J Headache Pain (2004) 5:S51S58
DOI 10.1007/s10194-004-0108-3Michele Feleppa
Walter Di Iorio
Donato M.T. SaracinoP300 and contingent negative variation in
migraineReceived: Accepted in revised form: M. Feleppa () W. Di Iorio
Department of Neuroscience,
Hospital G. Rummo,
Via dellAngelo, I-82100 Benevento, Italy
e-mail: [email protected]
Tel.: +39-0824-57492/57465
Fax: +39-0824-57465D.M.T. SaracinoDepartment of Medical Up-Grading,
Hospital G. Rummo, Benevento, ItalyAbstract We reviewed P300 and
contingent negative variation
(CNV) studies performed in
migraine in order to identify their
relevance in migraine and the role
of neurophysiology in migraine.
Publications available to us were
completed by a Medline search.
There is experimental and clinical
evidence for loss of cognitive habituation in migraine which may serve
as a specific diagnostic tool; therefore, we reviewed studies on
migraine that analyzed habituation
and lack of habituation by P300 and
CNV, performing short-term habituation (STH) and long-term habituation (LTH). Finally, we described
the two components of P300 (a andb) and of CNV (early and late
wave) and the two abnormalities
reported from the majority of studies on event-related potential in
migraine: increased amplitude of
average event-related potential and
lack of habituation. These abnormalities are especially related to the
early component characterizing orienting activity.Key words Migraine P300 CNV
HabituationThe event-related potential (ERP) component P300 is considered a cognitive neuroelectrical indicator of central
nervous system activity [1] involved with the processing
of new information when attention is engaged to update
memory representations [2]. P300 latency can be regarded
as a measure of the relative timing of the stimulus evaluation process, indicating an upper limit on categorization
and stimulus evaluation time [3], or the time taken to allocate resources and engage memory updating [4]. P300
amplitude is held to index attentional resource allocation
when memory updating is engaged [2]. The P300 component in many event-related brain potential (ERP) studies is
obtained using the so-called odd-ball paradigm, in which
two stimuli are presented in random order, with one occurring more frequently than the other [5]. The subject is
required to discriminate the infrequent or rare stimulus
(target stimulus) from the frequent or standard stimulus
(nontarget) by noting the occurrence of the target typically by pressing a button or mental counting [6, 7]. A
modification of the oddball paradigm has been developed
in which infrequent-nontarget stimuli are inserted into the
sequence of target and standard stimuli. When novel
stimuli (e.g., dog barks, color forms, etc.) are presented as
the infrequent nontargets in the series of more typical
targets and standard stimuli (e.g., tones, letters of the
alphabet, etc.), a P300 component that is large over the
frontal/central areas is produced. This component has
been dubbed the P3a, whereas the infrequent-target
stimulus elicits a parietal maximum P300 or P3b [8, 9].
Because this novel P300 exhibits an anterior/central scalp
distribution, short peak latency, and rapid habituation, it
has been interpreted as reflecting frontal lobe functionS52[10, 11]. In the 1960s, electrophysiologists described a
slow negative wave, called the contingent negative variation (CNV), that can be recorded on the brain cortical surface between two defined and contingent external stimuli[12]. The first stimulus serves as a warning (S1); it
announces an imperative stimulus (S2) which can be
directly acted upon by pressing a key. When the interval
between S1 and S2 is longer than 2 s, two CNV components can be distinguished: (1) an early component,
which has its greatest amplitude between 550 and 750 ms
after S1, and (2) a late component, which shows a maximum amplitude in the 200 ms preceding S2. The negativity is not seen in the raw EEG and can only be detected
with averaging [13]. The CNV amplitude is associated
with expectation, attention, preparation, motivation, and
readiness [12, 14, 15]. The expectation of stimuli can
selectively reduce brain activation [16], while the anticipatory state itself may be associated with increased blood
flow in the anterior cingulated cortex [17]. The early component is observed predominantly frontally and related to
the attention to stimulus; the late component has a more
central distribution and is associated with motor readiness[18]. The late CNV may consist of motor preparation and
stimulus preceding negativity, or SPN [19]. There is evidence for multiple sources of CNV [20]. The anterior cingulated gyrus, caudate nucleus, thalamus, and reticular
formation may be critical for the generation of the early
phase of CNV, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex may
be involved in the generation of the late phase [21]. The
early CNV may also originate in the frontal lobes [22].
The frontal cortex is known to be a crucial structure for
working memory, as confirmed in functional magnetic
resonance imaging, positron emission tomography (PET)
imaging, and direct neuronal recordings, and it may be
functionally specialized [23]. Loveless and Sanford [24]
called the two components of the CNV O-wave (or orienting wave following the warning signal) and E-wave
(or expectancy wave anticipating the imperative signal) in
order to denote their hypothesized functional significance.
According to Rohrbaugh et al. [25], the early CNV
observed in S1S2 paradigms with long interstimulus
intervals (3 s or more) is identical to the negative afterwave or slow negative wave (SNW) elicited by unpaired
stimuli. Both early CNV and SNW are sensitive to task
requirements enhancing the signal value of the stimuli[26]. For example, very much in the way the early CNV
responds to the signal value of the warning stimulus, the
amplitude of the SNW increases when a simple counting
task is imposed or discrimination of various stimuli is
required [27]. The increase seems to depend on the difficulty of the task [28]. The extent, however, to which these
waves may be held to be a manifestation of orienting
activity is, according to Rohrbaugh and Gaillard [29], an
important but largely unexplored problem. At least two
O-wave components were described [24, 27, 28]. The first
component peaking at about 500 ms or a little later is followed by a second broadly distributed wave. While the
first is negative at frontal scalp sites (slow negative wave
1, SNW1) and usually positive parietally (slow positive
wave, SPW), the later component of the O-wave (SNW2)
is uniformly negative at all sites, but tends to be strong
predominantly at frontal or central sites. Results of more
recent investigations suggest that the anterior negative
and the posterior positive aspects of the O-wave are independent and can be dissociated [10, 24]. Support for
assumptions relating the O-wave to orienting activity
comes from different lines of evidence. First, it is suggested by the bilateral distribution of the O-wave.
Particularly the late, SNW2, component appears to be
more pronounced over the right hemisphere [27]. This laterality agrees with hypotheses linking orienting and attention with functions of the right hemisphere [30].
Furthermore, the anterior-posterior gradient of the O-
wave is in accordance with neuropsychological and neurophysiological literature relating the frontal cortex to
attentional processes incorporated in orienting [31]. Also,
the effects of different experimental manipulations on
elicitation and magnitude of the O-wave seem to confirm
its relationship to orienting and attention [27]. The amplitude of the O-wave, for example, appears to depend on
manipulations affecting task relevance and stimulus significance [24, 27, 29]. In healthy subjects, the amplitude of evoked cortical
responses normally decreases with repetitive presentation
of a stimulus. This phenomenon is commonly referred to
as habituation [32], and one of its metabolic advantages
could be protection against overexcitation and lactate
accumulation in sensory cortices [33].Migraine patients, unlike healthy control subjects,
show a dissimilar electrophysiological behavior, as shown
for visual-evoked potentials (VEPs), which fail to habituate to repeated stimuli and sometimes even potentiate [34,35]. This also holds true for ERPs [36]. It has been
hypothesized that the simultaneous occurrence of this
deficit of habituation and a reduced mitochondrial energy
reserve might lead to the activation of the trigeminovascular system, culminating in the migraine attack [37].
Extensive research on EP habituation described two main
kinds of physiological phenomena: short-term habituation
(STH) and long-term habituation (LTH) [38]. The STH
occurs when individual stimuli are repeated at short interstimulus intervals and is often attributed to a refractory
period [39] or sensory gating [40]. It has been proposed
that, if the stimuli are presented in pairs, the first stimulus
activates excitatory inputs that cause a neuronal response
as well as inhibitory pathways that induce a suppressionS53of neuronal activity on the following stimulus. These
processes result in STH [41]. This form of habituation is
independent of stimulus intensity, psychological characteristics of the stimulus, and attentional demands, and is
possibly under strong genetic control [4042]. In particular, the STH of the P50 (P1), a positive peak occurring
about 4090 ms after stimulus onset, has been intensively
investigated because of its relevance in a number of psychiatric and neurological disorders [40, 42, 43]. The LTH
occurs across blocks of stimuli lasting minutes and is
often explained with the dual-process theory of
Thompson and Spencer [32] or the comparator theory of
Sokolov [44]. It has been suggested that the habituation of
a neuronal response occurs when the external events of
the environment and the internal neural model are
matched and no longer produce an orienting response of
attentional focus [32, 39, 44, 45]. This habituation
depends on the intensity, psychological significance of the
stimulus, and attentional resources [38, 39, 45, 46]. The
LTH was investigated most often for the P300 wave in the
odd-ball paradigm and requires allocation of attentional
resources [43, 47]. In summary, the STH is more probably
related to the preattentive screening out of irrelevant stimuli [3941], while the LTH is attributed to the attentive
processes of the orienting response [32, 43, 44, 47]. Migraine patients are also distinguished by a higher
dependence of their auditory-evoked cortical responses on
stimulus intensity than healthy volunteers [48, 49]. This
increased intensity dependence of auditory potentials
(IDAP) is, like sensitization, only detectable interictally[50]. The middle latency auditory potentials are complex
neuronal phenomena that depend on thalamo-cortico-thalamic activities and various state-setting subcortico-cortical
pathways; there is, nonetheless, indirect evidence that IDAP
is inversely correlated with central serotonergic transmission [51]. If deficient, the latter may lead to a decreased cortical preactivation level and possibly be the basis for the
observed lack of habituation in migraineurs [52].It has been hypothesized hat the strong IDAP found in
migraineurs might be due to a deficit of habituation to
high-intensity stimuli [53], and thus these two distinct
phenomena may in fact be expressions of the same dysfunction in habituation. Migraine patients are characterized as having attacks caused by a deficit of habituation,
or even potentiation, of cortical-evoked and ERPs.
Another interictal electrophysiological abnormality found
in migraineurs is a marked dependence on the intensity of
auditory-evoked cortical potentials. In contrast to habituation which is measured during repetition of a stimulus at
the same intensity, IDAP is obtained after stimulations of
increasing intensities. These two electrophysiological
phenomena are thought to reflect different aspects of cortical information processing, and their impairment in
migraine may be directly or indirectly related to decreased
activity in the state-setting monoaminergic projections to
the sensory cortices [54]. Habituation, i.e., amplitude
reduction of a cortical response to a sustained stimulus of
equal intensity, is considered to reflect an adaptive cortical mechanism protecting from sensory overstimulation[55] and lactate accumulation [33]. In the Aplysia gillwithdrawal reflex, habituation is controlled by serotonergic neurons [56]. Although habituation of long latency
cortical-evoked responses in the human brain is likely to
have more complex underlying molecular mechanisms,
there are some arguments suggesting that it might be
inversely related to serotonin activity. Evers et al. [57]
have shown in migraineurs that habituation, assessed by a
latency increase of the event-related visual P300 response,
varies inversely with platelet serotonin content; in particular, it arguments, i.e., normalizes, during the attack in
parallel with a decrease in platelet serotonin.
Normalization of amplitude habituation during the attack
was also found for VEPs [58] and CNV [59]. In a recent
study, Siniatchkin et al. [43] demonstrate that migraine
patients are characterized by two kinds of habituation
deficits compared with healthy subjects: (a) the sensory
gating deficit or reduced STH of the P50 wave, independent of attentional demands to the stimulation, and (b) the
LTH of the P300 wave under circumstances of increased
cortical processing and enhanced mobilization of attentional resources. These habituation deficits represent
abnormalities of different levels of information processing
in migraine. The reduced P50 STH possibly represents the
impairment of an early stage of the automatic screening
out of stimuli, whereas the P300 LTH deficit results from
disturbed cognitive processing. According to the authors,
how the STH and LTH are related, and which mechanisms
underlie these abnormalities, needs to be investigated in
further studies.The majority of studies on evoked and event-related
potentials in migraine have shown two abnormalities:
increased amplitudes of averages of large numbers of trials and lack of habituation in successive trial blocks [60].
At first sight, increased amplitudes of cortical-evoked
responses would favor the hypothesis of cortical hyperexcitability in migraine between attacks [61]. However, it is
useful to remember that evoked responses are averaged
over a large number of stimulations and that for low numbers of trials amplitudes were lower, not higher, than in
healthy volunteers [60]. The suggestion has been made
therefore that the increase of EP amplitude found in some
studies was not due to cortical hyperexcitability, but merely to the lack of habituation of the responses during sustained stimulation [34]. In the strict physiological sense,
hyperexcitability indeed indicates that the threshold to
obtain a response is decreased and/or that a greaterS54response is induced by a given suprathreshold stimulus[54]. Although habituation is a complex neurobiological
phenomenon, it might uniquely depend, for corticalevoked activation, on the preactivation excitability level.
According to the ceiling theory [62], which is most
often applied to explain the occurrence of an augmenting or reducing response to increasing stimulation
intensities, a low preactivation level of sensory cortices
allows a wider range of suprathreshold activation before
reaching the ceiling and initiating a reducing response,i.e., habituation. This theory, applied to EP findings in
migraineurs, would explain both the low first-block
amplitude for most EPs and the lack of habituation on trial
repetition [54]. There is evidence that the preactivation
level of cortical excitability depends on the so-called
state-setting, chemically addressed connections that originate in the brain stem and involve serotonin and noradrenaline as transmitters [63, 64]. Low interictal activity
of these systems, especially of the raphe-cortical serotoninergic pathway, could indeed be responsible for the
observed electrophysiological abnormalities in
migraineurs [65]. If this hypothesis is correct, one would
expect that decreasing cortical activation would produce
lack of habituation in healthy volunteers, and vice versa,
that increasing activation in migraineurs would normalize
their habituation patterns [54]. This was indeed recently
demonstrated by using repetitive transcranial magnetic
stimulation (rTMS). High-frequency rTMS of the occipital cortex, known to activate the underlying cortex, was
followed by a normalization of VEP habituation in
migraineurs, while low-frequency rTMS, which has an
inhibitory effect, induced a VEP habituation deficit in normal control subjects [66]. The precise relationship
between interictal abnormal cortical information processing and migraine pathogenesis remains to be determined.
The possibility that dysfunctional monoaminergic nuclei
in the brain stem may play a causative role in migraine
attacks [67], and may be responsible independently for
interictal lack of habituation of evoked potentials as an
epiphenomenon, cannot be excluded. We know that cortical habituation is also a protective mechanism against
overstimulation [33] and that the mitochondrial phosphorylation potential as studied by magnetic resonance spectroscopy is reduced interictally in the brain of migraineurs
[6870]. Lack of habituation may favor, under certain circumstances, a metabolic disequilibrium, which would
lead to activation of the major pain-signaling system of
the brain, the trigeminovascular system [71]. The ictal
normalization of EP amplitudes and habituation suggests
that there is, in close temporal proximity to the migraine
attack, an increase in the cortical preactivation level,
probably due to enhanced activity in raphe-cortical serotoninergic pathways [54]. Interestingly, the ictal normalization of visual-evoked P3 habituation is accompanied by
a decrease in platelet serotonin content [57], and two PET
studies have shown brain stem activation during the
migraine attack [67, 72]. According to the biobehavioral
theory of migraine [73], this ictal normalization of electrophysiological findings could be part of a homeostatic
process. Various studies have demonstrated a more negative bCNV in patients with migraine during foreperiods
that are shorter than 2 s [74]. Studies employing longer
foreperiods predominantly report a higher early wave
amplitude in migraine without aura [7577], but higher
late wave amplitudes have also been reported [75]. The
higher CNV amplitudes are believed to emerge as a result
of the slow habituations over trials [74, 76, 78]. Research
in cortical electrophysiology such as that embracing the
thalamic gating model [77] or the threshold regulation
theory [79] has linked the CNV to cortical negative brain
potentials, such as the CNV, reflecting the task-related
tuning of cortical areas. The threshold regulation theory
postulates that preparation is achieved by lowering neuronal firing thresholds in the appropriate cortical neuronal
networks and by selectively increasing cortical excitability. This is ultimately reflected in an enhanced negativity
in these particular cortical areas. In agreement with these
ideas, migraine researchers have generally attributed these
CNV findings to a hyperactivity of central catecholaminergic systems [80, 81]. High catecholaminergic activity is
thought to induce a state of cortical hyperexcitability and
arousal that presents normal habituation [82]. The early
wave increases even more during the days before an attack
but decreases to the level of healthy control subjects during the attack [82]. Along with this normalization of CNV
early wave amplitude, the impaired habituation also
resolves during the attack [77]. In the 23 days following
the attack, the CNV remains at this normal level after
which it gradually deteriorates again [83]. This normalization during an attack has been suggested to be related
to the depletion of noradrenaline activity combined with
an increased serotoninergic transmission [84]. During the
postattack period, Mulder et al. [82] demonstrated lower
CNV early and late wave amplitudes over the frontal cortex in migraineurs without aura compared with control
subjects, suggesting postattack hypoexcitability of the
frontal cortex. Concordant studies from different laboratories have shown that CNV amplitude is increased in
migraineurs between attacks, mainly in those suffering
from migraine without aura [75, 76, 78, 85, 86]. This
increase was more pronounced for the early component in
comparison with healthy volunteers or tension-type
headache patients [76], which can be interpreted as disturbed attention and possibly reflect an excess of excitatory versus inhibitory processes. When taking into account
age as a variable [87], decrease in early CNV amplitudeS55with aging was found in healthy volunteers but not in
migraineurs, which the authors interpreted as a disturbance of cerebral maturation. Disease duration also had an
effect on CNV abnormalities in migraineurs [88]. A strong
familial influence on CNV parameters was reported by
Siniatchkin et al. [89], who found abnormalities not only
in migraineurs but also in healthy subjects with a positive
family history for migraine [90]. The sensitivity of CNV
to different prophylactic agents used in migraine and the
relative specificity of the difference between headache
forms suggest that CNV is a useful method for studying
headaches [91]. Siniatchkin et al. [92] found differences
in habituation of the early component of CNV in migraine
and chronic daily headache (CDH); however, in the latter,
these differences have been related to lower baseline
amplitudes, and the authors conclude that CNV may be
considered a predictive variable for transformed migraine.
According to Aurora [93], depression may also have
played a role in the CNV difference in CDH, and also
comorbidity (i.e., depression and analgesic overuse) may
alter CNV.Studies of the P300 component of the classic odd-ball
paradigm of ERPs in migraine gave conflicting results. A
reduced P300 amplitude and a prolonged latency were
found when an auditory stimulus was used [94, 95], except
in one study [96]. In a paradigm with visual stimuli, however, there were no differences in P300 amplitude, but prolonged P300 latency in migraine without aura [36, 57, 97].
In the passive odd-ball paradigm, latency and amplitude of
the P3a component, which reflects automatic processing
of a novel stimulus, were on average normal interictally in migraine [98].References1. Regan D (1989) Human brain electrophysiology. Evoked potentials and
evoked magnetic fields in science and
medicine. Elsevier, New York2. Polich J (1996) Meta-analysis of P300
normative aging studies.
Psychophysiology 33:3343533. Coles MGH, Smid HGOM, Scheffers
MK, Otten LJ (1995) Mental chronometry and the study of human information processing. In: Rugg MD, Coles
MGH (eds) Electrophysiology of mind.
Oxford University Press, New York, pp
861314. Fjell AM, Walhovd KB (2001) P300
and Neuropsychological tests as measures of aging: scalp topography and
cognitive changes. Brain Topogr
14:25405. Demiralp T, Ademoglu A, Comerchero
M, Polich J (2001) Wavelet analysis of
P3a and P3b. Brain Topogr 13:252676. Picton TW (1992) The P300 wave of
the human event-related potential. J
Clin Neurophysiol 9:4564797. Polich J (1999) P300 in clinical applications: meaning, methods and measurements. In: Niedermeyer E, Lopes
da Silva FH (eds)
Electroencephalography: basic principles, clinical applications, and related
fields. William & Wilkins, Baltimore,
pp 107910918. Courchesne E, Hillyard SA, Galambos
R (1975) Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential
in man. Electroencephalogr Clin
Neurophysiol 39:1311439. Squires N, Squires K, Hillyard S
(1975) Two varieties of long-latency
positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
38:38740110. Friedman D, Simpson GV (1994) ERP
amplitude and scalp distribution to target and novel events: effects of temporal order in young, middle-aged and
older adults. Cog Brain Res 2:496311. Knight R (1996) Contribution of
human hippocampal region to novelty
detection. Nature 383:25625912. Walter WG, Cooper R, Aldrige VJ,
McCallum WC, Winter AL (1964)
Contingent negative variation: an electric sign of sensory-motor association
and expectancy in the human brain.
Nature 203:38038413. Kropp P, Gerber WD (1995)
Contingent negative variation during
migraine attack and interval: evidence
for normalization of slow cortical
potentials during the attack.
Cephalalgia 15:12312814. Rizzo PA, Amabile G, Caporali M,
Spadaro M, Zanasi M, Morocuti CA
(1978) CNV study in a group of
patients with traumatic head injuries.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
45:28115. Rockstroh B, Elbert T, Birbaumer N,
Lutzenberger W (1982) Slow brain
potentials and behavior. Urban &
Schwarzenberg, Munich16. Drevets WC, Burton H, Videen TO,
Snyder AZ, Simpson JR, Raichle ME
(1995) Blood flow changes in human
somatosensory cortex during anticipated stimulation. Nature 373:24925217. Murtha S, Chertkow H, Beauregard M,
Dixon R, Evans A (1996) Anticipation
caused increased blood flow to the
anterior cingulated cortex. Hum Brain
Mapp 4:10311218. Mnatsakanian EV, Tarkka IM (2002)
Task-specific expectation is revealed in
scalp recorded slow potentials. Brain
Topogr 15:879419. Brunia CH (1988) Movement and stimulus preceding negativity. Biol Psychol
26:16517820. Hamano T, Luder HO, Ikeda A,
Coldura TF, Comair YG, Shibasaki H
(1997) The cortical generators of the
contingent negative variations in
humans: a study with subdural electrodes. Electroencephalogr Clin
Neurophysiol 104:257268S5621. Rosahl SK, Knight RT (1995) Role of
prefrontal cortex in generation of the
CNV. Cereb Cortex 2:12312422. Cui RQ, Egkher A, Huter D, Lang W,
Lindinger G, Deecke L (2000) High
resolution spatiotemporal analysis of
the contingent negative variation in
simple or complex motor tasks and a
non motor task. Clin Neurophysiol
111:1847185923. Fletcher PC, Henson RNA (2001)
Frontal lobes and human memory.
Insights from functional neuroimaging.
Brain 124:84988124. Loveless NE, Sanford AJ (1974)
Effects of age on the contingent negative variation and preparatory set in a
reaction-time task. J Gerontol
29:526325. Rohrbaugh JW, Syndulko K, Lindsley
DB (1976) Brain wave components of
the contingent negative variation in
humans. Science 191:1055105726. Zimmer H, Demmel R (2000)
Habituation and laterality of orienting
processes as reflected by slow negative
waves. Biol Psych 53:1617627. Rohrbaugh JW (1984) The orienting
reflex: performance and central nervous system manifestations. In:
Parasuraman R, Davies R (eds)
Varieties of attention. Academic Press,
Orlando, Fl, pp 32337328. Rohrbaugh JW, Syndulko K, Lindsley
DB (1978) Cortical slow negative
waves following non-paired stimuli:
effects of task factors.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
45:55156729. Rorbaugh JW, Gaillard AWK (1983)
Sensory and motor aspects of the contingent negative variation. In: Gaillard
AWK, Ritter W (eds) Tutorials in
Event related potential research:
endogenous components. North-
Holland Publishing, Amsterdam, pp
26931030. Heilman KM, Van den Abell T (1980)
Right hemisphere dominance for attention: the mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetrics of inattention
(neglect). Neurology 30:32733031. Giard M-H, Perrin F, Pernier J,
Bouchet P (1990) Brain generators
implicated in the processing of auditory stimulus deviance: a topographic
event related potential study.
Psychophysiol 27:62764032. Thompson R, Spencer WA (1966)
Habituation. A model phenomenon for
the study of neuronal substrates of
behaviour. Psychol Rev 22:164333. Sappey-Marinier D, Galabrese G, Fein
G, Hugg JW, Biggins C, Weiner MW
(1992) Effect of photic stimulation on
human visual cortex lactate and phosphates using 1H and 31P magnetic resonance spectroscopy. J Cereb Blood F
Met 22:58459234. Schoenen J, Wang W, Albert A,
Delwaide PJ (1995) Potentiation
instead of habituation characterizes
visual evoked potentials in migraine
patients between attacks. Eur J Neurol
22:11512235. Afra J, Cecchini AP, De Pasqua V,
Albert A, Schoenen J (1998) Visual
evoked potentials during long periods
of pattern-reversal stimulation in
migraine. Brain 121:23324136. Evers S, Bauer B, Suhr B, Husstedt
IW, Grotemeyer KH (1997) Cognitive
processing in primary headache: a
study on event-related potentials.
Neurology 48:10811337. Schoenen J (1996) Deficient habituation of evoked cortical potentials in
migraine: a link between brain biology,
behavior and trigeminovascular activation. Biomed Pharmacother 22:717838. Woods DL, Elmasian R (1986) The
habituation of event-related potentials
to speech sounds and tones.
Electroencephal Clin Neurophysiol
65:44745939. Roemer RA, Shagass C, Teyler TJ
(1984) Do human evoked potentials
habituate? In: Peeke HV, Petrinovich L
(eds) Habituation, sensitization, and
behaviour. Academic Press, New York,
pp 32534640. Adler LE, Freedman R, Ross RG,
Olincy A, Waldo MC (1999)
Elementary phenotypes in the neurobiological and genetic study of schizophrenia. Biol Psych 46:81841. Freedman R, Waldo M, Bickford-
Wimer P, Nagamoto H (1991)
Elementary neuronal dysfunction in
schizophrenia. Schizophr Res
4:23324342. Schwarzkopf SB, Lamberti S, Smith
DA (1993) Concurrent assessment of
acoustic startle and auditory P50
evoked potential measures of sensory
inhibition. Biol Psych 33:8152843. Siniatchkin M, Kropp P, Gerber W-D
(2003) What kind of habituation is
impaired in migraine patients?
Cephalalgia 23:51151844. Sokolov E (1963) Perception and the
conditioned reflex. Pergamon, Oxford45. Loveless N (1983) The orienting
response and evoked potentials in man.
In: Siddle D (ed) Orienting and habituation: perspectives in human research.
John Wiley, New York, pp 7110846. Woods DL, Elmasian R (1986) The
habituation of event-related potentials
to speech sounds and tones.
Electroencephal Clin Neurophysiol
65:44745947. Polich J, McIsaac HK (1994)
Comparison of auditory P300 habituation from active and passive conditions. Int J Psychophysiol 17:253448. Wang W, Timsit-Berthier M, Schoenen
J (1996) Intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials is pronounced in
migraine: an indication of cortical
potentiation and low serotoninergic
neurotransmission. Neurology
22:1404140949. Siniatchkin M, Kropp P, Neumann M,
Gerber W, Stephani U (2000) Intensity
dependence of auditory evoked cortical
potentials in migraine families. Pain
22:2475450. fra J, Sandor PS, Schoenen J (2000)
Habituation of visual and intensity
dependence of auditory evoked cortical
potentials tends to normalize just
before and during the migraine attack.
Cephalalgia 22:71471951. Hegerl U, Juckel G (1993) Intensity
dependence of auditory evoked potentials as an indicator of central serotoninergic neurotransmission: a new
hypothesis. Biol Psychiatry
22:17318752. Schoenen J (1996) Deficient habituation of evoked cortical potentials in
migraine: a link between brain biology,
behavior and trigeminovascular activation. Biomed Pharmacother 22:717853. Wang W, Timsit-Berthier M, Schoenen
J (1996) Intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials is pronounced in
migraine: an indication of cortical
potentiation and low serotonergic neurotransmission. Neurology
22:14041409S5754. Ambrosini A, Rossi P, De Pasqua V,
Pierelli F, Schoenen J (2003) Lack of
habituation causes high intensity
dependence of auditory evoked cortical
potentials in migraine. Brain
126:2009201555. Thompson RF, Berry SD, Rinaldi PC,
Berger TW (1979) Habituation and orienting reflex: the dual-process theory
revised. In: Kimmel HD, van Olst EH,
Orlebeke JF (eds) The orienting reflex
in humans. Lawrence Erlbaum,
Hillsdale, NJ, pp 216056. Kandel ER (1991) Cellular mechanisms of learning and biological basis
of individuality. In: Kandel ER,
Schwartz JH, Jessell TM (eds)
Principles of neural science, 3rd edn.
Elsevier, New York, pp 1009103157. Evers S, Quibeldey F, Grotemeyer KH,
Suhr B, Husstedt IW (1999) Dynamic
changes of cognitive habituation and
serotonin metabolism during the
migraine interval. Cephalalgia
19:4859158. Judit A, Sndor P, Schoenen J (2000)
Habituation of visual and intensity
dependence of auditory evoked cortical
potentials tends to normalize just
before and during the migraine attack.
Echolalia 20:71471959. Kropp P, Gerber WD (1995)
Contingent negative variation during
migraine attack and interval: evidence
for normalization of slow cortical
potentials during the attack.
Cephalalgia 15:123860. Ambrosini A, de Noordhout A, Sndor
PS, Schoenen J (2003)
Electrophysiological studies in
migraine: a comprehensive review of
their interest and limitations.
Cephalalgia 23:133161. Welch KMA, DAndrea G, Tepley N,
Barkley G, Ramadan NM (1990) The
concept of migraine as a state of central neuronal hyperexcitability. Neurol
Clin 8:81782862. Knott JR, Irwin DA (1973) Anxiety,
stress and the contingent negative variation. Arch Gen Psychiatry
29:53854163. Hegerl U, Juckel G (1993) Intensity
dependence of auditory evoked potentials as an indicator of central serotonergic neurotransmission: a new hypothesis. Biol Psychiatry 33:17318764. Mesulam MM (1990) Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed
processing for attention, language and
memory. Ann Neurol 28:59761365. Schoenen J (1996) Abnormal cortical
information processing between
migraine attacks. In: Sandler M,
Ferrari M, Harnett S (eds) Migraine
pharmacology and genetics. Altman,
London, pp 23325366. Bohotin V, Fumal A, Vandenheede M,
Grard P, Bohotin C, Maertens de
Noordhout A et al (2002) Effects of
repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on visual evoked potentials in
migraine. Brain 125:11167. Weiller C, May A, Limmroth V, Juptner
M, Kaube H, Schayck RV et al (1995)
Brain stem activation in spontaneous
migraine attacks. Nature Med 1:65866068. Welch KMA, Levine SR, DAndrea G,
Schultz L, Helpern JA (1989)
Preliminary observations on brain
energy metabolism in migraine studied
by in vivo 31-phosphorus NMR spectroscopy. Neurology 39:53854169. Barbiroli B, Montagna P, Cortelli P,
Funicello R, Iotti S et al (1992)
Abnormal brain and muscle energy
metabolism shown by 31P magnetic
resonance spectroscopy in patients
affected by migraine with aura.
Neurology 42:1209121470. Montagna P, Cortelli P, Monari L,
Pierangeli G, Parchi P et al (1994)
31P-Magnetic resonance spectroscopy
in migraine without aura. Neurology
44:66666871. Schoenen J (1994) Pathogenesis of
migraine: the biobehavioural and
hypoxia theories reconciled. Acta
Neurol Belg 94:798672. Sndor PS, Dydak U, Schoenen J,
Crelier G, Kollias S et al (2000)
Functional magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging (fMRSI) shows cortical lactate changes during prolonged
visual stimulation in migraine with
aura: the metabolic equivalent of electrophysiological dyshabituation?
Cephalalgia 20:27773. Welch KMA (1986) Migraine: a biobehavioural disorder. Cephalalgia
[Suppl. 4]:10311074. Schoenen J, Maertens de Noordhout A,
Timsit-Berthier M, Timsit M (1986)
Contingent negative variation and efficacy of beta-blocking agents in
migraine. Cephalalgia 6:22923375. Bcker KB, Timsit-Berthier M,
Schoenen J, Brunia CH (1990)
Contingent negative variation in
migraine. Headache 30:60460976. Kropp P, Gerber WD (1993) Is
increased amplitude of contingent negative variation in migraine due to cortical hyperactivity or to reduced habituation? Cephalalgia 13:374177. Kropp P, Gerber WD (1995)
Contingent negative variation during
migraine attack and interval: evidence
for normalization of slow cortical
potentials during the attack.
Cephalalgia 15:12312878. Maertens de Noordhout A, Timsit-
Berthier M, Timsit M, Schoenen J
(1986) Contingent negative variation in
headache. Ann Neurol 19:788079. Rockstroh B, Elbert T, Canavan A,
LutzenbergerW, Birbaumer N (1989)
Slow cortical potentials and behavior.
Urban & Schwarzenberg, Baltimore80. Timsit-Berthier M, Mantanus H,
Poncelet M, Marissiaux P, Legros JJ
(1986) Contingent negative variation as
a new method to assess the catecholaminergic systems. In: Gallai V
(ed) Maturation of the CNS and evoked
potentials. Elsevier, New York, pp
26026881. Nagel-Leiby S, Welch KM, Dandrea
G, Grunfeld S, Brown E (1990) Eventrelated slow potentials and associated
catecholamine function in migraine.
Cephalalgia 10:31732982. Mulder EJCM, Linssen WHJP,
Passchier J, de Geus EJC (2001)
Interictal and postictal contingent negative variation in migraine without aura.
Headache 41:727883. Kropp P, Gerber WD (1998) Prediction
of migraine attacks using a slow cortical potential, the contingent negative
variation. Neurosci Lett 257:737684. Gerber WD, Schoenen J (1998)
Biobehavioral correlates in migraine:
the role of hypersensitivity and information-processing dysfunction.
Cephalalgia 18[Suppl 21]:51185. Schoenen J, Maertens A, Timsit-
Berthier, Timsit M (1985) Contingent
negative variation (CNV) as a diagnostic and physiopathologic tool in
headache patients. In: Rose FC (ed)
Migraine. Clinical and research
advances. Karger, Basel, pp 1725S5886. Schoenen J, Timsit-Berthier M, Timsit
M (1985) Correlations between contingent negative variation and plasma levels of catecholamines in headache
patients. Cephalalgia 5 [Suppl 1]:48087. Kropp P, Kirbach U, Detlefsen JO,
Siniatchkin M, Gerber WD, Stephani
U (1999) Slow cortical potentials in
migraine: a comparison of adults and
children. Cephalalgia 19[Suppl25]:606488. Kropp P, Siniatchkin M, Gerber WD
(2000) Contingent negative variation
as indicator of duration of migraine
disease. Funct Neurol 15[Suppl3]:788189. Siniatchkin M, Kirsch E, Kropp P,
Stephani U, Gerber WD (2000) Slow
cortical potentials in migraine families.
Cephalalgia 20:88189290. Siniatchkin M, Kropp P, Gerber WD
(2001) Contingent negative variation
in subjects at risk for migraine without
aura. Pain 94:15916791. Maertens de Noordhout A, Timsit-
Berthier M, Timsit M, Schoenen J
(1987) Effects of beta blockade on
contingent negative variation in
migraine. Ann Neurol 21:1111292. Siniatchkin M, Gerber W-D, Kropp P,
Vein A (1998) Contingent negative
variation in patients with chronic daily
headache. Cephalalgia 18:56556993. Aurora SK (1998) CNV in CDH.
Editorial commentary. Cephalalgia
18:53194. Drake ME, Pakalnis A, Padamadan H
(1989) Long-latency auditory event
related potentials in migraine.
Headache 29:23924195. Wang W, Schoenen J, Timsit-Berthier
M (1995). Cognitive functions in
migraine without aura between attacks:
a psychophysiological approach using
the oddball paradigm. Neurophysiol
Clin 25:31196. Mazzotta G, Alberti A, Cantucci A,
Gallai V (1995) The event-related
potential P300 during headache-free
period and spontaneous attack in adult
headache sufferers. Headache
35:21021597. Evers S, Bauer B, Grotemeyer KH,
Kurlemann G, Husstedt IW (1998)
Event-related potentials (P300) in primary headache in childhood and adolescence. J Child Neurol 13:32232698. Wang W, Schoenen J (1998) Interictal
potentiation of passive oddball auditory event-related potentials in
migraine. Cephalalgia 18:261265
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Springer-Verlag 2004
Abstract
We reviewed P300 and contingent negative variation (CNV) studies performed in migraine in order to identify their relevance in migraine and the role of neurophysiology in migraine. Publications available to us were completed by a Medline search. There is experimental and clinical evidence for loss of cognitive habituation in migraine which may serve as a specific diagnostic tool; therefore, we reviewed studies on migraine that analyzed habituation and lack of habituation by P300 and CNV, performing short-term habituation (STH) and long-term habituation (LTH). Finally, we described the two components of P300 (a and b) and of CNV (early and late wave) and the two abnormalities reported from the majority of studies on event-related potential in migraine: increased amplitude of average event-related potential and lack of habituation. These abnormalities are especially related to the early component characterizing orienting activity.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer