It appears you don't have support to open PDFs in this web browser. To view this file, Open with your PDF reader
Abstract
Evidence suggests that differences in motor function are an early feature of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). One aspect of motor ability that develops during childhood is postural control, reflected in the ability to maintain a steady head and body position without excessive sway. Observational studies have documented differences in postural control in older children with ASD. The present study used computer vision analysis to assess midline head postural control, as reflected in the rate of spontaneous head movements during states of active attention, in 104 toddlers between 16–31 months of age (Mean = 22 months), 22 of whom were diagnosed with ASD. Time-series data revealed robust group differences in the rate of head movements while the toddlers watched movies depicting social and nonsocial stimuli. Toddlers with ASD exhibited a significantly higher rate of head movement as compared to non-ASD toddlers, suggesting difficulties in maintaining midline position of the head while engaging attentional systems. The use of digital phenotyping approaches, such as computer vision analysis, to quantify variation in early motor behaviors will allow for more precise, objective, and quantitative characterization of early motor signatures and potentially provide new automated methods for early autism risk identification.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details


1 Duke University, Duke Center for Autism and Brain Development, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Durham, USA (GRID:grid.26009.3d) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7961)
2 University of Utah, Salt Lake City, USA (GRID:grid.223827.e) (ISNI:0000 0001 2193 0096)
3 Duke University, Duke Center for Autism and Brain Development, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Durham, USA (GRID:grid.26009.3d) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7961) ; Duke University, Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Durham, USA (GRID:grid.26009.3d) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7961)
4 Duke University, Department of Population Health Sciences, Durham, USA (GRID:grid.26009.3d) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7961)
5 New York University, New York, NYU Langone Child Study Center, New York, USA (GRID:grid.137628.9) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 8753)
6 Duke University, Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Durham, USA (GRID:grid.26009.3d) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7961)
7 Duke University, Department of Pediatrics, Durham, USA (GRID:grid.26009.3d) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7961)
8 Duke University, Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Durham, USA (GRID:grid.26009.3d) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7961) ; Computer Science, and Mathematics, Duke University, Departments of Biomedical Engineering, Durham, USA (GRID:grid.26009.3d) (ISNI:0000 0004 1936 7961)