self-report and interview measures, in addition to structural and fMRI scans. Parental report measures were also collected for some constructs. Local ethics research committees approved the study at each site. On the day of assessment, written consent was obtained from each parent or guardian, and verbal assent was obtained from each adolescent. Further details on recruitment, standardized instructions for administration of psychometric and cognitive behavioural measures, and other procedures are described in the Standard Operating Procedures for the IMAGEN project (http://www.imagen-europe.com/en/Publications_and_SOP.php
Web End =http://www.imagen-europe.com/ http://www.imagen-europe.com/en/Publications_and_SOP.php
Web End =en/Publications_and_SOP.php ). Subjects were included in the current study if they had valid data for all measures including the initial assessment at age 14 and
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14140 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 7
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140
Complete data (n=1,090)
Not in top quarter of novelty seeking at age 14 (n=807)
Problematic drug use criteria already met at age 14 (n=20)
Problematic drug use only at age 16 (n=191)
Not matched for drug use at age 14 (n=119)
Healthy at age 16 (n=72)
Problematic drug use at age 16 (n=72)
Figure 5 | Experimental design diagram depicting subject selection procedure. Out of 1090 subjects with full datasets, the top quarter of novelty seekers who had not already met criteria for problematic drug use at age 14 were selected. Those who showed problematic drug use at age 16 were matched with those who did not with respect to drug use at age 14 (n 72 per group, 144 total).
No gain
Large gain
follow-up at age 16 (see below). Based on these criteria, at the time of analysis, complete data were available for 1,090 adolescents.
Novelty seeking in this sample was initially assessed with the Novelty Seeking subscale of the Temperament and Character InventoryRevised66. From the original sample of 1,090 with full data sets, individuals scoring in the top 25th percentile (n 283) of novelty seeking at the initial assessment at age 14 were
selected. Based on the criteria described below, these subjects were then classied either as having PDU either at age 14 (excluded; n 20) or at age 16 (PDU group;
n 72) or as not having PDU at either ages 14 or 16 (control group; n 191). Since
we aimed to classify whether neural markers predict or result from drug use, we sought to directly compare the PDU group to the control group without statistically signicant differences in PDU at age 14. Thus, we further matched both groups with respect to size and average drug intake at age 14 (as dened by each individuals total drug intake score, described below). This procedure yielded 72 subjects in the PDU group and 72 matched subjects in the control group (Table 1; Fig. 5).
PDU criteria. PDU was operationally dened based on measures according to the European School Survey Project on Alcohol and Other Drugs (ESPAD)67. Unlike traditional clinical instruments (for example, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, version 5) these measures provided a preclinical index of drug use at ages 14 and 16. Thus, cutoff criteria for PDU were dened to capture problematic use of various legal or illicit drugs. With respect to legal drugs (alcohol and cigarettes), a threshold was set that indicated daily use. In particular, a score of 3 or higher on smoking (0: Not at all, 1: Less than 1 cigarette per week, 2: Less than 1 cigarette per day, 3: 15 cigarettes per day, 4: 610 cigarettes per day, 5: 1120 cigarettes per day, 6: More than 20 cigarettes per day) and a score of 5 or higher on alcohol consumption (0: 0 drinks per month, 1: 12 drinks per month, 2: 35 drinks per month, 3: 69, 4: 1019, 5: 2039 and 6: 40 or more) within the last 30 days were dened as PDU.
With respect to illicit drugs, PDU thresholds were based on lifetime use. Apart from cannabis, where the threshold was set to 39 lifetime occasions, the threshold for other drugs (glue, tranquilizers, amphetamine, lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), hallucinogenic mushrooms, 3,4 methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA), ketamine or liquid ecstasy) was set to 35 occasions or more. Use of a lifetime score and therefore a lower threshold criterion for these drugs was justied by the fact that early use of these drugs robustly predicts PDU later in life (for example, a threefold risk after early cannabis use60). Finally, the threshold for illicit drugs (for example, crack, cocaine, heroin and narcotics) was set to 12 or more occasions (Supplementary Figs 5 and 6 depict the distribution of scores for both groups at age 14 and 16, and cutoffs for each substance). None of the subjects in either group fell above the threshold for use of any substance at age 14 (Supplementary Fig. 5). Furthermore, most subjects were classied as PDU by means of daily cigarette use, followed by alcohol and cannabisonly a few were classied as PDU based on their use of other illicit drugs (for example, MDMA and amphetamines; Supplementary Figs 5 and 6).
Personality and psychopathology measures. Novelty seeking was assessed using a subscale of the Temperament and Character InventoryRevised66. Dimensions of personality were assessed using the 60-item Neuroticism-Extraversion-Openness Five-Factor Inventory, which indexes dimensions of Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness to Experience, as described by the Five-Factor Model of personality68. Adolescent psychiatric symptoms and their impact were assessed with the Development and Well-Being Assessment, which generates probabilities that individuals qualify for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (version 4) psychiatric diagnoses69.
Cognitive measures. Subjects completed a version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV70, which included the Perceptual Reasoning, Matrix Reasoning and Similarities, and Vocabulary scales to index intelligence. The Monetary-Choice Questionnaire (MCQ)51 was administered to assess delay discountingor individual differences in the tendency to choose sooner but smaller over later but larger rewards (this index correlates well with more precise but also more time-consuming measures71). Subjects also completed the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT)72, as a behavioural measure of risk seeking.
Demographics. The Puberty Development Scale73 assessed each subjects pubertal status. Socioeconomic status scores were assessed using a composite score that indexed the weighted sum of the following variables: Mothers Education Score, Fathers Education Score, Family Stress Unemployment Score, Financial Difculties Score, Home Inadequacy Score, Neighborhood Score, Financial Crisis Score, Mother Employed Score, Father Employed Score. Negative (that is, high risk) scores were reverse-coded.
Functional neuroimaging data acquisition and analysis. The task used to probe neural activity during reward anticipation was a modied version of the MID task34, which required subjects to respond after seeing a cue to a briey presented target by pressing one of two buttons as rapidly as possible to indicate whether the target appeared on the left or the right side of the screen (Fig. 6). If subjects responded while the target was on the screen, they received points, but if they responded before or after the targets disappearance, they received no points. Cues signalled each trials onset, and reliably indicated the position of the target as well as the number of points to be awarded for a successful response. Cues took one of three forms: a triangle indicated no points (No Gain), a circle with one line
+0 42
+2 44
+10 54
Small gain
Cue 250 ms
Figure 6 | Adapted Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) task trial structure. An initial cue signalled potential gain for each trial (no gain: 0 points; small gain: 2 points; or large gain: 10 points). After a variable delay, a target briey appeared. Responding during target display yielded the indicated gain, whereas late or early responses yielded no gain. Target durations adapted to approximate a 66% hit rate for each subject34.
Delay 44.5 s
Target 250400 ms
Feedback
1,450 ms
ITI3.54.15 s
8 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14140 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 ARTICLE
indicated 2 points (Small Gain) and a circle with three lines indicated 10 points (Large Gain) at stake. Behavioural data from this modied MID task included the proportion of hits on gain trials and hit reaction times.
At all sites, scanning was performed with 3 T whole-body magnetic resonance scanners produced by a variety of manufacturers (Siemens, Philips, General Electric and Bruker). For functional imaging, we acquired 300 volumes with 40 slices in descending order (2.4 mm slice thickness with 1 mm gap) using a gradient-echo T2*-weighted pulse sequence (EPI). The time to repetition for volume acquisition was set to 2,200 ms and the time to echo to 30 ms. In-plane resolution was 64 64 with a eld of view of 220 220 mm. The plane of acquisition was
tilted to parallel the anteriorposterior commissure line. For anatomical reference, a three-dimensional magnetization prepared gradient-echo sequence of the whole brain was obtained with time to repetition of 6.8 ms and a time to echo of 3.2 ms. These imaging parameters were chosen to ensure comparability of data across different scanners. Further details of the image acquisition protocols and quality control procedures have been described previously, including an extensive period of standardization across magnetic resonance scanners65.
Image preprocessing and analyses were performed with SPM8 and SPM12 software (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London). For structural preprocessing, we normalized individually segmented T1-weighted scans to a template generated by the rst 552 adolescents in the sample22,38 using the DARTEL toolbox74 as implemented in SPM8. For fMRI whole-brain analyses, single-subject echo-planar images were coregistered with their associated T1-weighted structural images. Functional images were then realigned and resliced to the rst volume. Single-subject statistical models analysed the resliced data using the following regressors: (1) anticipation of large gain; (2) anticipation of small gain; (3) anticipation of no gain; (4) feedback indicating large gain; (5) feedback indicating small gain; (6) and feedback indicating no gain. Each regressor was dened separately for successful (that is, hits) as well as unsuccessful (that is, misses) response trials. Thus, each model included a total of 12 orthogonal regressors. Trials in which subjects failed to respond were modelled similarly but separately as error trials. Rigid body movement parameters from the realignment procedure were included as six additional covariates. Next, contrast images of the parameter estimates were created for each subject. The present analyses focused on the gain anticipation phase. Analyses specically contrasted neural responses during anticipation of large versus small gain, since motor responses did not differ across incentivized conditions between groups (but were slightly lower in vulnerable subjects for no gain trials; Table 1). Single-subject contrast images were created by applying the DARTEL deformations to the contrast images, which were subsequently smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full width at half maximum. Normalized and smoothed single-subject contrast images were then entered into a second-level random-effects analysis (two-sample t-test contrasting PDU subjects versus controls). Although both groups were matched for gender, pubertal status, intelligence estimate, ESPAD composite score, novelty-seeking score and scanning site at the rst assessment, we included those variables as covariates to account for residual variance between and within groups. For whole-brain analyses, which were primarily intended to verify main effects of the neuroimaging task across groups and group differences in predicted regions, the threshold was set to Po0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons.
For targeted analyses that tested the critical hypotheses, bilateral ventral striatal spherical (12 mm diameter) volumes of interest (VOIs) were centred on Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates 14, 8, 8 (ref. 75), as documented in previous research on reward anticipation22,76. Similarly, bilateral midbrain spherical (12 mm diameter) VOIs were centred on coordinates 9, 15, 15 (ref. 77), as documented in previous research on reward anticipation22,76. Bilateral dorsolateral PFC spherical (40 mm diameter) VOIs were centred on coordinates 35, 36, 32 (ref. 54), based on previous research on executive control78. Tests for associations of activity in these VOIs at age 14 with eventual PDU versus healthy status at age 16 were Bonferroni-corrected for multiple comparisons (Po0.05/6 Po0.008). In
gures, results are displayed at a threshold of Po0.005 uncorrected with clusters including at least 10 contiguous voxels and projected onto the mean structural scan of all subjects, but these VOIs are superimposed for visualization purposes (Figs 24). Since the average adolescent brain at age 14 is smaller than the adult brain, we refrained from transforming individual brains into adult MNI space. However, since the predictions came from studies that reported data in MNI space, we estimated the parameters for linear transformation from MNI space to the space of our DARTEL template (that is, X 0.892, XMNI 0.008, YMNI 0.004, ZMNI 0.385; Y 0.017, XMNI 0.930, YMNI 0.025, ZMNI 6.799; Z 0.009, XMNI 0.005, YMNI 0.838, ZMNI 0.093).
Structural neuroimaging data analysis. Voxel-based morphometry analyses compared spatially normalized structural scans on a voxel-by-voxel basis. Instead of directly comparing image intensity, however, structural scans were segmented into grey and white matter, smoothed, and then the grey matter partition was subjected to a voxel-by-voxel statistical test. Thus, T1 images were segmented using the new segment routine as implemented in SPM8, then modulated and spatially normalized with DARTEL (see above). Resulting images were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full width at half maximum. Statistical analyses mirrored those applied to the fMRI contrasts (that is, two-sample t-test contrasting PDU
subjects versus controls, with gender, pubertal status, intelligence quotient estimate, ESPAD score, novelty-seeking score and scanning site entered as covariates of no interest). Targeted analyses compared grey matter density within the same VOIs that were constructed to compare functional activity.
Behavioural and neural prediction of PDU. After verifying key psychometric and neural variables at age 14, these variables relative ability to predict problematic abuse at age 16 was evaluated using a series of logistic regression models that included psychometric variables only, neural variables only (maximum peaks from VOIs), and the combination of psychometric and neural variables. Since data checks of pair-wise correlations revealed that activity in midbrain and bilateral ventral striatal VOIs was highly correlated (Supplementary Fig. 7), and based on previous evidence reliably implicating ventral striatal activity in reward anticipation76, we included coefcients for the right ventral striatal VOI in the models. Similarly, since activity in the two dorsolateral prefrontal VOIs was highly correlated, the prefrontal region whose activity was most closely associated with future PDU was included in the models. This initial variable selection averted collinearity and resulting instability that might arise from including highly correlated variables in the same model79. Cross-validation analyses veried that these variables could classify future PDU out of sample in each model. Classication was determined using 10-fold cross-validation over ts of a linear support vector machine, with 50% classication representing chance.
Data availability. Data are available via application to the IMAGEN project (http://www.imagen-europe.com
Web End =http://www.imagen-europe.com).
References
1. Nees, F. et al. Determinants of early alcohol use in healthy adolescents: the differential contribution of neuroimaging and psychological factors. Neuropsychopharmacol. 37, 986995 (2012).
2. Jupp, B. & Dalley, J. W. Behavioral endophenotypes of drug addiction: Etiological insights from neuroimaging studies. Neuropharmacology 76 (Pt B): 487497 (2014).
3. Whelan, R. et al. Neuropsychosocial proles of current and future adolescent alcohol misusers. Nature 512, 185189 (2014).
4. Steinberg, L. et al. Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Dev. Psychol. 44, 17641778 (2008).
5. Le Bon, O. et al. Personality prole and drug of choice; a multivariate analysis using Cloningers TCI on heroin addicts, alcoholics, and a random population group. Drug Alcohol Depend. 73, 175182 (2004).
6. Wills, T. A., Vaccaro, D. & McNamara, G. Novelty seeking, risk taking, and related constructs as predictors of adolescent substance use: an application of Cloningers theory. J. Subst. Abuse 6, 120 (1994).
7. Galvan, A. et al. Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J. Neurosci. 26, 68856892 (2006).
8. Steinberg, L. A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Dev. Rev. 28, 78106 (2008).
9. Ernst, M. The triadic model perspective for the study of adolescent motivated behavior. Brain Cogn. 89, 104111 (2014).
10. Haber, S. N. & Knutson, B. The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 426 (2010).
11. Gogtay, N. et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 81748179 (2004).
12. Giedd, J. N. & Rapoport, J. L. Structural MRI of pediatric brain development: what have we learned and where are we going? Neuron 67, 728734 (2010).
13. Christakou, A., Brammer, M. & Rubia, K. Maturation of limbic corticostriatal activation and connectivity associated with developmental changes in temporal discounting. Neuroimage 54, 13441354 (2011).
14. Sisk, C. L. & Foster, D. L. The neural basis of puberty and adolescence. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 10401047 (2004).
15. Cools, R. & DEsposito, M. Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions onhuman working memory and cognitive control. Biol. Psychiatry 69, e113e125 (2011).
16. Hommer, D. W., Bjork, J. M. & Gilman, J. M. Imaging brain response to reward in addictive disorders. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1216, 5061 (2011).
17. Blum, K., Cull, J. G., Braverman, E. R. & Comings, D. E. Reward deciency syndrome. Am. Sci. 84, 132145 (1996).
18. Melis, M., Spiga, S. & Diana, M. The dopamine hypothesis of drug addiction: hypodopaminergic state. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 63, 101154 (2005).
19. Bjork, J. M. et al. Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: similarities and differences from young adults. J. Neurosci. 24, 17931802 (2004).
20. Bjork, J. M., Smith, A. R., Chen, G. & Hommer, D. W. Adolescents, adults and rewards: comparing motivational neurocircuitry recruitment using fMRI. PLoS ONE 5, e11440 (2010).
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14140 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 9
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140
21. Lamm, C. et al. Longitudinal study of striatal activation to reward and loss anticipation from mid-adolescence into late adolescence/early adulthood. Brain Cogn. 89, 5160 (2014).
22. Peters, J. et al. Lower ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation in adolescent smokers. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 540549 (2011).
23. Ernst, M. et al. Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. Neuroimage 25, 12791291 (2005).
24. Leyton, M. & Vezina, P. Striatal ups and downs: their roles in vulnerability to addictions in humans. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 19992014 (2013).
25. Van Leijenhorst, L. et al. Adolescent risky decision-making: Neurocognitive development of reward and control regions. Neuroimage 51, 345355
2010:
26. Geier, C. F., Terwilliger, R., Teslovich, T., Velanova, K. & Luna, B. Immaturities in reward processing and its inuence on inhibitory control in adolescence. Cereb. Cortex 20, 16131629 (2010).
27. Hoogendam, J. M., Kahn, R. S., Hillegers, M. H. J., Van Buuren, M. & Vink, M. Different developmental trajectories for anticipation and receipt of reward during adolescence. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 113124 (2013).
28. Furby, L. & Beyth-Marom, R. Risk taking in adolescence: a decision-making perspective. Dev. Rev. 12, 144 (1992).
29. Males, M. Does the adolescent brain make risk taking inevitable? A skeptical appraisal. J. Adolesc. Res. 24, 320 (2009).
30. Spear, L. P. The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 417463 (2000).
31. Nelson, E. E., Leibenluft, E., McClure, E. B. & Pine, D. S. The social re-orientation of adolescence: a neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychol. Med. 35, 163174 (2005).
32. Wilson, M. & Daly, M. Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: the young male syndrome. Ethol. Sociobiol. 6, 5973 (1985).
33. Kreiser, P. M., Marino, L. D., Kuratko, D. F. & Weaver, K. M. Disaggregating entrepreneurial orientation: the non-linear impact of innovativeness, proactiveness and risk-taking on SME performance. Small Bus. Econ. 40, 273291 (2013).
34. Knutson, B., Westdorp, A., Kaiser, E. & Hommer, D. FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. Neuroimage 12, 2027 (2000).
35. Wu, C. C., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Katovich, K. & Knutson, B. Affective traits link to reliable neural markers of incentive anticipation. Neuroimage 84, 279289 (2014).
36. Schott, B. H. et al. Mesolimbic functional magnetic resonance imaging activations during reward anticipation correlate with reward-related ventral striatal dopamine release. J. Neurosci. 28, 1431114319 (2008).
37. Wrase, J. et al. Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxied alcoholics. Neuroimage 35, 787794 (2007).
38. Schneider, S. et al. Risk taking and the adolescent reward system:A potential common link to substance abuse. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 3946 (2012).
39. Volkow, N. D. et al. Decreased striatal dopaminergic responsiveness in detoxied cocaine-dependent subjects. Nature 386, 830833 (1997).
40. Reuter, J. et al. Pathological gambling is linked to reduced activation of the mesolimbic reward system. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 147148 (2005).
41. Nader, M. A. et al. PET imaging of dopamine D2 receptors duringchronic cocaine self-administration in monkeys. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 10501056 (2006).
42. Dalley, J. W. et al. Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science 315, 12671270 (2007).
43. Yacubian, J. et al. Dissociable systems for gain- and loss-related value predictions and errors of prediction in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 26, 95309537 (2006).
44. Tobler, P. N., Fiorillo, C. D. & Schultz, W. Adaptive coding of reward value by dopamine neurons. Science 307, 16421645 (2005).
45. Fiorillo, C. D., Tobler, P. N. & Schultz, W. Discrete coding of reward probability and uncertainty by dopamine neurons. Science 299, 18981902 (2003).
46. Ferenczi, E. A. et al. Prefrontal cortical regulation of brainwide circuit dynamics and reward-related behavior. Science 351, aac9698 (2016).
47. Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. An Integrative Theory of Prefrontal Cortical Function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167202 (2001).
48. Ballard, I. C. et al. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex drives mesolimbic dopaminergic regions to initiate motivated behavior. J. Neurosci. 31, 1034010346 (2011).
49. Mischel, W., Shoda, Y. & Peake, P. K. The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratication. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 687696 (1988).
50. Casey, B. J. et al. Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratication 40 years later. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 1499815003 (2011).
51. Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M. & Bickel, W. K. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 128, 7887 (1999).
52. Miedl, S. F., Peters, J. & Bchel, C. Altered neural reward representations in pathological gamblers revealed by delay and probability discounting. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 177186 (2012).
53. Bickel, W. K., Koffarnus, M. N., Moody, L. & Wilson, A. G. The behavioral-and neuro-economic process of temporal discounting: A candidate behavioral marker of addiction. Neuropharmacology 76 (Pt B): 518527 (2014).
54. DeYoung, C. G. et al. Testing predictions from personality neuroscience. Brain structure and the big ve. Psychol. Sci. 21, 820828 (2010).
55. Bogg, T. & Roberts, B. W. Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: a meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychol. Bull 130, 887919 (2004).
56. McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G. & Cohen, J. D. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306, 503507 (2004).
57. Ballard, K. & Knutson, B. Dissociable neural representations of future reward magnitude and delay during temporal discounting. Neuroimage 45, 143150 (2009).
58. Figner, B. et al. Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 538539 (2010).
59. Qu, Y., Galvan, A., Fuligni, A. J., Lieberman, M. D. & Telzer, E. H. Longitudinal changes in prefrontal cortex activation underlie declines in adolescent risk taking. J. Neurosci. 35, 1130811314 (2015).
60. Ellickson, P. L. & Morton, S. C. Identifying adolescents at risk for hard drug use: racial/ethnic variations. J. Adolesc. Health 25, 382395 (1999).
61. Shaffer, H. J. et al. Toward a syndrome model of addiction: multiple expressions, common etiology. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 12, 367374 (2004).
62. Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. F. & Miller, J. Y. Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early adulthood: Implications for substance abuse prevention. Psychol. Bull 112, 64105 (1992).
63. Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moftt, T. E., Silva, P. A. & McGee, R. Personality traits are differentially linked to mental disorders: a multitrait-multidiagnosis study of an adolescent birth cohort. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 105, 299312 (1996).
64. Johnson, S. B., Sudhinaraset, M. & Blum, R. W. Neuromaturation and adolescent risk taking: why development is not determinism. J. Adolesc. Res. 25, 423 (2010).
65. Schumann, G. et al. The IMAGEN study: reinforcement-related behaviour in normal brain function and psychopathology. Mol. Psychiatry 15, 11281139 (2010).
66. Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M. & Przybeck, T. R. A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 50, 975990 (1993).
67. Hibell, B. et al. The ESPAD Report 2003: Alcohol and Other Drug use Among Students in 35 European Countries (Swedish Council for Information on Alcohol and Other Drugs, Stockholm, 2007).
68. Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. Domains and facets: hierarchical personality assessment using the revised NEO personality inventory. J. Pers. Assess. 64, 2150 (1995).
69. Goodman, R., Ford, T., Richards, H., Gatward, R. & Meltzer, H. The development and well-being assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 41, 645655 (2000).
70. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for ChildrenFourth Edition (WISC-IV) (The Psychological Corporation, 2003).
71. Johnson, M. W. & Bickel, W. K. Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 77, 129146 (2002).
72. Rogers, R. et al. Dissociable decits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 20, 322339
1999:
73. Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M. & Boxer, A. A self-report measure of pubertal status: reliability, validity, and initial norms. J. Youth Adolesc. 17, 117133 (1988).
74. Ashburner, J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95113 (2007).
75. ODoherty, J. et al. Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science 304, 452454 (2004).
76. Knutson, B. & Greer, S. M. Anticipatory affect: Neural correlates and consequences for choice. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 37713786 (2008).
77. Schott, B. H. et al. The dopaminergic midbrain participates in human episodic memory formation: evidence from genetic imaging. J. Neurosci. 26, 14071417 (2006).
78. Blair, K. S. et al. Modulation of emotion by cognition and cognition by emotion. Neuroimage 35, 430440 (2007).
10 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14140 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 ARTICLE
79. Neter, J., Kutner, M. H., Nachtsheim, C. J. & Wasserman, W. Applied Linear Statistical Models. Vol. 4 (McGraw-Hill/Irwin, 1996).
Acknowledgements
IMAGEN received research funding from the European Communitys Sixth Framework Programme (LSHM-CT-2007-037286). C.B. is supported by the DFG (SFB 936), the ERC (ERC-2010-AdG_20100407) and BMBF (AERIAL). J.P. is supported by the DFG (PE 1627/4-1, PE 1627/5-1); B.K. is supported by a Stanford Neuroscience Institute Big Ideas Award to the Neurochoice Initiative. Research was also supported in part by DFG FOR 1617. This paper reects only the authors views and the Community is not liable for any use that may be made of the information contained therein. We thank spanlab and three anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions on earlier drafts of this paper.
Author contributions
All authors designed the study and acquired the data. C.B. and B.K. analysed the data and wrote the manuscript.
Additional information
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =http://www.nature.com/ http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =naturecommunications
Competing nancial interests: The authors declare no competing nancial interests.
Reprints and permission information is available online at http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =http://npg.nature.com/ http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =reprintsandpermissions/
How to cite this article: Bchel, C. et al. Blunted ventral striatal responses to anticipated rewards foreshadow problematic drug use in novelty-seeking adolescents. Nat. Commun. 8, 14140 doi: 10.1038/ncomms14140 (2017).
Publishers note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional afliations.
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articles Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in the credit line; if the material is not included under the Creative Commons license, users will need to obtain permission from the license holder to reproduce the material. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Web End =http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
r The Author(s) 2017
The IMAGEN ConsortiumMercedes Arroyo22, Eric Artiges8, Semiha Aydin13, Christine Bach14, Alexis Barbot8, Gareth Barker5, Ruediger Bruehl13, Anna Cattrell5, Patrick Constant25, Hans Crombag26, Katharina Czech12, Jeffrey Dalley22, Benjamin Decideur8, Sylvane Desrivieres5, Tahmine Fadai1, Mira Fauth-Buhler14, Jianfeng Feng27, Irinia Filippi15,16, Vincent Frouin8, Birgit Fuchs28, Isabel Gemmeke12, Alexander Genauck12, Eanna Hanratty5, Bert Heinrichs29, Nadja Heym19, Thomas Hubner23, Albrecht Ihlenfeld13, Alex Ing5, James Ireland30, Tianye Jia5, Jennifer Jones4, Sarah Jurk23, Mehri Kaviani19, Arno Klaassen31, Johann Kruschwitz12, Christophe Lalanne8, Dirk Lanzerath29, Mark Lathrop32, Claire Lawrence19, Herv Lemaitre15,16, Christine Macare5, Catherine Mallik5, Adam Mar22, Lourdes Martinez-Medina5, Eva Mennigen23, Fabiana Mesquita de Carvahlo5, Xavier Mignon25, Sabina Millenet14, Ruben Miranda15,16, Kathrin Mller23, Charlotte Nymberg5, Caroline Parchetka12, Yolanda Pena-Oliver26, Jani Pentilla15,16, Jean-Baptiste Poline8, Erin Burke Quinlan5, Michael Rapp12, Stephan Ripke23, Tamzin Ripley26, Gabriel Robert5, John Rogers30, Alexander Romanowski12, Barbara Ruggeri5, Christine Schmal14, Dirk Schmidt23, Sophia Schneider1, Florian Schubert13, Yannick Schwartz8, Wolfgang Sommer14, Rainer Spanagel14,Claudia Speiser28, Tade Spranger29, Alicia Stedman19, Dai Stephens26, Nicole Strache12, Andreas Strhle12, Maren Struve14, Naresh Subramaniam22, David Theobald22, Nora Vetter23, Helene Vulser15,16, Katharina Weiss12, Robert Whelan4, Steve Williams5, Bing Xu5, Juliana Yacubian1, Tao Yu5 & Veronika Ziesch23
25 PERTIMM, 92600 Asnieres-Sur-Seine, France. 26 Department of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer BN1 9QH, UK. 27 Warwick University, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK. 28 GABO:Milliarium mbH & Co., KG 80333 Munich, Germany. 29 Deutsches Referenzzentrum fur Ethik, D53113 Bonn, Germany. 30 Delosis, Twickenham, Middlesex TW1 4AE, UK. 31 Scito, F-75020 Paris, France. 32 Centre National de Genotypage, 91057 Evry, France.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:14140 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14140 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 11
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Nature Publishing Group Feb 2017
Abstract
Novelty-seeking tendencies in adolescents may promote innovation as well as problematic impulsive behaviour, including drug abuse. Previous research has not clarified whether neural hyper- or hypo-responsiveness to anticipated rewards promotes vulnerability in these individuals. Here we use a longitudinal design to track 144 novelty-seeking adolescents at age 14 and 16 to determine whether neural activity in response to anticipated rewards predicts problematic drug use. We find that diminished BOLD activity in mesolimbic (ventral striatal and midbrain) and prefrontal cortical (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) regions during reward anticipation at age 14 predicts problematic drug use at age 16. Lower psychometric conscientiousness and steeper discounting of future rewards at age 14 also predicts problematic drug use at age 16, but the neural responses independently predict more variance than psychometric measures. Together, these findings suggest that diminished neural responses to anticipated rewards in novelty-seeking adolescents may increase vulnerability to future problematic drug use.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer