About the Authors:
Tanya Onushko
Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: Department of Biomedical Engineering, Marquette University, Milwaukee, WI, United States of America
ORCID logo http://orcid.org/0000-0002-0144-7221
Timothy Boerger
Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Physical Therapy, Marquette University, Milwaukee, WI, United States of America
ORCID logo http://orcid.org/0000-0003-1587-3704
Jacob Van Dehy
Roles Investigation, Software, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Leidos, Inc. Reston, VA, United States of America
Brian D. Schmit
Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Department of Biomedical Engineering, Marquette University, Milwaukee, WI, United States of America
Introduction
A better understanding of the adjustments in spatiotemporal gait parameters individuals make to maintain stable gait on different walking surfaces may provide information on fall risk. Walking in challenging environments can increase the risk and concerns for falling causing individuals to adopt a more cautious gait pattern [1–4]. When gait stability is challenged, such as transitioning between different surfaces [5, 6] or walking on irregular surfaces [7–10], individuals will adjust their foot placement to increase stability and decrease fall risk. During daily activities, individuals encounter a wide variety of environments that challenge stability and stepping strategies could vary depending on size and direction of the destabilizing perturbation. Determining how people adjust foot placement while walking in a broader range of destabilizing environments is necessary to understand fundamental stepping strategies (i.e. spatiotemporal gait parameters) used to reduce the loss of balance.
Maintaining mediolateral (ML) stability during gait is an active control process. It requires the center of mass (CoM) motion (i.e. velocity and position) stays within the base of support (BoS). Based on theoretical models of walking, adjusting foot placement to control the CoM is an efficient technique for modifying balance while walking [11]. Healthy adults take shorter, quicker, and wider steps to increase the BoS and limit CoM motion when walking in an unstable environment [10, 12, 13]; a strategy that appears to be an efficient technique for stabilizing gait based on theoretical models of walking [14].
Dynamic balance can be assessed using the margin of stability (MoS). The MoS takes into consideration the CoM position and velocity with respect to the BoS, such that increasing the distance between the CoM and BoS theoretically increases dynamic stability [11]. In response to balance perturbations, healthy adults will effectively adjust their stepping to maintain or increase the MoS [10, 12, 15–17]. For example, when walking on a self-paced treadmill, healthy adults chose to alter their step parameters (walking with wider, shorter and more frequent steps) rather than alter gait speed in order to increase the ML MoS [12]. Previous studies have examined different factors that influence MoS, such as visual perturbations [10], perturbation intensity [12], or direction [17]. Yet, no study has evaluated how continuous rotations compared to translations of the walking surface affects spatiotemporal gait parameters and ML dynamic stability.
In this study, we examined changes in spatiotemporal gait parameters and ML dynamic stability in response to sinusoidal oscillations of different amplitude, type, and direction imposed to the walking surface. We hypothesized that participants would walk with wider, shorter and quicker steps, increased ML MoS, and MoS variability for all imposed oscillations. Specifically, we hypothesized that these changes: 1) would be the largest during ML compared to anteroposterior (AP) oscillations, 2) would be largest during rotations compared to translations of the walking surface, and 3) would scale with increasing oscillation amplitude.
Methods
Subjects
We recruited fifteen young adults (8 males) with average age, height and weight of 21.3 (SD 1.4) years, 1.7 (SD 0.1) m and 68.8 (SD 10.7) kg, respectively. Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to participating. All experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of Marquette University.
Equipment
Subjects walked on a custom-designed treadmill system (Fig 1A). A Woodway treadmill (Woodway USA Inc., Waukesha, WI, USA) was mounted on a six-axis motion base (Moog Inc., East Aurora, NY, USA) allowing for movement in 6 degrees-of-freedom (Fig 1B). A custom-written LabVIEW program (National Instruments Corp., Austin, TX, USA) was used to control the oscillations of the motion base system. Subjects wore a safety harness during all walking trials, which did not provide weight support.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 1. Experimental equipment.
(A) Subjects walked on a treadmill mounted on top of a 6 degree-of-freedom motion base. (B) Examples of the degrees of freedom the motion base can move.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212207.g001
Kinematic data were collected at 120 Hz from a 14-camera infrared motion capture system (NaturalPoint Inc., Corvallis, OR, USA). Subjects wore reflective markers attached to anatomical landmarks of the anterior superior iliac spine bilaterally, sacrum, greater trochanters, medial and lateral knee joints, medial and lateral ankle malleoli, and first and fifth metatarsal heads bilaterally. The locations of the anatomical landmarks were found by manual palpation. Marker clusters, consisting of 3 markers attached to plastic plates, were strapped around the thighs and shanks, and taped to the heels of the shoes.
Experimental protocol
Familiarization.
Prior to beginning the experiment, subjects were familiarized with the equipment. We incrementally increased belt speed until the subject self-reported a comfortable walking speed (mean 0.78 ±0.19 m/s). This speed was used for the experimental trials. Subjects then completed a control walking trial, in which they walked at their comfortable pace for 80 seconds when the motion base was stationary. This walking trial served as our control condition to which experimental trials were compared.
Experimental trials.
The protocol consisted of thirteen experimental conditions. For twelve conditions, we imposed continuous sinusoidal oscillations (0.12 Hz) to the treadmill in one degree-of-freedom (Fig 1B): Pitch, Roll, ML and AP. We tested a low, medium and high amplitude for each degree-of-freedom: ±5, ±10, and ±15 degrees for Pitch and Roll oscillations, and ±8, ±16.5 and ±25 cm for ML and AP oscillations. The motion base velocity and acceleration are provided in Table 1. Briefly, the same velocity and acceleration were applied during the ML and AP translation trials, and the same rotational velocity and acceleration were applied to the Roll and Pitch trials. Slower accelerations/velocities were selected in order to create challenging, yet achievable walking conditions. The thirteenth condition was a combination of Roll, Pitch and Yaw (RPY) oscillations with offset frequencies (0.15, 0.16, 0.17 Hz, respectively) at an amplitude of ±8 degrees. Subjects walked for 80 seconds per trial and were given 60 seconds rest between trials. Experimental conditions were presented in random order. All subjects were able to walk during the experimental trials without falling and without holding onto handrails.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. Velocity and acceleration of the motion base during the single degree-of-freedom sinusoidal oscillation trials.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212207.t001
Data analysis
Kinematic and spatiotemporal parameters.
Kinematic data were low-pass filtered using a 4th order Butterworth filter with a cut-off frequency of 6 Hz using Visual 3D (C-Motion Inc., Rockville, MD, USA). We used a local coordinate system to define model segments for the pelvis, thighs, shanks and feet. Kinematic data were then used to automatically approximate heel strikes, which were defined as times when the distance between the pelvis and heel markers was maximum [18], and the events were verified by visual inspection. Step width was calculated as ML distance between heel markers at moment of heel-contact. We approximated step length as the AP distance between heel markers at moment of heel-contact. Both step width and length were normalized to the subject’s leg length. During Roll, Pitch and RPY trials, step width and length were calculated using local coordinates with respect to the motion base (i.e. the angle of the base was taken into account). Step frequency was calculated as the average of inverse step duration for each step. These analyses were performed for the final 60 seconds of each trial, and were conducted in Matlab (The Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA).
Dynamic stability.
We calculated the ML margin of stability (MoSML) to quantify dynamic stability in the ML direction using previous methods [11, 13]. The MoSML is the minimum difference between the extrapolated center of mass (XCoM) and BoS (MoSML = BoS – XCoMML) for each step. The lateral malleolus marker defined the lateral boundary of the BoS. The XCoM was calculated using:where CoM and are position and velocity of CoM, respectively, g is acceleration due to gravity, and l is maximum height of the estimated CoM, which was calculated as 1.34 times trochanteric height. The CoM position was estimated using the pelvic segment [19]. During experimental trials, we corrected the MoS by subtracting the velocity factor of treadmill movement, from XCoM, where was either ML or AP linear or tangential velocity of the treadmill [20]. This maintained consistency when comparing the MoS among trials. We calculated the mean and variability (coefficient of variation (CoV)) of the MoSML during the final 60 seconds of each trial. Increasing MoSML indicates the XCoM is within the BoS increasing dynamic stability. Decreasing or negative MoSML indicates the CoM is nearing or outside of the BoS. The MoS variability was used as an indicator of fall risk–increasing MoS variability suggests increased risk of falling [21].
Statistics
A one-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted to compare the effect of walking surface oscillation on step frequency, step width, and step length and mean MoSML and CoV of MoSML. The Greenhouse-Geisser correction factor was used when Mauchly’s test of sphericity was violated. When a significant effect of walking surface oscillation was found, simple contrasts were conducted to test whether the dynamic walking condition differed from the control walking condition. We also conducted a multi-factor repeated measures ANOVA to compare oscillation Type (Rotation, Translation), Direction (AP, ML) and Amplitude (low, medium, high) on the dependent variables. For this analysis, we excluded the Control and RPY conditions. All statistics were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 21.0 (IBM, New York, NY, USA). The α level was set at 0.05 and a Bonferroni correction (α = 0.0038) was applied for post-hoc multiple comparisons. Data are reported as means (SD). Only significant main effects and contrasts are presented, unless otherwise noted.
Results
Subjects made significant changes to foot placement during walking surface oscillations (Fig 2). We observed a main effect of walking surface oscillation for step frequency (F13,182 = 8.66, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.382), step width (F13,182 = 16.24, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.537), and step length (F13,182 = 32.46, p = 0.008, ηp2 = 0.699). Subjects took quicker steps during RPY (p = 0.017), Pitch (low: p = 0.001; medium: p < 0.001; high: p = 0.013), Roll (medium: p = 0.002; high: p = 0.001), and ML oscillations (low: p = 0.001; medium: p < 0.001. respectively) compared to control walking. Subjects also increased step width for RPY (p < 0.001), Roll (medium: p < 0.001; high: p = 0.001), and ML oscillations (medium: p = 0.003). For all oscillating conditions, subjects walked with significantly shorter steps compared with the control condition (p < 0.001 for all comparisons, except: medium Roll, p = 0.007 and low AP, p = 0.007).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 2. Step parameters.
Step frequency (A), step width (B) and step length (C) for all walking conditions are shown for following conditions: control walking condition (C), roll, pitch, yaw combination (RPY), pitch (PL, PM, PH), roll (RL, RM, RH), mediolateral (MLL, MLM, MLH) and anteroposterior (APL, APM, APH) oscillations at low (L), medium (M) and high (H) amplitudes. Asterisks represent significant difference (p < 0.05) with respect to the control condition.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212207.g002
Subjects walked with larger MoSML during the oscillations than the control condition (F13,182 = 15.2, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.521; Fig 3A). Simple contrasts revealed that MoSML was greater for RPY, high Pitch, Roll and ML oscillations compared with the control condition (p < 0.001, p = 0.001, p = 0.001, p < 0.001, respectively). Subjects also exhibited greater MoSML variability during walking surface oscillations (Fig 3B). We found a main effect of walking surface oscillation on MoSML (F13,128 = 4.62, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.213) variability. Subjects walked with greater MoSML variability during RPY (p < 0.001), high Roll (p < 0.001), low ML (p < 0.001) and medium (p = 0.004) and high (p = 0.009) AP oscillations.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Fig 3. Dynamic stability.
The mean MoSML (A) and the coefficient of variation of MoSML (B) are shown for following conditions: control walking condition (C), roll, pitch, yaw combination (RPY), pitch (PL, PM, PH), roll (RL, RM, RH), mediolateral (MLL, MLM, MLH) and anteroposterior (APL, APM, APH) oscillations at low (L), medium (M) and high (H) amplitudes. Asterisks represent significant difference (p < 0.05) with respect to the control condition.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212207.g003
We also compared Type (Rotation vs. Translation), Direction (ML vs. AP) and Amplitude (low, medium and high) on stepping strategies and dynamic stability. The statistical results are reported in Table 2. We observed a main effect of Type and Amplitude for step width, length and frequency (p < 0.05). Subjects walked with wider, shorter and quicker steps during rotating oscillations (Pitch and Roll) compared to translating oscillations (ML and AP). These parameters also scaled with the oscillation amplitude. High amplitude oscillations resulted in the widest, shortest and quickest steps, and vice versa. We only observed a main effect of Direction on the step width. Subjects took wider steps during the ML directions (Roll and ML) compared to the AP directions (Pitch and AP). The interaction of Type x Direction and Type x Amplitude were significant for step length and frequency only. Post-hoc comparisons for Type x Direction interaction showed that subjects walked with shorter and quicker steps for the rotation oscillations compared to the translation oscillations, particularly for Pitch. Similarly, post-hoc comparisons for Type x Amplitude interaction showed subjects walked with shorter and quicker steps during rotation oscillations compared to translation oscillations at each amplitude (p < 0.05 for each comparison).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 2. Statistical results.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212207.t002
For dynamic stability, we observed significant main effects of Type and Amplitude on the MoSML. Subjects walked with greater MoSML during rotation oscillations (Pitch and Roll) compared to translation oscillations. The MoSML also scaled with the oscillation amplitude–the MoSML increased with increasing oscillation amplitude. We observed significant interaction effects between Type and Direction on MoS. Post-hoc comparisons showed a larger MoSML during rotation in the AP and ML-directions compared to translation in AP or ML (p < 0.05 for both comparisons). Lastly, we also observed a significant interaction between Type and Amplitude on the MoS. Post-hoc comparisons showed a larger MoSML during rotation oscillations at each amplitude compared to the translation oscillations (p < 0.05 for all comparisons).
Discussion
The aim of this study was to determine how continuous oscillations of the walking surface at differing amplitudes, types and directions affects stepping strategies and dynamic balance in healthy young adults. Compared to level walking, healthy adults took wider steps particularly for trials in the ML direction (Roll and ML translation). The oscillation amplitude and type (rotation trials Roll and Pitch) applied to the walking surface resulted in an increase in step frequency and decrease in step lengths, with rotations applied in the AP direction (Pitch) resulting in the greatest change in step length and frequency. Dynamic stability was also dependent on the type of movement. Rotation oscillations caused the largest increase in ML MoS compared to the translation oscillations.
As reported in similar studies, the observed stepping adjustments in response to different walking surface oscillations suggest that this is a natural strategy to increase dynamic stability, and potentially reduce loss of balance [10, 12, 13, 15, 17, 22]. From the current study, the increase in ML MoS and its relationship to loss of balance may be interpreted in several ways. First, an increased dynamic stability during the oscillating conditions suggests the participants were more stable, and thus, less likely to fall, especially during the more challenging walking conditions (i.e. RPY, RollH). Participants walked with wider steps to increase the distance between the XCoM and BoS thereby increasing ML MoS compared to the control walking condition. A second interpretation is that young healthy adults walked with wider steps to increase ML MoS, but their risk for falls may still be the same as the control walking condition. In this latter scenario, the walking environment itself imposes a challenge to balance and healthy adults naturally will adjust their stepping to maintain dynamic stability.
Another potential interpretation may be that subjects favored control of frontal plane balance over sagittal plane balance. Shortening step length can also be regarded as a strategy to decrease risk of falling when walking in unstable conditions [23]. For example, shortening step length will move the CoM near or even in front of the BoS (i.e. in front of the leading foot), which will reduce a chance of backwards loss of balance but at the cost of potentially inducing a forward loss of balance [12, 24, 25]. In the current study, we observed decreased step length, with the shortest step length occurring in the Rotation conditions at high amplitude. Together with taking wider steps, taking shorter steps could imply that subjects are still at risk for loss of balance in the sagittal plane in order to favor better frontal plane control. However, we cannot verify this from the current data because the XCoM model for AP stability does not consider angular momentum, which can influence AP stability, misrepresenting true stability in the sagittal plane during up/down hill walking [26]. Furthermore, the changes in knee and hip angles during the Pitch trials affect joint power and stiffness, which violates the assumption that the inverted pendulum model is a passive system [27]. Further work is needed to define the relationship between ML MoS, AP MoS and fall risk when walking on challenging environments.
One major finding is that subjects made greater adjustments to foot placement during rotating oscillations. We observed increased step width during the Roll oscillations, and decreased step length and increased step frequency during the Pitch oscillations. Furthermore, these stepping adjustments were more pronounced during the rotating oscillations compared to the translation oscillations for each oscillation amplitude (i.e. low, medium and high). Sloped walking requires a person to alter their ground reaction forces, and therefore joint moments, to maintain or increase stability. For example, during side-sloped walking, subjects will decrease hip joint moments but increase moment about the knee and ankle to keep the CoM stable [28]. While our results are similar to previous studies investigating sloped walking [29–31], it is important to note that we cannot make direct comparisons of the oscillation amplitudes between rotating and translating conditions– 15 degrees of rotation may not have the same influence on gait parameters and dynamic stability as 9.8 cm of translation. However, from observation, subjects largely walked in the center of the treadmill, near the rotational axes. This implies this area would have the lowest rotational velocity and acceleration imposed to the person from the motion base.
Walking under continuous sinusoidal oscillations of the walking surface increased MoSML variability. The observed increase in MoSML variability may indicate that imposing sinusoidal oscillations of the walking surface in different directions increases the risk for falling for healthy young adults [21]. While a greater MoS when walking can be considered an indicator of dynamic stability [11], MoS variability may be related to how well people control CoM and foot placement when stability is challenged [16, 32]. In our study, subjects maintained sufficient MoSML when walking during the oscillations, even though they exhibited greater MoSML variability. This could suggest that they were able to quickly adjust their gait and keep their balance while walking in a destabilizing environment. Conversely, sinusoidal oscillations of the walking surface could have increased MoS variability while participants remained dynamically stable. For example, walking on a laterally oscillating treadmill, healthy adults were shown to alter their stepping at particular motions of the walking surface resulting in increased step width variability [21].
Methodological considerations
Sinusoidal oscillations created a predictable walking surface that could encourage subjects to time their stepping with the oscillations (i.e. to entrain). Two factors, however, reduce the likelihood of entrainment. First, the average step frequency (1.6 Hz for the control walking condition) was much faster than the imposed sinusoidal frequency (0.12 Hz). Previous studies have shown that dynamic entrainment largely occurs if the period of the perturbation is close to the individual’s preferred cadence [33–35]. Second, rhythmic cueing reduces variability of stepping [36, 37] and we observed increasing levels of stepping variability during the destabilizing conditions.
Walking on elevated surfaces can increase postural threat and raise concern for falling leading to altered gait [38–41]. Walking on elevated surfaces increases the time spent in double support phase, reduces step length, velocity and cadence [38–40]. In our study, subjects walked ~100 cm above the ground. Walking at this height could have increased their fear, causing subjects to walk with a “cautious” gait pattern, and may explain why we observed a slower than normal walking speed for healthy young adults. Despite this, our subjects continued to exhibit changes in stepping similar to previous studies [10, 12, 13].
Clinical implications
These results enhance our knowledge on how healthy individuals modify their gait in challenging walking conditions and are useful for future studies investigating dynamic balance in balance-impaired individuals. Among clinical populations, walking over uneven terrain increases fall risk and limits mobility [8, 9, 16]. Quantifying dynamic balance of high-fall risk individuals in challenging walking conditions can tell us what factors are important in managing fall risk and how to address rehabilitation concerns. Findings from our study can be used to compare how balance-impaired individuals compensate for the same walking conditions or whether increasing the challenge imposed to balance affects balance-impaired individuals similarly.
Conclusion
This study demonstrates that healthy young adults adjust their gait pattern to increase dynamic stability when walking over destabilizing surfaces. Subjects took quicker, shorter and wider steps to increase dynamic stability when oscillations were imposed to the walking surface and were largely influenced by rotations and amplitude of oscillation. These results suggest that healthy young adults use a generalized gait strategy to maintain dynamic stability when their balance is challenged.
Supporting information
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
S1 Data.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212207.s001
(XLSX)
Citation: Onushko T, Boerger T, Van Dehy J, Schmit BD (2019) Dynamic stability and stepping strategies of young healthy adults walking on an oscillating treadmill. PLoS ONE 14(2): e0212207. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212207
1. Winter DA, Patla AE, Frank JS, Walt SE. Biomechanical walking pattern changes in the fit and healthy elderly. Phys Ther. 1990;70(6):340–7. Epub 1990/06/01. pmid:2345777.
2. Menz HB, Lord SR, Fitzpatrick RC. Age-related differences in walking stability. Age Ageing. 2003;32(2):137–42. Epub 2003/03/05. pmid:12615555.
3. Terrier P, Reynard F. Effect of age on the variability and stability of gait: a cross-sectional treadmill study in healthy individuals between 20 and 69 years of age. Gait Posture. 2015;41(1):170–4. Epub 2014/12/03. pmid:25455699.
4. Larocca NG. Impact of walking impairment in multiple sclerosis: perspectives of patients and care partners. Patient. 2011;4(3):189–201. pmid:21766914.
5. Oates AR, Frank JS, Patla AE. Control of dynamic stability during adaptation to gait termination on a slippery surface. Exp Brain Res. 2010;201(1):47–57. Epub 2009/10/17. pmid:19834697.
6. Sheehan RC, Gottschall JS. Older Adult Hill Transition Strategies are Determined by Task Demands and Cautious Gait Patterns. J Appl Biomech. 2015;31(3):133–41. Epub 2014/12/03. pmid:25449048.
7. Miller WC, Speechley M, Deathe B. The prevalence and risk factors of falling and fear of falling among lower extremity amputees. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2001;82(8):1031–7. pmid:11494181.
8. Richardson JK, Thies S, Ashton-Miller JA. An exploration of step time variability on smooth and irregular surfaces in older persons with neuropathy. Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon). 2008;23(3):349–56. pmid:18035458; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2277078.
9. Zurales K, DeMott TK, Kim H, Allet L, Ashton-Miller JA, Richardson JK. Gait Efficiency on an Uneven Surface Is Associated with Falls and Injury in Older Subjects with a Spectrum of Lower Limb Neuromuscular Function: A Prospective Study. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. 2016;95(2):83–90. pmid:26053187; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4670600.
10. McAndrew PM, Dingwell JB, Wilken JM. Walking variability during continuous pseudo-random oscillations of the support surface and visual field. J Biomech. 2010;43(8):1470–5. pmid:20346453; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2866814.
11. Hof AL, Gazendam MG, Sinke WE. The condition for dynamic stability. J Biomech. 2005;38(1):1–8. pmid:15519333.
12. Hak L, Houdijk H, Steenbrink F, Mert A, van der Wurff P, Beek PJ, et al. Speeding up or slowing down?: Gait adaptations to preserve gait stability in response to balance perturbations. Gait Posture. 2012;36(2):260–4. pmid:22464635.
13. Hak L, Houdijk H, Steenbrink F, Mert A, van der Wurff P, Beek PJ, et al. Stepping strategies for regulating gait adaptability and stability. J Biomech. 2013;46(5):905–11. pmid:23332822.
14. Bauby CE, Kuo AD. Active control of lateral balance in human walking. J Biomech. 2000;33(11):1433–40. pmid:10940402.
15. Hak L, Houdijk H, van der Wurff P, Prins MR, Mert A, Beek PJ, et al. Stepping strategies used by post-stroke individuals to maintain margins of stability during walking. Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon). 2013;28(9–10):1041–8. pmid:24200373.
16. Gates DH, Scott SJ, Wilken JM, Dingwell JB. Frontal plane dynamic margins of stability in individuals with and without transtibial amputation walking on a loose rock surface. Gait Posture. 2013;38(4):570–5. pmid:23481866; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3720773.
17. Madehkhaksar F, Klenk J, Sczuka K, Gordt K, Melzer I, Schwenk M. The effects of unexpected mechanical perturbations during treadmill walking on spatiotemporal gait parameters, and the dynamic stability measures by which to quantify postural response. PLoS One. 2018;13(4):e0195902. Epub 2018/04/20. pmid:29672558; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC5908091.
18. Zeni JA Jr., Richards JG, Higginson JS. Two simple methods for determining gait events during treadmill and overground walking using kinematic data. Gait Posture. 2008;27(4):710–4. Epub 2007/08/29. pmid:17723303; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2384115.
19. Winter DA. Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement. 3rd ed. New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 1990.
20. Beltran EJ, Dingwell JB, Wilken JM. Margins of stability in young adults with traumatic transtibial amputation walking in destabilizing environments. J Biomech. 2014;47(5):1138–43. Epub 2014/01/22. pmid:24444777; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4050449.
21. Su JL, Dingwell JB. Dynamic stability of passive dynamic walking on an irregular surface. J Biomech Eng. 2007;129(6):802–10. Epub 2007/12/11. pmid:18067383.
22. Brady RA, Peters BT, Bloomberg JJ. Strategies of healthy adults walking on a laterally oscillating treadmill. Gait Posture. 2009;29(4):645–9. pmid:19233653.
23. Pai YC, Patton J. Center of mass velocity-position predictions for balance control. J Biomech. 1997;30(4):347–54. pmid:9075002.
24. McAndrew Young PM, Dingwell JB. Voluntary changes in step width and step length during human walking affect dynamic margins of stability. Gait Posture. 2012;36(2):219–24. Epub 2012/04/05. pmid:22472707; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3387299.
25. Bierbaum S, Peper A, Karamanidis K, Arampatzis A. Adaptational responses in dynamic stability during disturbed walking in the elderly. J Biomech. 2010;43(12):2362–8. Epub 2010/05/18. pmid:20472240.
26. Pickle NT, Wilken JM, Fey NP, Silverman AK. A comparison of stability metrics based on inverted pendulum models for assessment of ramp walking. PLoS One. 2018;13(11):e0206875. Epub 2018/11/06. pmid:30395597; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC6218075.
27. Winter DA. Human balance and posture control during standing and walking. Gait & Posture. 1995;3:193–214.
28. Wade C, Breloff SP, Redfern MS, Andres BO. Joint kinetics while walking on an irregular slopped surface. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. 2014:1658–61.
29. Kawamura K, Tokuhiro A, Takechi H. Gait Analysis of Slope Walking—a Study on Step Length, Stride Width, Time Factors and Deviation in the Center of Pressure. Acta Med Okayama. 1991;45(3):179–84. WOS:A1991FV15000008. pmid:1891977
30. Leroux A, Fung J, Barbeau H. Postural adaptation to walking on inclined surfaces: I. Normal strategies. Gait Posture. 2002;15(1):64–74. Epub 2002/01/26. pmid:11809582.
31. Lay AN, Hass CJ, Gregor RJ. The effects of sloped surfaces on locomotion: a kinematic and kinetic analysis. J Biomech. 2006;39(9):1621–8. pmid:15990102.
32. McAndrew Young PM, Wilken JM, Dingwell JB. Dynamic margins of stability during human walking in destabilizing environments. J Biomech. 2012;45(6):1053–9. pmid:22326059; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3321251.
33. Nessler JA, Heredia S, Belair J, Milton J. Walking on a Vertically Oscillating Treadmill: Phase Synchronization and Gait Kinematics. PLoS One. 2017;12(1):e0169924. Epub 2017/01/19. pmid:28099517; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC5242471.
34. Ahn J, Hogan BL. Walking is not like reaching: evidence from periodic mechanical perturbations. PLoS One. 2012;3:e31767.
35. Ahn J, Hogan N. A simple state-determined model reproduces entrainment and phase-locking of human walking. PLoS One. 2012;7(11):e47963. Epub 2012/11/16. pmid:23152761; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3495952.
36. van Wegen E, de Goede C, Lim I, Rietberg M, Nieuwboer A, Willems A, et al. The effect of rhythmic somatosensory cueing on gait in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Sci. 2006;248(1–2):210–4. Epub 2006/06/20. pmid:16780887.
37. Wright RL, Bevins JW, Pratt D, Sackley CM, Wing AM. Metronome Cueing of Walking Reduces Gait Variability after a Cerebellar Stroke. Front Neurol. 2016;7:84. Epub 2016/06/18. pmid:27313563; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4887482.
38. Brown LA, Gage WH, Polych MA, Sleik RJ, Winder TR. Central set influences on gait. Age-dependent effects of postural threat. Exp Brain Res. 2002;145(3):286–96. Epub 2002/07/24. pmid:12136378.
39. Caetano MJ, Gobbi LT, Sanchez-Arias Mdel R, Stella F, Gobbi S. Effects of postural threat on walking features of Parkinson's disease patients. Neurosci Lett. 2009;452(2):136–40. Epub 2009/04/23. pmid:19383427.
40. Delbaere K, Sturnieks DL, Crombez G, Lord SR. Concern about falls elicits changes in gait parameters in conditions of postural threat in older people. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64(2):237–42. Epub 2009/02/07. pmid:19196645; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2655012.
41. Davis JR, Campbell AD, Adkin AL, Carpenter MG. The relationship between fear of falling and human postural control. Gait Posture. 2009;29(2):275–9. Epub 2008/10/31. pmid:18963992.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2019 Onushko et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Understanding how people modify their stepping to maintain gait stability may provide information on fall risk and help to understand strategies used to reduce loss of balance. The purpose of this study was to identify the stepping strategies healthy young individuals select to maintain balance while walking on a destabilizing surface in various directions. A treadmill mounted on top of a 6 degree-of-freedom motion base was used to generate support surface oscillations in different degrees of freedom and amplitudes. Fifteen healthy young adults (21.3 ± 1.4 years) walked at self-selected speeds while continuous sinusoidal oscillations were imposed to the support surface in a one degree of freedom: rotation or translation in the mediolateral (ML) direction and rotation or translation in the anteroposterior (AP) direction, with each condition repeated at three different amplitudes. We compared step width, length, and frequency and the mean and variability of margin of stability (MoS) during each experimental walking condition with a control condition, in which the support surface was stationary. Subjects chose a common strategy of increasing step width (p < 0.001) and decreasing step length (p = 0.008) while increasing mediolateral MoS (p < 0.001), particularly during oscillations that challenged frontal plane control, with rotations of the walking surface producing the greatest changes to stepping.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer