Abstract
Social wasps find prey mainly using olfactory cues when foraging. Flight area and the ability to return to the nest after this activity was studied for the predatory wasp Polistes lanio lanio (Fabricius) (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) using mark-recapture techniques. One hundred workers of this wasp were removed from 20 nests (5 per nest), marked on the mesothorax, and released after 2 d. Wasps were released at different times and distances to reduce memorization of the flight path. We determined that P lanio lanio workers' hypothetical foraging area was 13.2 km2 based on their greatest return capacity to nests.
Key Words: foraging; nests; social wasps; workers
Resumo
Vespas sociais localizam lagartas usando principalmente pistas olfativas quando forrageando. A área de voo e a capacidade de retornar ao ninho após esta atividade foram estudadas para a vespa predadora Polistes lanio lanio (Fabricius) (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) usando técnicas de marcaçao e recaptura. Cem operárias desta vespa foram removidas de 20 ninhos (5 por ninho) marcadas no mesotórax e liberadas após dois dias. As vespas foram liberadas em diferentes momentos e distancias para reduzir a memorizaçao do trajeto do voo. Nós determinamos que a área hipotética de forrageamento das operárias de P lanio lanio foi de 13.2 km2 com base em sua maior capacidade de retorno aos ninhos.
Palavras Chave: forrageamento; ninhos; operárias; vespas sociais
Polistinae are primitive eusocial insects forming colonies with dominant, reproductive, active, and subordinate foraging individuals (De Souza & Prezoto 2012). Polistes lanio lanio (Fabricius) (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) occurs from Guyana to Argentina (Richards 1978) with independent nest formation, and is active throughout the yr, principally in urban areas (Braun et al. 2008). All social insects must search for food resources (proteins, carbohydrates), water, and material for building nests (e.g., plant fibers and clay) (Richter 2000; Nascimento et al. 2015; Brügger et al. 2017, 2019a, b). Therefore, environmental factors such as humidity, sunlight, and temperature regulate these activities (Kovac et al. 2018). Foraging is the most complex behavior of social wasps (Richter 2000) with a behavioral sequence of travel, searching, preying, loading, return, and discharging the load in the nest (Brügger et al. 2019a). However, we believe that flight capacity of social wasps should be studied to define their foraging area (Mandal et al. 2017; Masciocchi et al. 2018) and to determine the predatory potential of these insects (Dejean 2017). Here we report on investigations of the flight area and return capacity of P lanio lanio workers in Seropédica, Rio de Janeiro State, Brazil.
Materials and Methods
This study was carried out from Jan 2004 to Jun 2005 at the Centro Integrado de Manejo de Pragas (CIMP) in the Universidade Federal Rural do Rio de Janeiro in Seropédica, Rio de Janeiro State, Brazil. Five post-emergent workers from 20 field colonies of P lanio lanio (100 individuals) were collected with an aerial net. Wasps were anesthetized for 25 s, marked on the mesothorax with 3 different colors using a Ceramic Twin Marker (water-based) (Marvy Uchida, Tokyo, Japan) (Fig. 1) to identify colony and individual wasp. Wasps were then placed in 500 mL clear polyethylene pots for 2 d with a 6.0 cm opening covered with cotton. Pots containing wasps were transported in black bags to release sites located at 450, 800, 1,000, 1,300, 1,400, 1,500, and 2,050 m from their colonies and released at different times to reduce memorization of flight path. Colonies were monitored for 72 h after wasp release.
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
The return speed of the wasps was obtained by dividing return time by distance of the initial release point. The hypothetical flight area of P lanio lanio was calculated with the equation: A = p x r2, where p is the constant and r the longest return distance, or theoretical flight range in relation to the central point of the respective nests (Prezoto & Gobbi 2005). Return speed data were obtained by dividing return time by release point distance of foraging workers. These data were transformed into cosine arc (x/100) 0.5 and subjected to analysis of variance; means were compared using the Scott-Knott test (P< 0.05) (Scott and Knott 1974).
Results
Polistes lanio lanio workers from 19 colonies returned to their nest and performed trophallaxis with the queen as the first behavior after arrival. Mean return time ranged from 0.6 to 24.3 h, with a travel speed of 4.8 to 62.4 m per s at the release distances of 450 to 1,400 m (Table 1). Foraging areas increased with increasing release distance; the largest forage area was 13.2 km2 and the largest release distance was 2.050 km2.
Discussion
The return distance of P lanio lanio from foraging confirms the capacity of Vespidae to travel great distances. Indeed, other authors have reported similar behavior for Polistes versicolor Olivier (Hymenoptera: Vespidae), with a maximum distance of 850 m and effective range of 200 m (Gobbi 1978). Ugolini (1986) reported that Polistes gallicus (Linnaeus) (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) had return distances of 300 to 400 m and > 600 m. Santos et al. (1994) found that Polistes canadensis canadensis (Linnaeus) (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) exhibited foraging distances from 50 to 250 m with 80% return of workers.
The foraging area of P lanio lanio workers and the increase of return time with distance from the release point was expected. A similar observation also was reported for Vespula germanica (Fab.) (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) by Moreyra et al. (2016). Wasps with resources such as prey, nectar, wood, and water reduce return time (Prezoto & Gobbi 2005). We also noted in our area that buildings and the Atlantic Forest may reduce return speed of P lanio lanio, because the absence of these 2 obstacles increased the speed of return to its respective colony. Golding et al. (2005) found that return speed of Vespula vulgaris (Linnaeus) (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) was lower, because they move back and forth in front of flowers before landing for feeding. In our study, P lanio lanio workers returning from the 2,050 m release point reinforced the notion that wasps use visual cues for reference points when returning to the colony. Indeed, Mandal et al. (2017) reported that foraging Ropalidia marginata (Lepeletier) (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) may travel 1,500 m to accomplish the same task. The foraging area of 13.2 km2 for P lanio lanio may be directly related to its larger body size. This was similarly observed by Prezoto and Gobbi (2005) where forage areas for Polistes simillimus Zikán (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) and Angipolybia pallens (Lepeletier) (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) were reported to be 7.6 and 1.8 km2, respectively.
Obstacles between P lanio lanio workers and their colony may reduce return speed, because this behavior depends on factors such as environmental preferences as it relates to nesting requirements and resource availability (Kasper et al. 2008; Moreyra et al. 2017). We found that P lanio lanio workers may return to their nests from long distances, although with greater speed up to 450 m. The return time of this wasp did not vary directly with nest distance due to the absence of obstacles along the flight path. However, the individual needs of each colony also influence this behavior.
Acknowledgments
We thank the following Brazilian agencies "Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Cientifico e Tecnológico, Coordenaçao de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nivel Superior (Finance Code 001), Funda- çâo de Amparo a Pesquisa do Estado de Minas Gerais" and "Programa Cooperativo sobre Proteçâo Florestal do Instituto de Pesquisas e Estudos Florestais" for scholarships and financial support.
*Corresponding author; E-mail: [email protected]
References Cited
Braun MR, Loyola RD, Kubota U, Perre P. 2008. Individual recognition and aggressive behavior in the paper wasp Polistes lanio (Hymenoptera: Vespidae). Sociobiology 51: 741-748.
Brügger BP, Castro BMC, Prezoto F, Serrao JE, Zanuncio JC. 2017. Feeding by the social wasp Polybia scutellaris (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) on Syzygium jambos (Myrtaceae) fruits in Minas Gerais, Brazil. Florida Entomologist 100: 172-173.
Brügger BP, Alcántara-de la Cruz R, De Carvalho AG, Soares MA, Prezoto F, Zanuncio JC. 2019a. Polybia fasticiosuscula (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) foraging activity patterns. Florida Entomologist 102: 264-265.
Brügger BP, Prezoto F, De Souza LSA, Zanuncio AJV, Soares MA. 2019b. Use of fruit juice as a method for the collection of social wasps. Florida Entomologist 102: 592-595.
Dejean A, Rodriguez-Perez H, Carpenter JM, Azémar F, Corbara B. 2017. The predatory behavior of the Neotropical social wasp Polybia rejecta. Behavioural Processes 140: 161-168.
De Souza AR, Prezoto F. 2012. Aggressive interactions for a decentralized regulation of foraging activity in the social wasp Polistes versicolor. Insectes Sociaux 59: 463-467.
Golding YC, Edmunds M, Ennos AR. 2005. Flight behaviour during foraging of the social wasp Vespula vulgaris (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) and four mimetic hoverflies (Diptera: Syrphidae) Sericomyia silentis, Myathropa florea, Helophilus sp. and Syrphus sp. Journal of Experimental Biology 208: 4523-4527.
Gobbi N. 1978. Determinaçao do raio de voo de operárias de Polistes versicolor (Hymenoptera, Vespidae). Ciencia e Cultura 30: 364-365.
Kasper ML, Reeson AF, Mackay DA, Austin AD. 2008. Environmental factors influencing daily foraging activity of Vespula germanica (Hymenoptera, Vespidae) in Mediterranean Australia. Insectes Sociaux 55: 288-295.
Kovac H, Stabentheiner A, Brodschneider R. 2018. Foraging strategy of wasps- optimisation of intake rate or energetic efficiency? Journal of Experimental Biology 221: jeb174169. doi: 10.1242/jeb.174169
Mandal S, Brahma A, Gadagkar R. 2017. Homing in a tropical social wasp: role of spatial familiarity, motivation and age. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 203: 915-927.
Masciocchi M, Martinez AS, Pereira AJ, Villacide JM, Corley JC. 2018. Dispersal behavior of yellowjacket (Vespula germanica) queens. Insect Science 25: 109-116.
Moreyra S, D'Adamo P, Lozada M. 2016. Learning in Vespula germanica social wasps: situations of unpredictable food locations. Insectes Sociaux 63: 381-384.
Moreyra S, D'Adamo P, Lozada M. 2017. Long-term spatial memory in Vespula germanica social wasps: the influence of past experience on foraging behavior. Insect Science 24: 853-858.
Nascimento S, Canale GR, Da Silva DJ. 2015. Abelhas euglossina (Hymenoptera: Apidae) associadas a monocultura de eucalipto no cerrado mato-grossense. Revista Árvore 39: 263-273.
Prezoto F, Gobbi N. 2005. Flight range extension in Polistes simillimus Zikán, 1951 (Hymenoptera, Vespidae). Brazilian Archives of Biology and Technology 48: 947-950.
Richards OW. 1978. The social wasps of the Americas excluding the Vespinae. British Museum (Natural History), London, United Kingdom.
Richter MR. 2000. Social wasp (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) foraging behavior. Annual Review of Entomology 45: 121-150.
Santos GDM, Marques OM, Carvalho CD. 1994. Raio de açao de Polistes canadensis canadensis (L., 1758) (Hymenoptera, Vespidae). Insecta 3: 20-24.
Scott AJ, Knott M. 1974. A cluster analysis method for grouping means in the analysis of variance. Biometrics 30: 507-512.
Ugolini A. 1986. Homing ability in Polistes gallicus (L.) (Hymenoptera Vespidae). Monitore Zoologico Italiano-Italian Journal of Zoology 20: 1-15.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2020. This work is published under https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0 (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Social wasps find prey mainly using olfactory cues when foraging. Flight area and the ability to return to the nest after this activity was studied for the predatory wasp Polistes lanio lanio (Fabricius) (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) using mark-recapture techniques. One hundred workers of this wasp were removed from 20 nests (5 per nest), marked on the mesothorax, and released after 2 d. Wasps were released at different times and distances to reduce memorization of the flight path. We determined that P lanio lanio workers' hypothetical foraging area was 13.2 km2 based on their greatest return capacity to nests.