Impact Summary
Emerging infections can have devastating impacts on their hosts, yet how hosts naturally evolve to deal with novels infections remains poorly understood. In particular, it remains unclear whether hosts evolve to become more resistant by mounting an immune response that will clear the infection, or whether they evolve to become more tolerant by lessening the symptoms of the infection. Understanding how hosts evolve to respond to infection is made all the more challenging by the fact that, for a given disease, there is natural variation in the level of aggression (i.e., virulence) of the infectious agents in circulation. Because more virulent pathogen variants are likely to give rise to stronger host responses, variation in virulence will impact our measurements of host responses to infection. As a result, if we want to understand how hosts evolve to deal with novel infections, we need to measure these responses against a range of pathogen variants that differ in their level of virulence. In this article, we take advantage of a naturally emerging infection in a wild North American songbird (house finches) to test whether hosts evolve resistance or tolerance. We experimentally infect house finches with 55 pathogen variants of differing virulence, and compare the response of finches from disease‐exposed populations that are known to have evolved in response to infection with that of finches from disease‐unexposed populations, which remain susceptible to the infection. We show that, relative to finches from disease‐unexposed populations, finches from disease‐exposed populations have evolved to be able to clear the infection through an immune response, and to limit the damages due to the infection. Thus, resistance and tolerance should be seen as complementary, rather than opposing, defense strategies.
Hosts can alleviate the costs of infection by evolving two distinct—although not necessarily mutually exclusive—strategies (Kover and Schaal ; Schneider and Ayres ). They can evolve resistance, which serves to reduce the establishment of infectious pathogens and/or to clear pathogens following establishment (Boots and Bowers ; Janeway ), and they can evolve tolerance. The latter serves to mitigate somatic damage caused by the infection without reducing pathogen load (Råberg et al. ; Medzhitov et al. ; Råberg ). Whether and when hosts evolve resistance, tolerance, or both in response to emerging pathogens have far‐reaching consequences for predicting virulence evolution and epidemiological dynamics (Miller et al. ), as well as for the design of novel pharmacological treatments (Vilaplana et al. ; Soares et al. ). Despite this, few experiments have been performed to investigate the relative importance of resistance versus tolerance in evolved responses to naturally emerging pathogens.
Hypotheses based on the evolution of resistance versus tolerance make contrasting predictions. First, if resistance has evolved, hosts will be better at clearing the infection and so show reduced pathogen load during an infection relative to nonevolved hosts (Miller et al. , ; Råberg et al. ). Second, if tolerance has evolved, hosts will be better able to mitigate the impact of an increasing pathogen load. Under the widely used “range tolerance” concept (Little et al. ), this would be detected as a shallower negative regression of fitness on pathogen load (Simms ). Alternatively, assuming that loss of fitness can be directly attributed to clinical symptom severity, we would predict a weaker positive regression of symptoms severity (rather than fitness) on pathogen load (Råberg et al. ; see Box ). Given these predictions, it is important to reiterate that damage‐limitation mechanisms could evolve in conjunction with resistance (e.g., by limiting immunity or initiating repair) (Restif and Koella ; Howick and Lazzaro ). Thus, resistance and tolerance mechanisms need not be mutually exclusive, and evidence for the evolution of one is not necessarily evidence against evolution of the other.
BOXDefinitions, predictions, and implications of key terms
Typically, it is assumed that resistance and tolerance represent mutually exclusive evolutionary responses to emerging pathogens (see “typical” predictions). However, there is no inherent reason why the two mechanisms cannot operate in tandem, which changes the predictions for each (see “generalized” predictions). Note that predictions may be stated in terms of “fitness” rather than proxies such as “clinical symptom severity.” The latter are more readily measured but carry implicit assumptions (e.g., that fitness declines with increasing symptom severity). The choice of the response variable also has implications for the distinction between point and range tolerance (see Fig. )
Definition | Predictions | Assumptions/Implications |
Resistance: Resistant hosts are better able to clear the infection than nonresistant hosts. (Note that we consider here resistance via immune activity) |
Typical: Resistant hosts display reduced pathogen load relative to nonresistant hosts, which results in reduced clinical symptoms but not in lower symptom severity for an equivalent pathogen load
Generalised: Resistant hosts display reduced pathogen load relative to nonresistant hosts, which results in reduced clinical symptoms |
Resistance and tolerance are mutually exclusive evolutionary strategies |
Resistance and tolerance can cooccur within the same evolutionary response | ||
Tolerance: Tolerant hosts are better able to mitigate the cost of infection than nontolerant hosts. | Typical: Tolerant hosts display reduced symptom severity relative to nontolerant hosts, but carry equivalent (i.e., not significantly different) pathogen load | Resistance and tolerance are mutually exclusive evolutionary strategies |
Generalised: Tolerant hosts display reduced symptom severity conditional on pathogen load (which may/may not be equivalent to nontolerant hosts). | Tolerance and resistance can cooccur within the same evolutionary response | |
Range tolerance: Tolerant hosts display reduced rate of symptom worsening as pathogen load increases than nontolerant hosts. | The slope of the regression between clinical symptoms and pathogen load is reduced for tolerant hosts relative to nontolerant ones. Usually estimated from symptom severity observed across a range of nonzero pathogen loads. |
If the measure of clinical symptom severity is assumed to equal 0 in the absence of infection (i.e., pathogen load = 0), range and point approaches to characterizing tolerance are equivalent (Fig. ). If this condition cannot be assumed biologically (e.g., because symptoms are nonspecific) and/or is not imposed analytically, empirical conclusions about tolerance may appear to differ depending on whether a slope or point approach is taken (see Fig. legend). |
Point tolerance: Tolerant hosts display lower clinical symptoms severity for a given pathogen load. | Tolerant hosts display reduced clinical symptoms for a given pathogen load relative to non‐tolerant hosts. Usually estimated from a symptom severity at a single nonzero pathogen load. | Note that if fitness rather than symptom severity is used, point tolerance approaches carry an implicit assumption of equal y‐intercepts (i.e., fitness in the absence of infection) that is unlikely to hold true. Violation of this assumption will bias conclusions about tolerance. |
Fig. B1. Schematic figure of the regression of symptoms severity on pathogen load for two host genotypes. (A) Host genotypes differing in tolerance, with the gold genotype being more tolerant to infection (i.e., symptoms severity increases more slowly with increasing pathogen load). If we assume that symptoms severity is equal to 0 in the absence of infection (i.e., pathogen load = 0), then any evidence of point tolerance (dots) will be equivalent to a difference in range tolerance (i.e., slopes). (B) and (C) For point tolerance to be distinct from range tolerance, regression slopes of symptoms on pathogen load need to have intercepts that can differ from 0. Hypothetically, host genotypes could then differ either (B) in range tolerance (i.e., slopes), but not in point tolerance (gray dot), or (C) in point tolerance (dots), but not in range tolerance. It is difficult to imagine that infection‐specific symptom severity is >0 in the absence of infection. Nonspecific indicator of health, on the other hand, could vary among uninfected individuals (as of course could fitness). Nevertheless, if the severity of specific symptoms is used, we suggest that evidence of point but not range tolerance (or vice versa) is an artefact of the sampling regime.
There have been few experimental tests of the predictions for the evolution of resistance and tolerance in response to naturally emerging pathogens, and the handful of studies to date have yielded rather mixed conclusions. For example, strong evidence for the evolution of resistance comes from observations of the epidemic of myxoma virus in European rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) in Australia (Kerr and Best ; Kerr et al. ). Following initially dramatic population declines, at seven years post‐outbreak, rabbits from disease‐exposed populations displayed mortality rates of only ∼25% in response to experimental infection. In contrast, mortality rates of over 88% were found in unexposed wild and domestic rabbits (Marshall and Fenner ; Marshall and Douglas ). Subsequent work confirmed that reduced mortality was mediated by the evolution of innate and cellular immune responses leading to significantly reduced pathogen loads (Best and Kerr ). By contrast, the endemic Hawaiian bird, Hawai‘i ‘Amakihi (Chlorodrepanis virens), was suggested to have evolved tolerance to Plasmodium relictum, following the pathogen's introduction to the archipelago around the 1930s (Van Riper et al. ). Experimentally infected ‘Amakihi from high‐altitude sites (where lower temperatures limit mosquito numbers and malaria parasite development; Van Riper et al. ; LaPointe et al. ), displayed significantly higher mortality and weight loss than did individuals from low‐altitude sites. Crucially, however, there was no significant difference in pathogen load (Atkinson et al. ).
Here, we test the role of resistance versus tolerance in evolutionary responses of North American house finches (Haemorhous mexicanus) to the emerging, conjunctivitis‐causing bacterium Mycoplasma gallisepticum, following its jump from poultry in 1994 (Dhondt et al. ; Nolan et al. ). Several previous experiments on this system have yielded apparently contradictory conclusions. Bonneaud et al. () concluded that resistance had evolved from standing genetic variation within 12 years of outbreak: finches from disease‐exposed populations displayed reduced pathogen load following infection with a virulent, contemporary 2007 isolate. By contrast, Adelman et al. () concluded that tolerance had evolved: finches from disease‐exposed populations in 2010 had similar pathogen load, but reduced symptoms (conjunctival swelling) relative to finches from unexposed populations and following inoculation with a low‐virulence bacterial isolate collected at epidemic outbreak (i.e., in 1994). Further experiments are clearly needed to understand the relative roles of resistance and tolerance in the response to this emerging pathogen. In addition, because previous support for tolerance evolution arises in part from a lack of significant differences in pathogen load (as would be predicted under resistance evolution; Råberg et al. ), here we use a greater number of host individuals and of pathogen isolates of varying levels of virulence to reduce the possibility of type II error.
We conducted a large‐scale infection experiment using 112 naïve house finches from disease‐exposed (N = 53) and unexposed (N = 59) populations, and 55 bacterial isolates collected from the epidemic outbreak (1994) and during the subsequent 20 years (until 2015). After emergence near Washington D.C. in 1994, M. gallisepticum spread throughout the entire eastern U.S. range of house finches within three years, killing millions of birds (Fischer et al. ; Dhondt et al. ). Although it later spread through much of the native western range (between 2000 and 2010; Duckworth et al. ; Dhondt et al. ), some populations remain unexposed to date (e.g., in Arizona; Staley et al. ). We have shown previously that the virulence of M. gallisepticum, defined as the amount of damage done to the house finch host, has increased over the course of the epidemic (Bonneaud et al. ; Tardy et al. ). Furthermore, we have shown that house finches from exposed populations display less severe symptoms than those from unexposed populations (Bonneaud et al. ). In this study, we test the key contrasting predictions set out above to determine whether this host evolutionary response is principally attributable to changes in resistance or tolerance.
First, if finches from exposed populations have evolved resistance, we would expect them to display lower pathogen loads during infection than birds from unexposed populations (i.e., populations that have not evolved resistance). Specifically, because resistance to M. gallisepticum is thought to be mediated through the ability to mount a cell‐mediated immune response (Bonneaud et al. ), given evolved resistance, finches from exposed populations are expected to show reduced pathogen load from approximately two weeks postinfection (i.e., the time required to mount a pathogen‐specific immune response). By contrast, if tolerance alone has evolved, we predict no differences in pathogen load over the course of the month‐long infection experiment between finches from the two populations. Further, we would expect that the relationship between symptom severity and pathogen load will be shallower in birds from exposed populations (range tolerance; Råberg et al. ), although reduced symptoms for a given pathogen load have also been given as evidence of (point) tolerance (Graham et al. ) (see Box ).
Methods
CAPTURE AND HOUSING
Wild hatch‐year house finches from populations that have never been exposed to M. gallisepticum (unexposed populations) were captured in urban areas and in suburban parks (see Bonneaud et al. ) in Arizona over a two‐week period of the summer 2015. We trapped, weighed, and banded each bird with a numbered metal tag for individual identification (N = 171; 93 males and 78 females). They were then immediately transported by car to an aviary at Arizona State University, where they were housed for the remainder of the experiment. On arrival, we sampled blood from each bird by brachial venipuncture (60 µL of whole blood) and also took a choanal swab. A lack of prior infection was confirmed for each bird by screening blood plasma samples for anti‐M. gallisepticum antibodies using a serum plate agglutination assay (Luttrell et al. ). Absence of current infection was verified using the choanal swabs in PCR amplification of M. gallisepticum DNA (Roberts et al. ). No prior or current M. gallisepticum infections were detected (as expected given no documented reports of M. gallisepticum from this area of Arizona; Staley et al. ). They were then allowed to acclimate in the aviary for >1 month, with ad libitum food and water. During this time, although none of the birds displayed any sign of infection with other diseases, all were treated prophylactically for Trichomonas gallinae with carnidazole (Spartrix, Janssen/Elanco) and Isospora spp with sulfadimethoxine in the first 40 days of captivity.
During the same time period, we also caught hatch‐year house finches from populations known to have been exposed to M. gallisepticum since the disease outbreak (i.e., currently maximally 20 host generations; exposed populations). All eastern house finch populations were exposed to M. gallisepticum within three years of outbreak, meaning that there is little variation in exposure duration among them (Dhondt et al. ). These were captured from urban areas and suburban parks in Alabama (see Bonneaud et al. ). Birds were similarly banded weighed, and sampled for blood and choanal swabs (N = 131). They were then immediately transported by car to aviaries at Auburn University, where they were housed separately in the same conditions as in the aviaries in Arizona and tested for prior and current infection as described above. Birds positive for either test were released immediately and not used for the study. In this way, we ensured that individuals from exposed populations used in the study had not themselves been previously infected with the pathogen. These remaining individuals (N = 53; 24 males and 29 females) underwent >30‐day quarantine period with ad libitum food and water, and during which they were treated prophylactically for Trichomonas gallinae and Isospora infections (see above). They were then transported in an air‐conditioned vehicle to the aviary at Arizona State University. Care was used to minimize travel time (<30 h), movement, and stress to the birds; food and water was provided ad libitum throughout the trip and the birds were regularly checked for any signs of distress or injury.
Following arrival at Arizona State University, 112 birds (53 birds from the exposed populations and 59 birds from the unexposed populations) were haphazardly selected for use in the present study (the remaining 112 individuals being used in another experiment). They were then allowed to acclimate in the aviary, with ad libitum food and water, for >1 month prior to experimental inoculation.
EXPERIMENTAL INOCULATION
We haphazardly inoculated each of the birds with one of 55 M. galliseptum isolates sampled over the course of the epidemic. We elected to use a large number of isolates in this paired design so that differences between exposed and unexposed host populations can be interpreted as averaged across any isolate‐specific effects. This study is thus designed to draw maximally general inference on between‐host population differences in response to M. gallisepticum infection. It is not designed to fully characterize differences among pathogen isolates, an aim that would be better served by using fewer isolates replicated across multiple hosts per population. Isolates were originally obtained from naturally infected, wild‐caught house finches by swabbing the conjunctiva of a symptomatic bird and placing the swab in SP4 growth medium. Isolates were collected over a 20‐year period and obtained from various urban and suburban sites in eight different States in the eastern United States (mainly from Alabama; Bonneaud et al. ). Isolates were administered via 20 µL of culture containing 1 × 104 to 1 × 106 color changing units/mL of M. gallisepticum in both eyes. To quantify conjunctival swelling, we photographed the right and left eyes at 0, 6, 13, and 25 days postinoculation (dpi) from a standardized distance. We then measured the average area of the conjunctiva swelling across the two eyes and at each day as: the area of the outer ring minus the area of the inner ring at 6, 13, or 25 dpi—the area of the outer ring minus the area of the inner ring at 0 dpi (see Staley et al. ). Measurements were blind with respect to the isolate inoculated and the population of origin of the bird. The experiment was stopped at 35 dpi and all birds were euthanized. Protocols were approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees of Auburn University (protocol # PRN 2015–2721) and of Arizona State University (protocol #15‐1438R), and by Institutional Biological Use Authorizations to Auburn University (# BUA 500).
PATHOGEN LOAD
Bacterial load was measured by quantitative amplification of M. gallisepticum DNA from pooled conjunctival and tracheal swabs obtained at 8, 14, 21, and 28 dpi. DNA was extracted using a QIAGEN DNeasy® Blood and Tissue Kit according to the manufacturer's standard protocol (Qiagen, Germany). For each sample, we ran a multiplex quantitative PCR of the M. gallisepticum‐specific gene mgc2, which encodes a cytadhesin protein, and the house finch recombination‐activation gene rag1, using an Applied Biosystems™ StepOnePlus™ Real‐Time PCR system (Applied Biosystems, USA) (Tardy et al. ). Each reaction contained: 2 µL of sample genomic DNA template, 1 µL each of 10 µM mgc110‐F/R, and rag1‐102‐F/R primers (total 4 µL), 0.5 µL each of 10 µM Mgc110‐JOE and Rag1‐102‐6FAM fluorescent hydrolysis probes (total 1 µL), 10 µL of 2× qPCRBIO Probe Mix HI‐ROX (PCR BIOSYSTEMS) and 3 µL Nuclease‐free water (Ambion®, USA). Reactions were then run at 95°C for 3 min, followed by 45 cycles of 95°C for 1 s and 60°C for 20 s. Samples were run in duplicate alongside serial dilutions of plasmid‐based standards (range of standards for mgc2: 1.6 × 108 – 1.6 × 103 copies; range of standards for rag1: 8.0 × 107 – 8.0 × 102 copies). Amplification data were exported to LinRegPCR version 2017.1 for calculation of individual reaction efficiencies and quantification of low‐amplification samples (Ruijter et al. ; Tuomi et al. ); between run variation was normalized using Factor qPCR version 2016.0 (Ruijter et al. ), with plasmid standard serial dilutions used for factor correction.
STATISTICAL ANALYSES
All statistical analyses were conducted in R 3.3.2 (Team RC ) using linear mixed effect models fitted in lme4 (Bates et al. ), and figures were made using ggplot2 (Wickham ). We previously showed that the probability of developing conjunctivitis following experimental inoculation did not differ between birds from exposed versus unexposed populations, but the former subsequently displayed less severe symptoms (Bonneaud et al. ). To test the roles of resistance and tolerance in this host evolutionary response, we restricted our analyses only to individuals that became symptomatic (N = 83) and further removed 3 individuals that died during the course of the experiment due to incomplete data. We thus analyzed data from 80 symptomatic individuals (N = 34 birds from exposed populations each inoculated with a distinct isolate and 46 birds from unexposed populations inoculated with 1 of 45 isolates (one isolate from was inoculated into two birds from unexposed populations)). When fitted as response variables, pathogen load was transformed using natural logarithm, whereas peak conjunctival swelling was square root transformed to ensure normal residuals. In total, we ran three different models. First, we investigated the effects of year of pathogen sampling and whether finches were obtained from disease‐exposed or unexposed populations on peak pathogen loads (ln transformed) using a mixed linear model, with pathogen isolate fitted as a random intercept. Second, to test for evidence of population differences in the rate of pathogen clearance, we ran a mixed effects model with ln(pathogen load) as the response term, host population (exposed vs. unexposed), dpi (as a continuous covariate), and their interaction as explanatory terms. Bacterial isolate identity was included as a random intercept. Bird identity was included as an additional random term to account for repeated measures of loads over the course of the experiment. Third, we also used a mixed model to test for population differences in the association between pathogen load and clinical symptoms severity. Here peak conjunctival swelling (square root transformed) was the response term, with host population, peak pathogen load (square root transformed), and their interactions as fixed effects. Bacterial isolate identity was again included as a random intercept, but not individual bird identity (because each bird was only represented once). We note that square root transformations can stabilize variance and, in this case, that risks removing (or at least reducing) any signature of a population × pathogen interaction on symptom severity. Because this interaction is key to our hypothesis testing (i.e., we predict a steeper regression of symptom severity on pathogen load in unexposed populations if range tolerance has evolved), we reran this second analysis using un‐transformed conjunctival swelling data. As conclusions were unaltered, we elected not present that analysis here (but see results in Supplementary Results and Fig. S1). In addition, we tested whether evidence of point tolerance might be a manifestation of range tolerance by rerunning the third model, but forcing the intercept at 0, as would be expected under the assumption that with no pathogen there are no symptoms.
Results
PATHOGEN LOAD
Over the course of the experiment, the median peak bacterial load observed across all four measures of all individuals used was 78 bacteria per host cell. There was marked variation around this median (IQR of 42–154, total range of 1–522 bacteria per host cell), arising in part from effects of bacterial isolate identity. Specifically, the mixed model analysis revealed that isolate identity explained 19% of the variance in peak load. Further, as expected, year of pathogen sampling showed a substantial positive effect on peak pathogen load, with later isolates achieving higher peak load than early isolates (mixed GLM; linear estimate ± SE = 4.68 ± 1.15, χ2 = 4.99, df = 1, P = 0.025; quadratic estimate ± SE = –2.53 ± 1.15, χ2 = 4.97, df = 1, P = 0.026). Finally, however, peak loads were similar in birds from exposed and unexposed populations on average (population effect (unexposed relative to exposed) ± SE = 0.004 ± 0.26, χ2 = 0.0003, df = 1, P = 0.99). This latter result is not sufficient to distinguish the roles of resistance and tolerance in the evolutionary response to infection.
The absence of a population difference in peak load is not incompatible with evolved resistance if pathogen loads are peaking prior to the time when genetically resistant birds are able to mount an effective immune response. Indeed, we found here that bacterial loads were highest (on average) in birds from both populations at 8 dpi and thereafter declined significantly on average (mixed GLM; main dpi effect: estimate ± SE = –0.07 ± 0.009, χ2 = 54.7, df = 1, P < 0.0001). However, there was a significant population by dpi interaction (estimate ± SE = 0.07 ± 0.02, χ2 = 13.9, df = 1, P < 0.0002), with birds from exposed populations clearing the pathogen approximately three times faster than those from unexposed populations (Fig. ). Differential clearing rates are such that birds from exposed populations have a fourfold lower bacterial load than birds from unexposed populations by 28 dpi. These findings support the hypothesis that genetic resistance through acquired immunity has evolved in exposed populations.
Fig. 1. Changes in pathogen load over the experiment. We show pathogen load (log‐transformed) at 8, 14, 21, and 28 dpi for birds from exposed and unexposed populations. Raw values are shown as triangles (exposed) or circles (unexposed populations); lines are predicted from the model (solid = exposed; dashed = unexposed), with SEs represented by ribbons.
CONJUNCTIVAL SWELLING AS A FUNCTION OF PATHOGEN LOAD
The key symptom of M. gallisepticum infection in house finches is conjunctivitis, which, when severe, causes blindness and death in the wild through starvation or predation (Roberts et al. ; Kollias et al. ; Adelman et al. ). Using the area of conjunctival swelling to measure clinical symptom severity, we found no obvious support for the hypothesis that tolerance, as measured by the regression slope of peak symptom severity on peak pathogen load, has evolved. Across all individuals used, the mean measure of peak conjunctival swelling was 64.4 ± 34.0 pixels, and swelling increased with peak pathogen load as expected (mixed GLM; pathogen load main effect: estimate ± SE = 0.21 ± 0.052, χ2 = 15.30, df = 1, P < 0.0001). However, the slope of this regression did not differ between exposed versus unexposed populations (population × peak pathogen load interaction effect: estimate ± SE = = –0.073 ± 0.10, χ2 = 0.52, df = 1, P = 0.47) (Fig. ). Nonetheless, birds from exposed populations did have 24% lower clinical symptom severity for any given pathogen load, which is predicted under the point tolerance concept (population main effect: estimate ± SE = 1.30 ± 0.50, χ2 = 6.88, df = 1, P = 0.009). We reran this analysis using the integral of pathogen load rather than peak pathogen load as our predictor variable, but results were qualitatively unchanged (see Fig. S1). Thus, our results suggest that mechanisms to limit immune damage have evolved in tandem with resistance.
Fig. 2. Association between pathogen load and clinical symptom severity. We show peak conjunctival swelling (square root‐transformed; in pixels) as a function of peak pathogen load (square root‐transformed) for birds from exposed and unexposed populations. Raw values are shown as triangles (exposed) or circles (unexposed populations); lines are predicted from the model (solid = exposed; dashed = unexposed), with SEs represented by ribbons. Boxplot show the median and range peak conjunctival swelling and peak pathogen load for each population. Birds from exposed populations displayed significantly lower peak conjunctival swelling than those from unexposed populations (estimate ± SE = –1.13 ± 0.54, χ2 = 4.36, df = 1, P = 0.037), but equivalent peak pathogen load (see Results).
POINT VERSUS RANGE TOLERANCE
Although the evidence above is consistent with the concept of point rather than range tolerance, the apparent distinction between the two might be an artefact of whether asymptomatic hosts are sampled (see Box ). For example, because in our study all hosts were infected, the regression slopes of pathogen loads on conjunctival swelling had intercepts in excess of zero. However, it may be more reasonable to assume that with no pathogen, there are no symptoms, and as a consequence the regression needs to originate at 0 (see Fig. ). Where this reasonable assumption is made, any difference in points will derive from a difference in slope. To test this possibility, we reran the model presented above, but wherein we force the intercept to be 0. As expected, doing so generates a significant difference in the slopes, with finches from exposed populations showing reduced slope as expected under range tolerance (mixed GLM; population × pathogen load interaction effect: estimate ± SE = –0.15 ± 0.06, χ2 = 4.9, df = 1, P = 0.027: Fig. ). These results suggest that there is no distinction between point and range tolerance, and any apparent evidence of point tolerance is in fact evidence of range tolerance (Box and Fig. ).
Fig. 3. Association between pathogen load and clinical symptom severity with intercept forced at 0. For birds from exposed and unexposed populations, we show peak conjunctival swelling (square root‐transformed values) as a function of peak pathogen load (square root‐transformed). Raw values are shown as triangles (exposed) or circles (unexposed populations); lines are predicted from the model (solid = exposed; dashed = unexposed), with SEs represented by ribbons.
Discussion
To test whether house finches from disease‐exposed populations have evolved resistance or tolerance to infection to the emerging bacterial pathogen M. gallisepticum, we conducted an inoculation experiment of house finches from disease‐unexposed and exposed populations using isolates collected over a 20‐year period from epidemic outbreak and differing in virulence. We found that birds from exposed and unexposed populations had comparable peak pathogen loads, which were maximal at 8 dpi in birds from both populations. However, thereafter birds from previously exposed populations cleared the pathogen more rapidly and to a greater extent during our experiment. That bacterial loads only started to differ between exposed and unexposed finch populations after 14 dpi is consistent with our prior evidence that evolved finches clear M. gallisepticum through cell‐mediated immunity (Bonneaud et al. ). We interpret these patterns as evidence that, in the exposed populations, hosts have evolved resistance in response to the emerging pathogen M. gallisepticum.
In contrast to the evidence supporting the hypothesis of evolved resistance, evidence for the evolution of tolerance in the exposed finch population was more ambiguous. Notably, the gradient of the regression of symptom severity on pathogen load was comparable in birds from exposed and unexposed populations. In other words, because we did not observe the predicted shallower regression slope for finches from exposed populations, our results are ostensibly inconsistent with the hypothesis that range tolerance to M. gallisepticum has evolved (sensu Little et al. ). Nonetheless, on average, birds from exposed populations did exhibit lower clinical symptoms for a given pathogen load, which suggests that a “tolerance” mechanism has evolved to limit damage (i.e., symptom severity). Because we found a difference in reaction norm intercept, but not slope, between unexposed and exposed finch populations, strictly our results would more consistent with the evolution of “point” than “range” tolerance (see, e.g., Graham et al. and references therein for further discussion).
That said, we suggest that the distinction between point and range tolerance might be rather artificial, at least when symptom severity rather than fitness is used on the y‐axis, as we do here (see Box ). Most notably, if one makes the intuitive assumption that potential hosts are asymptomatic prior to infection, then any regression slope of symptom severity on pathogen loads must intercept zero. And, where this is the case, any significant point difference must result from a difference in slopes. Thus, ostensible evidence of point tolerance might be a manifestation of range tolerance, but wherein the range of pathogen loads fail to include zero (we stress this is not necessarily true when fitness used as variation in intercepts is expected even among uninfected individuals). In support, when we forced the regression slopes of symptom severity on pathogen load for the two host populations through zero, we found a significant difference between the slopes: finches from exposed populations showed reduced slope as expected under range tolerance.
Semantics over the definition and labels of tolerance notwithstanding, what is clear is that any change in tolerance that has occurred has been accompanied by a change in resistance. This novel finding based on a large‐scale inoculation experiment using >50 isolates collected throughout the epidemic helps to clarify previous ambiguity in this system. For instance, using a low‐virulence 1994 isolate, Adelman et al. () concluded a significant role for tolerance, but not resistance, because inoculated finches from exposed populations displayed comparable loads than those from unexposed populations, but lower peak eye lesion scores. By contrast, Bonneaud et al. () used a more virulent 2007 strain and found population differences in pathogen load, consistent with the evolution of resistance. We now know that there is substantial among‐isolate variation in peak pathogen load (this study), and that differences in symptom severity between exposed and unexposed host populations are more apparent under infection with late‐epidemic bacterial isolates (Bonneaud et al. ). It therefore seems likely that studies performed on a restricted subset of isolates (typically 1 isolate) will provide an incomplete picture of host evolutionary responses to selection.
We would be surprised if our key finding that both resistance and tolerance have evolved in response to the emerging pathogen were not general. The overarching implication is that although resistance and tolerance can be viewed as distinct host defense strategies, this does not mean they must be either mechanistically independent or mutually exclusive. Indeed, immune cells are increasingly recognized as playing a dual role in resistance and damage‐limitation processes in the broad sense (Wynn and Vannella ; Kubes ). Clearly then there is value in future studies addressing the role of evolved damage‐limitation mechanisms that curtail and resolve immune responses to prevent autoimmunity, remove cellular debris and stimulate tissue repair and regeneration (Wynn and Vannella ). The results of our study highlight that future tests of resistance versus tolerance evolution in response to naturally emerging pathogens require: (i) inoculations with sufficient numbers of pathogen isolates taken from varying time points of the host–pathogen interaction and varying in virulence; and (ii) analyses of pathogen load over a sufficient infection duration to encompass the consequences of both innate and adaptive immune processes. Comparisons of the results presented here with those published previously on the house finch system (Adelman et al. ), including by ourselves (Bonneaud et al. ), suggests that failure to do so is likely to lead to reduced coherence regarding host responses to emerging pathogens.
Not distinguishing between the contributions of resistance and tolerance to evolved host defense will negatively impact the ability to predict coevolutionary dynamics. For instance, although resistance is implicated in antagonistic host–pathogen coevolution (Gandon et al. ; Gandon et al. ), it is often noted that the emergence of tolerance should benefit both parties, allowing interactions to evolve toward commensalism (Roy and Kirchner ; Miller et al. ). In fact, we argue this latter prediction is likely contingent on the assumption that virulence is a by‐product of pathogen replication rates rather than a direct target of selection on pathogens (Anderson and May ; Ebert ; Mackinnon and Read ,b; Gandon et al. ; Miller et al. ). In this case, tolerance alleviates the cost of virulence, thus allowing the pathogen to evolve high replication rates without causing damage to coevolved (tolerant) hosts. However, in M. gallisepticum and many other diseases (e.g., respiratory tract infections with aerosol transmission), increased symptom severity may itself drive higher transmission (Hornef et al. ). If so, the evolution of host tolerance will actually impose selection for increased damage (and so transmission) in coevolved (tolerant) hosts.
In the current context, M. gallisepticum requires virulence because transmission occurs through ocular fluid exudates (Dhondt et al. ), and so depends on the bacterium causing a misdirected inflammatory response to disrupt the mucosal surface of the conjunctiva and respiratory tract (Gaunson et al. ; Lam and DaMassa ; Ganapathy and Bradbury ; Gaunson et al. ). Given that high virulence is broadly expected to favor the evolution of host resistance, while low virulence should favor tolerance (Restif and Koella ), it is intuitive that obligately virulent pathogens should lead to the evolution of resistance. In finches, resistance to M. gallisepticum via an effective cell‐mediated immune response does seem to have evolved, but has likely been accompanied by the ability to resist the pathogen‐driven activation of an inflammatory response (Bonneaud et al. ; Adelman et al. ). This interpretation is consistent with our finding that the slopes of the relationships between pathogen load and clinical symptoms severity (i.e., “range” tolerance) were equivalent between finches from disease‐exposed and unexposed populations, but those from the latter displayed higher symptoms overall.
In conclusion, we provide evidence that house finches have evolved resistance following the infectious outbreak of the bacterial pathogen, M. gallisepticum, with finches from disease‐exposed populations likely reducing pathogen load through acquired immune processes (Bonneaud et al. ). Further, however, we also found evidence to suggest that the ability to tolerate infection and limit damage caused by the pathogen has evolved in tandem with resistance. Thus, while tolerance and resistance have been widely conceptualized as evolutionary alternatives (Råberg ), presumably because of their differing implications for host–pathogen coevolution, from a host perspective they are better viewed as complementary strategies that are likely to evolve together to fight infection and reduce damage.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was supported by a Natural Environment Research Council standard grant to C.B. and A.W. (NE/M00256X). We thank A. Russell, the Associate Editor K. Lythgoe, and two anonymous referees for helpful discussion and/or constructive comments on the manuscript. We thank M. Staley for growing and shipping the pathogen isolates, M. Cook for assisting with bird captures in Arizona, A. Santos, W. R. Hood, and the undergraduates in the Hood lab for assisting with bird captures in Alabama, and A. K. Ziegler for assisting with the experiment in Arizona. The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
CB conceived and designed the study. GEH, KJM, MG, and CB obtained the animals and/or bacterial isolates. MG, KJM, and LT conducted the experiment. LT conducted the molecular work. CB and AW analyzed the data and wrote the paper.
DATA ARCHIVING
Data reported in this paper have been deposited in Dryad Digital Repository (
Associate Editor: K. Lythgoe
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2019. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Understanding how hosts minimize the cost of emerging infections has fundamental implications for epidemiological dynamics and the evolution of pathogen virulence. Despite this, few experimental studies in natural populations have tested whether, in response to disease emergence, hosts evolve resistance, which reduces pathogen load through immune activation, or tolerance, which limits somatic damages without decreasing pathogen load. Further, none has done so accounting for significant natural variation in pathogen virulence, despite known effects on host responses to infection. Here, we investigate whether eastern North American house finches (Haemorhous mexicanus) have evolved resistance and/or tolerance to their emerging bacterial pathogen, Mycoplasma gallisepticum. To do so, we inoculated finches from disease‐exposed and disease‐unexposed populations with 55 distinct isolates of varying virulence. First, although peak pathogen loads, which occurred approximately eight days postinoculation, did not differ between experimentally inoculated finches from disease‐exposed versus unexposed population, pathogen loads subsequently decreased faster and to a greater extent in finches from exposed populations. These results suggest that finches from exposed populations are able to clear the infection through adaptive immune processes. Second, however, finches from exposed populations also displayed lower symptom severity for a given pathogen load, suggesting that a damage‐limitation mechanism, or tolerance, has accompanied the evolution of immune clearance. Our results highlight that resistance and tolerance should be seen as complementary, not alternative, defense strategies: the evolution of resistance benefits from the concomitant evolution of tolerance mechanisms that protect against the damage of immune activation, whereas the evolution of tolerance without resistance will risk runaway selection on pathogen virulence.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Details
1 Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, Penryn, Cornwall, United Kingdom
2 Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, Penryn, Cornwall, United Kingdom; School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona; Current address: Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary Research on Cancer, UMR CNRS/IRD/UM 5290 MIVEGEC, Montpellier, France
3 Department of Biological Sciences, Auburn University, Auburn, Alabama
4 School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona