About the Authors:
József Vuts
Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: Department of Biointeractions and Crop Protection, Rothamsted Research, Harpenden, United Kingdom
ORCID logo http://orcid.org/0000-0001-6240-0905
Christine M. Woodcock
Roles Investigation
Affiliation: Department of Biointeractions and Crop Protection, Rothamsted Research, Harpenden, United Kingdom
Lisa König
Roles Investigation
Affiliation: Karl-Franzens-University, Graz, Austria
Stephen J. Powers
Roles Formal analysis
Affiliation: Department of Computational and Analytical Sciences, Rothamsted Research, Harpenden, United Kingdom
John A. Pickett
Roles Funding acquisition, Methodology, Writing – original draft
Affiliation: School of Chemistry, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
Árpád Szentesi
Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Systematic Zoology and Ecology, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
Michael A. Birkett
Roles Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Biointeractions and Crop Protection, Rothamsted Research, Harpenden, United Kingdom
Introduction
Of the ca. 450,000 known phytophagous insect species, at least 70% are mono- and oligophagous, consuming a narrow range of species within one plant family [1,2]. Due to their making up a large proportion of the Insecta class (45%), the evolution of herbivorous insects strongly influences theories on the evolution of insect diversity [3]. Within herbivores, mono- and oligophagy is a more frequent strategy than polyphagy (ca. 70% vs 30%, respectively), which might reflect the relatively higher rates of speciation and extinction among mono- and oligophages [4]. The forces thought to drive the evolution of host plant specialisation in herbivorous insects include i) autonomous hereditary changes in the chemosensory system [5], ii) distribution of plant secondary metabolites [6], iii) interspecific competition [7], iv) escape from predation [8], or v) ecological fitting [9]. Populations of species undergoing host shift may then become reproductively separated by natural and/or sexual selection [10].
Sexual selection has been suggested as another mechanism driving host shift in phytophagous insects, proposing that host specialization can result from female mate choice being driven by certain diet-derived male pheromones, which induce egg-laying on a specific host plant [11]. The model predicts that, after a host shift, the ability to sequester host plant compounds into an attractive pheromone evolves first, accompanied by a pre-existing female preference for that plant-derived pheromone, which would drive specialization on that host plant by indirectly selecting for oviposition preference genes. Two recent examples emphasize the role of sexual selection in prezygotic isolation by chemical signalling during host range shift. Individuals of a leaf beetle, Phaedon cochleariae Fabr. (Coleoptera: Chrysomelidae), that develop on an alternative host plant show differences in their cuticular hydrocarbon profile compared to individuals on the ancestral host, and prefer to mate with beetles of the same host line [12]. Assortative mating has been observed among individuals of another chrysomelid, the dried bean beetle (Acanthoscelides obtectus Say), that were reared on the ancestral host, common bean (Phaseolus vulgaris L.) [13], as opposed to those reared on an acceptable non-host, chickpea (Cicer arietinum L.) (Fabaceae). (For a definition of an acceptable non-host, see [14].) Although chickpea-reared individuals had reduced mate discrimination characteristics, low level of mate choice isolation between the two host lines was found, in which the role of cuticular hydrocarbons as contact pheromones [15] was proposed. The authors speculate that plant-induced phenotypic divergence in mate recognition cues can lead to reproductive isolation between host lines [12,13].
A. obtectus is an oligophagous seed predator specializing on the Ph. vulgaris group [16]. Originating in the Neotropics, it has become cosmopolitan through human-mediated migrations since the domestication and distribution of beans [17], and can have several generations a year in the tropics and subtropics, multiplying both in the field and in granaries [18]. The species has served as a model for life history studies. Profound differences in life history traits, such as developmental time, body mass or lifespan, were found between two host lines developed on bean and chickpea [19]. Short-term (plastic) effects of host shift were also observed on certain physiological properties. For example, one-generation shift from bean to chickpea significantly increased the activity of phosphatase enzymes [20] and altered cuticular hydrocarbon profiles [15], reflecting immediate plastic responses to nutritional changes. Chemical communication in A. obtectus not only involves contact chemoreception [15,21], but also a male-produced volatile sex pheromone [22], highlighting the complex nature of intraspecific signalling in this species. In the present study, we tested the hypothesis that artificial host shift induces segregation in sex pheromone communication between two host lines of A. obtectus. Specifically, the following hypotheses were tested: 1) volatile chemical signalling changes plastically with host line formation; 2) such changes are maintained under breeding on acceptable non-host (chickpea) for multiple generations; and 3) altered chemical communication facilitates host shift that may eventually lead to reproductive isolation.
Materials and methods
Insect rearing
Acanthoscelides obtectus beetles were obtained from a laboratory population reared either on dry ‘Cannellini’ beans (Ph. vulgaris) (bean line) or chickpea seeds (C. arietinum) (chickpea line). A third `host line`was created by rearing beetles from chickpea on beans (chickpea/bean line). The original laboratory population was from a natural infestation on Ph. vulgaris in Hungary. Maintenance conditions were as follows: artificial lighting with a 16:8 h L:D photoperiod, a constant temperature of 20°C, and 60% RH. In order to obtain virgin insects, seeds were kept individually in wells of an Eppendorf rack and covered with a piece of transparent acetate sheet until beetle emergence, at which point the sexes were separated immediately for use in experiments.
Dynamic headspace collection (air entrainment)
To determine the composition of pheromone blends, aeration extracts [23] were prepared from male A. obtectus of the three different host lines. Air was pumped (12VDC pump; KNF, Reiden, Switzerland) through an activated charcoal filter and Teflon tubing at 600 mL/min into a glass chamber (10 cm diam., 5 cm height), enclosing a single 6-8-day-old beetle, to provide a positive pressure of clean air. Males of this age were used because their volatile release peaks at this time (S1 Fig). A glass tube (8 cm × 0.3 cm i.d.) containing 50 mg Porapak Q adsorbent sandwiched between glass wool plugs was inserted into the air outlet. Air was drawn from the chamber through the tube under negative pressure at a flow rate of 500 mL/min., and at a constant temperature of 20°C. In this way, volatile compounds were collected on Porapak Q traps for 24 h and were then eluted from the adsorbent with 750 μL freshly distilled diethyl ether. Extracts were concentrated to 50 μL under a gentle nitrogen stream and kept at −20°C until use. Six biological replicates were prepared from male beetles developed in each host seed type.
Analyses of male headspace extracts
The identity of peaks in the aeration extracts were confirmed by comparison of their gas chromatographic (GC) and mass spectrometric (GC-MS) properties with those of authentic standards, and by GC peak enhancement using authentic samples. GC conditions were: an Agilent 6890A GC equipped with a cool on-column injector, a flame ionization detector (FID) and a 50 m × 0.32 mm i.d. HP-1 column (J & W Scientific, Folsom, CA, USA). The oven temperature was maintained at 30°C for 1 min, then programmed to increase at 5°C/min to 150°C, then held for 0.1 min, then programmed to increase at 10°C/min to 250°C and then held for 20 min. The carrier gas was hydrogen. GC-MS conditions were: a Micromass Autospec Ultima magnetic sector mass spectrometer (Waters, Milford, MA) attached to an Agilent 6890N GC (fitted with a 50 m × 0.32 mm i.d. x 0.52 μm film thickness non-polar HP-1 column, J & W Scientific), and equipped with a cool-on-column injector. Ionization was by electron impact (70 eV, 220°C). The GC oven temperature was maintained at 30°C for 5 min and then programmed to increase at 5°C/min to 250°C, with a 70-min run time. Six compounds, representing the volatile sex pheromone constituents of male A. obtectus, were of interest: methyl (2E,4Z,7Z)-2,4,7-decatrienoate, methyl (2E,4Z)-2,4-decadienoate, (3Z,6E)- and (3E,6E)-α-farnesene, methyl (E,R)-2,4,5-tetradecatrienoate and octadecanal. Because the total amounts of compounds varied between sampled individuals, analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied (completely randomised design) to the normalised peak area data, the normalisation consisting of calculating the ratios of the areas under the peaks for the 6 compounds to the total area. This normalisation accounts for some of the overall differences between beetles. A log-to-base 10 transformation was used to account for some heterogeneity of variance over the three treatments, with an adjustment of 0.001 to account for zero observations. ANOVA, providing an F-test for the overall difference between host lines, was followed by application of Fisher’s least significant difference (LSD) test (p < 0.05) for the statistical separation of means of most interest. The Genstat (2015, 18th edition, VSN International Ltd, Hemel Hempstead, UK) statistical package was used for the analysis.
Authentic standards of the six constituents of the male A. obtectus pheromone were obtained according to the parenthetical references as follows: methyl (2E,4Z)-2,4-decadienoate [24], methyl (E,R)-2,4,5-tetradecatrienoate and methyl (2E,4Z,7Z)-2,4,7-decatrienoate [25], (3Z,6E)- and (3E,6E)-α-farnesene [26,27], octadecanal (by tetrapropylammonium perruthenate (TPAP)-mediated oxidation of octadecanol [28]).
Behavioural assays
Two types of behaviour assay were used to compare the response of females from the three different host lines to the three synthetic chemical blends of pheromones of male beetles from these host lines or control (air). This provided a three (host lines) by four (blends + control) factorial treatment structure for assessment.
Firstly, a four-arm olfactometer [29] was used to measure female responses to synthetic blends. Glass arms (narrow part: 50 mm length × 2.5 mm diam., wide part: 90 mm length × 20 mm diam.) were attached through a 3 mm diameter hole to the end of each of the four arms (Figure A in S2 File). Prior to each experiment, all glassware was washed with Teepol (Orpington, UK) detergent, rinsed with acetone and distilled water and baked in an oven overnight at 160°C. Perspex components were washed with Teepol solution, rinsed with 80% ethanol solution and distilled water, and left to air-dry. The olfactometer was illuminated from above by diffuse uniform lighting from two 18W/35 white fluorescent light bulbs screened with red acetate. It was surrounded by black paper to remove any external visual stimuli. Test compounds were applied onto filter paper strips (ca. 2 cm2, Whatman, Little Chalfont, UK) in proportions and doses in such a way that the amounts released per hour were similar to those emitted by one male beetle over 1 h specific to each host line (Table 1). When only one blend was tested, three control arms were treated with 10 μL redistilled hexane, whereas in the case of two treatments, these were positioned in two opposite arms, accompanied by two control arms. After allowing the solvent to evaporate for 30 s, the strips were inserted into the glass arms. This setup ensured the robustness of the experiment by making it less likely for an insect to accidentally walk in or out of the treated region. A single virgin, 3-6-day-old female was introduced through a hole in the top of the olfactometer. (In comparison, mated females give weak positive response to the male pheromone; J. Vuts, pers. comm.). Air was drawn through the central hole by a vacuum pump and, consequently, pulled through each of the four side arms (75 mL/min/arm) and subsequently exhausted from the room. Each beetle was given 2 min to acclimatize in the olfactometer (the room temperature was 20°C and RH 60%), after which the experiment was run for 16 min. The olfactometer was rotated 90° every 4 min to control for any directional bias. The olfactometer was divided into four regions, corresponding to each of the four arms, and the time spent in each arm was recorded. The structure of the experiment was such that there were 18 assays (3 individual host lines plus 3 pairs of host lines by 3 blends) by 10 replicates each, giving 180 olfactometer runs altogether, but there were either two or three blend treatments (including control) tested in each one, giving an unbalance that precludes use of ANOVA. Instead, a linear mixed model (LMM), fitted using the method of residual maximum likelihood (REML), was applied to the data, which takes account of the design structure of olfactometer replicate runs and areas within them (as split-plots) before testing (F-tests) for the main effects and interaction between the factors of host line and blend. Three analyses were done: using data from all assays, using data only from the assays assessing blends in pairwise fashion, and using data only from the assays comparing individual blends. A square root transformation of the time spent data was used to account for some heterogeneity of variance over the treatment combinations, assessment of residuals revealing that they endorsed conformation to the assumptions of the analysis. Predicted means for statistically relevant (p < 0.05, F-test) terms from the model were output for comparison using Fisher’s LSD test (p < 0.05). Genstat was used for this analysis.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. Composition (ng/μL) of male blends used in olfactometer bioassays.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.t001
Ten females from a population reared on chickpea for 50 generations were also tested against the bean and chickpea male blends and control, in 10 replicate olfactometer runs. Data from this assay was also analysed using a linear mixed model, albeit in this case to test the main effects and interaction between the factors of number of generations (1 or 50) and type of male blend (bean, chickpea or control).
Secondly, a still-air bioassay was developed to model natural situations in A. obtectus chemical communication that may occur in granaries where bean and chickpea seeds can be stored in close proximity (Figure A in S3 File). Ten virgin females from each of the bean and chickpea host lines were released in a Petri dish (14 cm diam. × 2 cm height). Individuals from the chickpea line were marked with a water-based white marker (Tipp-Ex Ecolutions, Societe Bic, France) by placing a small dot on the dorsal surface of the thorax. The arena was connected via 9 mm tubes (custom-made pipette tips, Sigma-Aldrich, UK) to three plastic vials (2.5 cm diam. × 9 cm height, Starlab, Muri, Switzerland). At the bottom of each of 2 of these vials, two male-equivalent amounts of one of the synthetic pheromone blends on a filter paper strip were placed, either typical of males from the bean or chickpea host line. In the third vial, an equal volume (20 μL) of hexane was added on a paper strip as control. This experiment was replicated 7 times. Environmental conditions were the same as those for the olfactometer tests. The number of beetles trapped inside each vial, as well as in the main arena, was determined after 24 h. The resulting data in the form of counts in a two (bean or chickpea host line female) by four (bean, chickpea, control or Petri dish) outcomes contingency table for each replicate were analysed by fitting a generalized linear model (GLM) assuming a Poisson distribution and using a natural log link function (ln), taking the replicates as a blocking factor. Having tested (Chi-squared test) the interaction between the two factors, the predicted counts for statistical comparison on the transformed scale and for presentation on the back-transformed scale were output. Comparisons were made using Fisher’s LSD test (p < 0.05). There was no evidence of over-dispersion given the assumption of the Poisson distribution. Genstat was used for this analysis.
Oviposition assays
Oviposition assays were set up to assess if development in different host seeds affects subsequent female host preference (Hopkins’ host selection principle, [30]). A Petri dish (5.5 cm diam. × 1.5 cm height) was divided into quarters by a 1 cm high cross-section made of two black filter paper strips. A disc from the same type of filter paper covered the bottom of the dish to make the white eggs more visible. Three bean seeds were put in each of two opposite sectors, and three chickpea seeds were put in the other two opposite sectors. A pair of virgin A. obtectus was released into each dish, the cross division allowing free access for the beetles to the entire arena. Temperature was maintained at 20°C, and each arena was left for 5 days. There were 10 replicates for each host line. Egg numbers were summed for each seed type in each dish at the end of the experiment, and the two seed types were compared using a paired t-test (p < 0.05) for each female host line.
Larval assays
To determine seed preference of 1st instar larvae originating from eggs laid by 1st generation females, 10 eggs per host line were placed inside a Petri dish (5.5 cm diam. × 1.5 cm height) in the middle of a space surrounded by two bean and two chickpea seeds in an alternated manner and in close proximity to each other (0.1 cm) and from the eggs (1 cm). The replication was 10 Petri dishes for the bean and chickpea lines and 7 for the chickpea/bean line. The same test was performed with eggs from females bred for over 50 generations on chickpea (10 replicate dishes, 20 eggs per replicate). The number of emerging adults was determined for each seed type, and data was analysed by paired t-test (p < 0.05) for each host line and number of generations.
Electroantennography (EAG)
To test neurophysiological responses in the olfactory periphery of female A. obtectus to male odour (sex pheromone) characterising different host lines, EAG tests were conducted using the whole antennae of virgin females from the three host lines. Antennal recordings were made using Ag–AgCl glass electrodes filled with saline solution. An antenna was freshly amputated at the base from a live A. obtectus and suspended between the two electrodes. The tip of the terminal process of the antenna was removed to ensure a good contact with a high-impedance amplifier (UN-06; Ockenfels Syntech GmbH, Kirchzarten, Germany). The base of the antenna was connected to a grounded electrode. Test stimuli consisted of five virgin males from each host line in a Pasteur pipette, through which 1 mL of air was puffed, using a stimulus controller, into a humidified airstream flowing towards the antenna. Live beetles were used as stimuli [31], because EAG responses to synthetic blends are problematic to interpret owing to the different vapour pressure of constituents in the headspace inside the pipette [32]. Stimuli were presented at random order, and the absolute negative amplitude changes in response to the stimuli were recorded. A blank Pasteur pipette served as control. The experiment provided a 3 female host lines by 4 odours (three male host lines plus control) factorial treatment structure. There were 5 replicate antennae used per female host line and odour. ANOVA was used to assess the main effects and interaction between the two factors (F-tests), followed by Fisher’s LSD test (p < 0.05) to compare female responses to male odour from different host lines.
Results
GC and GC-MS analysis showed significant differences (p<0.05, F-tests) in the sex pheromone composition of A. obtectus between males originating from beans and chickpeas (Fig 1; S1 File). Intriguingly, there was an almost complete lack of methyl (2E,4Z,7Z)-2,4,7-decatrienoate in the headspace extract of 1st generation males reared on chickpea when compared to males reared on bean. However, the emission of this compound returned after rearing 1st generation chickpea beetles on bean seeds again (chickpea/bean line). The amount of methyl (E,R)-2,4,5-tetradecatrienoate doubled in males after the bean-chickpea-bean transitions.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 1. Composition of volatile extracts of male Acanthoscelides obtectus beetles derived from different seeds (black bars: Bean, grey bars: chickpea, empty bars: Chickpea/bean).
Asterisks mark pheromone compounds the ratio of which differ significantly between host lines [n = 6 males/host line, methyl (2E,4Z,7Z)-2,4,7-decatrienoate p = 0.004, methyl (E,R)-2,4,5-tetradecatrienoate p = 0.006, ANOVA p = 0.05, LSD test].
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.g001
Females gave strong positive behavioural responses to all types of male blend when they were presented on their own (Fig 2; Figures B-C in S2 File). When faced with pairwise choices, a host line-specific pattern emerged: females reared on beans did not discriminate between the three blend types, whereas chickpea-reared ones and those from the chickpea/bean line preferred their own type of blend to the blend of males from beans, not differentiating, however, between the chickpea and chickpea/bean types (Fig 3; Figures D-E in S2 File; the statistical analysis of the full set of single blend and pairwise blend choice assays together is given in Figures F-G in S2 File).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 2. Behavioural responses of 1st generation virgin Acanthoscelides obtectus females to synthetic male pheromone blends, representing compositions observed for the three host lines, in olfactometer bioassays.
n = 10/female host line/male blend type; linear mixed model, fitted using the method of residual maximum likelihood.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.g002
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 3. Behavioural responses of 1st generation virgin Acanthoscelides obtectus females to synthetic male pheromone blends, representing compositions observed for the three host lines, in pairwise comparisons in olfactometer bioassays.
n = 10/female host line/male blend type; linear mixed model, fitted using the method of residual maximum likelihood; columns with same letter not differing significantly (p<0.05, LSD).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.g003
Still-air bioassays gave similar results to those from olfactometer assays, showing that females reared on beans did not discriminate between blends of males reared on bean and chickpea, whereas females reared on chickpea preferred the pheromone blend of chickpea-reared males compared to bean-reared males (Table 2). There was no significant difference between female numbers within each blend. There was some partial evidence of an interaction between treatment (blend) and female population (p = 0.156, Chi-squared test; Figure B in S3 File).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 2. Predicted means for the number of virgin Acanthoscelides obtectus females (±SE) attracted to male blends in still-air bioassays.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.t002
Response patterns of the primary olfactory organ in female A. obtectus, the antenna, showed that independent of seed origin, females gave the strongest EAG responses to the odour of bean males, not differentiating between the other two host lines, or control (Fig 4; S4 File).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 4. EAG responses of antennae of 1st generation virgin Acanthoscelides obtectus females to the odour of live males from different host lines.
The insert exemplifies a typical EAG response (n = 5/female host line; black bars: bean, grey bars: chickpea, empty bars: chickpea/bean; control = blank air; columns with same letter within a host line not differing significantly by ANOVA and Fisher`s LSD test, p<0.05).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.g004
In oviposition assays, 1st generation females from all host lines laid the greatest number of eggs in sectors with bean seeds as compared to sectors with chickpea seeds (Table 3). Host-searching first instar larvae, on the other hand, did not discriminate between seeds (Table 4).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 3. Results of oviposition choice tests with Acanthoscelides obtectus.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.t003
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 4. Seed choice by 1st instar Acanthoscelides obtectus larvae from different host lines, measured as number of emerged adults.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.t004
When investigating the consequences of breeding A. obtectus for 50 generations on chickpea, a strong behavioural preference for the sex pheromone blend of chickpea males over that of bean males was found in 50th generation females, which was similar to that of 1st generation ones (Fig 5A; Figures H-I in S2 File). However, the greatest number of eggs was laid in sectors with bean seeds (Fig 5B).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 5. Behavioural and oviposition responses of virgin Acanthoscelides obtectus females reared on chickpea for 50 generations.
A: Behavioural responses to synthetic male pheromone blends in olfactometer bioassays; n = 10; columns with same letter not differing significantly, linear mixed model, fitted using the method of residual maximum likelihood. B: Oviposition responses; n = 10; p<0.001, paired t-test; asterisk: statistical difference.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.g005
Discussion
Artificially-induced host shifts from beans to chickpeas has been shown to cause changes in the physiology and life history traits of A. obtectus [19,20]. After the 1st instar larvae have entered the seed, their development is determined by the chemical composition of the endosperm, which is reflected in the difference in life history and physiological traits between bean- and chickpea-reared A. obtectus laboratory populations. Nasonia vitripennis Walker wasps (Hymenoptera: Pteromalidae) benefited from an additional supply of linoleic acid in their diet, as they produced increased amounts of pheromone [33]. N. vitripennis can synthesize linoleic acid from oleic acid de novo, and males have higher pheromone titres when they develop on hosts that have been experimentally enriched in the unsaturated fatty acid precursors. We found that A. obtectus males reared on chickpea produce only trace amounts of methyl (2E,4Z,7Z)-2,4,7-decatrienoate, but this compound reappears in the volatile bouquet of descendant males reared on beans. The biosynthesis of methyl (2E,4Z,7Z)-2,4,7-decatrienoic acid, i.e., the acid part of the ester, can be rationalized either via lipoxygenase-mediated cleavage of (9Z,12Z,15Z)-9,12,15-octadecatrienoic acid (α-linolenic acid) or by a sequence of four β-oxidation steps and rearrangement of the same precursor [22]. As male A. obtectus do not feed as adults, the nutritional composition of the seed endosperm during larval development must determine which precursors are available in the adult stage for de novo pheromone production. As bean (Ph. vulgaris) seeds contain five times more α-linolenic acid than chickpea seeds [34], this creates a platform for new hypotheses to be formed on the biosynthetic origins of methyl (2E,4Z,7Z)-2,4,7-decatrienoate. Methyl (E,R)-2,4,5-tetradecatrienoate is a key component of the A. obtectus male sex pheromone [22]. Together with octadecanal, it is abundant in the cuticular wax layer of the thorax and the elytra [21], whereas the more volatile C10 methyl esters and farnesenes can only be extracted from the headspace of live males (J. Vuts, pers. observation). Methyl (E,R)-2,4,5-tetradecatrienoate is also utilised as a mate-recognition signal and as an anti-aphrodisiac, and may also be key to host line discrimination in mate-finding, as its amounts changed significantly during the chickpea-bean shift, highlighting the parsimonious use of the compound. All this points to multiple ecological functions [35] for the complex, six-component male volatile blend of A. obtectus. Considering the diversity of the genus Acanthoscelides (several hundred species; [36]), the pheromone may have roles for reproductive isolation encoded in its complexity.
Single-generation shift from bean to chickpea causes rapid changes in several physiological and behavioural traits in A. obtectus, including alterations in the composition of cuticular hydrocarbons, causing a certain level of sexual isolation between host lines [13,15]. As well as the altered male volatile sex pheromone composition after the bean-chickpea shift, we showed that females from the two host lines respond differently to male chemical signals. Bean-reared females did not differentiate between the bean and chickpea male pheromone blends, possibly reflecting a broad acceptance range of sex pheromone composition. Such high degree of phenotypic plasticity was also reported for other behavioural traits [19]. A novelty effect, i.e., exposure to a novel, unusual stimulus [37,38], may explain why the chickpea male blend was also found highly attractive by bean-reared females in our laboratory bioassays. However, we argue that the activity of the chickpea blend would decrease during repeated exposure because of habituation [39,40]. In contrast, females reared on chickpea showed significant preference for the male pheromone blend of their own host line, even in the first generation, indicating a plastic phenotypic process in action in response to the new environment. Their response profiles may be attributed to a high excitatory state of the central nervous system (CNS) [41], causing increased reactivity to their own host line blend (sensitisation). Chickpea-reared A. obtectus was described as behaviourally less plastic [19], which, in the light of data presented here, may also include their sexual behaviour. According to West-Eberhard ([42], p. 253), “…the probability of producing a favourable variant (in a new or challenging situation at a particular time) is greater for behaviour (and other relatively plastic traits) than for morphology.” Also, “The key to the evolutionary role of behaviour, however, is not lability alone, but a combination of lability and the consistency with which given behaviors occur in given conditions: a genetic (evolutionary) response to selection depends on a repeated association, under selection, of a particular phenotype… and a particular fitness-affecting condition…” It is argued that adaptive behavioural plasticity evolves more easily than adaptive morphological plasticity, due to the greater abundance of potential cues for regulating the expression of an immediate (behavioural) adaptive response [42]. Thus, (olfactory) sensitisation via continuous reinforcement is suggested to be maintained in A. obtectus chickpea females breeding on chickpea by regular exposure to the male pheromone (environmental conditioning). This new phenotype can then become fixed in the experimental environment (chickpea host) by epigenetic inheritance [43]. However, the phenotype sensitised to the chickpea male pheromone can only be maintained in F1 chickpea/bean females if it has an adaptive value in the new environment. To show that the narrowly tuned response profile of chickpea beetles can be inherited by the F1 generation developing on a new host (bean), cross-testing studies will be necessary.
The oviposition assays in this paper indicate no change in host plant preference in any of the first-generation host lines, or in beetles that are continuously reared on chickpea. This contradicts previous work, which found that chickpea-reared females laid more eggs on chickpea than on bean [44]. Experience can narrow down the inherited (potential) host range of certain polyphagous larvae, i.e., feeding on one host species can reduce the acceptability of other hosts in later instars (induced preference; [45]). However, only a few examples of Hopkins’ host selection principle exist [46–48]), and the general agreement is that larval experience does not influence oviposition behaviour ([30,49] and references therein). Also, again contrary to previous work [44], there seems to be no correlation between female oviposition preference and larval performance in A. obtectus. Females of all host lines preferred to lay eggs on bean seeds, whereas larvae entered both beans and chickpeas with equal frequency, measured as adult emergence (performance). No obvious correlation was found between maternal host preference and F1 performance either, concluding that the female’s oviposition decisions are suboptimal [50]. She most probably assesses the physico-chemical characteristics of the seed testa before egg-laying, which, however, does not convey information about the endosperm, the primary factor influencing larval survival. First-instar larvae entering the seeds chew through the seed coat and then spit it out, a behaviour performed possibly to avoid toxic lignin, polyphenolic or condensed tannin compounds present in the testa [51]. Being mobile, they can actively choose between host plants [52] and thus may increase their chance of survival if given the choice between seeds. Apart from the genetically determined host range, there is no information exchange between the female and the L1 larvae about the suitability of substrates, resulting in the lack of positive correlation between female oviposition preference and larval performance. Thus, the preference-performance hypothesis [53], i.e., female insects preferentially oviposit on plants that maximise the survival and performance of their larvae, has no support in A. obtectus.
It was suggested [4,54] that behavioural changes can be induced in phytophagous insects by mutations that alter chemoreceptor sites, facilitating diversification on to new host plants. Recent studies highlight the key role of both the central and peripheral nervous system in semiochemical-based herbivore host specialisation [55,56] and the importance of mutations affecting metabolic pathways and the olfactory system in shaping host specialisation processes [57]. Host shift in phytophagous insects is suggested to be characterised by the loss of recognising the original host plant (‘switch-over’), and is not an extension of host range [58]. It is argued that a loss of sensitivity to deterrents of the potential new host is a prerequisite for host switch [59]. However, others suggest the terminology of ‘host range expansion’ for cases when a population colonizes a new host plant but continues to use previous host plants [60]. It is not possible to determine which process may be true for A. obtectus, simply because the phenomenon has only been studied under experimental situations, which was pointed out before [61]. It was shown that laboratory-reared A. obtectus larvae can develop in suboptimal host seeds, such as chickpea [62–64]. However, this is reported also in natural populations in chickpea-producing areas [65–67]. Where beans and chickpeas are cultivated and stored in close proximity, for example in India, the Middle East or East Africa, host shift to chickpea may happen under natural circumstances. Thus, studies presented in this paper must be extended to such sympatric wild populations.
Since antennae of both first-generation A. obtectus females and those reared on chickpea continuously gave very similar electrophysiological (EAG) responses to the odour of males across the host lines included in this study, we conclude that plastic processes in the CNS, but not in the peripheral nervous system, determine female behaviour towards male pheromones. The modulatory effect of the environment, e.g., starvation, on EAG responses has been shown in a number of insect species [68], but such peripheral sensory modifications do not appear to be involved in A. obtectus chemical communication under host shifts. Although significant alterations were observed in chemical signalling immediately after shift from one host to another (our hypothesis 1), which were maintained over generations in chickpea lines (hypothesis 2), there is no support for hypothesis 3 (altered chemical communication facilitates host shift that may eventually lead to reproductive isolation) from our studies. Both the behavioural and electrophysiological data suggest that the more common bean line would assimilate rarer populations of the chickpea line in sympatry, because bean-reared females do not discriminate between host line pheromone blends. Also, because the level of male mate discrimination reduces after shift from bean to chickpea [13], narrow mate preference of chickpea females would be counteracted by non-discriminatory chickpea males. In addition, the effect of assortative mating in bean-reared males [13] would be similarly weakened by non-discriminatory bean females.
Assuming that the hypothesis by Quental et al. [11] holds true, i.e., newly acquired compounds sequestered from alternative hosts influence female mate preferences and drives sexual isolation and host shift, bean-reared females would be expected to select for their own host line pheromone; bean and chickpea lines should embody plastic changes occurring in the CNS into morphological changes in the olfactory periphery at the neuronal level (although morphological changes are rarer to acquire than behavioural; [42]); oviposition substrate choices should change accordingly. According to the hierarchy-threshold model [69], each plant species the insect encounters has an intrinsic acceptability rank, and if stimuli received from the plant exceed a certain threshold, the plant will be accepted. The model predicts “the maintenance of significant genetic variance for acceptability of hosts which are not used in nature, since this genetic variance is not subject to selection” ([69], p.61). Thus, oviposition on an acceptable non-host in A. obtectus may be a result of changes in the hierarchy threshold for that plant (i.e., chickpea), a phenomenon also indicated for Vigna radiata (L.) R. Wilczek/Vigna unguiculata L. and Callosobruchus maculatus Fabr., another legume-bruchid system [70]. Host shift in field A. obtectus populations would require a fine balance among several factors, including the female’s ability to recognise the pericarp and accept the fruit of the acceptable non-host. As larvae can develop in chickpea but larval experience does not influence oviposition choice, the process breaks at every egg-laying event. How pheromones may facilitate host shift remains to be answered, for which studies with individuals from sympatric wild populations will be necessary, as outlined above. For A. obtectus and other bruchids, such experiments may also provide key information for the design of novel, semiochemical-based management strategies [71].
Supporting information
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
S1 Fig. Age-specific release of Acanthoscelides obtectus sex pheromone components by virgin males from the bean line.
Raw data included.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.s001
(DOCX)
S1 File. Raw data for pheromone release from males from different host lines.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.s002
(DOCX)
S2 File. Olfactometer data and analysis.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.s003
(DOCX)
S3 File. Still-air bioassay data and analysis.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.s004
(DOCX)
S4 File. EAG data for females from different host lines.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144.s005
(DOCX)
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Lydia Bird and Zafirah Hussain for help with the bioassays.
Citation: Vuts J, Woodcock CM, König L, Powers SJ, Pickett JA, Szentesi Á, et al. (2018) Host shift induces changes in mate choice of the seed predator Acanthoscelides obtectus via altered chemical signalling. PLoS ONE 13(11): e0206144. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206144
1. Bernays EA, Chapman RE. Host-plant selection by phytophagous insects. Boston, MA: Springer; 1994.
2. Schoonhoven LM, van Loon JJA, Dicke M. Insect-plant biology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005.
3. Labandeira CC. The history of associations between plants and animals. In: Herrera CM, Pellmyr O, editors. Plant-animal interactions. An evolutionary approach. Malden-Oxford-Victoria: Wiley-Blackwell; 2002. pp. 26–76.
4. Jermy T, Lábos E, Molnár I. Stenophagy of phytophagous insects–a result of constraints on the evolution of the nervous system. In: Maynard-Smith J, Vida G, editors. Organisational constraints on the dynamics of evolution. Manchester, UK: Manchester Univ. Press; 1990. pp. 157–166.
5. Jermy T. Evolution of insect-plant relationships—a devil's advocate approach. Entomol Exp Appl. 1993; 66: 3–12.
6. Fraenkel GS. The raison d`être of secondary plant substances. Science. 1959; 129: 1466–1470. pmid:13658975
7. Ehrlich PR, Raven PH. Butterflies and plants: a study in coevolution. Evolution. 1964; 18: 586–608.
8. Bernays EA. Host specificity in phytophagous insects: selection pressure from generalist predators. Entomol Exp Appl. 1988; 49: 131–140.
9. Janzen DH. On ecological fitting. Oikos. 1985; 45: 308–310.
10. The Marie Curie SPECIATION Network. What do we need to know about speciation? Trend Ecol Evol. 2012; 27: 27–39. pmid:21978464
11. Quental TB, Patten MM, Pierce NE. Host plant specialization driven by sexual selection. Am Nat. 2007; 169: 830–836. pmid:17479468
12. Geiselhardt S, Otte T, Hilker M. Looking for a similar partner: host plants shape mating preferences of herbivorous insects by altering their contact pheromones. Ecol Lett. 2012; 15: 971–977. pmid:22708843
13. Stojković B, Savković U, Đorđević M, Tucić N. Host-shift effects on mating behavior and incipient pre-mating isolation in seed beetle. Behav Ecol. 2014; 25: 553–564.
14. de Boer G, Hanson FE. Foodplant selection and induction of feeding preference among host and non-host plants in larvae of the tobacco hornworm Manduca sexta. Entomol Exp Appl. 1984; 35: 177–193.
15. Savković U, Vučković I, Stojković B. The growth on different stored legume species affects the profiles of cuticular hydrocarbon (CHC) in Acanthoscelides obtectus (Say). J Stored Prod Res. 2012; 50: 66–72.
16. Imura O. Life histories of stored product insects. In: Fujii K, Gatehouse AMR, Johnson CD, Mitchel R, Yoshida T, editors. Bruchids and legumes: Economics, ecology and coevolution. Netherlands: Kluwer; 1990. pp. 257–269.
17. Alvarez N, McKey D, Hossaert-McKey M, Born C, Mercier L, Benrey B. Ancient and recent evolutionary history of the bruchid beetle, Acanthoscelides obtectus Say, a cosmopolitan pest of beans. Mol Ecol. 2005; 14: 1015–1024. pmid:15773933
18. Paul UV, Hilbeck A, Edwards PJ. Pre-harvest infestation of beans (Phaseolus vulgaris L.) by Acanthoscelides obtectus Say (Coleoptera: Bruchidae) in relation to bean pod maturity and pod aperture. Int J Pest Manag. 2010; 56: 41–50.
19. Savković U, Đorđević M, Šešlija JD, Lazarević J, Tucić N, Stojković B. Experimentally induced host-shift changes life-history strategy in a seed beetle. J Evol Biol. 2016; 29: 837–847. pmid:26790127
20. Janković-Tomanić M, Jovanović DŠ, Savković U, Đorđević M, Stojković B, Lazarević J. Host expansion modifies activity of phosphatases in a legume store pest Acanthoscelides obtectus (Say). J Stored Prod Res. 2015; 62: 32–35.
21. Vuts J, Powers SJ, Caulfield JC, Pickett JA, Birkett MA. Multiple roles of a male-specific compound in the sexual behaviour of the dried bean beetle, Acanthoscelides obtectus. J Chem Ecol. 2015; 41: 287–293. pmid:25786892
22. Vuts J, Francke W, Mori K, Zarbin PHG, Hooper AM, Millar JG, et al. Pheromone bouquet of the dried bean beetle, Acanthoscelides obtectus (Col.: Chrysomelidae), now complete. Eur J Org Chem. 2015; 2015: 4843–4846.
23. Birkett MA. The chemistry of plant signalling. In: Baluška F, Ninkovic V, editors. Plant communication from an ecological perspective. Berlin–Heidelberg: Springer; 2010. pp. 21–42.
24. Mori K. Pheromone synthesis. Part 249: Syntheses of methyl (R,E)-2,4,5-tetradecatrienoate and methyl (2E,4Z)-2,4-decadienoate, the pheromone components of the male dried bean beetle, Acanthoscelides obtectus (Say). Tetrahedron. 2012; 68: 1936–1946.
25. Mori K. Pheromone synthesis. Part 257: Synthesis of methyl (2E,4Z,7Z)-2,4,7-decatrienoate and methyl (E)-2,4,5-tetradecatrienoate, the pheromone components of the male dried bean beetle, Acanthoscelides obtectus (Say). Tetrahedron. 2015; 71: 5589–5596.
26. Svatoš A, Attygalle AB. Characterization of vinyl-substituted, carbon-carbon double bonds by GC/FT-IR analysis. Anal Chem. 1997; 69: 1827–1836. pmid:9164159
27. Ramaiah P, Pegram JJ, Millar JG. A short synthesis of (3Z,6E)-α-farnesene. J Org Chem. 1995; 60: 6211–6213.
28. Griffith WP, Ley SV, Whitcombe GP, White AD. Preparation and use of tetra-n-butylammonium per-ruthenate (TBAP reagent) and tetra-n-propylammonium per-ruthenate (TPAP reagent) as new catalytic oxidants for alcohols. J Chem Soc, Chem Commun. 1987; 1987: 1625–1627.
29. Pettersson J. An aphid sex attractant I. Biological studies. Insect Syst Evol. 1970; 1: 63–73.
30. Barron AB. The life and death of Hopkins' host-selection principle. J Insect Behav. 2001; 14: 725–737.
31. Pouzat J. The role of sense organs in the relations between bruchids and their host plants. In: Labeyrie V, editor. The ecology of bruchids attacking legumes (pulses). Series Entomol. 1981; 19: 61–72.
32. Roelofs WL. The scope and limitations of the electroantennogram technique in identifying pheromone components. In: McFarlane NR, editor. Crop protection agents–their biological evaluation. New York: Academic Press; 1977. pp. 147–165.
33. Brandstetter B, Ruther J. An insect with a delta-12 desaturase, the jewel wasp Nasonia vitripennis, benefits from nutritional supply with linoleic acid. Sci Nat. 2016; 103: 40. pmid:27116611
34. Grela ER, Samolińska W, Kiczorowska B, Klebaniuk R, Kiczorowski P. Content of minerals and fatty acids and their correlation with phytochemical compounds and antioxidant activity of leguminous seeds. Biol Trace Element Res. 2017; 180: 338–348. pmid:28357649
35. Blum MS. Semiochemical parsimony in the Arthropoda. Annu Rev Entomol. 1996; 41: 353–374. pmid:15012333
36. Johnson CD. Relations of Acanthoscelides with their plant hosts. In: Labeyrie V, editor. The ecology of bruchids attacking legumes (pulses). Series Entomol. 1981; 19: 73–81.
37. Bernays EA, Bright K, Howard JJ, Raubenheimer D, Champagne D. Variety is the spice of life: frequent switching between foods in the polyphagous grasshopper Taeniopoda eques Burmeister (Orthoptera: Acrididae). Anim Behav. 1992; 44: 721–731.
38. Soibam B, Shah S, Gunaratne GH, Roman GW. Modeling novelty habituation during exploratory activity in Drosophila. Behav Proc. 2013; 97: 63–75. pmid:23597866
39. Groves PM, Thompson RF. Habituation: A dual-process theory. Psych Rev. 1970; 77: 419–450.
40. Szentesi Á, Jermy T. The role of experience in host plant choice by phytophagous insects. In: Bernays EA, editor. Insect-plant interactions, vol. 2. Boca Raton-Ann Arbor-Boston: CRC Press; 1990. pp. 39–74.
41. Dethier VG, Solomon RL, Turner LH. Sensory input and central excitation and inhibition in the blowfly. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1965; 60: 303. pmid:5839258
42. West-Eberhard MJ. Phenotypic plasticity and the origins of diversity. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1989; 20: 249–278.
43. Jablonka E. Epigenetic inheritance and plasticity: The responsive germline. Progr Biophys Mol Biol. 2013; 111: 99–107. pmid:22975443
44. Tucić N, Milanović D, Mikuljanac S. Laboratory evolution of host plant utilization in the bean weevil (Acanthoscelides obtectus). Genet Select Evol. 1995; 27: 491–502.
45. Jermy T, Hanson FE, Dethier VG. Induction of specific food preference in lepidopterous larvae. Entomol Exp Appl. 1968; 11: 211–230.
46. Smith MA, Cornell HV. Hopkins`host-selection in Nasonia vitripennis and its implications for sympatric speciation. Anim Behav. 1979; 27: 365–370.
47. Thöming G, Larsson MC, Hansson BS, Anderson P. Comparison of plant preference hierarchies of male and female moths and the impact of larval rearing hosts. Ecology. 2013; 94: 1744–1752. pmid:24015518
48. Cahenzli F, Wenk BA, Erhardt A. Female butterflies adapt and allocate their progeny to the host-plant quality of their own larval experience. Ecology. 2015; 96: 1966–1973. pmid:26378318
49. Jermy T The role of experience in the host selection of phytophagous insects. In: Chapman RF, Bernays EA, Stoffolano JG Jr., editors. Perspectives in chemoreception and behaviour. New York-Berlin-Heidelberg: Springer; 1987. pp. 143–157.
50. Szentesi Á. Relationships between Hungarian bruchids and their leguminous host plants: behaviour, ecology and evolution. Doctoral thesis, Hungarian Academy of Sciences; 2007. [in Hungarian]
51. Stamopoulos DC. Toxic effect of lignin extracted from the tegument of Phaseolus vulgaris seeds on the larvae of Acanthoscelides obtectus (Say) (Col., Bruchidae). J Appl Entomol. 1988; 105: 317–320.
52. Ohtsuka Y, Toquenaga Y. Pioneer-follower dilemma game in Acanthoscelides obtectus (Coleoptera: Bruchidae). J Ethol. 2009; 27: 131–136.
53. Jaenike J. On optimal oviposition behaviour in phytophagous insects. Theor Popul Biol. 1978; 14: 350–356. pmid:751265
54. Dethier VG. Evolution of receptor sensitivity to secondary plant substances with special reference to deterrents. Am Nat. 1980; 115: 45.
55. Linz J, Baschwitz A, Strutz A, Dweck HKM, Sachse S, Hansson BS, Stensmyr MC. Host plant-driven sensory specialization in Drosophila erecta. Proc R Soc B. 2013; 280: 20130626. pmid:23595274
56. Tait C, Batra S, Ramaswamy SS, Feder JL, Olsson SB. Sensory specificity and speciation: a potential neuronal pathway for host fruit odour discrimination in Rhagoletis pomonella. Proc R So. B. 2016; 283: 20162101. pmid:28003447
57. Lavista-Llanos S, Svatoš A, Kai M, Riemensperger T, Birman S, Stensmyr MC, Hansson BS. Dopamine drives Drosophila sechellia adaptation to its toxic host. eLife. 2014; 3: 03785. pmid:25487989
58. Levins R, MacArthur R. An hypothesis to explain the incidence of monophagy. Ecology. 1969; 50: 910–911.
59. Chapman RF. It's all in the neurones. Entomol Exp Appl. 1999; 91: 259–265.
60. Agosta SJ. On ecological fitting, plant-insect associations, herbivore host shifts, and host plant selection. Oikos. 2006; 114: 556–565.
61. Labeyrie V. Is it sufficient to consider plant-insect relationships to conclude about evolution? In: Maynard-Smith J, Vida G, editors. Organisational constraints on the dynamics of evolution. Manchester, UK: Manchester Univ. Press; 1990. pp. 373–384.
62. Larson A, Fisher LK. The bean weevil and the southern cowpea weevil in California. Tech Bull USDA. 1930; 593: 1–70.
63. Jermy T, Szentesi Á. The role of inhibitory stimuli in the choice of oviposition site by phytophagous insects. Entomol Exp Appl. 1978; 24: 458–471.
64. Milanović D, Aleksić I, Tucić N. Nonrandom association between host choice and fitness in bean weevil (Acanthoscelides obtectus). J Zool Syst Evol Res. 1991; 29: 108–114.
65. Distribution Hariri G. and importance of bruchid attacks on different species of pulses consumed in the Near East. In: Labeyrie V, editor. The ecology of bruchids attacking legumes (pulses). Series Entomol. 1981; 19: 215–221.
66. Shade RE, Pratt RC, Pomeroy MA. Development and mortality of the bean weevil Acanthoscelides obtectus (Coleoptera: Bruchidae) on mature seeds of tepary beans, Phaseolus acutifolius, and common beans, Phaseolus vulgaris. Environ Entomol. 1987; 16: 1067–1070.
67. Lüthi C, Álvarez-Alfageme F, Ehlers JD, Higgins TJV, Romeis J. Resistance of αAI-1 transgenic chickpea (Cicer arietinum) and cowpea (Vigna unguiculata) dry grains to bruchid beetles (Coleoptera: Chrysomelidae). Bull Entomol Res. 2013; 103: 373–381. pmid:23458831
68. Reisenman CE. Hunger is the best spice: Effects of starvation in the antennal responses of the blood-sucking bug Rhodnius prolixus. J Insect Physiol. 2014; 71: 8–13. pmid:25280630
69. Courtney SP, Chen GK, Gardner A. A general model for individual host selection. Oikos. 1989; 55: 55–65.
70. Messina FJ. How labile are the egg-laying preferences of seed beetles? Ecol Entomol. 2004; 29: 318–326.
71. Rodríguez SA. Semiochemistry of the Bruchidae species. J Stored Prod Res. 2018; 77: 148–155.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2018 Vuts et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
The mechanisms of host shift in phytophagous insects are poorly understood. Among the many proposed processes involved, sexual selection via semiochemicals has recently been suggested. This hypothesizes that sexual communication using pheromones is modified as a result of development on a new host, and such plant-induced phenotypic divergence in mate recognition cues can lead to reproductive isolation between host lines. We tested this hypothesis on Acanthoscelides obtectus, an oligophagous bruchid of Phaseolus vulgaris beans worldwide, which also develops in acceptable non-hosts, such as chickpea (Cicer arietinum L.). Male sex pheromone blends of the bean, chickpea and chickpea/bean host lines during artificially induced host shifts showed different composition. Bean-reared females did not distinguish between blends, whereas chickpea and chickpea/bean females preferred the chickpea male pheromone. However, electrophysiological (EAG) responses to male odour of antennae of the three female host lines were similar, all preferring bean-reared males. Egg-laying choice tests revealed a uniform preference for bean seeds across female host lines, even after multiple generations, whereas larvae did not distinguish between bean and chickpea seeds. We conclude that the development of divergent chemical signalling systems during host shifts does not facilitate the evolution of host races in A. obtectus, because oviposition preferences remain unaffected.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer